Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 15 October 2025

Sec. Autism

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1575940

Red flags in global autism data: a forensic analysis of prevalence patterns and official aid dependencies

  • College of Education and Humanities, Nanchang Institute of Technology, Nanchang, China

Introduction: The literature extensively examines the global incidence rate of autism, emphasizing the need to scrutinize reported figures for potential anomalies, particularly addressing overdiagnosis concerns.

Methods: Our forensic analysis employing Benford's Law and Mean Absolute Deviation indicates significant statistical irregularities and potential overdiagnosis, especially post-DSM-5 implementation, suggesting diagnostic criteria changes drive upward trends. The segmented analysis reveals this relationship intensified in low-income countries post-DSM-5 while remaining non-significant in high-income nations.

Results: Based on 206 countries over 1990-2019, our findings suggest official aid received causes upward trends in autism cases for both genders. Sub-sample analysis indicates positive effects are pronounced in countries with low income, health expenditures, mental health services, government effectiveness, and weak democracies. Results remain robust through instrumental variable and lagged analyses addressing endogeneity concerns.

Discussion: While Benford's Law suggests overdiagnosis patterns, both genuine increases and diagnostic inflation produce similar empirical results, preventing definitive conclusions. Nevertheless, these statistical red flags warrant future research and governmental vigilance when monitoring dramatic prevalence increases. This research addresses a critical literature gap, encouraging scholarly inquiry into reported autism prevalence complexities.

Key message

Global autism diagnosis rates are potentially distorted by international aid and diagnostic criteria changes, particularly in low-income countries. Our study using Benford’s Law reveals complex interactions between financial support, healthcare systems, and medical reporting that challenge conventional understanding of autism prevalence.

1 Introduction

The Signal Detection Theory, a generally applied framework in psychology and epidemiology, highlights the significance of discriminating genuine signals from background noise (1). In the context of autism spectrum disorder prevalence, the term ‘signal’ implies the authentic rise in cases, while ‘noise’ comprehends probable distortions or misreporting. This theoretical framework becomes particularly relevant when examining global autism prevalence patterns, as failure to address anomalies poses the risk of misconstruing authentic epidemiological trends or, conversely, neglecting systemic issues within reported data and diagnostic processes. The historical evolution and prevalence of autism spectrum disorder exemplify this challenge, having undergone significant shifts across different parts of the world that reflect changes in diagnostic criteria, awareness, and societal discernments. Initially identified in the mid-20th century, autism was considered rare, and its prevalence was underestimated due to limited awareness and diagnostic tools. However, contemporary examination of global incidence rates reveals the need to not only assess the extent of reported increases but also systematically examine these figures for probable anomalies that may confound our understanding of true epidemiological patterns.

Over time, advancements in research and refinements in diagnostic criteria, such as the introduction of the DSM-III, DSM-IV, and DSM-5 (24), contributed to an apparent surge in reported autism cases. Theories including the broadening of the autism spectrum and greater awareness leading to more precise diagnoses have been proposed to explain this rise (5, 6). However, awareness alone cannot fully account for the increase, as it overlooks additional contributory factors (7). Recent theoretical frameworks, particularly the prevalence inflation hypothesis, argue that mental health awareness campaigns may inadvertently inflate reporting by both improving recognition of genuine cases and encouraging overinterpretation of subclinical symptoms (5, 8, 9). Complicating matters further, potential diagnostic overlap between autism spectrum disorder and personality disorders, given shared features in social communication, emotional regulation, and interpersonal functioning, can result in misattribution and diagnostic uncertainty (10).

These complexities are intensified by systemic incentives, as individuals with ASD diagnoses often gain preferential access to services and support compared to those with other developmental conditions. For instance, the Israel Ministry of Health in 2008 announced that children with ASD are eligible for an extended basket of care (14 hours weekly), additional treatments, and special education facilities from their first year of life through the age of 7, with added treatments available for older children and adolescents up to the age of 18 (11). Conversely, children identified with other developmental or mental disorders often do not have access to these extensive facilities. Therefore, the convergence of shared symptomology, increased awareness, destigmatization, and special ASD-specific services may create inducements for the overdiagnosis of ASD in comparison to other conditions (12).

Data from the Global Burden of Disease Study (13) reflect these trends, revealing that high-income countries exhibited only a modest 20 percent relative increase in autism incidence between 1990 and 2019 (14), whereas low-income countries experienced a striking 114 percent absolute increase, with diagnosed cases rising from 344,606 in 1990 to 736,655 in 2019 (Figure 1). This disparity underscores the importance of examining contextual influences such as healthcare infrastructure, diagnostic capacity, and awareness, since understanding these dynamics is vital not only for identifying potential anomalies in reporting but also for shaping future research directions in the field (15). Researchers have consistently emphasized the inadequate healthcare resources in low-income countries, suggesting that these nations may face challenges in identifying and addressing mental health issues such as ASD (1619).

Figure 1
Four stacked area charts showing neurodevelopmental disorder cases from 1990 to 2019 by gender and income level: Lower Middle, High, Middle, and Low Income (World Bank). Purple areas represent females, orange areas represent males. All charts indicate a gradual increase in cases over time for both genders. Data source: IHME, Global Burden of Disease (2019).

Figure 1. Gender-based statistics of autism cases. Data supported by the institute for health metrics and evaluation (IHME).

Nevertheless, a closer inspection of secondary data spanning from 1990 to 2019 discloses a surprising inclination. Contrary to expectations (20, 21), there is a consistent and even elevated growth of autism cases in low-income countries, surpassing the rates observed in high-income countries. This unforeseen rise prompts an exploration of potential influencers that underwrite this phenomenon. In a study conducted by Davidovitch et al. (22), clinicians expressed concerns regarding the perceived overdiagnosis of both Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Consequently, one plausible explanation is the heightened emphasis on the disorder might have led to overdiagnosis, contributing to the observed growth. Furthermore, researchers highlight the overlap in symptoms between ASD and some other conditions, including intellectual and learning disabilities, as well as developmental language disorders (2325). These diagnostic challenges are compounded by significant limitations in epidemiological surveillance systems, particularly age-restricted data collection that focuses primarily on eight-year-old populations while providing limited information on autism prevalence during adolescence, youth, or adulthood (26). Additionally, substantial gender disparities in diagnostic practices contribute to systematic underestimation, as traditional diagnostic criteria and assessment tools may inadequately capture autism presentations in females, leading to potential underdiagnosis in this population and skewing overall prevalence estimates toward male-dominated samples.

To systematically detect potential reporting anomalies and overdiagnosis patterns, we employ Benford’s Law, which has proven effective in healthcare fraud detection (2729) and identifying fabricated data in scientific research that subsequently led to retractions (30). The law is particularly suited for autism prevalence data as it naturally spans multiple orders of magnitude across countries, a key requirement for Benford’s Law applicability (31), and can detect systematic reporting bias when external incentives influence diagnostic patterns (32). Probing deeper into this proposition, we examined the correlation between the growth pattern of autism cases and the influx of health-related aid, which exposed a significant connection. This nuanced analysis sheds light on the interplay of factors prompting the identification and reporting of autism cases on a global scale. Our contribution centers around a comprehensive inspection of reported autism cases across 206 countries from 1990 to 2019, marking the first wide-ranging analysis for potential anomalies and overdiagnosis in this extensive panel data approach. Leveraging statistical methodologies such as Benford’s Law (28) and Mean Absolute Deviation (MAD) (33), we systematically examined the dataset to reveal previously unexplored insights. Building upon previous research that underscores the overdiagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) due to shared symptomology, heightened awareness, and exclusive ASD-specific services (22, 3439), we extend this argument through a unique comparative lens.

Current research explored the association between net funding received for healthcare by countries and reported autism cases, with a precise focus on gender-specific data. To strengthen the rigor of our study, we introduced vital control variables, including maternal/neonatal disorders, mental health policy, government effectiveness, out-of-pocket expenditures, population size, sex ratio at birth, and gross domestic product. The breakdown of our data based on mental health service levels, income, health expenditure, and government effectiveness adds granularity to our findings. Ensuring the robustness of our results, we conducted thorough analyses across various subsamples, employed instrumental variable techniques with carefully justified lagged aid instruments that address bidirectional causality concerns through established economic theory, and explored different lagged selections with comprehensive diagnostic testing to verify instrument validity. This comprehensive approach strengthens the validity of our insights and contributes valuable information for policymakers, healthcare practitioners, and researchers aiming to enhance the accuracy of autism diagnoses globally. This strategic research not only provides a deeper understanding of the upsurge in autism cases but also establishes a robust foundation for weighing the reliability and validity of the reported statistics.

2 Literature and theoretical foundation

2.1 Autism prevalence and history of diagnosis in developmental disorders

Over the past century, the understanding and prevalence of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have endured a significant revolution. Initially thought of as a rare condition with inadequate recognition, the term “autism” was devised by Leo Kanner in 1943 (40). As diagnostic criteria evolved, awareness improved, societal understanding enhanced, and reported prevalence rates surged from the 1990s to the early 2000s. Recent research has revealed the intricate interplay of genetic, environmental, and epigenetic aspects contributing to ASD development, with developments in genetics shedding light on associated risk genes (41, 42). Existing prevalence approximations, exemplified by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) reporting 1 in 54 children with ASD in the United States as of 2021 (43), reflect not only an increased actual incidence but also heightened awareness and sophisticated diagnostic criteria. This change in perception from scarcity to a more nuanced understanding highlights the complex nature of autism’s prevalence. The steady rise in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) prevalence stands out among some severe developmental disorders (44). While a genetic source is well-established, with numerous probable contributing genes identified (45), it’s notable that hereditary and de novo gene variations account for only 10–20% of ASD cases (46). Current studies link ASD risk to various issues, including advanced parental age, maternal pregnancy-related factors, birth complications, the use of technological devices in early childhood and environmental effects (47; 4851).

Despite these factors, ASD lacks biological indicators, trusting solely on developmental history assessment, clinical observations, questionnaires, and semi-structured interviews for diagnosis. Some researchers suggest that factors related to seeking a diagnosis and the diagnostic procedure itself underwrite the rising ASD trend. These comprise diagnostic expansion, the presence of other neurodevelopmental disorders, diagnostic substitution, coupling to services, awareness, resolution, and overdiagnosis (15, 5256). The application of Benford’s Law to medical diagnostic data has emerged as a valuable forensic tool for detecting potential anomalies in healthcare reporting (57). Studies have successfully applied Benford’s Law to identify irregularities in clinical trial data, pharmaceutical research, and epidemiological surveillance systems, demonstrating its effectiveness in healthcare contexts where systematic reporting bias may occur (30, 58). Particularly relevant to autism prevalence analysis, Benford’s Law has proven to be able to detect fabricated or manipulated data in scientific research that subsequently led to journal retractions, indicating its utility for identifying anomalous patterns in medical datasets that span multiple orders of magnitude across different populations (28, 59).

