Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 29 August 2025

Sec. Adolescent and Young Adult Psychiatry

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1590441

This article is part of the Research TopicAdolescent Emotional Disorders and Suicide Self-Harm Crisis InterventionView all 27 articles

The first study of a mentalizing mediator model in a community sample of nonsuicidal self-injured adolescents and its association with salivary oxytocin

  • 1Department of Psychology, School of Humanities and Management, Guangdong Medical University, Dongguan, China
  • 2Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University, Guangzhou, China
  • 3The Affiliated Hospital of Guangdong Medical University, Zhanjiang, China
  • 4Department of Child & Adolescence Psychology, Zhongshan Third People’s Hospital, Zhongzhan, China
  • 5Department of Child & Adolescence Psychology, Dongguan Seventh People’s Hospital, Dongguan, China

Background: Nonsuicidal self-injurious behavior (NSSI) is highly prevalent in adolescents and strongly associated with early trauma. Emerging theories have indicated that the occurrence of NSSI results from the interaction of individual biological vulnerability and environmental risk; however, the underlying mechanisms are not yet clear. This study sought to investigate the psychological and pathophysiological mechanisms underlying the development of self-injury behavior through environmental, psychological, and physiological factors. We hypothesized that mentalization mediates the relationship between childhood trauma and NSSI, and further explored whether oxytocin (OT) has the potential to serve as an informative biomarker of social functioning for people with NSSI.

Methods: This study investigated NSSI, childhood trauma, and mentalizing in 1313 junior high school students to develop and test a mediated model of mentalizing in which childhood trauma affects NSSI. Subsequently, the relationship between peripheral salivary OT levels and NSSI in a cohort of 109 individuals with suicidal self-injurious behavior and 113 healthy controls.

Results: The NSSI detection rate was 28.2% in this study. Females had a greater frequency of NSSI, hypomentalization and lower hypermentalization.The structural equation modeling (SEM) results revealed that the indirect effect of childhood trauma on NSSI through hypomentalization was 0.091 (95% CI [0.066, 0.120], P< 0.001). The indirect effect of childhood trauma on NSSI through hypermentalization was 0.037 (95% CI [0.025, 0.049], P < 0.001). OT levels were not significantly correlated with hypermentalization, hypomentalization, or childhood trauma (P > 0.05).

Conclusions: The present study revealed that mentalizing partially mediated the associations between childhood trauma and NSSI, suggesting that the mentalizing trauma model is equally applicable to the community NSSI population. This is the first study to explore the relationship between peripheral OT levels and NSSI behavior, with results suggesting that baseline salivary OT concentrations are not reliable biomarkers for NSSI in community samples.

Introduction

Nonsuicidal self-injurious (NSSI) behavior is closely associated with psychiatric disorders, such as personality disorders, particularly borderline personality disorder (1), and self-harm is a major symptom of Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) (2). NSSI is also an important predictor of suicide, and previous studies have often examined the mechanisms of suicide and NSSI together (3). However, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-5), independently identifies NSSI as a diagnostic category for psychiatric disorders, and although the two are closely related (4), some studies have confirmed differences in their pathogenesis (5, 6). The prevalence of NSSI among adolescents has been increasing rapidly in recent years, and research on its mechanisms has received increasing amounts of attention.

The concept of mentalizing was first introduced in people with BPD. Mentalizing reflects an individual’s ability to visualize abstract cues to perceive abstract cues, such as tone of voice and facial expressions, in oneself and others, and to process one’s own cognitive ability to learn about both one’s own and others’ mental states (including intentions, desires, emotions, and beliefs) and infer the behavior of others (7). Mentalizing is considered to be a more complex and advanced cognitive function (8). Fonagy proposed that reflective functioning (RF) could be used to measure mentalizing ability and reported that impaired RF was associated with borderline symptoms in adolescents. He also proposed a model of mentalizing development (9), suggesting that BPD increases the risk of impaired mentalizing when experiencing early trauma (10). Patients with BPD frequently confuse reality with subjective imagination when perceiving the inner and outer worlds, struggle to set boundaries between external reality and the inner world in understanding the social world, manifest as self-centered, experience fragmentation in their interactions with others, and undergo identity-self dissociation. These challenges increase the risk of undesirable emotions and behaviors, such as self-harm, in an attempt to change their current social relationships.

