BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychiatry, 23 June 2025

Sec. Autism

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1540985

Visual recursion without recursive language? a case study of a minimally verbal autistic child

  • 1Department of Catalan Philology and General Linguistics, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 2Center for Music in the Brain, Aarhus University & Royal Academy of Music Aarhus/Aalborg, Aarhus, Denmark
  • 3Center for Brain and Cognition, Pompeu Fabra University, Barcelona, Spain
  • 4SCAN-Unit, Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

The human faculty to generate an infinite set of structured expressions in language, present in most cultures and normal ontogeny, is the most substantial evidence of the human capacity for recursion. In contrast, strong evidence of this capacity in other domains has been sparse, inviting the speculation that recursion is primarily linguistic and co-opted into other domains. Here, we present a case report of a minimally verbal 11-y.o. autistic child with poor language comprehension whose speech rarely exceeds two-word commands despite remarkable hyperlexia (i.e., mechanical reading in Spanish, Catalan, and English) and a visually-based, mainly nominal lexicon acquired through reading. Importantly, medium-range scores in visual tasks and hyperlexia suggest that he can detect complex visual patterns despite low fluid intelligence. Against this background, we tested whether this child could represent recursive hierarchical embedding in vision, despite no evidence of it in language. We found that 1) his accuracy was above chance and 2) it was not significantly different from that of typically developing children. Accordingly, we suggest that a core capacity of recursion, interfacing with a sensory modality and a visuospatial conceptual system, is sufficient to process recursive patterns in vision. In contrast, linguistic recursion may require more complex sensorimotor and conceptual-intentional machinery.

Introduction

The human Faculty of Language allows the generation of an infinite set of structured expressions out of a finite set of words and rules. This property is assumed to result from a recursive process that embeds expressions within other expressions (1), creating complex hierarchical objects. The property of representing recursive hierarchical embedding (henceforth recursion) has been claimed in various domains (17). A crucial question remains whether recursion is domain-general, multiply-domain-specific (one recursive capacity per domain), or primarily linguistic and then used in other domains through linguistic computations (810).

In typical development cases and most populations, recursion emerges in syntax (1, 1113) and at the level of linguistic discourse (1416). This widespread and effortless use has led to the hypothesis that recursion is primarily linguistic, with its use in other domains dependent on the human Faculty of Language (17). Recursion has also been proposed in the domains of action planning (1821), visuospatial processing (22), Theory of Mind (ToM) (23, 24), and music (25, 26). However, the prevalence, hierarchical depth, and automaticity of use in these domains seem limited compared to language.

Recursion has also been investigated in atypical development. Some studies have investigated the contribution of linguistic recursion to second-order ToM in autism (27, 28). Their results show that recursive syntax (sentential complementation) predicts second-order false beliefs (28) and that training with sentential complementation improves ToM, with most gains for children with mild autism (27). However, this relationship might be limited to explicit ToM and explained by the role of working memory in both domains (2830).

In line with findings on specific autistic cognition, language development is heterogeneous in autism (31). Highly atypical developmental trajectories have been reported, including cases where missing early communication skills, such as joint attention, do not preclude the emergence of structural language (32). Similarly, literacy skills suggestive of implicit meaning-making have been observed in nonspeaking autistic individuals, offering a potential alternative route to language acquisition (33). As for syntax, syntactic deficits can occur even when vocabulary and sentence repetition are intact (3436), and 20-30% of individuals with autism are non-verbal or minimally verbal (37). In this subgroup, linguistic recursion is absent, similar to cases in which children were not exposed to language in critical periods (see 38). In the latter, though, fluency and lexical abilities are preserved.

Linguistic comprehension in non-verbal and minimally verbal individuals with autism is vastly understudied. Slušná et al. (39) studied individuals with non- or minimally verbal autism and has found that expressive language (General level of no echolalic language, measured with the ADOS; see Table 1 for details) correlates with receptive language (or verbal mental age, measured with the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–III (PPVT), or with the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF) when PPVT score reached a verbal mental age (VMA) of 3 years) but not with non-verbal IQ. This study also shows a strong association between sense-making/ComFor (Forerunners in Communication) and Verbal Mental Age/Receptive Language.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of (diagnostic) tests applied to A. Testing was done in the context of the Slušná et al. (39) study on non-/minimally verbal individuals with ASD.

In another study, Vicente et al. (40) investigated whether autistic adults with minimally receptive vocabulary could comprehend two-word phrases presented auditorily where a familiar noun is modified by a familiar adjective (e.g., red broom). They found that only one out of eight participants succeeded. Interestingly, the pattern was reversed with a visual version of the same task, as all but one participant could form simple symbolic compositions with iconic representations in all trials. This opens the question of whether visual-spatial recursion could also be available to minimally verbal populations with autism and, more broadly, whether language is necessary for recursion in vision.