Behavioral psychologists are known to come across challenges in differentiating between ASD and other developmental disorders owing to overlapping behavioral features. Down Syndrome, with its shared challenges in terms of communication and social interaction, might get misdiagnosed as ASD, especially during periods of decreasing prevalence (60, 61). Likewise, ADHD, with symptoms similar to impulsivity, hyperactivity, and inattention, could lead to overlaps in clinical assessments and probable misdiagnoses (62, 63). Figure 2, shows a comprehensive inspection of the growth of numerous developmental disorders and intelligence quotient (IQ) measured globally from years 1990 to 2019. Notably, this representation captures dynamic trends in Down Syndrome, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), and autism spectrum disorder (ASD), alongside male and female IQ scores. The latter part of the figure shows a significant spike in the co-occurrence of developmental disorders in 2016 and 2017, as demonstrated by substantial positive values in Down Syndrome, ADHD, and IQ measures. This period proposes a potential surge in the simultaneous growth of these conditions and there exists a potential for misdiagnosis, particularly concerning ASD.

Figure 2
Stacked area chart showing trends from 1990 to 2019 for various conditions including Down syndrome, ADHD, IQ for females and males, idiopathic IQ, autism females, and autism males. The chart indicates a significant peak for autism males in 2017, with other conditions varying over the years. Colors differentiate each category for clarity.

Figure 2. Growth rate of neurological disorders from 1990-2019.

Challenges arise from the lack of a standard definition and complications in quantifying overdiagnosis, complicating research, policy, and communication (6466). In response to the challenges posed by the lack of a standard definition and difficulties in quantifying overdiagnosis in ASD, our research takes a comprehensive method. We employ a panel data methodology, analyzing reported ASD cases globally from 1990 to 2019. This technique allows us to report the intricacies of the growing prevalence while considering several factors that may influence the accuracy of diagnoses. To ensure a robust analysis, we meticulously control for possible influences from coexisting factors, including maternal/neonatal disorders, mental health policy, government effectiveness, out-of-pocket expenditures, population size, sex ratio at birth, and gross domestic product. By integrating these variables into our study, we aim to illuminate the nuanced interplay between these factors and the reported upsurge in ASD cases. This holistic approach enables a more comprehensive understanding of the dynamics contributing to the growing landscape of ASD prevalence.

2.2 Healthcare funding and ASD incidence

Contrary to the idea, “Giving some help to many in need is preferable to giving only some people a lot of help”, the situation for children with autism in developing countries is different. Given the novelty of the disorder and its lack of widespread understanding, families often find themselves caught off guard. In the pursuit of assistance, parents readily turn to specialized centers and institutes that place children in devoted care programs from the outset (67, 68). While early challenges could be managed by general specialists working with various developmental disorders (69), parents often incur additional expenses for exclusive care prompted by diagnoses from behavioral practitioners (70, 71). While wealthier families can pay for private practitioners, those with scarce resources may even incur debt in their desperation to support their children. Research in low-income countries (7274) frequently noted the inclination to establish distinct services for autism assessment and interventions.

Despite effective lobbying for specialist provisions in affluent countries, these services are derived at a high cost. This not only poses financial loads but also hinders access to help for children with other developmental difficulties, resulting in waiting lists and a concentration of services in specific urban areas, leaving rural communities unsupported (75, 76). As special institutions allocate significant resources to accommodate autism cases, accompanied by substantial financial contributions from parents, there exists a potential for misdiagnosis/overdiagnosis (77, 78). This pattern of resource concentration and potential overdiagnosis has been documented across various contexts, yet existing research examining these dynamics has several critical limitations. Previous investigations (77, 78) have primarily relied on cross-sectional designs in high-income settings, failing to establish temporal relationships between funding patterns and diagnostic changes in developing countries where infrastructure is rapidly expanding through international aid. While earlier studies have observed these phenomena, they lacked systematic examination of the underlying economic mechanisms that drive such patterns, particularly how targeted mental health aid creates specific institutional incentives in resource-constrained environments.

Moreover, the theoretical mechanism connecting healthcare funding to diagnostic behavior works through multiple interconnected pathways that align with supplier-induced demand theory in health economics (79). Healthcare funding, mainly targeted aid for mental health services, creates systematic incentives that can influence diagnostic patterns through three primary channels: infrastructure development, professional training, and service expansion (80, 81). Firstly, increased funding leads to the establishment of specialized diagnostic facilities and recruitment of trained professionals, creating institutional capacity that must be utilized to justify continued investment. Second, funding-dependent training programs may inadvertently emphasize autism recognition over differential diagnosis, leading to diagnostic confirmation bias where practitioners are more likely to identify autism symptoms rather than alternative explanations (8284). Third, it is proposed that the availability of autism-specific services funded through targeted aid creates systematic incentives for autism diagnoses over other developmental conditions that lack equivalent funding streams, as families and practitioners navigate toward better-resourced diagnostic categories (11, 8486). This supplier-induced demand framework suggests that the increased reporting of autism cases may be systematically related to amplified health grants through these institutional and professional incentive structures rather than reflecting genuine epidemiological changes.

This misdiagnosis phenomenon entwines with the burden on the health sector, generating a complex situation as there is an increased prevalence of coexisting physical health conditions in autistic individuals when contrasted with the broader population (8789). The increased reporting of autism cases could be related to amplified net health grants, establishing a link where heightened funding might inadvertently lead to an uptick in reported autism cases. Notably, data specifies that adults on the autism spectrum, in contrast to their non-autistic counterparts, display a higher probability of receiving diagnoses for conditions such as epilepsy, cardiovascular diseases encompassing dyslipidemia and hypertension, along diabetes (77, 90). Moreover, individuals with ASD face a heightened risk of premature mortality in comparison to the general population. Considering data from two extensive nationwide population-based Swedish registers, documented significantly elevated mortality rates amongst autistic individuals when compared to the general population (91). This intricate connection highlights the importance of careful consideration in the allocation of resources, stressing the need for accurate diagnoses to evade the inflation of autism statistics. This study advances beyond descriptive accounts from prior research by providing both longitudinal analysis and a comprehensive theoretical framework to examine whether well-intentioned healthcare aid policies may produce unintended diagnostic consequences, challenging the widespread assumption that increased mental health funding automatically leads to improved diagnostic accuracy.

2.3 Sub-sample analysis of healthcare aids received and autism incidence

To determine the potential existence of overdiagnosis, it is imperative to establish a consistent and distinct definition while also differentiating it from other related issues. Furthermore, the development of effective assessment methods is crucial for gauging the prevalence of overdiagnosis accurately. In light of these considerations, our research endeavors to inspect the correlation between net official development assistance and official aid allocated for healthcare by countries and the reported cases of ASD. This study extends across various dimensions, including the stratification of countries in terms of their mental health services, sub-sample analysis considering income levels, scrutiny of health expenditure, examination of government effectiveness, and exploration of The Economist Democracy Index.

Researchers emphasize the pivotal role of mental health services in influencing the prevalence and reporting patterns of such disorders (92, 93). The stratification based on income levels draws theoretical backing from economic frameworks, aligning with the principles posited by Kawachi et al. (94), which emphasize the substantial influence of financial resources and healthcare accessibility on the identification and reporting of health-related conditions. The investigation of health expenditure finds theoretical grounding in the Health Belief Model, initially proposed by Rosenstock (95) wherein people are predisposed to search for diagnosis and treatment for conditions perceived as severe and necessitating financial investment. The analysis of government effectiveness is theoretically reinforced by Institutional Theory (96), which posits that the efficacy of governance structures significantly affects the implementation of health policies and hence, the reporting patterns of developmental disorders. In addition, the consideration of The Economist Democracy Index presents a political lens, acknowledging the role of democratic governance in nurturing transparency and inclusivity in healthcare reporting (97). This configuration with established theories and empirical evidence highlights the scholarly rigor of our study, aiming to contribute nuanced insights into the intricate relationships between external aid for healthcare and reported autism cases, while accounting for diverse socio-economic and political contexts.

3 Methodology

3.1 Empirical methodology and baseline model specification

To examine the effect of official aid assistance on ASD, the following baseline line is developed (Equation 1):

AUTi,t= β1(ODAit)+ β2(Xit)+ μi+ γt+ϵit(1)

where AUT signifies the total, female, and male autism cases in country i and time t. ODAit means the official development assistance received, and Xit encompasses a set of control variables on behalf of various factors such as environmental influences, maternal health, mental health policies, government effectiveness, out-of-pocket expenditures, population size, sex ratio at birth, and gross domestic product. This baseline model incorporates country and time-fixed effects denoted by μi and γt, respectively to account for unobserved heterogeneity. The error term is denoted by ϵit. The assortment of this model is grounded in theoretical considerations and is aligned with empirical research suggesting potential links between development assistance and health outcomes (98, 99). The utilization of fixed effects and control variables augments the internal validity of the model, allowing for a more nuanced scrutiny of the impact of ODA on autism prevalence, while concurrently accounting for other influential factors. This comprehensive empirical methodology warrants a rigorous analysis of the relationship between development assistance and autism, providing valuable insights into the potential determinants of autism prevalence worldwide.

3.2 Endogeneity concern and instrumental variable

Conventional ordinary least squares (OLS) estimation cannot adequately address the endogeneity concerns inherent in examining the relationship between official aid and autism spectrum disorder cases. The endogeneity problem manifests through bidirectional causality, whereby aid may enhance autism reporting via improved diagnostic infrastructure and training, while simultaneously, countries demonstrating higher autism prevalence may attract increased targeted health assistance from donors responding to documented epidemiological needs. Furthermore, omitted variable bias emerges from unobserved heterogeneity in healthcare system quality, government effectiveness, and international engagement levels that concurrently influence both aid allocation decisions and diagnostic capacity for autism spectrum disorders. These confounding factors preclude the isolation of the true causal effect of official aid on reported autism cases, as the relationship operates through multiple interconnected and potentially simultaneous pathways.