Childhood is an essential period for the development of mentalizing skills. Trauma during childhood, which exceeds the capacity for psychological regulation, can severely affect the development of mentalizing abilities during adolescence, leading to disruptions in mentalizing abilities and distortion of one’s cognitive state and the understanding of others in social interactions, causing interpersonal distress and increasing the risk of self-injury (11, 12). Neurodevelopmentally, childhood trauma acts a negative stressor that increases susceptibility to NSSI, as it disrupts the structure and function of the corresponding brain regions. The use of maladaptive behaviors (e.g., NSSI) is a maladaptive strategy used by adolescents who have experienced childhood trauma to cope with environmental interpersonal stress. Individuals with early adverse experiences are inclined to overanalyze the mental states of others, and such negative interpersonal interactions can activate the individual’s attachment system increasing emotional arousal to a level that temporarily inhibits mentalizing and may lead to acting out through self-harm (13). In a nonclinical sample, individuals with high externalizing symptoms (e.g., violence, impulsivity) were found to have lower RF scores (14), implying that mentalizing deficits were significantly associated with psychopathic traits as well as active aggressive behavior (15). The present study hypothesized that impaired mentalizing may be a potential risk for the occurrence of ambulatory NSSI in individuals exposed to childhood trauma, providing new insights into the mechanisms of NSSI.

Previous pro-social effects of oxytocin (OT) and its ability to promote empathy have been extensively reported in animal and human behavior studies. Studies have shown that OT deficiency, abnormal metabolism, and dysfunctional OT receptors are significantly associated with self-injurious behavior, social interpersonal problems, emotional dysregulation, and depression (16, 17). Ditzen et al. (18) reported that OT was related to pro-social behavior in adolescents. Pro-social behavior increases adolescents’ willingness to cooperate with and trust others, enhances their willingness to communicate, and promotes interpersonal skills, thus helping to reduce the occurrence of NSSI behaviors (18). OT is a social cognitive physiological hormone that improves social cognition and fosters positive social relationships (19). Recent studies have revealed reduced OT in BPD patients, who are at greater risk for self-harm and mentalizing deficits (2022).

This study hypothesized that mentalization and OT influenced the risk of NSSI at the psychosocial and physiological levels, respectively. Therefore, this study investigated the pathogenesis of NSSI in adolescents through a combination of sociophysiological, psychological, and environmental factors in an ambulatory population, providing a scientific basis and theoretical foundation for the detection and prevention of NSSI.

The first part of this two-part study examines whether mentalizing deficits are involved in the pathogenesis of NSSI from childhood trauma through a mediation model. The second part explores whether the biological factor OT is involved in the pathological process of NSSI through a case-control study.

Materials and methods

Participants

The participants were recruited from three middle schools in Dongguan, Guangdong Province, China, with the participating students aged 11–15 years. The participants were guided by pretrained data collectors to complete paper questionnaires in the classroom. The Adolescent Nonsuicidal Self-injury Assessment Questionnaire (ANSAQ) was used to complete the basic screening for NSSI situations. A total of 1400 questionnaires were distributed, and 1313 valid questionnaires were returned, yielding an effective recovery rate of 93.93%. Among the participants, 668 were male students, 645 were female students, 938 were in the first year of junior high school, and 375 were in the second year of junior high school. A total of 109 NSSI individuals and 113 healthy controls were recruited from these screening samples, and saliva was collected at a uniform date and time point to complete the case-control study. In the NSSI group (109 participants), 53 participants were male and 56 were female. In the non-NSSI group (113 participants), 55 participants were male and 58 were female. The study was authorized by the Research Ethics Committee of Guangdong Medical University. All participants signed an informed consent form, and consent was obtained from guardians and class teachers. The ethical approval number is PJKT2022-051.