Previous work on the relationship between language and visual recursion paints an ambiguous picture. On the one hand, verbal or motor interference does not affect the capacity to perform adequately in a visual recursion task (10). Moreover, when testing well-trained participants with motor, visual, and music recursion, fMRI experiments show domain-specific patterns of brain activity (5, 7, 41). Thus, each domain could implement similar recursive computations on independent biological systems (8, 9). On the other hand, when testing untrained participants, the behavioral and neural evidence is compatible with shared resources in acquiring recursive rules across domains (6, 42, 43). For instance, stroke patients with deficits in parsing sentences with two levels of embedding are also impaired in the processing of visual recursion, and visual recursion seems to activate a left-lateralized brain network, including the classic language brain areas (42, 43). However, these studies were performed in adults with fully developed language.

Perhaps recursion is domain-general at first, but acquires specializations depending on the degree of exposure and training with specific domains. The adult neural pattern associated with processing natural and artificial languages only emerges after extensive training and in specific development stages (4447). More extensive exposure and training could explain why recursion is expressed first and more prevalently in language than in other domains. However, recursion might also become available later to other domains.

In our previous work in the visual domain, we found that children could master visual recursion around age 9 but not before (48). This is later than the acquisition of syntax. However, the age of acquisition of recursion may vary across linguistic forms, being available for simple adjectives and verbal compounds at age 2–3 and more complex sentence complements at age 5-7 (49). We also found that prior acquisition of a simpler iterative task was necessary to master recursion within the same developmental stage (48). This effect resembles the acquisition constraints in language, in which conjunctive constructions must be mastered before recursion (50, 51). In conjunction and iteration, items are added within a fixed hierarchical level without generating new levels (e.g., very, very, very big).

Interestingly, we also found that grammar comprehension (controlling for IQ) equally predicted accuracy in visual recursion and iteration. However, these grammar comprehension tasks included sentences with a maximum of one embedded clause. Unlike our study with stroke patients mentioned above, they were not designed to test for the similarities between linguistic and visual recursion (42).

This manuscript explores the relationship between visual and syntactic recursion from a developmental perspective by presenting a case report from an 11-year-old child (A.) with autism and hyperlexia (mechanical reading), whose linguistic skills do not exceed those of minimally verbal individuals despite possessing a visually-based, predominantly nominal vocabulary acquired by reading. We investigated whether visual recursion can dissociate from linguistic syntax by testing A. with a visual recursion task and a control visual iteration task. We compared his accuracy levels with 26 Typical Development (TD) children attending the 4th grade. If linguistic recursion is a prerequisite for visual recursion, we would expect A. to fail in the former. However, he could still succeed in the control iteration task due to his preserved visual cognitive performance. Conversely, if recursion in vision is not dependent on language, we expect A. to pass both recursion and iteration tasks.

Methods

Case report

A. is a minimally verbal 11-year-old child with autism and hyperlexia. Crucially, a minimally verbal condition includes a lack of language comprehension. A.’s language knowledge consists of absent spontaneous production and a receptive vocabulary biased towards nouns (and a few adjectives) corresponding to objects (and properties) that can be represented in pictures. His language abilities appeared for the first time at the age of 2.6 y.o. in the form of precocious oral reading of words as captions of object pictures presented in cards. His speech is infrequent and, when not echolalic, comprises a maximum of two-word combinations for requests (e.g., ‘batido rosa’, which means ‘pink shake’ in English). A slower and over-articulated simplified Spanish has spontaneously emerged in his family to ensure that A. understands the behavioral regulatory instructions that the linguistic exchange with him mainly consists of. In this family code, verb and noun grammar is kept to a minimum, wh-questions are replaced with simplified alternative questions (e.g., A., pear, or banana)?, and no pronouns exist. Like his minimally verbal (and nonverbal) peers, A. has neither declarative pointing nor iconic gestures. Unlike them, he does not use alternative augmentative communication systems (e.g., PECS, Picture Exchange Communication System).

A.’s score on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III), which yields a verbal mental age of 4.3 years, stems from his proficiency in labeling pictures with nouns, thus clearly overstating his linguistic knowledge. In contrast, 4-year-olds produce and understand complex utterances and are active conversationalists. Moreover, previous research has shown that while PPVT vocabulary knowledge scores and verbal mental age correlate well in normal development, this correlation is less clear in autism (52).

