ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 29 May 2025

Sec. Public Mental Health

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1572963

Smartphone addiction and academic procrastination among college students: a serial mediation model of self-control and academic self-efficacy

Xiuli Zhao&#x;Xiuli Zhao1†Huahua Wang&#x;Huahua Wang2†Zaoming Ma,Zaoming Ma3,4Libing ZhangLibing Zhang5Tian Chang*Tian Chang6*
  • 1Academy of Art and Design, Guangdong AIB Polytechnic, Guangzhou, China
  • 2School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
  • 3University International College, Macau Unversity of Science and Technology, Macau, Macau SAR, China
  • 4The National Research Institute for Teaching Materlals for Hong Kong, Macau and Taiwan, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China
  • 5Faculty of Education, Henan Normal University, Xinxiang, China
  • 6Institute of International and Comparative Education, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China

Introduction: Researches have highlighted the individual roles of smartphone addiction (SA), self-control (S-C), and academic self-efficacy (AS-E) in predicting academic procrastination (AP), but studies on how these variables combine to affect AP are scarce. Drawing inspiration from the conceptual model of procrastination, this research endeavors to examine a serial mediation model in which SA serves as a precursor, with S-C and AS-E acting as serial mediators in predicting AP among college students who are prone to it.

Methods: Participants were 1269 Chinese undergraduates (989 females, Mage = 19.03±1.00) from seven major regions in China. Data were collected using an online questionnaire. Descriptive and mediation analyses were conducted in SPSS 25.0.

Results: The findings revealed that SA is indirectly linked to AP through independent and sequential mediation by S-C and AS-E. People with high SA reported lower levels of S-C, which in turn was associated with lower AS-E, and these were associated with higher reports of AP.

Discussion: These findings offer significant contributions to the current body of research on AP, laying the groundwork for the development of interventions focused on reducing AP among college students.

1 Introduction

Academic procrastination (AP) is a pervasive phenomenon characterized by an individual’s deliberate postponement of academic tasks despite full awareness of their responsibility to complete them promptly (1). This behavior poses a significant concern among college students, as it is marked by an alarmingly high incidence rate and its detrimental, long-lasting impacts on personal and academic development. Among Chinese college students, a significant majority (74%) exhibit patterns of AP (2). Recent studies have shown that AP hinders academic success by increasing academic stress (3) and eroding dedication to learning (4), and it also correlates with reduced school connectedness, life satisfaction (5), and subjective well-being (6). Consequently, addressing AP is essential for cultivating better academic outcomes and promoting a healthier, more fulfilling student experience.

1.1 Theoretical background

The conceptual model of procrastination (7) integrates prominent psychological factors influencing procrastination into a structured framework. It categorizes these factors into three groups: task-related, personality-related, and other dynamic variables. Task-related factors include frustration, self-efficacy, and similar factors that shape how individuals perceive and approach tasks. For instance, lower self-efficacy tends to increase procrastination by undermining confidence in task completion. Personality-related elements, such as impulsiveness, affect decision-making processes. High impulsiveness is linked to greater procrastination due to a preference for immediate rewards over long-term goals. Dynamic variables include ego depletion, temptations, and other factors that divert attention from goal-directed activities. This model provides a theoretical framework for understanding the complex mechanisms of procrastination and lays the foundation for subsequent empirical research.

While previous studies on AP have primarily focused on task-related and personality-related factors (8), less attention has been given to other factors. For example, extensive research has demonstrated the role of academic stress (task-related factors), perfectionism (personality-related factors), and self-esteem (personality-related factors) in contributing to AP (810). Despite the limited focus on other factors in earlier research, the temptations posed by smartphones and related variables has been increasingly recognized as playing a critical role in AP (11, 12). Due to the diverse functionalities, real-time interactivity, and ease of operation of smartphones, they have become a significant temptation in the daily study and lives of college students (13). If college students are unable to resist the temptation of smartphones, excessive use can easily lead to SA. SA refers to a potent and enduring craving for and reliance on smartphones, leading to excessive engagement in phone-mediated activities that markedly impair individuals’ social and psychological well-being (14). Studies have indicated that SA can disrupt the learning process, consume valuable study time and effort, and inevitably hinder academic engagement and lead to AP (15, 16). Using the decision tree algorithm, Song and colleagues (17) found that SA is a significant factor in predicting AP among Chinese college students.