Scale assessment tools

Adolescent nonsuicidal self-injury assessment questionnaire

The Chinese version of the NSSI behavior questionnaire for adolescents, developed by Liu et al. (23), was selected to obtain the occurrence of NSSI among the participants in the past year. This scale consists of 2 subquestionnaires: the behavioral questionnaire and the functional questionnaire. The behavioral questionnaire included 12 forms of injury, including intentionally yanking one’s own hair, intentionally biting, and cutting oneself, among others. The number of occurrences of self-injurious behaviors was used as the overall self-injury frequency. The higher the total self-injury score, the more severe the self-injury. In accordance with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-5) criteria, subjects with a frequency > 5 times were judged as the “self-injury group.” The functional questionnaire contains 19 items, each with a five-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (fully). In this study, positive entries were defined as those resulting from 3 (consistent) and 4 (fully consistent). One or several positive entries in each dimension were defined as having an NSSI function. In this study, simultaneous self-injury frequency >5 times and at least one positive NSSI function were classified as NSSI enrollment criteria. The Cronbach’s α values of the behavioral and functional questionnaires in this study were 0.862 and 0.885, indicating good internal consistency, respectively.

Assessment of mentalization level

The simplified Chinese version of the Reflective Functioning Questionnaire-8 (RFQ-8) by Xu (24) was selected to assess the level of mentalization of secondary school students.The same four items were used to calculate the scores for both subscales, with these items being reverse-scored between RFQ_C and RFQ_U (e.g., “I don’t always know why I do what I do”; “When I’m angry, I say things I later regret”; “If I’m unsure of myself, I may act in ways that offend others”; “Sometimes I do things without exactly knowing why”). The remaining four items (two for each subscale) were unique to their respective subscales (e.g., for RFQ_C: “When I’m angry, I say harsh words without knowing why I said them”; for RFQ_U: “I always know what I’m feeling”). The scale consists of eight items, with responses ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree,” scored as “0,0,0,0,0,1,2,3” and “3,2,1,0,0,0,0.”. Hypermentalization reflects high levels of imagination and reasoning, while hypomentalization is the opposite, lacking objective content and an inability to understand one’s own and others’ mental states. Higher scores on the certainty subsection scale and lower scores on the uncertainty subsection scale indicate greater mentalizing ability. Conversely, lower scores on the certainty subsection scale and higher scores on the uncertainty subsection scale indicate greater deficits in mentalizing ability. The Cronbach’s α for RFQU was 0.691, and for RFQC, it was 0.793, indicating adequate reliability.

Childhood trauma questionnaire

The CTQ was constructed by Bernstein et al. (25), a clinical psychologist in New York, USA, as an instrument to assess traumatic experiences in childhood. It contains 28 items, with each subscale containing five items, and each item is rated on a 5-point scale (1, 2, 3, 4, 5 for never, occasionally, sometimes, often, always, respectively), with each abuse subscale scoring 5–25 and a total score of 25–125, with higher scores indicating higher levels of trauma. The subscales were composed of emotional abuse (EA), physical abuse (PA), sexual abuse (SA), emotional neglect (EN) and physical neglect (PN). In this study, Cronbach’s α was 0.605.

OT collection and detection

Saliva collection

Each participant was instructed not to eat or drink anything for 2 hours prior to saliva collection. Prior to the measurement, each participant was permitted to rest quietly in a room (outside the test room) for 30 min. They were then taken to the nearby room, where a baseline saliva sample was collected with a cotton roll (Neutral Salivettes®, SARSTEDT, Numbrecht, Germany), while being monitored by a laboratory technician. The participants held the cotton roll in their mouths, performing circular movements to avoid soaking the cotton, prior to replacing it with the stopper part of the Salivette® tube. Saliva samples were stored at −20°C until they were centrifuged twice, 2 days apart, at 40°C at 1,500 × g for 20 min. Liquid samples were stored at −80°C.

OT detection

On the day of the assay, the samples were redissolved in liquid and concentrated four times in buffer before immunoassay with a Human Oxytocin ELISA Kit (catalog #MK0807A; Sumeike Biological Technology Co., Jiangsu, China). Measurements were performed in duplicate, and the concentration was calculated via a microplate reader (RT-6100, China) according to relevant standard curves.

Quality control

The collectors were trained uniformly via uniform guidelines and were informed of the principle of confidentiality. The questionnaires and saliva samples were collected in a standardized manner via collective administration. Mediated effects tests were performed with the bootstrap method, with random sampling of the original sample 5000 times to redistribute the samples and obtain robust standard errors and confidence intervals for the parameter estimates. A 95% CI not containing 0 was considered a statistically significant difference. The test level was set at a=0.05.