Hyperlexia occurs when reading decoding skills surpass reading comprehension (53, 54). While autism and hyperlexia are strongly associated (53), the combination of hyperlexia and minimally verbal autism is exceptional; it has only been described once in the literature, but in a much younger autistic child of 4 years of age (see 55). Hyperlexia-based interventions in verbal autism seem to improve comprehension but not expressive language (56), which aligns with A.’s extremely poor expressive language despite hyperlexia allowing him to fluently read aloud in Spanish, Catalan, and English.

A. received his autism spectrum disorder diagnosis at Sant Joan de Déu Hospital in Barcelona (a children’s hospital) at the age of two. An overview of his assessment is shown in Table 1. His gross motor milestones were achieved at the lower end of the normal age range, while his fine motor skills, influenced by hand hypotonia and palm hypersensitivity, proved atypical. Although A. can respond to joint attention, initiating it is challenging. He was evaluated with the ADI-r (Autism Diagnostic Interview-revised), the ComFor (a test probing which augmentative communication system is more appropriate for the tested individual, 57), and the Leiter-3 (Leiter International Performance Scale, measuring non-verbal intelligence). Notably, A. demonstrated deficits in social interaction (27; cutoff 10), communication (14; cutoff 8), restricted and repetitive behaviors (10; cutoff 3), and atypical development detectable by 36 months or before (3; cutoff 1), based on ADI-r scoring, consistent with his ASD diagnosis. A. reached the representation level in the scale of “perception and sense-making of non transient forms of communication” measured by the ComFor (58). This score is similar to his nonverbal/minimally verbal peers. On Leiter-3’s subtests assessing fluid intelligence, A. scored 79 IQ points. In the specific subtasks, he scored 5 in Figure-Ground, a visual interference task where the target object is embedded in an increasingly complex background; 5 in Form Completion, where the subject must arrange parts in a whole; 8 in Classification and Analogies, which tests pattern analysis and prediction of “what goes next”; and 6 in the capacity to analyze Sequential Order (59).

Finally, A. has a remarkable ability to type and copy colorful computer displays. Thanks to his familiarization with computers and screens, we tested him with a tactile screen presenting the visual recursion and iteration tasks.

Visual recursion task (REC) and visual iteration task (ITE)

In the visual Recursion task (REC) (Figure 1A), participants are shown a sequence of three images (steps 1, 2, and 3) depicting the generation of a visual fractal and asked to discriminate, from two choices, the image corresponding to the correct continuation of the previous sequence of three (i.e., the fourth step). One of the choices is the correct image, and the other is a foil (Figure 1B). Stimuli varied along two categories, each with three levels: visual complexity (the number of increasing elements: 3, 4, or 5) and type of foils. The latter could be ‘odd constituent’ foils (two elements within the whole hierarchy were misplaced), ‘positional error’ foils (all elements within new hierarchical levels were internally consistent but inconsistent with the previous iterations), and ‘repetition’ foils (no additional iterative step was performed after the third iteration). Combined, these two categories generated 9 different kinds of stimuli. The tasks comprised three instances of each of these nine kinds, resulting in 27 trials.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental paradigm [adapted from (42)]. (A) The presentation of the visual recursion/iteration task (REC, ITE) comprised 4 steps, including a successive presentation of steps 1 to 3 at the top of the screen, followed by the two options for a forced choice at the bottom. The location of the correct image was randomized and counterbalanced (e.g., left in the ITE and right in the REC example provided). (B) Examples of fractals used in REC. There were different categories of ‘visual complexity’ — 3, 4, and 5 — and different categories of foils (the task comprised three trials of each of the nine categories). In ‘odd constituent’ foils, two elements within the whole hierarchy were misplaced —foil, second panel step 4; in ‘positional error’ foils, all elements within new hierarchical levels were internally consistent but inconsistent with the previous iterations —foil, second panel step 4; in ‘repetition’ foils, no additional iterative step was performed after the third iteration —foil, second panel step 4.

Participants presented their answers by touching their choice directly on the screen within a maximum of 30 seconds, after which there was a trial timeout. No response feedback was given. The total duration of the task (27 trials) was about 12 minutes. The task is an adaptation of what is used and described in detail elsewhere (5, 42, 48). The task is well-validated and designed to prevent the use of simple visual strategies (22). For example, choosing the image more similar to step 3 would yield incorrect answers as the repetition foil is identical to the latter. In previous work, we found that low-frequency visual information was insufficient to determine the correct choice (10), rendering simple Gestalt-based strategies unlikely. Finally, we found that REC’s scores correlate with correctly representing hierarchical information in the musical, action, linguistic, and logic domains when the variance shared with simple iteration is accounted for (6, 42, 43).