To address these methodological challenges, we employ instrumental variable two-stage least squares (IV-2SLS) estimation with fixed effects. Lagged values of official aid serve as instrumental variables based on the theoretical justification that past aid allocations are correlated with current aid levels (satisfying the relevance condition) but are predetermined and thus uncorrelated with contemporary unobserved factors affecting current autism reporting (satisfying the exogeneity condition). The utilization of lagged values as instrumental variables is well-established in the empirical literature, particularly in contexts where identifying truly exogenous external instruments proves methodologically challenging or theoretically implausible (100102).

The validity and strength of our instrumental variables are rigorously assessed through comprehensive diagnostic testing: first-stage relevance is evaluated using Sanderson-Windmeijer F-statistics and Chi-square tests, overidentification restrictions are tested via Hansen J-statistics to ensure instrument exogeneity, under identification is examined through Kleibergen-Paap rank LM tests to confirm instrument relevance, and weak instrument concerns are addressed using Kleibergen-Paap rank F-statistics to verify sufficient instrument strength for reliable inference.

3.3 Data description and summary of statistical techniques

3.3.1 Data description

We have considered country-level autism cases from the Institute for Health Metrics and Evaluation (IHME). Our data set comprised 6180 observations from 206 countries, from the year 1990 to 2019. The details of the variables’ descriptions and sources of data collection are given in Table 1. The official development aid received by the country for healthcare initiatives was the dependent variable and the data was collected from the World Bank and measured in constant US$. The reported cases of autism spectrum disorder were considered inclusive of those with autism, Asperger Syndrome, and other autistic spectrum disorders. The cases were measured in total and differentiated by sex. Apart from the main independent variable, several control variables are also incorporated including biological, psychological, and socioeconomic factors to address omitted variable bias. Maternal and neonatal disorders, for instance, play a crucial role in shaping the trajectory of autism cases. Research has constantly shown that prenatal complications, such as maternal infections and nutritional deficiencies, may contribute to an increased risk of ASD in offspring (103, 104). This emphasizes the significance of understanding and addressing maternal health as a key determinant in the prevalence of autism cases. Therefore, in this research maternal and neonatal disorders were assessed in the form of a principal component (PC) derived from three health constructs: communicable, maternal, neonatal, and nutritional diseases; maternal and neonatal disorders; and neonatal disorders specifically. Furthermore, the construction and implementation of mental health policies at the state level significantly impact the reported cases of autism. Nations with comprehensive mental health policies are well-equipped to sense, diagnose, and offer appropriate interventions for individuals with ASD. The absence or inadequacy of such policies may result in misreporting and false identification of autism cases (105). This highlights the need for robust mental health infrastructure and policies to effectively address the challenges posed by autism.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Description of variables and data sources.

Government effectiveness, as measured by the sub-component of World Governance Indicators (WGI), is one more critical factor influencing reported autism cases. Efficient and responsive governance guarantees the allocation of resources for early intervention programs, research initiatives, and support services, consequently positively affecting the accuracy and comprehensiveness of autism reporting (106). The theoretical underpinning stands in the role of governance in shaping public health outcomes, emphasizing the necessity for effective governance to alleviate the impact of autism. Socioeconomic factors, such as out-of-pocket expenditures for healthcare, likewise play a role in reported cases of autism. Families with limited financial resources may experience challenges in accessing diagnostic and therapeutic services, possibly leading to misreporting in economically disadvantaged regions (107). Population size, sex ratio at birth, and gross domestic product (GDP) are additional aspects that can impact the reported cases of autism. Larger populations may exhibit higher absolute numbers of autism cases, while variations in sex ratio at birth may contribute to gender-specific differences in prevalence (108110). Moreover, the GDP of a country correlates with its healthcare infrastructure and resources, influencing the discovery and reporting of autism cases (111).

In conclusion, the reported cases of autism are related to a multitude of factors, encompassing maternal and neonatal health, mental health policies, government effectiveness, socioeconomic variables, and demographic indicators. Understanding and addressing these factors through a multidimensional method is crucial for truthful reporting, effective intervention, and the development of targeted policies to support individuals with ASD and their families. This all-inclusive perspective aligns with the bio-psycho-social model of health (112, 113), highlighting the need to consider biological, psychological, and social determinants to comprehend and address the complexity of autism prevalence.

3.3.2 Summary statistics

Table 2 presents summary statistics showing substantial variability in autism prevalence with males demonstrating significantly higher rates than females, consistent with existing literature. Official aid varies considerably across entities, while approximately half have formal mental health policies in place. Control variables include government effectiveness, out-of-pocket health expenditures, population sizes, and a principal component of maternal and neonatal disorders derived from communicable, maternal, neonatal, and nutritional disease constructs.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary statistics.

4 Empirical results

4.1 Preliminary analysis

Benford’s Law and the Mean Absolute Deviation (MAD) are statistical tools that fulfill a central objective, which is the possibility of anomalies inherent in reported global incidences of autism. Benford’s Law inspects the distribution of leading digits in numerical data, and deviations from this law may specify irregularities or anomalies. Likewise, we employed another robust analysis called, MAD, a measure of the dispersion of data points from the mean, providing insights into the overall inconsistency of a dataset. Our implementation pools autism case data across all countries within each income group over the 2015–2019 period, generating sufficient observations (1,920 for low-income and 4,260 for high-income countries) for reliable first-digit distribution analysis rather than conducting yearly country-specific tests. The rationale behind selecting the years 2015 to 2019 for the analysis of global autism cases is grounded in a cautious strategy to examine a post-Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) era, intentionally circumventing the instantaneous repercussions of the DSM-5’s release in 2013. This step delivers a unique opportunity for a nuanced exploration of inclinations and patterns in reported autism cases, independent of the immediate impact stemming from the revised diagnostic criteria introduced by the DSM-5. The research justification for this timeframe lies in its potential to disclose stabilized and assimilated diagnostic practices on a global scale. Moving past the initial adjustment period to the new criteria, healthcare systems, clinicians, and researchers are expected to have adapted to the DSM-5 guidelines, potentially influencing case identification and reporting practices. By concentrating on the years 2015 to 2019, the analysis aims to encapsulate a duration during which the field has possibly acclimated to the DSM-5 revisions. Benford’s Law, applied to the leading digits of the observed values, is a method to assess the conformity of the data distribution to a theoretically expected pattern. Moreover, MAD assisted as a valuable metric to gauge the average absolute difference of observed values from their mean. This indicates the overall variability in reported autism cases.

 Mean Absolute Deviation= 1N1NfiXiX¯

Given the concerning spike in multiple developmental disorders observed during 2016-2017 (Figure 2), our analysis focuses on the 2015–2019 period to capture these anomalous patterns while allowing sufficient post-DSM-5 (2) adjustment time. Benford’s Law violations are assessed using chi-square goodness-of-fit tests comparing observed versus expected first-digit distributions across 1,920 low-income and 710 high-income country observations. The chi-square statistic measures the magnitude of deviation from Benford’s theoretical distribution, with higher values indicating greater irregularities. Low-income countries show χ² = 72.91 (p = 0.694) compared to high-income countries at χ² = 37.09 (p = 0.695), indicating substantially greater systematic deviations from expected patterns. Similarly, MAD analysis reveals low-income countries exhibit nearly double the variability (1.647 vs 0.830), corroborating systematic differences in reporting patterns. These statistical disparities suggest potential data quality concerns in autism prevalence reporting, particularly within resource-constrained settings where external incentives may influence diagnostic practices.

The low-income countries’ dataset discloses varying levels of deviation from the mean, as reflected by the calculated MAD values. Meanwhile, Benford’s Law analysis on the leading digits allows for an additional layer of scrutiny, recognizing potential irregularities in the distribution of observed values. These analytical techniques assist as valuable tools for identifying patterns, trends, or discrepancies that warrant further investigation. Likewise, high-income countries undergo the same analytical process. MAD values provide insights into the dispersion of reported autism cases from the mean, while Benford’s Law analysis sheds light on the distribution of leading digits. A comparative analysis amongst the low and high-income countries’ datasets can further clarify potential disparities or similarities in the reported autism cases. Upon graphically representing these datasets, the visualizations display the trends, deviations, and distributions (Figures 3, 4).

Figure 3
Bar chart comparing observed values and Benford's law across digits one to nine. Blue bars represent observed values, decreasing steadily. Orange bars labeled “X” are minimal. A gray line shows Benford's expected distribution, with a clear decline. A yellow line indicates minimal mean absolute deviation (MAD) across all digits.

Figure 3. Autism prevalence in high-income countries (2015-2019): MAD and Benford’s Law compliance.

Figure 4
Bar and line chart comparing observed frequencies to Benford's Law. The blue bars represent observed data, and the grey line shows Benford's predictions. Orange bars signify additional data labeled as X, while the yellow line depicts the Mean Absolute Deviation (MAD). Data points range from one to nine on the x-axis and zero to forty on the y-axis. Observed frequencies generally decrease as predicted by Benford's Law, with minor deviations.

Figure 4. Autism prevalence in low-income countries (2015-2019): MAD and Benford’s Law compliance.

4.2 Main results

Table 3 elaborates on the baseline proposition behind this research which is the influence of official aid received on the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder. The study discovers this relationship by employing two models: Model 1, which eliminates fixed effects, and Model 2, which includes both year and country fixed effects. In Model 1, the focal variable, Official Aid Received, appears as a statistically significant predictor of the prevalence of autism across all categories, overall prevalence, male and female prevalence. The coefficients of 0.085, 0.084, and 0.088 for overall prevalence, male, and female prevalence, respectively, indicate a high statistical significance. This infers that, on average, a one-unit increase in official aid received is associated with the corresponding increase in prevalence across the specified categories. Moving to Model 2, which accounts for fixed effects, the robustness of the relationship between official aid received and ASD prevalence is constant. The coefficients rise slightly to 0.094, 0.094, and 0.096 for overall prevalence, male prevalence, and female prevalence, respectively, and retain their high level of significance. The inclusion of fixed effects increases the model’s control for potential confounding factors, reinforcing the integrity of the findings.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The impact of official aid received on the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder.

The positive relationship between official aid received and reported autism incidence identified in this study reflects broader patterns documented in the literature regarding unintended consequences of targeted healthcare funding. This finding aligns with research by Sarr et al. (84), who emphasize how increased funding to specific healthcare sectors can produce inadvertent effects on diagnostic practices. Similarly, the economic impact literature demonstrates how resource influxes can alter healthcare delivery patterns, as shown by Smith et al. (114) in their analysis of pandemic influenza funding effects on health outcomes. Marty et al. (115), further illuminate these dynamics through their examination of healthcare resource allocation, particularly highlighting how financial resource surges can lead to unintended consequences including over-diagnosis tendencies.

