- Department of Neuroscience, Imaging and Clinical Sciences, “G. D’Annunzio” University, Chieti, Italy
1 Introduction
The Predictive Coding theory (1) posits that the brain constantly generates predictions about the environment, allocating neural resources primarily to processing stimuli that violate these predictions (i.e., prediction errors), in order to update the brain’s predictive models of the environment. Sensory prediction is described as a hierarchical process, wherein neural activity encodes top-down expectations of sensory experiences, which are sent to lower-level brain regions. Any resulting discrepancies are then utilized to update internal models, facilitating the formation of new predictions (2, 3). This processing loop, known as the Predictive Coding framework, is based on the idea that the brain generates inferential models of the world to efficiently process the vast amount of incoming sensory information (2).
According to this model, impaired belief updating is thought to underlie the hallucinations and delusions characteristic of schizophrenia (4, 5). For example, an inappropriate overweighting of prior beliefs relative to incoming sensory information might lead to the perception of hearing intelligible words in response to ambiguous stimuli (6).
It is now well established that “urbanicity” -the impact of living in urban areas at a given time- constitutes a significant risk factor for the later development of psychosis and schizophrenia (7–9). A higher prevalence of schizophrenia in urban centers has been consistently confirmed (10, 11). Furthermore, the higher incidence of the disorder among migrant groups and ethnic minorities is one of the most consistent findings in schizophrenia epidemiology (12–14). Supporting this evidence, studies have observed a reduction in the risk of psychosis among children born in cities who later move to rural areas, as well as an increased risk among individuals raised in rural environments who move to cities during adolescence (15, 16).
With this article, we aim to propose an interpretation of the influence of urbanicity through the lens of the Predictive Coding model in the development of psychosis and schizophrenia. We suggest that the urban environment may shape predictive processes in the brain, potentially altering the balance between prior beliefs and sensory information. This perspective may offer new insights into the mechanisms underlying the increased risk of psychosis in urban settings and contribute to a more comprehensive understanding of schizophrenia.
2 Discussion
According to our hypothesis, urbanicity, a form of social stress (7), represents a form of overstimulation for individuals who are not accustomed to it. In such cases, internal predictive models may fail to generate coherent predictions of reality, potentially leading to the development of psychosis and schizophrenia.
Additionally, epidemiological studies have shown that environmental green spaces contribute to reducing the incidence of psychosis within the population (17–19).
From the perspective of the predictive coding model, it can be hypothesized that predicting reality is easier in a context with fewer stimuli, such as environments with ample green spaces, compared to densely populated, stimulus-rich urban areas. In other words, it appears to be easier to generate predictive models of reality in less reactive contexts. This may explain why green spaces act as a protective factor against the development of schizophrenia, whereas urbanicity is a significant risk factor, particularly for individuals who migrate from rural areas to high-density urban settings.
Converging experimental evidence indicates that exposure to natural environments modulates the neurobiological circuits governing the stress response, whereas urban living systematically imposes an “uncertainty load” on cognitive and biological systems. For instance, an fMRI study involving 63 healthy participants found that current urban residence was associated with heightened amygdala reactivity during a social−threat task; moreover, city dwellers showed stronger amygdala responses to unpredictable social evaluation, while an urban childhood engaged the perigenual anterior cingulate cortex—both regions pivotal for stress regulation (20). Complementing these findings, a randomized “one-hour walk” trial found that a single 60-min stroll in a forest, compared with a busy urban boulevard, significantly decreases amygdala activity, suggesting that even brief nature exposure can attenuate neural stress signatures (21). Epidemiologically, an analysis of 9,830 Chinese “new-generation migrants” revealed that migration from rural to urban areas is linked to higher levels of psychological distress (β = 0.305) and perceived stress (β = 0.328) than in peers who remained in rural settings, independent of socioeconomic covariates (22). Youth from disadvantaged urban neighborhoods display altered limbic reactivity when threat cues switch from predictable to unpredictable (23); irregular, impulsive traffic noise typical of metropolitan streets provokes stronger cortisol release and performance decrements than continuous noise (24); and life−history studies link the unsafe, chaotic conditions of low−SES urban settings with objectively measured environmental unpredictability and downstream stress−related psychopathology (25, 26). Taken together, these findings, demonstrating that moving from green rural settings to stimulus–rich urban environments increases stress vulnerability, also suggest, consistent with our hypothesis, that such a transition may elevate the unpredictability load on cerebral predictive mechanisms.