The control task, the visual Iteration task (ITE), also involved a stepwise procedure applied to hierarchical structures without recursive embedding. However, in ITE, additional elements are added to a preexisting hierarchical structure without producing new hierarchical levels (Figure 1A, bottom right). As for REC, understanding this stepwise procedure is necessary to predict the next step correctly.

Procedure

The procedure comprised one training session and two test sessions. In the training session, comprised of 20 trials, A. was habituated to the touchscreen apparatus and tested with simple (non-recursive) geometrical reasoning tasks. Similarly to REC and ITE, he learned to follow the consecutive presentation of three figures on the upper part of the screen and touch the correct figure from the two alternatives at the bottom. The trials provided success or error auditory feedback. In the first 8 trials, he was asked to identify the identity of geometric shapes (e.g., three circles on the top row, then a circle vs. a square in the bottom row). In the following 12 trials, he had to grasp how a pattern was incrementally built from an initial image in the top row consisting of one central big circle alone (4 trials), one big and one small circle (4 trials), or one big and two small circles (4 trials). Each initial arrangement was sequentially incremented in the top row, yielding 1, 2, 3 circles; 2, 3, 4 circles; and 3, 4, 5 circles, respectively. The participant then had to choose between the two bottom images: the correct one with the congruent increment (i.e., 4 circles, 5 circles, and 6 circles, respectively) and the incorrect one with no increment (i.e., repeating the third occurrence of the top row).

Feedback sounds were removed from the test sessions. Five months later, the first test session was performed, which comprised ITE and REC. Three months later, the second session comprised REC, followed by ITE, presenting the stimuli in a different order.

Videos of the testing sessions will be available on demand with the permission of A.’s parents.

We compared A.’s performance to 26 children with Typical Development (TD) (11 female, ages 9-10, 4th-grade monolingual students from Austria). These children were tested with REC and ITE in a counterbalanced order (13 children ITE-REC and 13 REC-ITE) and were part of a previous study (48).

We analyzed and visualized the recorded data using Jamovi (60), Matlab (61), and Raincloud plots (62).

Results

Accuracy

In the first test session, A. hesitated to answer the initial 10 ITE trials and timed out after 30 seconds (see Reaction Times in Supplementary Figure S1). We labeled these trials as “incorrect” in terms of accuracy to apply a stringent criterion. Across both sessions, ITE accuracy was 64.8% and REC 72.2%, both above chance (ITE binomial GLM intercept: z = 9.97, p <.001; REC: z = 11.80, p <.001) (Figure 2). There were no significant associations between correct answers and the order of each session, neither for ITE (first session 59.3%; second session 70.4%, chi-square test of independence: χ 2 (1,54) = 0.73, p = .393) nor for REC (first session 77.8%, second session 66.7%; χ 2 (1, 54) = 0.83, p = .362). These results indicate that A. performed adequately and equally well in REC and ITE.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Percentage of correct responses given by A. and the children with Typical Development (TD). Both groups were tested with the visual recursion task (REC) and the visual Iteration task (ITE), with counterbalanced order. A.’s first session order was ITE-REC (light color), and the second session was REC-ITE (dark color). The dashed line marks 50% accuracy as the chance level..

Comparison with children with typical development

A.’s mean accuracy did not differ from children with TD of similar age, neither in ITE (64.8 ± 7.8%, vs. 77.9 ± 18.0%; independent sample t-test: t(26) = -1.007, p = .323), nor in REC (72.3 ± 7.8% vs. 79.6%, ± 21.2%; t(26) = -0.482, p = .634). Following the procedure in Huber et al. (63) to compare case reports with control groups, we ran a Generalized Mixed Model to ascertain the effects of Task (REC, ITE), Group (A., children with TD), and their interaction on the likelihood of correct responses (1/0). As random effects, we included Task: ID and 1:Trial. Confirming the t-test analysis, we found neither differences between groups (B = 0.8, 95% CI [0.28, 18.2], z= 0.75, p= .45), tasks (B = 0.33, 95% CI [0.71, 2.76], z= 0.96, p= .34), nor an interaction between group and task (B = -0.04, 95% CI [0.24, 3.75], z= -0.06, p= .95).

While we could not compare the reaction times between A. and the children with TD due to A. multitasking behavior (see the comments and data visualizations in Supplementary Materials Figure S1), we were able to explore the effects of presentation order of the tests. As in Martins et al. (48), the tests showed that the order ITE-REC (i.e., iteration test first, recursion test second) may have benefited children with TD (see Supplementary Materials Table S1), but not A., who performed slightly worse in the first session with the ITE-REC order (see Supplementary Materials; Figures S2, S3; Tables S2, S3).