Under the framework of the conceptual model of procrastination (7), researchers have separately explored the impacts of other factors (e.g., SA) (17), personality-related factors (e.g., self-control, S-C) (18), and task-related factors (e.g., academic self-efficacy, AS-E) (19) on AP. However, how these factors interact with each other to influence AP remains unclear. Based on this, the present study aims to further explore the specific roles of S-C and AS-E in connecting SA with AP.

1.2 The mediating effect of self-control

S-C, which refers to the ability to consciously regulate automatic responses and align behavior with personal standards and long-term goals, is a positive trait that evolves throughout life (20). Its benefits have been well documented in terms of facilitating response inhibition, resisting temptation, and achieving academic success (21, 22). According to the reinforcement pathway of the I-PACE model, excessive Internet/smartphone use weakens inhibitory control (e.g., S-C), resulting in further negative consequences (23). This model suggest that S-C may mediate the relationship between SA and AP.

SA can contribute to a decline in S-C. Researchers have revealed that the addictive nature of smartphone use disrupts the neural mechanisms essential for maintaining S-C, particularly in areas of the brain responsible for inhibitory control and cognitive regulation (24). SA can divert attention and reduce cognitive control, causing the cognitive control system to become “idle” and leading to a decrease in S-C (25, 26). Recent studies have also shown that individuals with more severe SA exhibit poorer S-C (26, 27). Poor S-C is also a primary triggering factor for AP (28). Individuals with inadequate S-C often prioritize immediate rewards—such as the instant gratification provided by smartphones—over future benefits, such as completing academic assignments or studying for exams. This tendency to favor short-term pleasures over long-term goals is a hallmark of procrastinatory behavior (28). This deficit in S-C hinders students’ ability to focus on their current learning tasks, making them susceptible to distractions from surrounding stimuli and ultimately leading to AP (29). Empirical studies have revealed a negative connection between S-C and AP (18, 21). Taking into account both theoretical insights and empirical findings, this study proposes Hypothesis 1: S-C can mediate the relationship between SA and AP.

1.3 The mediating effect of academic self-efficacy

AS-E represents students’ confidence in their ability to achieve particular educational objectives effectively (30). It is an important proximal factor that influences academic motivation, engagement, and procrastination (19, 31). Self-efficacy not only promotes positive expectations for task outcomes but also reduces negative experiences during the task process, thereby decreasing procrastination (32, 33). Furthermore, self-efficacy plays a crucial role in shaping an individual’s approach to task initiation and persistence (34). Students who possess robust AS-E are more inclined to embark on and persevere through learning tasks, driven by their confidence in their abilities. Conversely, those with diminished AS-E, plagued by doubts about their capacity to complete such tasks, often struggle to initiate or maintain their efforts, leading to AP (35). Multiple studies have consistently demonstrated the negative relationship between AS-E and AP (19, 33, 36).

Research has shown that smartphone use serves as a good indicator for predicting self-efficacy among college students (37). Excessive smartphone usage, particularly when it becomes problematic, has been shown to negatively impact students’ learning capabilities, memory retention, and overall academic adaptability, thereby undermining their confidence in their academic competence (38, 39). For instance, a study involving 821 university students revealed a negative correlation between problematic smartphone behavior and students’ perceptions of their academic effectiveness, suggesting that overreliance on smartphones can erode their belief in their ability to succeed academically (37). According to Tuckman and Sexton, self-beliefs serve as a bridge between external factors and self-regulatory performance (40). Consistent with this perspective, recent research has found that AS-E acts as a mediator between smartphone use and AP (19, 36). Consequently, this study puts forward Hypothesis 2: AS-E mediates the relationship between SA and AP.

1.4 The serial mediating effect of self-control and academic self-efficacy

S-C is closely associated with AS-E. Numerous studies have examined how self-efficacy and S-C sequentially mediate between external influences and problematic behaviors (41, 42). However, no research has yet explored the serial mediation role of S-C and AS-E in this context. Based on the cyclical model of self-regulated learning (43), self-directed learning operates in a continuous loop, with S-C also influencing the formation of AS-E. Empirical evidence suggests that individuals who have stronger S-C generally exhibit greater self-efficacy (44, 45). They typically set and pursue long-term and valuable goals, living a more planned life, which contributes to enhancing their AS-E (46). Moreover, their ability to successfully resist temptations and avoid short-term detrimental behaviors aids them in achieving their goals and accumulating successful experiences, thereby further boosting their self-efficacy (47). Considering the theoretical and empirical evidence regarding the relationship between S-C and AS-E, this study proposes Hypothesis 3: S-C and AS-E have a serial mediation effect on the link between SA and AP.