The scatter plot analysis (Figure 5) visually demonstrates the differential correlation patterns observed in our statistical models. Low-income countries show a positive correlation between official aid and autism prevalence, while high-income countries display a less pronounced association. These observed patterns are consistent with our regression findings across income groups, though the correlation analysis alone cannot distinguish between genuine diagnostic improvement and potential reporting irregularities. The visual data supports the statistical evidence of systematic differences between income groups but requires interpretation alongside our broader analytical framework that includes controls for institutional capacity and healthcare infrastructure to understand the underlying mechanisms driving these relationships. Given these differential patterns, we conducted comprehensive sub-sample analyses to examine how various country characteristics moderate the aid-autism relationship, the results of which we report in the following sections.

Figure 5
Scatter plot showing the correlation of autism with official aid by income group. Blue dots represent low-income groups, and red triangles represent high-income groups. Trend lines for both groups are included. Autism is measured on the vertical axis, and official aid on the horizontal axis.

Figure 5. Scatter plot of correlation between official aid and autism prevalence by income group.

Table 4 presents sub-sample analysis results examining how mental health service availability moderates the relationship between official aid and autism prevalence. The analysis divides countries into low and high mental health services groups based on median values (≤8.03 vs >8.03). In low mental health service countries, official aid demonstrates statistically significant positive associations with overall autism prevalence, female autism prevalence, and male autism prevalence. Conversely, in high mental health service countries, these associations become non-significant, indicating that robust mental health infrastructure attenuates the aid-prevalence relationship. Control variables generally behave as expected, with government effectiveness showing negative associations and population size showing positive associations across both groups.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The impact of official aid received on the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder: sub-sample analysis of countries with high and low mental health services.

These findings directly support the supplier-induced demand theory outlined in our theoretical framework, where healthcare funding creates systematic incentives that influence diagnostic patterns through infrastructure development, professional training, and service expansion (79, 80). This aligns with research by Greenspoon and Saklofske (116) Matson and Kozlowski (117), and Zablotsky et al. (118), who highlighted the critical role that mental health service availability and convenience play in ensuring correct autism diagnoses. The significant aid-prevalence relationship in low-service countries exemplifies how funding-dependent training programs may emphasize autism recognition over differential diagnosis, creating confirmation bias toward autism symptoms rather than alternative explanations (84). This is particularly concerning given documented overlapping behavioral features between ASD, Down Syndrome, and ADHD that complicate clinical assessments (60, 62). The moderation effect validates our theoretical proposition that autism-specific services funded through targeted aid create systematic incentives for autism diagnoses over other developmental conditions lacking equivalent funding streams.

In well-resourced healthcare systems with robust mental health infrastructure, standardized diagnostic protocols and professional training reduce susceptibility to these funding influences, explaining the non-significant results in high-service countries. Conversely, in resource-constrained settings, the three-channel mechanism operates more prominently, where increased funding enhances diagnostic capacity without corresponding improvements in differential diagnostic rigor. These results underscore the importance of context-specific approaches to healthcare aid allocation, where policymakers should prioritize establishing robust diagnostic frameworks and professional training standards before expanding screening initiatives in resource-poor settings.

Table 5 explains the relationship between official aid and autism prevalence across four income-level country categories. The results reveal contrasting patterns across income groups. In high-income countries, official aid demonstrates significant negative associations with autism prevalence (β = -0.519 for total, -0.584 for females, -0.508 for males; all p < 0.001). Upper-middle-income countries show similar negative effects with reduced magnitude. The relationship reverses in lower-middle-income countries, where positive associations emerge (β = 0.370, 0.378, 0.367 respectively; all p < 0.001), with lower-income countries displaying comparable positive patterns with smaller coefficients.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. The impact of official aid received on the prevalence of ASD: sub-sample analysis for the level of income.

This income-stratified pattern provides compelling evidence for the Economic Theory of Health Behavior and aligns with research by Kawachi et al. (94), highlighting how financial resources influence healthcare identification and reporting. The findings support broader literature on diagnostic trends (119121), identifying systematic overuse of healthcare facilities particularly prevalent in low- and middle-income countries. This pattern reflects the three-channel mechanism we established theoretically, where funding influxes in resource-constrained settings create diagnostic capacity without corresponding improvements in differential diagnostic frameworks. As Born et al. (122) and Albarqouni et al. (123) demonstrate, augmented attention to health issues through external funding can intensify diagnostic efforts, potentially leading to the positive aid-prevalence associations observed in lower-income settings. The WHO’s call for context-specific strategies to address overdiagnosis (124) becomes particularly relevant here, suggesting that aid effectiveness depends critically on existing healthcare infrastructure and institutional capacity rather than funding levels alone.

Table 6 is revealing distinct patterns that support our theoretical framework on healthcare resource allocation. In low health expenditure countries, official aid demonstrates significant positive associations with autism prevalence (β = 0.239 for total, 0.231 for females, 0.242 for males; all p < 0.001). This unexpected positive association suggests a complex relationship between official aid and reported ASD prevalence in countries with limited healthcare spending. Medium health expenditure countries show no significant associations, with coefficients near zero and small standard errors, demonstrating a lack of meaningful connection between official aid and reported ASD prevalence. High health expenditure countries display significant negative associations (β = -0.032 for total, -0.064 for females, -0.026 for males; p < 0.05), creating a clear gradient from positive to negative effects as health expenditure capacity increases. This negative association aligns more closely with expectations, suggesting that in countries with high health expenditure, financial assistance may contribute to lower reported ASD prevalence.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. The impact of official aid received on the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder: sub-sample analysis for the level of health expenditure.

These findings align with the Health Belief Model framework (95), where healthcare expenditure capacity influences how financial resources translate into diagnostic behavior. In low health expenditure countries, the influx of financial aid likely contributes to heightened global attention on healthcare issues, including neurodevelopmental disorders like autism. Increased awareness and resources might lead to enhanced identification and reporting of ASD cases, possibly resulting in an apparent rise in prevalence, consistent with the overdiagnosis phenomenon (120).

This further aligns with previous research (118, 125) highlighting how awareness and diagnostic practices influence reported ASD prevalence. In medium-expenditure countries, the absence of significant effects suggests these nations maintain a balance in their healthcare systems, with adequate resources for ASD identification and reporting, consequently reducing susceptibility to overdiagnosis due to external financial aid. This interpretation is supported by research from Ehsan et al. (126) and Zeidan et al. (127) underscoring healthcare infrastructure’s importance in influencing ASD prevalence rates. The negative associations in high-expenditure settings raise intriguing questions, suggesting that well-established healthcare systems and diagnostic practices may lead to more accurate identification and reporting of ASD cases, minimizing potential for overdiagnosis. This interpretation aligns with Tekola et al. (19) and Xu et al. (128) emphasizing advanced healthcare infrastructure’s role in refining ASD prevalence estimate accuracy. The unexpected positive association between official aid and reported ASD prevalence in countries with low health expenditure raises important questions about the complex relationship between financial assistance and public health outcomes.

Government effectiveness emerges as a critical moderator in the relationship between official aid and autism prevalence, as demonstrated in Table 7’s sub-sample analysis across three governance categories. Countries with low government effectiveness show significant positive associations between official aid and autism prevalence, indicating that higher aid levels correlate with increased reported ASD cases. Medium government effectiveness countries display similar patterns with comparable coefficients, suggesting persistent positive aid-prevalence relationships in moderate governance contexts. However, high government effectiveness countries exhibit dramatically different results, with strong negative associations, where official aid correlates with lower reported autism prevalence.

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. The impact of official aid received on prevalence of autism spectrum disorder: sub-sample analysis for level of government effectiveness.

This governance-based gradient aligns with Institutional Theory (96) established in our literature, which posits that governance structure efficacy significantly affects health policy implementation and reporting patterns. The positive associations in low and medium effectiveness settings correspond with healthcare system constraints that highlight systematic limitations in accurate symptom detection and reporting capabilities within these governance contexts (74, 119, 124). In these contexts, external aid may serve as critical intervention addressing diagnostic capacity gaps, potentially leading to heightened awareness and increased identification efforts that could contribute to apparent prevalence increases. This mechanism exemplifies our three-channel theoretical framework, where funding influxes create diagnostic infrastructure without corresponding improvements in differential diagnostic rigor, particularly problematic when governance structures cannot effectively oversee diagnostic quality standards. Conversely, the strong negative associations in high effectiveness countries suggest that robust governance structures enable more stringent diagnostic criteria and better control over potential overdiagnosis (35). These countries likely possess the institutional capacity to channel aid toward improving diagnostic accuracy rather than simply expanding identification efforts, reflecting the Social Determinants of Health framework (129), where governance quality shapes how economic resources translate into health outcomes. The governance moderation effect provides compelling evidence that aid effectiveness depends critically on existing institutional capacity to maintain diagnostic standards and quality control mechanisms.

The sub-sample analysis examining official aid’s impact on ASD prevalence across different regime types reveals striking governance-dependent patterns (Table 8). Full democracies show significant negative associations while flawed democracies, hybrid regimes, and authoritarian regimes exhibit positive associations. This divergent pattern aligns with institutional theory and the “selectivity hypothesis” that aid works better in environments with strong governance institutions (131133), Recent evidence also supports this, showing that “the aggregate effect of aid on democracy has become more positive after the Cold War” and that “stable inflows of ‘governance aid’ drive aggregate net positive effects” (134136). Democratic contexts benefit from “institutional efficiency” (137), where transparency and civil society oversight ensure effective resource allocation toward genuine healthcare improvements (138). This strengthens diagnostic infrastructure and reduces stigma (139, 140). Conversely, non-democratic settings suffer from “extractive institutions” (141) that enable elite capture and strategic manipulation of health statistics. Weaker institutional frameworks result in aid misallocation, limited transparency, and perverse incentives that distort diagnostic practices (132, 142, 143). The absence of democratic accountability creates information asymmetries that facilitate diagnostic inflation and resource misappropriation (144).

Table 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. The impact of official aid received on the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder: sub-sample analysis for the level of types of regimes by the economist democracy index.