Discussion

We presented a case report of an 11 y.o. minimally verbal but hyperlexic autistic child who can represent visual recursion despite showing no evidence of this capacity in linguistic production or comprehension. His accuracy was above chance and was not significantly different from that of 9–10 y.o. children with TD. This suggests that visual recursion is not dependent on a primary linguistic recursive capacity but could be domain-general or multiply domain-specific.

These findings resonate with prior research on autistic cognition, which shows marked dissociations between verbal and nonverbal intelligence (64). Many minimally verbal autistic individuals demonstrate significantly higher scores on nonverbal reasoning tasks such as Raven’s Progressive Matrices despite low scores on verbal subtests of traditional IQ measures. A.’s case similarly suggests that the capacity for recursion may be preserved in visuospatial domains while remaining inaccessible in language.

Within the generative framework (65), language recursion is the core combinatorial capacity that builds an “infinite array of expressions” that are interpreted at two interfaces, a conceptual-intentional (C-I) system connecting the same mental expressions to reasoning and planning among “other activities of the internalized mental world” and a sensorimotor (S-M) system connecting with the external world via language production and perception (externalization). We surmise that A.’s impairment in linguistic recursion might result from deficits in both the C-I and the S-M systems required for language, which may prevent recursion to manifest linguistically.

Visual recursion is likely to hinge on different machinery. First, it does not involve an intentional component. Second, it has a sensory component but does not involve a structured motor output like linguistic utterances. Thus, while language recursion requires intact loops involving core, conceptual-intentional, and sensorimotor systems, visual recursion can operate with core and visual sensory and conceptual systems without intentional and motor components. As reviewed above, visual conceptual and combinatorial properties might be preserved in minimally verbal autism (40).

This interpretation remains agnostic regarding whether recursion is domain-general or multiply domain-specific (10). However, it hypothetically (but not necessarily) allows the same core capacity to interface with specialized sensorimotor/sensory and conceptual-intentional/conceptual systems. In this regard, A.’s hyperlexia suggests a strong visual sensory system and the ability to associate visual patterns with vocal labels but without linguistic semantics. Interestingly, although his visual IQ was low in most subtasks in the Leiter-3, he had a medium-range score in the subtest Classification and Analogies, which explicitly assesses the capacity for general pattern analysis and prediction of “what goes next”. The latter can indicate a capacity for recognizing abstract visual patterns and rules (i.e., a visual conceptual system).

In sum, specialization of the sensory and conceptual abilities to the visual domain could afford the interaction of these systems with a core combinatorial capacity and the detection of visual recursion. Conversely, a faulty integration of the subcomponents making the C-I and S-M systems would impede expressive and receptive recursion in language.

This study has two significant limitations. First, broader generalizations are impossible, as this case report presents a single subject with a rare profile: he is a minimally verbal autistic child with hyperlexia in 3 languages. Second, we did not directly test A. with a linguistic recursion comprehension task. As such, we do not know whether A. could demonstrate recursion in a reading or writing task, should he be taught to read or write. Recent evidence suggests that nonspeaking autistic adolescents and adults may display implicit knowledge of English orthography in response time-based spelling tasks, suggesting foundational literacy skills (33). However, it is unclear whether this implicit capacity for orthography translates into semantic and, more importantly, syntactic understanding (66). Here, we infer limited syntactic recursion from A.’s limited expressive compositionality (i.e., two-word phrase requests at most; e.g., pink milkshake) and his low (and likely overestimated) score on the vocabulary test PPVT. Since previous research has shown an association between receptive and expressive language (39, 52) and very limited phrase comprehension in minimally verbal autism (40), we adduce that A. was also limited in linguistic recursion.

Finally, it is important to acknowledge that a lack of functional language might stem from impairments at either abstract computational abilities, such as recursion, or from an absence of verbs and/or grammar, which are fundamental and specific to language. Our results show that recursion is present, at least in vision, albeit possibly made available by simpler domain-specific machinery. However, it is also possible that a domain-general system is not available to language due to the latter set of limitations.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: https://osf.io/aju83/.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the IRB of the Dpt. of Catalan Philology and General Linguistics, University of Barcelona. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s), and minor(s)’ legal guardian/next of kin, for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

JR: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AC-M: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MM: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. ACM was supported by the Spanish Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad FPI grant (BES-2014-070547) and the Danish National Research Foundation (DNRF117).