1.5 The current study

There remains a lack of clarity regarding how SA, S-C, and AS-E collectively influence AP among college students. Drawing inspiration from the conceptual model of procrastination (7), and based on the reinforcement pathway of the I-PACE model (23) and the cyclical model of self-regulated learning (43), this study aims to establish a serial mediation model to test the three hypotheses mentioned above.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants and procedure

Following approval from the Ethics Committee of School of Education, South China Normal University, this study was conducted via an online questionnaire on the Wenjuanxing platform. Prior to participation, the researchers read the informed consent form out loud to the participants, outlining the confidentiality of all study-related information and its exclusive use for research purposes. Participants can choose for themselves whether to participate or not to participate in the online survey. Upon acknowledging and consenting to these terms, participants proceeded to complete the survey. This cross-sectional study. A total of 1,505 questionnaires were collected. Following the exclusion of inattentive responses (including patterned answering and completion times under 100 seconds), 1,269 valid responses were retained. The final group of participants consisted of 1,269 university students (989 females and 280 males). Participants’ ages ranged from 17 to 24 years (M = 19.03, SD = 1.00). Additional demographic details are presented in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of participants.

2.2 Measures

2.2.1 Smartphone addiction

Smartphone Addiction Scale–Short Version (SAS-SV) is a scale for smartphone addiction that consisted of 10 items (48). It includes five sub-factors: daily-life disturbance, withdrawal, cyberspace-oriented relationship, overuse, and tolerance. Students in our study completed the Chinese version of SAS-SV (49). They rated their agreement with 10 statements on a scale of 1 to 6. All item employ positive scoring. As the mean score increases, the levels of SA intensify. In this study, Cronbach’s α of this scale was 0.92.

2.2.2 Self-control

Participants completed a self-control scale that is widely applicable to Chinese college students (50). This scale was adapted by Chinese scholars from the Brief Self-Control Scale (51). It includes two sub-factors: impulse control (4 items), and self-discipline (3 items). Students report how well each item describes themselves on a scale of 1 to 5. Items 2, 4, 6 and 7 employ reverse scoring. As the mean score increases, the levels of S-C intensify. In this study, Cronbach’s α of this scale was 0.75.

2.2.3 Academic self-efficacy

A 9-item Academic Self-Efficacy Scale (AS-ES), widely applicable to Chinese college students (52) was used to measure participants’ AS-E levels. This scale was translated by Chinese scholars from Pintrich’s and De Groot’s original AS-ES (53). Participants responded by indicating their level of agreement with the 9 statements on a scale of 1 to 5. This scale is a unidimensional measure with no reverse-scored items. As the mean score increases, the levels of AS-E intensify. In this study, Cronbach’s α of this scale was 0.92.

2.2.4 Academic procrastination

College students completed the Academic Procrastination Inventory (API), which was translated by Chinese scholars (54), originating from Aitken (55). API is widely used in Chinese college students (54). Participants indicated whether each of the 19 procrastination behaviors applies to their own circumstances on a scale of 1 to 5. As a unidimensional measure, this scale contains 19 items, among which Items 2, 4, 7, 11, 12, 14, 16, 17, and 18 require reverse scoring. As the mean score increases, the levels of AP intensify. In this study, Cronbach’s α of this scale was 0.85.

2.2.5 Control variables

Based on the findings of previous studies (3, 56), gender, age, and parental education levels may affect AP. This study added these three factors as control variables in the statistical analysis. Gender is dummy coded as 1 for males and 0 for females. Father’s and mother’s educational levels were evaluated across eight categories, from the lowest (uneducated) to the highest (PhD).

2.3 Data Analysis

We first carried out a preliminary assessment for potential common method variance, followed by descriptive and correlational analyses with SPSS 25.0. Our hypothesized serial mediation model was tested via PROCESS Macro Model 6 (57). The hypothesized serial mediation model is presented in Figure 1. Utilizing bootstrapping (n = 5000), a technique particularly suitable for testing mediation models without assuming normality in the data, we further investigated the significance of the mediation effect.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The hypothesized serial mediation model.