The increased likelihood of psychosis in migrants living in environments where they represent a minority also reflects the impact of perceived social stress (27), including social isolation and social defeat (13, 28).
Social capital is defined as “the set of resources, such as information, influence, and support, embedded in and accessible through social networks, sustained by trust and shared norms of reciprocity, that facilitate coordination and cooperation for mutual benefit” (29).
Multiple studies have demonstrated that social capital is markedly higher in rural than in urban settings, presumably because smaller, more homogeneous networks foster stronger, trust–based ties (30–32). Echoing this pattern, an inverse association between population density and social capital has been documented, indicating that less–dense, and therefore often smaller, communities tend to sustain higher levels of social capital (33). Elevated social capital has, in turn, been proposed to buffer the risk of psychosis, whereas social fragmentation and low social capital in urban neighborhoods may heighten that risk (11, 34).We hypothesize that smaller, traditional social structures may provide greater predictability and, consequently, lower stress levels compared to the urban social landscape, particularly for a migratory population accustomed to a more structured social life than the social disorganization found in urban neighborhoods.
Additionally, the sensory overload typical of metropolitan areas may compromise sensory gating, an automatic, pre−attentive inhibitory filter that dampens neural responses to repetitive or irrelevant input, (35, 36). Impaired gating exposes the brain to a flood of unfiltered information and can trigger aberrant salience, the inappropriate assignment of motivational significance to otherwise neutral stimuli (37). This process has been implicated in the emergence of delusional beliefs (38, 39). In line with our hypothesis, we propose that a state of “hypersalience” arises precisely when predictive systems fail to anticipate -or effectively model- the exceptionally high volume and unpredictability of urban sensory input, thereby overloading cortical prediction−error mechanisms. Within the predictive−coding framework, the sensory overload typical of urban settings weakens sensory gating (35), generating numerous, noisy prediction errors. To preserve perceptual coherence, the brain re−balances precision, down−weighting bottom−up evidence and conferring excessive confidence -hyper−precision- on higher−level predictions through dopaminergic modulation (4, 40). This imbalance drives aberrant salience, the motivational tagging of otherwise irrelevant stimuli (37), and stabilizes high−certainty hypotheses that withstand contradictory data, charting a computational path toward the formation of delusional beliefs (2, 39).
The phenomenological tradition has highlighted that self-awareness is inherently permeated by alterity (41), while empirical research has identified a basic pathology, described as a global alteration of subjectivity, known as self-disorders, which are central to schizophrenia (42, 43). Self-disorders involve disturbances in the dynamic structure of the first-person perspective, characterized by a fragile sense of self-presence and a tendency toward hyperreflexivity which implies active or passive modes of increased and intrusive self-awareness (44–46).
According to our hypothesis, the fragile sense of self-presence and the tendency toward hyperreflexivity, which disrupt the experience of alterity in schizophrenia, could be explained by a deficit in the ability to “predict” the behavior of others. In schizophrenia, this impaired ability to construct stable and coherent representations of social interactions leads to a sense of unpredictability and unfamiliarity in interpersonal contexts (47). Individuals with schizophrenia often struggle to anticipate the intentions, emotions, and actions of others, resulting in heightened anxiety and withdrawal from social environments (48). This impairment may manifest as difficulties in theory of mind, leading to misinterpretations of others’ behavior and contributing to delusional ideation (48, 49). Ebisch et al. (50) have already hypothesized that self-processing plays a role in mediating the beneficial effects of green spaces on psychosis. Moreover, we hypothesize that this failure in predictive social processing may contribute to the experience of ego-boundary disturbances, wherein individuals feel an abnormal permeability between the self and others (51). This can lead to phenomena such as thought insertion, passivity experiences, and delusions of influence, all of which reflect an altered sense of agency and a disrupted experience of self-other distinction (41, 48). Although intriguing, this proposal remains speculative and calls for rigorous empirical testing.