The instrumental variable two-stage least squares results (Table 9) provide crucial validation of the causal relationship between official aid and autism prevalence, effectively addressing endogeneity concerns that could bias conventional regression estimates. The IV-2SLS approach yields consistently positive and statistically significant coefficients across all specifications, establishing that the observed aid-autism relationship is genuinely causal rather than merely correlational (145). This causal identification definitively rules out reverse causality, where donors might systematically target countries with higher disease burdens, and eliminates concerns about omitted variable bias that could confound the relationship (146). The robust first-stage diagnostics, including F-statistics well above conventional thresholds, confirm instrument strength and validate the reliability of these causal inferences, providing compelling econometric evidence that previous findings reflect true causal effects rather than spurious associations (147).

Table 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Instrumental variable two-stage least square (IV-2SLS) regression estimations.

Table 10 demonstrates winsorization as a robustness check, capping extreme values to mitigate outlier influence while preserving sample size (145). In order to confirm that our results are not driven by outliers or extreme values, we applied winsorization at 1% and 99% cutoffs. For brevity, the subsample of low and high-income countries were divided by median value of GDP. The analysis confirms our core findings with low GDP countries showing significant positive aid coefficients (0.332***) versus negligible effects (0.015) in high GDP countries. This technique demonstrates that observed patterns reflect genuine structural differences rather than statistical anomalies from extreme observations (36), strengthening our inference about systematic differences across economic development levels.

Table 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Winsorized analysis of aid-autism relationship by GDP Level.

The regression results in Table 11 reveal valuable insights into the prevalence of autism, mainly highlighting the significance of lag effects. The inclusion of lagged variables in the regression model consents for the examination of the delayed effects of official aid on autism prevalence. This approach is important in understanding the temporal dynamics of the relationship between official assistance and reported cases of autism. Coefficients for L1, L2, and L3 are constantly positive and significant, indicating that an increase in official aid in one year positively influences the prevalence of autism in the following three years. The results of the analysis deliver compelling evidence in favor of the hypothesis, indicating a noteworthy connection between official aid and the subsequent increase in reported autism cases.

Table 11
www.frontiersin.org

Table 11. 3-years lagged analysis of official aid received on autism prevalence.

The segmented analysis addressing the DSM-5 implementation reveals contrasting patterns in how official aid affects autism prevalence across income groups and diagnostic periods (Table 12; Figure 6). In low-income countries, official aid demonstrates consistently positive associations with autism prevalence (0.042, p<0.01), with the interaction term (Official Aid*DSM-5) showing significant additional effects ranging from 0.016 to 0.023 (p < 0.05 to p < 0.01), indicating that the aid-autism relationship intensified following DSM-5 implementation. Conversely, high-income countries exhibit no significant associations between aid and autism prevalence, with negligible interaction effects across all specifications. Figure 6 illustrates these divergent patterns: low-income countries display sharply steepening slopes post-DSM-5, while high-income countries maintain flat relationships regardless of diagnostic period. These findings present an interpretive challenge, as the observed patterns are consistent with both improved diagnostic capacity in previously under-resourced settings and potential diagnostic inflation enabled by broader DSM-5 criteria and aid-related incentives.

Table 12
www.frontiersin.org

Table 12. Interaction effects of DSM-5 implementation on official-aid ASD prevalence relationship by income group.

Figure 6
Two line graphs compare predictive margins with ninety-five percent confidence intervals for low and high income countries before and after DSM-5. The left graph for low income countries shows a positive trend for both lines. The right graph for high income countries shows a slight decline for both lines. The horizontal axis represents official aid, and the vertical axis represents linear prediction. Blue lines indicate data before DSM-5, while red lines represent data after DSM-5.

Figure 6. Moderating effect of DSM-5 implementation on the relationship between official aid and ASD prevalence by income level.

Summary. The pronounced positive relationship between official aid and autism prevalence in low-income countries with limited healthcare infrastructure suggests a complex interplay of legitimate diagnostic improvements and potential systematic biases. In these under-resourced settings, aid likely enables genuine case identification by providing access to specialized diagnostic services, training healthcare providers in autism recognition, and establishing screening programs that previously did not exist, thereby capturing a substantial population of undiagnosed individuals who lacked access to appropriate evaluation. However, the intensification of this relationship following DSM-5 implementation, particularly in countries with weak governance structures and limited mental health oversight, indicates that expanded diagnostic criteria may have created conditions conducive to both improved detection and diagnostic inflation operating simultaneously. The contrast with high-income countries, where established healthcare systems and robust oversight mechanisms prevent such pronounced aid-diagnosis correlations, suggests that institutional quality plays a crucial role in determining whether increased resources translate into accurate diagnostic improvements or systematic over-reporting. These findings underscore that while aid can meaningfully enhance autism identification capacity in previously neglected populations, the absence of adequate regulatory frameworks and professional oversight may allow legitimate diagnostic expansion to drift toward systematic inflation, necessitating careful consideration of both the beneficial and potentially distortive effects of targeted healthcare funding.

5 Clinical and policy implications

The findings of this research carry profound implications for both policymakers and clinical practitioners operating within the global health architecture. If the overdiagnosis hypothesis is confirmed, international donors and recipient governments must fundamentally reconsider aid allocation mechanisms to prevent perverse diagnostic incentives that distort epidemiological surveillance systems. Policymakers should implement robust oversight frameworks that decouple healthcare funding from reported disease prevalence, instead tying aid disbursements to process indicators such as healthcare infrastructure development, provider training quality, and diagnostic protocol adherence. Clinical practitioners in aid-recipient countries require enhanced training not only in accurate autism identification but also in resisting institutional pressures that may encourage diagnostic inflation, necessitating the establishment of independent clinical review boards and peer consultation networks that can validate complex neurodevelopmental diagnoses.

Conversely, if the relationship reflects genuine diagnostic improvement, the implications suggest that targeted aid interventions can effectively enhance autism detection capacity in under-resourced settings, warranting continued investment in specialized healthcare infrastructure and provider education. However, even under this interpretation, the dramatic differences observed across governance contexts indicate that aid effectiveness is highly conditional on institutional quality, suggesting that donors should prioritize governance strengthening alongside healthcare capacity building. For clinical practice, these findings underscore the critical importance of implementing standardized diagnostic protocols and ensuring adequate specialist supervision, particularly during periods of rapid healthcare system expansion. Regardless of the underlying mechanism, the research demonstrates an urgent need for enhanced data verification systems, independent diagnostic auditing processes, and transparent reporting mechanisms that can distinguish between authentic epidemiological trends and artifact-driven prevalence increases, ensuring that autism prevalence data serves as a reliable foundation for evidence-based policy and clinical decision-making.

6 Limitations

Several methodological constraints limit definitive causal interpretation beyond the interpretive ambiguity already discussed. The reliance on IHME data introduces significant constraints, as these estimates assume relatively uniform diagnostic criteria application globally, which may not reflect substantial heterogeneity in clinical practice, healthcare infrastructure, and cultural understanding of autism across diverse national contexts. The Benford’s Law findings themselves may be influenced by variations in local diagnostic practices, cultural interpretations of autism symptoms, and country-specific reporting protocols that could generate digit irregularities independent of deliberate data manipulation. The analysis lacks granular data on diagnostic severity distributions, healthcare provider incentives, and specific aid conditionality mechanisms that would enable clearer mechanistic understanding. Furthermore, the study cannot account for country-specific variations in DSM-5 implementation timing, training quality, or institutional oversight that may influence diagnostic practices. The temporal aggregation of data may also mask important within-period variations in both aid flows and diagnostic practices that could provide additional insights into underlying causal mechanisms.

7 Conclusion

The relationship between international aid and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) prevalence presents a paradoxical empirical puzzle that challenges conventional assumptions about development assistance effectiveness in health outcomes. This research reveals a substantial positive association between official aid received by countries worldwide and subsequent ASD incidence, yet this relationship embodies complex interpretive challenges that extend beyond simple causality. The study employed rigorous statistical controls, including instrumental variable analysis establishing causal validation, to address potential confounding variables and endogeneity concerns, thereby enhancing result robustness. The segmented analysis examining DSM-5 implementation (2013) reveals particularly pronounced patterns whereby the aid-autism relationship intensified in low-income countries following broader diagnostic criteria introduction, while high-income countries demonstrated no significant associations. This divergence raises critical questions regarding whether observed increases reflect genuine diagnostic improvements in previously under-resourced settings or constitute potential diagnostic inflation facilitated by expanded criteria and aid-related incentives. The examination of potential ASD data misreporting across income groups employed Median Absolute Deviation analysis, revealing significant variability in low-income countries, while Benford’s Law application provided compelling statistical evidence of irregularities that corroborate systematic distortion concerns in autism reporting, particularly within aid-dependent settings.

The study’s nuanced data segregation reveals contextual variations in aid-autism relationships, with countries possessing limited mental health services demonstrating distinct positive correlations with official aid, whereas this association became non-significant in nations with well-established mental health services. The observed patterns suggest that while aid may enhance diagnostic capacity in under-resourced settings, it may simultaneously create conditions conducive to systematic over-reporting, necessitating cautious policy formulation given the interpretive ambiguity between diagnostic improvement and potential overdiagnosis. International health organizations should prioritize both data integrity in ASD reporting and robust oversight mechanism development that distinguishes between genuine case detection and diagnostic inflation. Enhanced data accuracy and verification systems prove crucial for informed decision-making and resource allocation, ensuring interventions address authentic needs rather than artificially inflated prevalence figures. This research emphasizes the critical importance of maintaining vigilance in health data quality monitoring while acknowledging that aid-autism prevalence relationships reflect complex institutional, economic, and diagnostic factors requiring further investigation to definitively resolve underlying causal mechanisms.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the article/Supplementary Material.

Ethics statement

The study did not involve any direct human participation and data was collected from secondary data sources available online. The study was approved by the institutional Ethics committee of Nanchang Institute of Technology. The study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin in accordance with the national legislation and institutional requirements.

Author contributions

JQ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AH: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1575940/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Banks WP. Signal detection theory and human memory. psychol Bull. (1970) 74:81. doi: 10.1037/h0029531

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. American Psychiatric Association, D and Association, A. P. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (Vol. 5). Washington, DC: American psychiatric association (2013).

Google Scholar

3. Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (1994).

Google Scholar

4. Association, A. P, Nomenclature, C. o, and Statistics. DSM-II: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association (1975).