3 Results

3.1 Basic analysis

Before conducting mediation analysis, we perform a basic analysis. First, the results of the Harman single-factor test indicate that the amount of variation explained by the first factor is 27.11%, which is less than 40% (58). Therefore, this study does not suffer from serious common method bias. Second, the results of correlation analyses revealed that SA has a negative link with S-C and AS-E, but a positive link with AP. S-C has a positive correlation with AS-E and a negative link with AP. Moreover, AS-E also has a negative link with AP (see Table 2).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations among main variables.

3.2 Mediating analysis

After accounting for control variables, our results, presented in Tables 3, 4, revealed a negative association between SA and S-C (β = −0.63, p < 0.001) as well as between SA and AS-E (β = −0.21, p < 0.001). Furthermore, S-C (β = −0.46, p < 0.001) and AS-E (β = −0.17, p < 0.001) demonstrated a negative association with AP. Bootstrapping results further confirmed the significance of S-C’s (effect size = 0.29, 95% CI [0.24, 0.34]) and AS-E’s (effect size = 0.04, 95% CI [0.02, 0.05]) mediation in the SA-AP relationship. Notably, S-C exhibited a positive correlation with AS-E (β = 0.31, p < 0.001). Moreover, the serial mediation effect of S-C and AS-E in the SA-AP connection was found to be significant, with an effect size of 0.03 (95% CI [0.02, 0.05]), as indicated by the bootstrapping results. Given that gender imbalance may potentially influence the results of this study, we conducted a gender-specific analysis to examine the robustness of the findings. The results indicated that the chain mediation effect of S-C and AS-E was significant for both male and female students (males: effect size = 0.03, 95% CI [0.01, 0.04]; females: effect size = 0.03, 95% CI [0.01, 0.04]), as presented in Supplementary Table S1-S3. These findings suggest that the observed chain mediation effects are stable across genders within the study sample.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of the serial mediation model.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The mediating role of self-control and academic self-efficacy.

4 Discussion

A serial mediation framework was constructed to assess the independent mediation roles of S-C and AS-E, along with their sequential mediation effect, in linking SA to AP. In this study, all hypotheses have been well validated. The results revealed that SA was indirectly associated with AP through both independent and sequential mediation by S-C and AS-E. Specifically, higher SA correlated with lower S-C and AS-E, which in turn were linked to higher AP. These findings align with the conceptual model of procrastination (7) and extend prior research by integrating SA, S-C, and AS-E into a unified framework.

Consistent with Hypothesis 1, our study uncovered a significant mediation effect of S-C in the connection between SA and AP. This result supports the reinforcement pathway of the I-PACE model (23), suggesting that S-C may play an important role in the relationship between SA and AP. Previous studies have also identified S-C as a crucial internal psychological mechanism through which problematic smartphone use or Internet addiction influences students’ procrastination behaviors (18, 59). For example, a longitudinal study of 622 college students found that S-C could significantly mediate the link between problematic smartphone use and sleep procrastination (59). The strength model of S-C (20) indicated that S-C requires the expenditure of individuals’ limited psychological resources, such that depletion of these resources in one domain leads to a corresponding decrease in available S-C strength in other domains. Individuals addicted to smartphones may need these resources to suppress impulses when faced with smartphone temptations (21). Reduced S-C capacity could potentially lead to difficulties in sustaining focus on learning tasks, potentially contributing to AP (18, 29).

Supporting Hypothesis 2, our research revealed that AS-E plays a significant intermediary role in the connection between SA and AP. This result tallies with earlier research findings (19, 36), which have identified AS-E as a key factor linking smartphone usage to academic procrastination. Previous research has highlighted the importance of AS-E in predicting academic performance and behavioral outcomes (35). In the context of SA, students who exhibit higher levels of SA tend to report lower levels of AS-E (15, 37). This association may reflect that excessive smartphone use can interfere with study habits and routines, potentially reducing students’ confidence in their ability to succeed academically. Other studies also found that individuals with high AS-E often exhibit a stronger belief in their capacity to manage academic tasks effectively, making them more inclined to take the initiative and complete them on time, thereby reducing their propensity for procrastination (36). Conversely, college students who lack of confidence may doubt their capacity to complete academic tasks, prompting them to procrastinate before starting these tasks (35).