In this context, Sartre’s statement “Hell is other people” (52) gains new significance, emphasizing how encounters with others create anxiety in individuals with schizophrenia. The unpredictability of others -alterity- is known to be impaired in patients with schizophrenia (41).
Ultimately, from the perspective of predictive coding, a stressful factor can be understood as an event that is difficult to predict in relation to one’s internal predictive models of reality. When this “threshold of unpredictability” is exceeded, it can lead to psychopathological decompensation. In this sense, resilience may be defined as the ability to cope with events that are difficult to predict.
Ultimately, fear−related disorders such as PTSD are propelled mainly by hyper−reactive threat circuits and impaired fear−extinction mechanisms, systems in which prediction−error weighting is largely preserved. Schizophrenia, by contrast, hinges on a fundamental misallocation of precision between top−down predictions and incoming sensory evidence, a disturbance that clinically manifests as hallucinations and delusions. Predictive−coding failure thus stands as a cornerstone for understanding schizophrenia, even if further empirical work is required to refine and substantiate this framework.
Author contributions
AM: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Investigation, Methodology, Resources. MP: Conceptualization, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. GM: Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Ford JM and Mathalon DH. Efference copy, corollary discharge, predictive coding, and psychosis. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2019) 4:764–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.07.005
2. Friston K. The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat Rev Neurosci. (2010) 11:127–38. doi: 10.1038/nrn2787
3. Picard F and Friston K. Predictions, perception, and a sense of self. Neurology. (2014) 83:1112–8. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000798
4. Adams RA, Stephan KE, Brown HR, Frith CD, and Friston KJ. The computational anatomy of psychosis. Front Psychiatry. (2013) 4:47. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00047
5. Sterzer P, Adams RA, Fletcher P, Frith C, Lawrie SM, Muckli L, et al. The predictive coding account of psychosis. Biol Psychiatry. (2018) 84:634–43. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.05.015
6. Corlett PR, Horga G, Fletcher PC, Alderson-Day B, Schmack K, and Powers AR. Hallucinations and strong priors. Trends Cogn Sci. (2019) 23:114–27. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.001
7. Abrahamyan Empson L, Baumann PS, Söderström O, Codeluppi Z, Söderström D, and Conus P. Urbanicity: The need for new avenues to explore the link between urban living and psychosis. Early Interv Psychiatry. (2020) 14:398–409. doi: 10.1111/eip.12861
8. Grover S, Varadharajan N, and Venu S. Urbanization and psychosis: an update of recent evidence. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2024) 37:191–201. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000931
9. Krabbendam L and van Os J. Schizophrenia and urbanicity: a major environmental influence–conditional on genetic risk. Schizophr Bull. (2005) 31:795–9. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi060
10. Allardyce J and Boydell J. Review: the wider social environment and schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2006) 32:592–8. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl008
11. Kirkbride JB, Jones PB, Ullrich S, and Coid JW. Social deprivation, inequality, and the neighborhood-level incidence of psychotic syndromes in East London. Schizophr Bull. (2014) 40:169–80. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs151
12. Akdeniz C, Tost H, Streit F, Haddad L, Wüst S, Schäfer A, et al. Neuroimaging evidence for a role of neural social stress processing in ethnic minority-associated environmental risk. JAMA Psychiatry. (2014) 71:672–80. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.35
13. Morgan C and Fearon P. Social experience and psychosis insights from studies of migrant and ethnic minority groups. Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. (2007) 16:118–23. doi: 10.1017/s1121189x00004723