Google Scholar

5. Hansen SN, Schendel DE, and Parner ET. Explaining the increase in the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders: the proportion attributable to changes in reporting practices. JAMA Pediatr. (2015) 169:56–62. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.1893, PMID: 25365033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Russell G, Stapley S, Newlove-Delgado T, Salmon A, White R, Warren F, et al. Time trends in autism diagnosis over 20 years: a UK population-based cohort study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2022) 63:674–82. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13505, PMID: 34414570

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Jensen CM, Steinhausen H-C, and Lauritsen MB. Time trends over 16 years in incidence-rates of autism spectrum disorders across the lifespan based on nationwide Danish register data. J Autism Dev Disord. (2014) 44:1808–18. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2053-6, PMID: 24554161

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Foulkes L and Andrews JL. Are mental health awareness efforts contributing to the rise in reported mental health problems? A call to test the prevalence inflation hypothesis. New Ideas Psychol. (2023) 69:101010. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2023.101010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Waterhouse L. Autism overflows: increasing prevalence and proliferating theories. Neuropsychol Rev. (2008) 18:273–86. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9074-x, PMID: 19015994

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Lugo-Marin J, Magan-Maganto M, Rivero-Santana A, Cuellar-Pompa L, Alviani M, Jenaro-Rio C, et al. Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in adults with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2019) 59:22–33. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2018.12.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Rosen B, Samuel H, Merkur S, and Organization WH. Israel: health system review. Copenhagen: World Health Organization, Regional Office for Europe / European Observatory on Health Systems and Policies (2009).

Google Scholar

12. Ro C. Why autism diagnoses are on the rise. BBC Future (2025). Available online at: https://www.bbc.com/future/article/20250509-why-autism-diagnoses-are-on-the-rise (Accessed May 20, 2025).

Google Scholar

13. Institute for Health Metrics and Evaluation. Global Burden of Disease study 2019 [Data set]. https://www.healthdata.org/research-analysis/gbd (as cited in Our World in Data, https://ourworldindata.org/neurodevelopmental-disorders).

Google Scholar

14. Murray CJ. Findings from the global burden of disease study 2021. Lancet. (2024) 403:2259–62. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(24)00769-4, PMID: 38762327

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Fombonne E. Is autism overdiagnosed Vol. 64. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Online Library (2023) p. 711–4.

Google Scholar

16. Barbui C, Purgato M, Abdulmalik J, Acarturk C, Eaton J, Gastaldon C, et al. Efficacy of psychosocial interventions for mental health outcomes in low-income and middle-income countries: an umbrella review. Lancet Psychiatry. (2020) 7:162–72. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30511-5, PMID: 31948935

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. McMahon DE, Peters GA, Ivers LC, and Freeman EE. Global resource shortages during COVID-19: Bad news for low-income countries. PloS Negl Trop Dis. (2020) 14:e0008412. doi: 10.1371/journal.pntd.0008412, PMID: 32628664

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Sarikhani Y, Bastani P, Rafiee M, Kavosi Z, and Ravangard R. Key barriers to the provision and utilization of mental health services in low-and middle-income countries: a scope study. Community Ment Health J. (2021) 57:836–52. doi: 10.1007/s10597-020-00619-2, PMID: 32285371

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Tekola B, Baheretibeb Y, Roth I, Tilahun D, Fekadu A, Hanlon C, et al. Challenges and opportunities to improve autism services in low-income countries: lessons from a situational analysis in Ethiopia. Global Ment Health. (2016) 3:e21. doi: 10.1017/gmh.2016.17, PMID: 28596889

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Balasubramanian P, Burchi F, and Malerba D. Does economic growth reduce multidimensional poverty? Evidence from low-and middle-income countries. World Dev. (2023) 161:106119. doi: 10.1016/j.worlddev.2022.106119

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Isaksson S, Salomäki S, Tuominen J, Arstila V, Falter-Wagner CM, and Noreika V. Is there a generalized timing impairment in Autism Spectrum Disorders across time scales and paradigms? J Psychiatr Res. (2018) 99:111–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.12.008, PMID: 29438910

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Davidovitch M, Shmueli D, Rotem RS, and Bloch AM. Diagnosis despite clinical ambiguity: physicians’ perspectives on the rise in Autism Spectrum disorder incidence. BMC Psychiatry. (2021) 21:1–6. doi: 10.1186/s12888-021-03151-z, PMID: 33711966

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Kim S-K, McKay D, Cepeda SL, Schneider SC, Wood J, and Storch EA. Assessment of improvement in anxiety severity for children with autism spectrum disorder: The matched correspondence analysis approach. J Psychiatr Res. (2022) 145:175–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.12.004, PMID: 34923358

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. King M and Bearman P. Diagnostic change and the increased prevalence of autism. Int J Epidemiol. (2009) 38:1224–34. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyp261, PMID: 19737791

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Shattuck PT. The contribution of diagnostic substitution to the growing administrative prevalence of autism in US special education. Pediatrics. (2006) 117:1028–37. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-1516, PMID: 16585296

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Navarro-Pardo E, López-Ramón F, Alonso-Esteban Y, and Alcantud-Marín F. Diagnostic tools for autism spectrum disorders by gender: analysis of current status and future lines. Children (Basel Switzerland). (2021) 8:262. doi: 10.3390/children8040262, PMID: 33805261

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Gorenc M. Benford’s law as a useful tool to determine fraud in financial statements. Management. (2019) 14:19–31. doi: 10.26493/1854-4231.14.19-31

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Miller SJ. Benford’s law. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press (2015).

Google Scholar

29. Nigrini MJ. Audit sampling using Benford’s Law: A review of the literature with some new perspectives. J emerging Technol accounting. (2017) 14:29–46. doi: 10.2308/jeta-51783

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Eckhartt GM and Ruxton GD. Investigating and preventing scientific misconduct using Benford’s Law. Res Integrity Peer Rev. (2023) 8:1. doi: 10.1186/s41073-022-00126-w, PMID: 37041616

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hill TP. A statistical derivation of the significant-digit law. Stat Sci. (1995) 10:354–63. doi: 10.1214/ss/1177009869

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Diekmann A. Making use of “Benford’s Law” for the randomized response technique. Sociological Methods Res. (2012) 41:325–34. doi: 10.1177/0049124112452525

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Konno H and Koshizuka T. Mean-absolute deviation model. Iie Trans. (2005) 37:893–900. doi: 10.1080/07408170591007786

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. AMIHAESEI IC and Stefanachi E. Autism, an overwhelming condition: history, etiopathogenesis, types, diagnosis, therapy and prognosis. Medical-Surgical J. (2013) 117:654–61. doi: 10.21103/Article3(3)_OA5, PMID: 24502031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Charman T. The prevalence of autism spectrum disorders: Recent evidence and future challenges. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2002) 11:249–56. doi: 10.1007/s00787-002-0297-8, PMID: 12541002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Cremieux P-Y and Ouellette P. Omitted variable bias and hospital costs. J Health Econ. (2001) 20:271–82. doi: 10.1016/S0167-6296(00)00085-0, PMID: 11252374

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. De Rose A, Arezzo MF, Amore M, Cuomo A, De Filippis S, La Pia S, et al. Vulnerabilities in mental health due to covid-19 pandemic: the response of the italian physicians. Soc Indic Res. (2023) 168:1–18. doi: 10.1007/s11205-023-03111-y, PMID: 37362172

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Rowland D. Differential diagnosis of autism. Recent Developments Med Med Res. (2021) 30:30–5. doi: 10.9734/bpi/rdmmr/v5/4188F

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Rowland D. Epidemic of false diagnoses of autism. Res Highlights Dis Health Res Vol. (2023) 1:41–52. doi: 10.9734/bpi/rdhhr/v1/4188F

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Harris J. Leo Kanner and autism: a 75-year perspective. Int Rev Psychiatry. (2018) 30:3–17. doi: 10.1080/09540261.2018.1455646, PMID: 29667863

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Genovese A and Butler MG. The autism spectrum: behavioral, psychiatric and genetic associations. Genes. (2023) 14:677. doi: 10.3390/genes14030677, PMID: 36980949

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Yoon SH, Choi J, Lee WJ, and Do JT. Genetic and epigenetic etiology underlying autism spectrum disorder. J Clin Med. (2020) 9:966. doi: 10.3390/jcm9040966, PMID: 32244359

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Shaw KA, Bilder DA, McArthur D, Williams AR, Amoakohene E, Bakian AV, et al. Early identification of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 4 years—Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States. MMWR Surveillance Summaries. (2023) 72:1. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss7201a1, PMID: 34855727

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Elsabbagh M, Divan G, Koh YJ, Kim YS, Kauchali S, Marcín C, et al. Global prevalence of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. Autism Res. (2012) 5:160–79. doi: 10.1002/aur.239, PMID: 22495912

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Woodbury-Smith M and Scherer SW. Progress in the genetics of autism spectrum disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2018) 60:445–51. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13717, PMID: 29574884

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Rylaarsdam L and Guemez-Gamboa A. Genetic causes and modifiers of autism spectrum disorder. Front Cell Neurosci. (2019) 13:385. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2019.00385, PMID: 31481879

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Detroja S and Bhatia G. Early screen exposure and developmental abnormalities: understanding the trepidations of “Virtual autism. Indian J psychol Med. (2024) 0:1–3. doi: 10.1177/02537176241263310, PMID: 39564297

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Sarfraz S, Shlaghya G, Narayana SH, Mushtaq U, Shaman Ameen B, Nie C, et al. Early screen-time exposure and its association with risk of developing autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Cureus. (2023) 15:e42292. doi: 10.7759/cureus.42292, PMID: 37614255

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Chien Y-L, Chou M-C, Chou W-J, Wu Y-Y, Tsai W-C, Chiu Y-N, et al. Prenatal and perinatal risk factors and the clinical implications on autism spectrum disorder. Autism. (2019) 23:783–91. doi: 10.1177/1362361318772813, PMID: 29950101

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Conde-Agudelo A, Rosas-Bermudez A, and Norton MH. Birth spacing and risk of autism and other neurodevelopmental disabilities: a systematic review. Pediatrics. (2016) 137(5):e20153482. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-3482, PMID: 27244802

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Modabbernia A, Velthorst E, and Reichenberg A. Environmental risk factors for autism: an evidence-based review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Mol Autism. (2017) 8:1–16. doi: 10.1186/s13229-017-0121-4, PMID: 28331572

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Bolton S, McDonald D, Curtis E, Kelly S, and Gallagher L. Autism in a recently arrived immigrant population. Eur J Pediatr. (2014) 173:337–43. doi: 10.1007/s00431-013-2149-6, PMID: 24085582

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Graf WD, Miller G, Epstein LG, and Rapin I. The autism “epidemic”: ethical, legal, and social issues in a developmental spectrum disorder. Neurology. (2017) 88:1371–80. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003791, PMID: 28275086

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Idring S, Lundberg M, Sturm H, Dalman C, Gumpert C, Rai D, et al. Changes in prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in 2001–2011: findings from the Stockholm youth cohort. J Autism Dev Disord. (2015) 45:1766–73. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2336-y, PMID: 25475364

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. May J and Roberts B. Poverty diagnostics using poor data: Strengthening the evidence base for pro-poor policy making in Lesotho. Soc Indic Res. (2005) 74:477–510. doi: 10.1007/s11205-004-8208-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Thombs B, Turner KA, and Shrier I. Defining and evaluating overdiagnosis in mental health: a meta-research review. Psychother psychosomatics. (2019) 88:193–202. doi: 10.1159/000501647, PMID: 31340212

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Balashov VS, Yan Y, and Zhu X. Using the Newcomb–Benford law to study the association between a country’s COVID-19 reporting accuracy and its development. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:22914. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-02367-z, PMID: 34824323

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Nigrini MJ. Benford’s Law: Applications for forensic accounting, auditing, and fraud detection. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons (2012).