Another crucial finding of this study is that S-C and AS-E serially mediated the relationship between SA and AP, aligning with Hypothesis 3. This discovery resonates with the cyclical model of self-regulated learning (43), indicating that S-C is positively correlated with AS-E. Moreover, our findings are consistent with previous research, which highlight that individuals with strong S-C are better equipped to regulate their behaviors, resist distractions, and maintain focus on long-term academic goals, which may enhance their confidence in their ability to succeed academically (44, 45). However, SA may undermines S-C by depleting students’ cognitive resources and making them more susceptible to external distractions (26, 47). This study suggests that higher levels of S-C are associated with greater confidence in one’s ability to succeed academically (AS-E), and may further escalate the propensity for AP (60). However, it should be noted that these interpretations are speculative and solely based on correlational data. While our findings align with existing theoretical frameworks and empirical evidence, the cross-sectional nature of this study limits our ability to establish causal relationships or determine the temporal sequence of these processes. Future longitudinal or experimental studies are necessary to validate these associations and clarify the directionality of the relationships between SA, S-C, AS-E, and AP.

There is evidence to confirm the relationship between SA, S-C, AS-E, and AP, respectively (1719). However, how these factors interact with each other to influence AP is unclear. Based on the conceptual model of procrastination (7), this study integrated SA, S-C, AS-E, and AP into a serial mediation model to explore the underlying mechanisms through which SA influences AP among college students. The results indicate that S-C and AS-E serve as serial mediators between SA and AP. As well as enriching the content of the conceptual model of procrastination, this study also enhances understanding of the pathways through which SA, S-C, and AS-E jointly impact AP. The results imply that intervening in any single variable within the serial mediation — comprising S-C and AS-E — can disrupt the interconnected sequence. First, enhancing S-C is vital. Mindfulness training, particularly brief mindfulness meditation, has proven effective in rapidly developing S-C abilities, even in environments with limited resources (61, 62). By practicing mindfulness meditation, students can strengthen their S-C mechanisms, making them less prone to smartphone distractions and more proficient in managing their academic responsibilities efficiently. In addition to enhancing S-C, interventions targeting AS-E may also play a significant role in breaking the serial mediation chain. Self-care programs based on the Orem model have been shown to effectively improve AS-E (63).

Limitations that merit consideration are as follows. First, the data collected solely relied on participants’ self-reports, potentially introducing social desirability bias. To enhance the accuracy of assessments of students’ SA and AP, future research could integrate reports from teachers and parents. Second, cross-sectional design was unable to establish causality between the examined variables. To rigorously explore the chained mediation effects of S-C and AS-E in the association between SA and AP, future research could adopt longitudinal or cross-lagged approaches. Third, while the procrastination conceptual model (7) categorizes influential factors into task-related, personality-related, and other factors, this study confined its focus to the mechanisms through which SA, S-C, and AS-E impact AP. Future studies could broaden the scope to investigate the mechanisms through which task-related factors such as autonomous motivation, personality-related factors such as impulsiveness, and other factors such as mood influence AP. Finally, although this study conducted gender-specific analyses and found that gender imbalance did not affect the stability of the results, future research would benefit from using samples with a more balanced proportion of male and female participants.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the School of Education, South China Normal University (SCNU- 288 005-2024-12, September 28, 2024).

Author contributions

XZ: Writing – original draft. HW: Writing – original draft. ZM: Data curation, Writing – review & editing. LZ: Data curation, Writing – original draft. TC: Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by the Humanities and Social Science Fund of the Ministry of Education of China (20YJC190024), Guangdong Provincial Education Science Planning Project (2023GXJK826), and the “13th Five-Year Plan” Education Science Research Project of Guangdong Province (2020GXJK536).

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the participants who took part in this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1572963/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Senécal C, Koestner R, and Vallerand RJ. Self-regulation and academic procrastination. J Soc Psychol. (1995) 135:607–19. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1995.9712234

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Zhang Y, Dong S, Fang W, Chai X, Mei J, and Fan X. Self-efficacy for self-regulation and fear of failure as mediators between self-esteem and academic procrastination among undergraduates in health professions. Adv Health Sci Educ. (2018) 23:817–30. doi: 10.1007/s10459-018-9832-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Jin Y, Zhou W, Zhang Y, Yang Z, and Hussain Z. Smartphone distraction and academic anxiety: The mediating role of academic procrastination and the moderating role of time management disposition. Behav Sci. (2024) 14:820. doi: 10.3390/bs14090820