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank A. and his family, especially his mother, for helping to keep A. focused on the experiment. We also thank Elisabet Vila Borrellas for conducting the ADI-r interview with A.’s mother. Finally, we acknowledge the FIDMAG Research Foundation for providing the touchscreen laptop used in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1540985/full#supplementary-material.

References

1. Hauser MD, Chomsky N, and Fitch WT. The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Sci (New York N.Y.). (2002) 298:1569–79. doi: 10.1126/science.298.5598.1569

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Greenfield PM. Language, tools and brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior. Behav Brain Sci. (1991) 14:531–51. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00071235

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Jackendoff R and Pinker S. The nature of the language faculty and its implications for evolution of language (Reply to Fitch, Hauser, and Chomsky). Cognition. (2005) 97:211–25. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.04.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Lerdahl F and Jackendoff RS. A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge, Massachusetts, US: MIT Press (1982).

Google Scholar

5. Martins MD, Fischmeister FP, Puig-Waldmüller E, Oh J, Geißler A, Robinson S, et al. Fractal image perception provides novel insights into hierarchical cognition. NeuroImage. (2014) 96:300–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.064

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Martins MD, Gingras B, Puig-Waldmueller E, and Fitch WT. Cognitive representation of “musical fractals”: Processing hierarchy and recursion in the auditory domain. Cognition. (2017) 161:31–45. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.01.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Martins MD, Bianco R, Sammler D, and Villringer A. Recursion in action: An fMRI study on the generation of new hierarchical levels in motor sequences. Hum Brain Mapp. (2019) 40:2623–38. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24549

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Dehaene S, Al Roumi F, Lakretz Y, Planton S, and Sablé-Meyer M. Symbols and mental programs: A hypothesis about human singularity. Trends Cogn Sci. (2022) 26:751–66. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.06.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Fedorenko E and Shain C. Similarity of computations across domains does not imply shared implementation: the case of language comprehension. Curr Dir psychol Sci. (2021) 30:526–34. doi: 10.1177/09637214211046955

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Martins MD, Muršič Z, Oh J, and Fitch WT. Representing visual recursion does not require verbal or motor resources. Cogn Psychol. (2015) 77:20–41. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.01.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Berwick RC and Chomsky N. Why only us: language and evolution. Cambridge, Massachusetts, US: MIT Press (2016).

Google Scholar

12. Chomsky N. Syntactic structures. In: Syntactic structures. Berlin , Germany: De Gruyter Mouton (1957). doi: 10.1515/9783112316009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Friederici AD, Chomsky N, Berwick RC, Moro A, and Bolhuis JJ. Language, mind and brain. Nat Hum Behav. (2017) 1:713–22. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0184-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Everett DL. What does Pirahã grammar have to teach us about human language and the mind? WIREs Cogn Sci. (2012) 3:555–63. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1195

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Levinson SC. Recursion in pragmatics. Language. (2013) 89:149–62. doi: 10.1353/lan.2013.0005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Watumull J, Hauser MD, Roberts IG, and Hornstein N. On recursion. Front Psychol. (2014) 4:1017. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Fitch WT, Hauser MD, and Chomsky N. The evolution of the language faculty: Clarifications and implications. Cognition. (2005) 97:179–210. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Barceló-Coblijn L and Gomila A. Evidence of recursion in tool use. Behav Brain Sci. (2012) 35:219–20. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X11001865

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Corballis M. C.. The recursive mind: The origins of human language, thought, and civilization-updated edition In New Jersey: The Recursive Mind. Princeton University Press. (2014).

Google Scholar

20. Jackendoff R. What is the human language faculty?: Two views. Language. (2011) 87:586–624. doi: 10.1353/lan.2011.0063

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Vicari G and Adenzato M. Is recursion language-specific? Evidence of recursive mechanisms in the structure of intentional action. Consciousness Cogn. (2014) 26:169–88. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.03.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Martins MD, Martins IP, and Fitch WT. A novel approach to investigate recursion and iteration in visual hierarchical processing. Behav Res Methods. (2015) 48:1421–42. doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0657-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. De Villiers J, Hobbs K, and Hollebrandse B. Recursive complements and propositional attitudes. In: Roeper T and Speas M, editors. Recursion: complexity in cognition, vol. 43. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing (2014). p. 221–42. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-05086-7_10

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. De Villiers J and De Villiers PA. The role of language in theory of mind development. Topics Lang Disord. (2014) 34:313–28. doi: 10.1097/TLD.0000000000000037

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Lerdahl F and Jackendoff R. An overview of hierarchical structure in music. Music Perception. (1983) 1:229–52. doi: 10.2307/40285257

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Rohrmeier M. Towards a generative syntax of tonal harmony. J Mathematics Music. (2011) 5:35–53. doi: 10.1080/17459737.2011.573676