14. van Os J, Hanssen M, Bijl RV, and Vollebergh W. Prevalence of psychotic disorder and community level of psychotic symptoms: an urban-rural comparison. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2001) 58:663–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.7.663
15. Marcelis M, Takei N, and van Os J. Urbanization and risk for schizophrenia: does the effect operate before or around the time of illness onset? Psychol Med. (1999) 29:1197–203. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799008983
16. Pedersen CB and Mortensen PB. Evidence of a dose-response relationship between urbanicity during upbringing and schizophrenia risk. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2001) 58:1039–46. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.11.1039
17. Boers S, Hagoort K, Scheepers F, and Helbich M. Does residential green and blue space promote recovery in psychotic disorders? A cross-sectional study in the province of Utrecht, The Netherlands. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2018) 15:2195. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15102195
18. Tost H, Champagne FA, and Meyer-Lindenberg A. Environmental influence in the brain, human welfare and mental health. Nat Neurosci. (2015) 18:1421–31. doi: 10.1038/nn.4108
19. Verheij RA, Maas J, and Groenewegen PP. Urban—Rural health differences and the availability of green space. Eur Urban Regional Stud. (2008) 15:307–16. doi: 10.1177/0969776408095107
20. Lederbogen F, Kirsch P, Haddad L, Streit F, Tost H, Schuch P, et al. City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature. (2011) 474:498–501. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.08.018
21. Sudimac S, Sale V, and Kühn S. How nature nurtures: Amygdala activity decreases as the result of a one-hour walk in nature. Mol Psychiatry. (2022) 27:4446–52. doi: 10.1038/s41380-022-01720-6
22. Zhou F, Peng B, Chu M, Zhang H, Shi L, and Ling L. Association between migration paths and mental health of new-generation migrants in China: The mediating effect of social integration. Front Psychiatry. (2022) 13:967291. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.967291
23. Huggins AA, Morales S, Perlman SB, Britton JC, Tallman E, Schultz DH, et al. Neighborhood disadvantage and neural response to predictable and unpredictable threat in youth. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2022) 17:327–36. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsac014
24. Schäffer B, Grether T, Markert A, and Hübner S. Effects of impulsive versus continuous urban traffic noise on annoyance, cognitive performance and endocrine stress response. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:8422. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19148422
25. Ellis BJ, Figueredo AJ, Brumbach BH, and Schlomer GL. Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: The impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Hum Nat. (2009) 20:204–68. doi: 10.1007/s12110-009-9063-7
26. Chang L, Lu HJ, Lansford JE, Skinner AT, Bornstein MH, Steinberg L, et al. Environmental harshness and unpredictability, life history, and social and academic behavior of adolescents in nine countries. Dev Psychol. (2019) 55(4):890–903. doi: 10.1037/dev0000655
27. Boydell J, van Os J, McKenzie K, and Murray RM. The association of inequality with the incidence of schizophrenia–an ecological study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2004) 39:597–9. doi: 10.1007/s00127-004-0789-6
28. Selten J-P, Cantor-Graae E, and Kahn RS. Migration and schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2007) 20:111–5. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328017f68e
29. Putnam RD. Making Democracy Work: Civic Traditions in Modern Italy. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press (1993).
30. Sørensen JFL. Rural–urban differences in bonding and bridging social capital. Regional Stud. (2014) 50:391–410. doi: 10.1080/00343404.2014.918945
31. Debertin DL and Goetz SJ. Social capital formation in rural, urban and suburban communities (Staff Paper No. 159102). University of Kentucky: Department of Agricultural Economics (2013).