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Tian J, Zhao J, Xu J, Li Q, Sun T, Zhao C, et al. Mobile phone addiction and academic procrastination negatively impact academic achievement among chinese medical students. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:758303. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.758303

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Çıkrıkçı Ö and Erzen E. Academic procrastination, school attachment, and life satisfaction: A mediation model. J Rat-Emo Cognitive-Behav Ther. (2020) 38:225–42. doi: 10.1007/s10942-020-00336-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Albulescu I, Labar A-V, Manea A-D, and Stan C. The mediating role of cognitive test anxiety on the relationship between academic procrastination and subjective wellbeing and academic performance. Front Public Health. (2024) 12:1336002. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1336002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Procee R, Kamphorst B, Wissen AV, and Meyer J. A formal model of procrastination (2013). Available online at: https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/A-formal-model-of-procrastination-Procee-Kamphorst/6a8e45afb81405138810e1ffcc0e74d1344e39d0 (Accessed October 20, 2024).

Google Scholar

8. Ashraf MA, Sahar N, Kamran M, and Alam J. Impact of self-efficacy and perfectionism on academic procrastination among university students in Pakistan. Behav Sci. (2023) 13:537. doi: 10.3390/bs13070537

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Park H and Doehee A. The relationships between self-regulation, time management, academic stress, and academic procrastination of university students. J Learner-Centered Curriculum Instruction. (2018) 18:867–91. doi: 10.22251/jlcci.2018.18.22.867

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Li C, Qu S, and Ren K. The role of self-esteem and depression in the relationship between physical activity and academic procrastination among chinese undergraduate students: A serial mediation model. PRBM. (2024) 17:3721–9. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S477659

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Liu F, Xu Y, Yang T, Li Z, Dong Y, Chen L, et al. The mediating roles of time management and learning strategic approach in the relationship between smartphone addiction and academic procrastination. Psychol Res Behav Manag. (2022) 15:2639–48. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S373095

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Zhou X, Yang F, Chen Y, and Gao Y. The correlation between mobile phone addiction and procrastination in students: A meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2024) 346:317–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2023.11.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Tian Y, Ding H, and Guo Z. The bidirectional association between negative life events and mobile addiction among university freshmen: The role of perceived social support. psychol Dev Educ. (2024) 4:579–88. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2025.04.14

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Odacı H and Kalkan M. Problematic Internet use, loneliness and dating anxiety among young adult university students. Comput Educ. (2010) 55:1091–7. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2010.05.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kuss DJ and Griffiths MD. Online social networking and addiction–a review of the psychological literature. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2011) 8:3528–52. doi: 10.3390/ijerph8093528

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Wang P and Lei L. How does problematic smartphone use impair adolescent self-esteem? A moderated mediation analysis. Curr Psychol. (2021) 40:2910–6. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00232-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Song P, Liu X, Cai X, Zhong M, Wang Q, and Zhu X. Predictive analysis of college students’ academic procrastination behavior based on a decision tree model. Hum Soc Sci Commun. (2024) 11:869. doi: 10.1057/s41599-024-03300-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Liu M, Sun W, Chen H, and Fan J. The impact of internet addiction on college students’ academic procrastination: A moderated mediation model. Soc Behav Pers. (2024) 52:e13402. doi: 10.2224/sbp.13402

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Parmaksız İ. The effect of phubbing, a behavioral problem, on academic procrastination: The mediating and moderating role of academic self-efficacy. Psychol Schools. (2023) 60:105–21. doi: 10.1002/pits.22765

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, and Tice DM. The strength model of self-control. Curr Dir psychol Sci. (2007) 16:351–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Li Y, Liu C, Huo Z, Zhang L, Han J, Li Q, et al. Task aversiveness and outcome value mediate the relationship between self-control and academic procrastination among primary school students. Eur J Psychol Educ. (2024) 39:3029–45. doi: 10.1007/s10212-024-00851-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Nederkoorn C, Houben K, Hofmann W, Roefs A, and Jansen A. Control yourself or just eat what you like? Weight gain over a year is predicted by an interactive effect of response inhibition and implicit preference for snack foods. Health Psychol. (2010) 29:389–93. doi: 10.1037/a0019921