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Durrleman S, Bentea A, Prisecaru A, Thommen E, and Delage H. Training syntax to enhance theory of mind in children with ASD. J Autism Dev Disord. (2023) 53:2444–57. doi: 10.1007/s10803-022-05507-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Polyanskaya I, Eigsti I-M, Brauner T, and Blackburn P. Second-order false beliefs and linguistic recursion in autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2022) 52:3991–4006. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05277-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Grosse Wiesmann C, Schreiber J, Singer T, Steinbeis N, and Friederici AD. White matter maturation is associated with the emergence of Theory of Mind in early childhood. Nat Commun. (2017) 8(1):14692. doi: 10.1038/ncomms14692

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Grosse Wiesmann C, Friederici AD, Singer T, and Steinbeis N. Two systems for thinking about others’ thoughts in the developing brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2020) 117:6928–35. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1916725117

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Gernsbacher MA, Morson EM, and Grace EJ. Chapter 70—Language development in autism. In: Hickok G and Small SL, editors. Neurobiology of language. Cambridge, Massachusetts, US: Academic Press (2016). p. 879–86. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00070-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kissine M, Saint-Denis A, and Mottron L. Language acquisition can be truly atypical in autism: Beyond joint attention. Neurosci Biobehavioral Rev. (2023) 153:105384. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105384

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Jaswal VK, Lampi AJ, and Stockwell KM. Literacy in nonspeaking autistic people. Autism: Int J Res Pract. (2024) 28:2503–14. doi: 10.1177/13623613241230709

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Brynskov C, Eigsti I-M, Jørgensen M, Lemcke S, Bohn O-S, and Krøjgaard P. Syntax and morphology in danish-speaking children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2017) 47:373–83. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2962-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Eigsti I-M, de Marchena AB, Schuh JM, and Kelley E. Language acquisition in autism spectrum disorders: A developmental review. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2011) 5:681–91. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.09.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Vogindroukas I, Stankova M, Chelas E-N, and Proedrou A. Language and speech characteristics in autism. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2022) 18:2367–77. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S331987

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Tager-Flusberg H and Kasari C. Minimally verbal school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder: The neglected end of the spectrum. Autism Research: Off J Int Soc Autism Res. (2013) 6:468–78. doi: 10.1002/aur.1329

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Curtiss S.. The case of Chelsea: The effects of late age at exposure to language on language performance and evidence for the modularity of language and mind. UCLA Work. Pap. Linguist (2014) 18:115–146.

Google Scholar

39. Slušná D, Rodríguez A, Salvadó B, Vicente A, and Hinzen W. Relations between language, non-verbal cognition, and conceptualization in non- or minimally verbal individuals with ASD across the lifespan. Autism Dev Lang Impairments. (2021) 6:23969415211053264. doi: 10.1177/23969415211053264

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Vicente A, Barbarroja N, and Castroviejo E. Linguistic, concept and symbolic composition in adults with minimal receptive vocabulary. Clin Linguistics Phonetics. (2023) 38(2):155–71. doi: 10.1080/02699206.2023.2180670

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Martins MD, Fischmeister FPS, Gingras B, Bianco R, Puig-Waldmueller E, Villringer A, et al. Recursive music elucidates neural mechanisms supporting the generation and detection of melodic hierarchies. Brain Structure Funct. (2020) 225:1997–2015. doi: 10.1007/s00429-020-02105-7. 0123456789.

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Martins MD, Krause C, Neville DA, Pino D, Villringer A, and Obrig H. Recursive hierarchical embedding in vision is impaired by posterior middle temporal gyrus lesions. Brain. (2019) 142:3217–29. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz242

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Scholz R, Villringer A, and Martins MD. Distinct hippocampal and cortical contributions in the representation of hierarchies. bioRxiv. (2022), 2022–06. doi: 10.1101/2022.06.15.496057. 2022.06.15.496057.

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Jeon H-A and Friederici AD. Two principles of organization in the prefrontal cortex are cognitive hierarchy and degree of automaticity. Nat Commun. (2013) 4(1):2041. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Jeon H-A and Friederici AD. Degree of automaticity and the prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. (2015) 19:244–50. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.03.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Klein CC, Berger P, Goucha T, Friederici AD, and Grosse Wiesmann C. Children’s syntax is supported by the maturation of BA44 at 4 years, but of the posterior STS at 3 years of age. Cereb Cortex. (2023) 33:5426–35. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac430

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Skeide MA and Friederici AD. The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2016) 17(5):323-32. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.23

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Martins MD, Laaha S, Freiberger EMEM, Choi S, and Fitch WT. How children perceive fractals: Hierarchical self-similarity and cognitive development. Cognition. (2014) 133:10–24. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.05.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Roeper T. The prism of grammar: how child language illuminates humanism. A bradford book. (2007).