32. Westlund, H and Kiyoshi K. Social Capital and Rural Development in the Knowledge Society.Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar Publishing (2013). doi: 10.4337/9781782540601
33. Kokubun K and Yamakawa Y. Social capital mediates the relationship between social distancing and COVID–19 prevalence in Japan. Inquiry: A J Med Care Org Provision Financing. (2021) 58:469580211005189. doi: 10.1177/00469580211005189
34. Veling W, Hoek HW, and Mackenbach JP. Perceived discrimination and the risk of schizophrenia in ethnic minorities: a case-control study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2008) 43:953–9. doi: 10.1007/s00127-008-0381-6
35. Freedman R, Adler LE, MylesWorsley M, Nagamoto HT, Miller C, Kisley M, et al. Inhibitory gating of an evoked response to repeated auditory stimuli in schizophrenic and normal subjects: Human recordings, computer simulation, and an animal model. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1996) 53:1114–21. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830120052009
36. Golembiewski JA. The designed environment and how it affects brain morphology and mental health. HERD. (2016) 9:161–71. doi: 10.1177/1937586715609562
37. Kapur S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. (2003) 160:13–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.13
38. Corlett PR, Honey GD, and Fletcher PC. From prediction error to psychosis: Ketamine as a pharmacological model of delusions. J Psychopharmacol (Oxf). (2007) 21:238–52. doi: 10.1177/0269881107077716
39. Fletcher PC and Frith CD. Perceiving is believing: A Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2009) 10:4858. doi: 10.1038/nrn2536
40. Haarsma J, Fletcher PC, Griffin JD, Taverne HJ, Ziauddeen H, Spencer TJ, et al. Precision weighting of cortical unsigned prediction error signals benefits learning, is mediated by dopamine, and is impaired in psychosis. Mol Psychiatry. (2021) 26:5320–33. doi: 10.1038/s4138002008038
41. Zahavi D. Self-Awareness and Alterity: A Phenomenological Investigation. Evanston, Ill: Northwestern University Press (1999).
42. Parnas J, Møller P, Kircher T, Thalbitzer J, Jansson L, Handest P, et al. EASE: examination of anomalous self-experience. Psychopathology. (2005) 38:236–58. doi: 10.1159/000088441
43. Parnas J and Handest P. Phenomenology of anomalous self-experience in early schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry. (2003) 44:121–34. doi: 10.1053/comp.2003.50017
44. Parnas J and Henriksen MG. Mysticism and schizophrenia: A phenomenological exploration of the structure of consciousness in the schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Conscious Cogn Int J. (2016) 43:75–88. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.05.010
45. Parnas J and Sass L. The structure of self-consciousness in schizophrenia. In: Gallagher S, editor. The Oxford Handbook of the Self. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press (2011).
46. Sass LA and Parnas J. Schizophrenia, consciousness, and the self. Schizophr Bull. (2003) 29:427–44. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007017
47. Langdon R, Connors MH, and Connaughton E. Social cognition and social judgment in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res Cogn. (2014) 1(4):171–4. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2014.10.001
48. Pienkos E. Intersubjectivity and its role in schizophrenic experience. Humanist Psychol. (2015) 43:194–209. doi: 10.1080/08873267.2014.990459
49. Ebisch SJH and Gallese V. A neuroscientific perspective on the nature of altered self-other relationships in schizophrenia. J Conscious Stud. (2015) 22:220–40.
50. Ebisch SJH. The self and its nature: A psychopathological perspective on the risk-reducing effects of environmental green space for psychosis. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:531840. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.531840
51. Cermolacce M, Naudin J, and Parnas J. The “minimal self” in psychopathology: Re-examining the self-disorders in the schizophrenia spectrum. Conscious Cogn Int J. (2007) 16:703–14. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.05.013
Keywords: schizophrenia, self-disorders, predictive coding, urbanicity, psychosis
Citation: Mosca A, Pettorruso M and Martinotti G (2025) “Hell is other people”. Schizophrenia and urbanicity in the light of predictive coding theory. Front. Psychiatry 16:1574944. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1574944
Received: 11 February 2025; Accepted: 11 August 2025;
Published: 28 August 2025.
Edited by:
Eric D. Achtyes, Western Michigan University, United StatesReviewed by:
Lars Muckli, University of Glasgow, United KingdomBeier Yao, McLean Hospital, United States
Jessica Fattal, Northwestern University, United States
Copyright © 2025 Mosca, Pettorruso and Martinotti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Alessio Mosca, YWxlc3Npby5tb3NjYTkwOUBnbWFpbC5jb20=