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Brand M, Young KS, Laier C, Wölfling K, and Potenza MN. Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific Internet-use disorders: An Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model. Neurosci Biobehavioral Rev. (2016) 71:252–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Baler RD and Volkow ND. Drug addiction: the neurobiology of disrupted self-control. Trends Mol Med. (2006) 12:559–66. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2006.10.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Dou K, Wang L-X, Li J-B, Wang G-D, Li Y-Y, and Huang Y-T. Mobile phone addiction and risk-taking behavior among chinese adolescents: A moderated mediation model. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:5472. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17155472

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Park Y and Lee S. Gender differences in smartphone addiction and depression among Korean adolescents: Focusing on the internal mechanisms of attention deficit and self-control. Comput Hum Behav. (2022) 136:107400. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2022.107400

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Świątek AH, Szcześniak M, Aleksandrowicz B, Zaczkowska D, Wawer W, and Ścisłowska M. Problematic smartphone use and social media fatigue: The mediating role of self-control. PRBM. (2023) 16:211–22. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S389806

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Steel P and König CJ. Integrating theories of motivation. Acad Manage Rev. (2006) 31:889–913. doi: 10.5465/amr.2006.22527462

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Zheng Y and Xu Y. The effect of mindfulness on academic procrastination of college students in online learning environments:Resilience and self-control as chain mediator. J psychol Sci. (2022) 45:1450–7. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20220623

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev. (1977) 84:191–215. doi: 10.1037//0033-295x.84.2.191

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Fredricks JA, Blumenfeld PC, and Paris AH. School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the evidence. Rev Educ Res. (2004) 74:59–109. doi: 10.3102/00346543074001059

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Bandura A. Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development and functioning. Educ Psychol. (1993) 28:117–48. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep2802_3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Liu G, Cheng G, Hu J, Pan Y, and Zhao S. Academic self-efficacy and postgraduate procrastination: A moderated mediation model. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:1752. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01752

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Haycock LA, McCarthy P, and Skay CL. Procrastination in college students: The role of self-efficacy and anxiety. J Couns Dev. (1998) 76:317–24. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.1998.tb02548.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Chen C, Shen Y, Xiao F, Ni J, and Zhu Y. The effect of smartphone dependence on learning burnout among undergraduates: the mediating effect of academic adaptability and the moderating effect of self-efficacy. Front Psychiatry. (2023) 14:1155544. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1155544

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Li L, Gao H, and Xu Y. The mediating and buffering effect of academic self-efficacy on the relationship between smartphone addiction and academic procrastination. Comput Educ. (2020) 159:104001. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2020.104001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Li SC, Chan JWW, Lui AKF, Lui M, and Wong RWP. Mindfulness mitigates the adverse effects of problematic smartphone use on academic self-efficacy: A structural equation modelling analysis. Educ Technol Soc. (2024) 27:114–33. doi: 10.30191/ETS.202407_27(3).RP07

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Tanil CT and Yong MH. Mobile phones: The effect of its presence on learning and memory. PloS One. (2020) 15:e0219233. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0219233

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Wang X, Liu Y, Ying B, and Lin J. The effect of learning adaptability on Chinese middle school students’ English academic engagement: The chain mediating roles of foreign language anxiety and English learning self-efficacy. Curr Psychol. (2023) 42:6682–92. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02008-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Tuckman BW and Sexton TL. The effects of informational feedback and self-beliefs on the motivation to perform a self-regulated task. J Res Pers. (1992) 26:121–7. doi: 10.1016/0092-6566(92)90048-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Sun R, Gao Q, and Xiang Y. Perceived parental monitoring of smartphones and problematic smartphone use in adolescents: Mediating roles of self-efficacy and self-control. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2022) 25:784–92. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2022.0040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Tian X, Ming H, and Wei L. Test anxiety and academic burnout of high school studentsthe chain mediating role of academic self-efficacy and self-control. Sci Explor Educ. (2023) 41:67–73.