Google Scholar

50. Roeper T. The acquisition of recursion: how formalism articulates the child’s path. BIOLINGUISTICS. (2011) 5(1-2):057–086. doi: 10.5964/bioling.8831

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Yang C, Hu Y, Fan J, Dong X, and Jeschull L. An experimental study of Mandarin-speaking children’s acquisition of recursion under a formal definition/classification system for recursion. Lingua. (2022) 271:103228. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2021.103228

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Kwok EYL, Brown HM, Smyth RE, and Oram Cardy J. Meta-analysis of receptive and expressive language skills in autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2015) 9:202–22. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2014.10.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Ostrolenk A, Forgeot d’Arc B, Jelenic P, Samson F, and Mottron L. Hyperlexia: Systematic review, neurocognitive modelling, and outcome. Neurosci Biobehavioral Rev. (2017) 79:134–49. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Silberberg NE and Silberberg MC. Hyperlexia—Specific word recognition skills in young children. Exceptional Children. (1967) 34:41–2. doi: 10.1177/001440296703400106

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Atkin K and Lorch MP. Hyperlexia in a 4-year-old boy with autistic spectrum disorder. J Neurolinguistics. (2006) 19:253–69. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2005.11.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Macdonald D, Luk G, and Quintin E-M. Early reading comprehension intervention for preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder and hyperlexia. J Autism Dev Disord. (2022) 52:1652–72. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05057-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Verpoorten R, Noens I, and van Berckelaer-Onnes I. ComFor – Forerunners in Communication. Manual (2008).

Google Scholar

58. Noens I, van Berckelaer-Onnes I, Verpoorten R, and van Duijn G. The ComFor: An instrument for the indication of augmentative communication in people with autism and intellectual disability. J Intellectual Disability Research: JIDR. (2006) 50:621–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00807.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Roid GH and Koch C. Leiter-3: nonverbal cognitive and neuropsychological assessment. In: McCallum RS, editor. Handbook of nonverbal assessment. New York, US: Springer International Publishing (2017). p. 127–50. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-50604-3_8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. The jamovi project (Version Version 1.6) (2021). Available online at: https://www.jamovi.org. Computer software (Accessed May 5, 2020).

Google Scholar

61. The MathWorks Inc. MATLAB version: 9.7 (2019b) (2019). Available online at: https://www.mathworks.com. Computer software (Accessed February 20, 2020).

Google Scholar

62. Allen M, Poggiali D, Whitaker K, Marshall TR, van Langen J, and Kievit RA. Raincloud plots: A multi-platform tool for robust data visualization. Wellcome Open Res. (2021) 4:63. doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15191.2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Huber S, Klein E, Moeller K, and Willmes K. Comparing a single case to a control group – applying linear mixed effects models to repeated measures data. Cortex. (2015) 71:148–59.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

64. Dawson M, Soulières I, Gernsbacher MA, and Mottron L. The level and nature of autistic intelligence. psychol Sci. (2007) 18:657–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01954.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Berwick RC, Friederici AD, Chomsky N, and Bolhuis JJ. Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends Cogn Sci. (2013) 17:89–98. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Beals KP. Illusions of literacy in nonspeaking autistic people: A response to Jaswal, Lampi & Stockwell 2024. Evidence-Based Communication Assess Intervention. (2023) 17:218–29. doi: 10.1080/17489539.2024.2381445

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: recursion, iteration, minimally verbal autism, hyperlexia, visual patterns

Citation: Rosselló J, Celma-Miralles A and Martins MD (2025) Visual recursion without recursive language? a case study of a minimally verbal autistic child. Front. Psychiatry 16:1540985. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1540985

Received: 06 December 2024; Accepted: 28 April 2025;
Published: 23 June 2025.

Edited by:

Antonio M. Persico, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Italy

Reviewed by:

Lucie Bouvet, Université de Toulouse, France
Andrea Marini, University of Udine, Italy

Copyright © 2025 Rosselló, Celma-Miralles and Martins. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Joana Rosselló, am9hbmEucm9zc2VsbG9AdWIuZWR1

These authors have contributed equally to this work

ORCID: Joana Rosselló, orcid.org/0000-0001-6429-2135
Alexandre Celma-Miralles, orcid.org/0000-0003-2557-6308
Mauricio Dias Martins, orcid.org/0000-0003-0247-8473

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.