Google Scholar

59. Diekmann A. Not the first digit! Using benford’s law to detect fraudulent scientif ic data. J Appl Stat. (2007) 34:321–9. doi: 10.1080/02664760601004940

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Furzer J, Dhuey E, and Laporte A. ADHD misdiagnosis: Causes and mitigators. Health Econ. (2022) 31:1926–53. doi: 10.1002/hec.4555, PMID: 35763436

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Hamner T, Hepburn S, Zhang F, Fidler D, Rosenberg CR, Robins DL, et al. Cognitive profiles and autism symptoms in comorbid down syndrome and autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2020) 41:172–9. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000745, PMID: 31599791

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Evans WN, Morrill MS, and Parente ST. Measuring inappropriate medical diagnosis and treatment in survey data: The case of ADHD among school-age children. J Health Econ. (2010) 29:657–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2010.07.005, PMID: 20739076

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL, Mayes RD, and Molitoris S. Autism and ADHD: Overlapping and discriminating symptoms. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2012) 6:277–85. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.05.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Brodersen J, Schwartz LM, Heneghan C, O’Sullivan JW, Aronson JK, and Woloshin S. Overdiagnosis: what it is and what it isn’t. BMJ Evidence-Based Med. (2018) 23:1. doi: 10.1136/ebmed-2017-110886, PMID: 29367314

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Carter SM, Degeling C, Doust J, and Barratt A. A definition and ethical evaluation of overdiagnosis. J Med ethics. (2016) 42:705–14. doi: 10.1136/medethics-2015-102928, PMID: 27402883

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Pifarré i Arolas H and Dudel C. An ordinal measure of population health. Soc Indic Res. (2019) 143:1219–43. doi: 10.1007/s11205-018-2010-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Mandell DS and Novak M. The role of culture in families’ treatment decisions for children with autism spectrum disorders. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. (2005) 11:110–5. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20061, PMID: 15977313

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Pruckner GJ, Schneeweis N, Schober T, and Zweimüller M. Birth order, parental health investment, and health in childhood. J Health Econ. (2021) 76:102426. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2021.102426, PMID: 33529856

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Doehring P, Reichow B, Palka T, Phillips C, and Hagopian L. Behavioral approaches to managing severe problem behaviors in children with autism spectrum and related developmental disorders: a descriptive analysis. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clinics. (2014) 23:25–40. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2013.08.001, PMID: 24231165

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Breuer E, De Silva MJ, Shidaye R, Petersen I, Nakku J, Jordans MJ, et al. Planning and evaluating mental health services in low-and middle-income countries using theory of change. Br J Psychiatry. (2016) 208:s55–62. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.153841, PMID: 26447178

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Durkin MS, Elsabbagh M, Barbaro J, Gladstone M, Happe F, Hoekstra RA, et al. Autism screening and diagnosis in low resource settings: Challenges and opportunities to enhance research and services worldwide. Autism Res. (2015) 8:473–6. doi: 10.1002/aur.1575, PMID: 26437907

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Jones AM and Wildman J. Health, income and relative deprivation: Evidence from the BHPS. J Health Econ. (2008) 27:308–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2007.05.007, PMID: 18207266

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Lenart A and Pasternak J. Resources, problems and challenges of autism spectrum disorder diagnosis and support system in Poland. J Autism Dev Disord. (2023) 53:1629–41. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05142-1, PMID: 34345979

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. McConkey R. Responding to autism in low and middle income countries (Lmic): what to do and what not to do. Brain Sci. (2022) 12:1475. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12111475, PMID: 36358400

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Bottini S, Wiseman K, and Gillis J. Burnout in providers serving individuals with ASD: The impact of the workplace. Res Dev Disabil. (2020) 100:103616. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103616, PMID: 32120047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. O’Toole B. Community-based rehabilitation (CBR): problems and possibilities. Eur J special needs Educ. (1987) 2:177–90. doi: 10.1080/0885625870020303

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Croen LA, Zerbo O, Qian Y, Massolo ML, Rich S, Sidney S, et al. The health status of adults on the autism spectrum. Autism. (2015) 19:814–23. doi: 10.1177/1362361315577517, PMID: 25911091

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Howlin P and Magiati I. Autism spectrum disorder: Outcomes in adulthood. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2017) 30:69–76. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000308, PMID: 28067726

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Seyedin H, Afshari M, Isfahani P, Hasanzadeh E, Radinmanesh M, and Bahador RC. The main factors of supplier-induced demand in health care: a qualitative study. J Educ Health promotion. (2021) 10. doi: 10.4103/jehp.jehp_68_20, PMID: 34084796

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Balsa AI and McGuire TG. Statistical discrimination in health care. J Health Econ. (2001) 20:881–907. doi: 10.1016/S0167-6296(01)00101-1, PMID: 11758051

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Richardson JR and Peacock SJ. Supplier-induced demand: reconsidering the theories and new Australian evidence. Appl Health Econ Health Policy. (2006) 5:87–98. doi: 10.2165/00148365-200605020-00003, PMID: 16872250

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Cook J, Hull L, and Mandy W. Improving diagnostic procedures in autism for girls and women: A narrative review. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2024) 20:505–14. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S372723, PMID: 38469208

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Cumin J, Pelaez S, and Mottron L. Positive and differential diagnosis of autism in verbal women of typical intelligence: A Delphi study. Autism. (2022) 26:1153–64. doi: 10.1177/13623613211042719, PMID: 34514874

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Sarr R, Spain D, Quinton AM, Happé F, Brewin CR, Radcliffe J, et al. Differential diagnosis of autism, attachment disorders, complex post-traumatic stress disorder and emotionally unstable personality disorder: A Delphi study. Br J Psychol. (2025) 116:1–33. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12731, PMID: 39300915

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Ellis RP and McGuire TG. Cost sharing and patterns of mental health care utilization. J Hum Resour. (1986) 21:359–79. doi: 10.2307/145968

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Murray AL, Allison C, Smith PL, Baron-Cohen S, Booth T, and Auyeung B. Investigating diagnostic bias in autism spectrum conditions: An item response theory analysis of sex bias in the AQ-10. Autism Res. (2017) 10:790–800. doi: 10.1002/aur.1724, PMID: 27891820

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Bishop-Fitzpatrick L and Kind AJ. A scoping review of health disparities in autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2017) 47:3380–91. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3251-9, PMID: 28756549

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Bishop-Fitzpatrick L and Rubenstein E. The physical and mental health of middle aged and older adults on the autism spectrum and the impact of intellectual disability. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2019) 63:34–41. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2019.01.001, PMID: 31768189

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Cashin A, Buckley T, Trollor JN, and Lennox N. A scoping review of what is known of the physical health of adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Intellectual Disabil. (2018) 22:96–108. doi: 10.1177/1744629516665242, PMID: 27623754

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Qian M, Chou S-Y, and Lai EK. Confirmatory bias in health decisions: Evidence from the MMR-autism controversy. J Health Econ. (2020) 70:102284. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2019.102284, PMID: 32057491

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Hirvikoski T, Mittendorfer-Rutz E, Boman M, Larsson H, Lichtenstein P, and Bölte S. Premature mortality in autism spectrum disorder. Br J Psychiatry. (2016) 208:232–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.160192, PMID: 26541693

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Liang D, Mays VM, and Hwang W-C. Integrated mental health services in China: challenges and planning for the future. Health Policy Plann. (2018) 33:107–22. doi: 10.1093/heapol/czx137, PMID: 29040516

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Wang K, Link BG, Corrigan PW, Davidson L, and Flanagan E. Perceived provider stigma as a predictor of mental health service users’ internalized stigma and disempowerment. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 259:526–31. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.11.036, PMID: 29156425

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Kawachi I, Adler NE, and Dow WH. Money, schooling, and health: Mechanisms and causal evidence. Ann New York Acad Sci. (2010) 1186:56–68. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05340.x, PMID: 20201868

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Rosenstock IM. The health belief model and preventive health behavior. Health Educ Monogr. (1974) 2:354–86. doi: 10.1177/109019817400200405

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Scott WR. Introduction: institutional theory and organizations. institutional construction organizations. (1995), 11–23.