Google Scholar

43. Zimmerman BJ. Investigating self-regulation and motivation: Historical background, methodological developments, and future prospects. Am Educ Res J. (2008) 45:166–83. doi: 10.3102/0002831207312909

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Cheng F, Xu L, Zhou Y, Chen Z, Wang Y, and Li T. Self-control and meaning in life among Chinese young adults: The role of self-efficacy. Pers Individ Dif. (2024) 229:112770. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2024.112770

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Džinović V, Đević R, and Đerić I. The role of self-control, self-efficacy, metacognition, and motivation in predicting school achievement. Psihologija. (2019) 52:35–52. doi: 10.2298/PSI180202027D

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Azizli N, Atkinson BE, Baughman HM, and Giammarco EA. Relationships between general self-efficacy, planning for the future, and life satisfaction. Pers Individ Dif. (2015) 82:58–60. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.03.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Annesi JJ. Effects of self-regulatory skill usage on weight management behaviours: Mediating effects of induced self-efficacy changes in non-obese through morbidly obese women. Br J Health Psychol. (2018) 23:1066–83. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12338

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Kwon M, Kim D-J, Cho H, and Yang S. The smartphone addiction scale: Development and validation of a short version for adolescents. PloS One. (2013) 8:e83558. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083558

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Xiang M, Wang Z, and Ma B. Reliability and validity of Chinese version of the smartphone addiction scale in adolescents. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2019) 27:959–64. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2019.05.022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Luo T, Cheng L, Qin L, and Xiao S. Reliability and validity of Chinese version of brief self-control scale. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2021) 29:83–6. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2021.01.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Morean ME, DeMartini KS, Leeman RF, Pearlson GD, Anticevic A, Krishnan-Sarin S, et al. Psychometrically improved, abbreviated versions of three classic measures of impulsivity and self-control. psychol Assess. (2014) 26:1003–20. doi: 10.1037/pas0000003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Huang T and Yang X. Making a scale of self-worth for young students. psychol Sci. (1998) 382:289–92. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.1998.04.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Pintrich PR and De Groot EV. Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. J Educ Psychol. (1990) 82:33–40. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Chen X, Dai X, and Dong Q. A research of aitken procrastination inventory applied to chinese college students. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2008) 76:22–3.

Google Scholar

55. Aitken ME. A personality profile of the college student procrastinator. Dissertation Abstracts Int Section A: Humanities Soc Sci. (1982) 43:722–3.

Google Scholar

56. Hortacsu N. Parents education level, popularity, individual cognitions, and academic-performance - an investigation with turkish children. J Genet Psychol. (1994) 155:179–89. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1994.9914770

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Hayes AF. Model templates for PROCESS for SPSS and SAS. Model Templates PROCESS SPSS SAS. (2013) 42:185–227.

Google Scholar

58. Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee JY, and Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J Appl Psychol. (2003) 88:879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Chen S, Liao J, Wang X, Wei M, and Liu Y. Bidirectional relations between problematic smartphone use and bedtime procrastination among Chinese university students: Self-control as a mediator. Sleep Med. (2023) 112:53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2023.09.033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Klassen RM, Krawchuk LL, and Rajani S. Academic procrastination of undergraduates: Low self-efficacy to self-regulate predicts higher levels of procrastination. Contemp Educ Psychol. (2008) 33:915–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.07.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Friese M, Messner C, and Schaffner Y. Mindfulness meditation counteracts self-control depletion. Conscious Cognit. (2012) 21:1016–22. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.01.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Liu X, Xiao R, and Tang W. The impact of school-based mindfulness intervention on bullying behaviors among teenagers: mediating effect of self-control. J Interpers Violence. (2022) 37:NP20459–NP20481. doi: 10.1177/08862605211052047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Seyednazari MA, Amini K, Ramezani-Badr F, and Markani AK. Effect of a structured Orem’s self-care model nursing intervention on sleep quality and academic self-efficacy in nursing students. BMC Nurs. (2025) 24:248. doi: 10.1186/s12912-025-02886-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: smartphone addiction, academic procrastination, self-control, academic self-efficacy, college students

Citation: Zhao X, Wang H, Ma Z, Zhang L and Chang T (2025) Smartphone addiction and academic procrastination among college students: a serial mediation model of self-control and academic self-efficacy. Front. Psychiatry 16:1572963. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1572963

Received: 08 February 2025; Accepted: 28 April 2025;
Published: 29 May 2025.

Edited by:

María del Mar Simón Márquez, University of Almeria, Spain

Reviewed by:

Perihan Gürbüz, İnönü University, Türkiye
Deniz Mertkan Gezgin, Trakya University, Türkiye
Zhou Min, Lingnan Normal University, China

Copyright © 2025 Zhao, Wang, Ma, Zhang and Chang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tian Chang, NzgxOTk5OTczQHFxLmNvbQ==

†These authors share the first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.