Google Scholar

97. Walker ME. Democracy and access to health services. Saskatoon, SK: University of Saskatchewan (2013).

Google Scholar

98. Boyce MR, Meyer MJ, Kraemer JD, and Katz R. Financial assistance for health security: effects of international financial assistance on capacities for preventing, detecting, and responding to public health emergencies. Int J Health Policy Manage. (2022) 11:2054. doi: 10.34172/ijhpm.2021.120, PMID: 34634886

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Dieleman JL, Schneider MT, Haakenstad A, Singh L, Sadat N, Birger M, et al. Development assistance for health: past trends, associations, and the future of international financial flows for health. Lancet. (2016) 387:2536–44. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)30168-4, PMID: 27086170

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Li J, Hania A, Yahya F, Hussain M, and Waqas M. Fostering sustainable futures through global peace and eco-innovation: A cross-country evidence. Technol Soc. (2025) 103008. doi: 10.1016/j.techsoc.2025.103008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Umar M, Yahya F, and Rashid A. Climate risk and loan pricing: the moderating role of trilemma policy choices. Economic Change Restructuring. (2025) 58:63. doi: 10.1007/s10644-025-09904-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Yahya F, Ali SA, and Rashid A. Unpacking mundell-fleming trilemma tradeoffs: asymmetric pathways to energy diversification. Rev Dev Econ. (2025) 29:1–20. doi: 10.1111/rode.70002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Dastamooz S, Tham CC, Yam JC, Li M, Wong SH, and Sit CH. A systematic review and meta-analysis on the ocular characteristics in children and adolescents with neurodevelopmental disorders. Sci Rep. (2023) 13:19397. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-46206-9, PMID: 37938638

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Gardener H, Spiegelman D, and Buka SL. Perinatal and neonatal risk factors for autism: a comprehensive meta-analysis. Pediatrics. (2011) 128:344–55. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1036, PMID: 21746727

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Lyall K, Croen L, Daniels J, Fallin MD, Ladd-Acosta C, Lee BK, et al. The changing epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. Annu Rev Public Health. (2017) 38:81–102. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031816-044318, PMID: 28068486

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Durkin MS, Maenner MJ, Baio J, Christensen D, Daniels J, Fitzgerald R, et al. Autism spectrum disorder among US children, (2002–2010): Socioeconomic, racial, and ethnic disparities. Am J Public Health. (2017) 107:1818–26. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2017.304032, PMID: 28933930

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Liptak GS, Benzoni LB, Mruzek DW, Nolan KW, Thingvoll MA, Wade CM, et al. Disparities in diagnosis and access to health services for children with autism: data from the National Survey of Children’s Health. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2008) 29:152–60. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e318165c7a0, PMID: 18349708

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Baron-Cohen S, Lombardo MV, Auyeung B, Ashwin E, Chakrabarti B, and Knickmeyer R. Why are autism spectrum conditions more prevalent in males? PloS Biol. (2011) 9:e1001081. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001081, PMID: 21695109

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Chiarotti F and Venerosi A. Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders: a review of worldwide prevalence estimates since 2014. Brain Sci. (2020) 10:274. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10050274, PMID: 32370097

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Zaroff CM and Uhm SY. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders and influence of country of measurement and ethnicity. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2012) 47:395–8. doi: 10.1007/s00127-011-0350-3, PMID: 21293843

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Baxter AJ, Brugha T, Erskine HE, Scheurer RW, Vos T, and Scott JG. The epidemiology and global burden of autism spectrum disorders. psychol Med. (2015) 45:601–13. doi: 10.1017/S003329171400172X, PMID: 25108395

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Bolton D and Gillett G. The biopsychosocial model of health and disease: New philosophical and scientific developments. New York, NY: Springer Nature (2019).

Google Scholar

113. Lehman BJ, David DM, and Gruber JA. Rethinking the biopsychosocial model of health: Understanding health as a dynamic system. Soc Pers Psychol compass. (2017) 11:e12328. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12328

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Smith RD, Keogh-Brown MR, and Barnett T. Estimating the economic impact of pandemic influenza: an application of the computable general equilibrium model to the UK. Soc Sci Med. (2011) 73:235–44. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.05.025, PMID: 21708419

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Marty R, Dolan CB, Leu M, and Runfola D. Taking the health aid debate to the subnational level: the impact and allocation of foreign health aid in Malawi. BMJ Global Health. (2017) 2:e000129. doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2016-000129, PMID: 28588997

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Greenspoon PJ and Saklofske DH. Toward an integration of subjective well-being and psychopathology. Soc Indic Res. (2001) 54:81–108. doi: 10.1023/A:1007219227883

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Matson JL and Kozlowski AM. The increasing prevalence of autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2011) 5:418–25. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.06.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Zablotsky B, Pringle BA, Colpe LJ, Kogan MD, Rice C, and Blumberg SJ. Service and treatment use among children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. J Dev Behav pediatrics: JDBP. (2015) 36:98. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000127, PMID: 25650952

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Albarqouni L, Palagama S, Chai J, Sivananthajothy P, Pathirana T, Bakhit M, et al. Overuse of medications in low-and middle-income countries: a scoping review. Bull World Health Organ. (2023) 101:36. doi: 10.2471/BLT.22.288293, PMID: 36593777

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Glasziou PP, Jones MA, Pathirana T, Barratt AL, and Bell KJ. Estimating the magnitude of cancer overdiagnosis in Australia. Med J Aust. (2020) 212:163–8. doi: 10.5694/mja2.50455, PMID: 31858624

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Kirschbaum C, Pirke K-M, and Hellhammer DH. The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’–a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. (1993) 28:76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004, PMID: 8255414

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Born K, Levinson W, and de Freitas L. Reducing harm from overuse of healthcare Vol. 379. London: British Medical Journal Publishing Group (2022).

Google Scholar

123. Albarqouni L, Pathirana T, and Moynihan R. 89 Overdiagnosis and overuse of healthcare in limited-resource settings: a workshop. BMJ Evidence-Based Med. (2022) 27:A16–7. doi: 10.1136/bmjebm-2022-PODabstracts.34

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Pathirana T, Yu MW, Martiny F, Rupasinghe M, Kottahachchi D, Wijewickrama A, et al. 8 drivers and potential solutions for overdiagnosis: perspectives from the low and middle income countries. BMJ Evidence-Based Med. (2019) 24:A6. doi: 10.1136/bmjebm-2019-POD.13

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Zablotsky B, Rast J, Bramlett MD, and Shattuck PT. Health care transition planning among youth with ASD and other mental, behavioral, and developmental disorders. Maternal Child Health J. (2020) 24:796–804. doi: 10.1007/s10995-019-02858-6, PMID: 31897930

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Ehsan U, Sakib N, Haque MM, Soron T, Saxena D, Ahamed SI, et al. Confronting autism in urban Bangladesh: unpacking infrastructural and cultural challenges. EAI Endorsed Trans Pervasive Health Technol. (2018) 4:e5–5. doi: 10.4108/eai.13-7-2018.155082

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Zeidan J, Fombonne E, Scorah J, Ibrahim A, Durkin MS, Saxena S, et al. Global prevalence of autism: A systematic review update. Autism Res. (2022) 15:778–90. doi: 10.1002/aur.2696, PMID: 35238171

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Xu G, Strathearn L, Liu B, Yang B, and Bao W. Twenty-year trends in diagnosed attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among US children and adolescents 1997-2016. JAMA network Open. (2018) 1:e181471–e181471., PMID: 30646132

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

129. Marmot M. Social determinants of health inequalities. Lancet. (2005) 365:1099–104. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)71146-6, PMID: 15781105

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Unit EI. Democracy index 2022 (2023). Available online at: https://www.eiu.com/n/campaigns/democracy-index-2022 (Accessed February 10, 2025).

Google Scholar

131. Burnside C and Dollar D. Aid, policies, and growth. Am economic Rev. (2000) 90:847–68. doi: 10.1257/aer.90.4.847

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Gisselquist RM, Niño-Zarazúa M, and Samarin M. Does aid support democracy? A systematic review of the literature. (2021). doi: 10.35188/UNU-WIDER/2021/948-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Svensson J. When is foreign aid policy credible? Aid dependence and conditionality. J Dev Econ. (2000) 61:61–84. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3878(99)00061-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Asatullaeva Z, Aghdam RFZ, Ahmad N, and Tashpulatova L. The impact of foreign aid on economic development: A systematic literature review and content analysis of the top 50 most influential papers. J Int Dev. (2021) 33:717–51. doi: 10.1002/jid.3543

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Dietrich S and Wright J. Foreign aid allocation tactics and democratic change in Africa. J Politics. (2015) 77:216–34. doi: 10.1086/678976

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Maruta AA, Banerjee R, and Cavoli T. Foreign aid, institutional quality and economic growth: Evidence from the developing world. Economic Model. (2020) 89:444–63. doi: 10.1016/j.econmod.2019.11.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

137. North DC. Institutions, institutional change and economic performance. Cambridge: Cambridge university press (1990).

Google Scholar

138. Rahman MM, Dyuti TI, Tareque M, and Alnour M. Health expenditure, governance and SDG3 nexus: a longitudinal analysis in BRICS economies. Globalization Health. (2025) 21:18. doi: 10.1186/s12992-025-01113-8, PMID: 40205434

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Baird G, Simonoff E, Pickles A, Chandler S, Loucas T, Meldrum D, et al. Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Lancet. (2006) 368:210–5. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69041-7, PMID: 16844490

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Micali N, Chakrabarti S, and Fombonne E. The broad autism phenotype: findings from an epidemiological survey. Autism. (2004) 8:21–37. doi: 10.1177/1362361304040636, PMID: 15070545

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Robinson JA and Acemoglu D. Why nations fail: The origins of power, prosperity and poverty. London: Profile Books (2012).

Google Scholar

142. Moynihan R, Doust J, and Henry D. Preventing overdiagnosis: how to stop harming the healthy. Bmj. (2012) 344:e3502. doi: 10.1136/bmj.e3502, PMID: 22645185

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Ryan MP. Collaborative governance in a developing non-democracy: Uganda’s organizational success fighting HIV/AIDS. Am Rev Public Administration. (2022) 52:175–90. doi: 10.1177/02750740211059113

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Carnegie A and Marinov N. Foreign aid, human rights, and democracy promotion: Evidence from a natural experiment. Am J Political Sci. (2017) 61:671–83. doi: 10.1111/ajps.12289

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Wilms R, Mäthner E, Winnen L, and Lanwehr R. Omitted variable bias: A threat to estimating causal relationships. Methods Psychol. (2021) 5:100075. doi: 10.1016/j.metip.2021.100075

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Rajan RG and Subramanian A. Aid and growth: What does the cross-country evidence really show? Rev Econ Stat. (2008) 90:643–65. doi: 10.1162/rest.90.4.643

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Clemens MA, Radelet S, Bhavnani RR, and Bazzi S. Counting chickens when they hatch: Timing and the effects of aid on growth. Economic J. (2012) 122:590–617. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2011.02482.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorder, incidence, panel data, financial aid, Benford law

Citation: Qiu J and Hania A (2025) Red flags in global autism data: a forensic analysis of prevalence patterns and official aid dependencies. Front. Psychiatry 16:1575940. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1575940

Received: 06 March 2025; Accepted: 30 September 2025;
Published: 15 October 2025.

Edited by:

Morufu Olalekan Raimi, Federal University, Nigeria

Reviewed by:

Francisco Alcantud-Marín, University of Valencia, Spain
Noor Kamal Al-Qazzaz, University of Baghdad, Iraq

Copyright © 2025 Qiu and Hania. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alishba Hania, YWxpc2hiYV9oYW5pYTkyQGhvdG1haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.