Data analysis

Preliminary analysis revealed that the OT and questionnaire data were not normally distributed. The nonnormally distributed measures were expressed as M (P25, P75), and the two groups were compared via the Mann–Whitney U test, depending on the type of variable. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient was used to assess associations between OT levels, sociodemographic variables, and questionnaire data. Mediated effects analysis and model fitting were performed via AMOS 24.0. Model fit was assessed based on the following criteria: the degrees of freedom (χ2/DF) < 3, the goodness-of-fit index (GFI) > 0.90, the adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI) > 0.90, the comparative fit index (CFI) > 0.90, the incremental fit index (IFI) > 0.90, the Tucker–Lewis coefficient (TFI) > 0.90, the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) <0.08, and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) < 0.08. If the model met these criteria, it was considered to fit well.

Results

Common method deviation test

The Harman one-factor test was used to control the procedure (e.g., reverse scoring of some items). All the collected question items were tested for common method bias via Harman’s one-way test, and the results of the unrotated exploratory factor analysis extracted a total of 14 factors with characteristic roots greater than 1. The maximum factor variance explained was 19.76% (< 40%) for this study, so there was no serious common method bias in this study.

Detected situation and frequency of nonsuicidal self-injury

Among the adolescents surveyed, the self-injurious behavior detection rate was 47.6% (625/1313), with 370 cases of self-injurious behavior occurring five times or more, and the NSSI detection rate was 28.2% (Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Distribution of the frequency of NSSI behaviors.

Mann–Whitney U tests for sex and grade level in this study

There were significant sex differences (P < 0.01, Table 2) in hypermentalization, hypomentalization, and NSSI behavior. Females had a greater frequency of NSSI, hypomentalization and lower hypermentalization; the grade level differences were not significant (P > 0.05).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Tests for differences in grade and gender.

Correlation analysis of childhood trauma, mentalizing, and frequency of nonsuicidal self-injurious behavior

The Spearman rank correlation test was used due to the non-normal distribution of the measurements. The results (Table 3) revealed that hypomentalization was positively correlated with total childhood trauma scores, scores for various trauma type, and NSSI behaviors at significant levels (P < 0.001). Hypermentalization was negatively associated with childhood trauma, scores for various trauma types (except SA), and NSSI behavior at significant levels (P < 0.001). Childhood trauma and various trauma types were positively associated with NSSI (P < 0.001).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlation analysis of childhood trauma, mentalizing, and NSSI frequency.

Regression analysis of mentalizing and NSSI frequency in adolescent childhood trauma

In this study, multiple linear regression was performed to further explore the relationships among childhood trauma, mentalizing, and NSSI among junior high school students. The total childhood trauma score, each trauma dimension, and each dimension of mentalizing were used as independent variables, with NSSI behaviors as the dependent variables.

Regression analysis revealed that hypermentalization (β = −0.062, t = −2.142, P < 0.05) significantly and negatively predicted NSSI behavior. PA (β = 0.061, t = 2.055, P < 0.05), EA (β = 0.446, t = 14.465, P < 0.01), childhood trauma (β = 0.440, t = 17.742, P < 0.01) and hypomentalization (β = 0.382, t = 12.651, P < 0.01) were significant positive predictors of NSSI behavior. The EN, SA, and PN dimensions of childhood trauma did not significantly affect NSSI behavior (P > 0.05) (Table 4). Among them, the SA dimension of childhood trauma did not significantly impact NSSI behavior, which may be related to the low incidence of sexual abuse, resulting in insufficient statistical power.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Regression analysis of early trauma and mentalizing on the frequency of nonsuicidal self-injury.

Analysis of the intermediary model

SPSS process macro procedure bootstrap method test

To explore this further, this study conducted a mediated effects test using childhood trauma and its dimensions as independent variables, NSSI frequency as the dependent variable, and two dimensions of mentalizing as mediating variables, controlling for gender. The nonparametric percentile bootstrap method, advocated by Zhonglin Wen (26), was used. A bootstrap sample of 5000 was first drawn, with robust standard parameter errors and confidence intervals estimated, using a 95% CI excluding 0. The sample was tested for mediation effects via the SPSS Process macro program prepared by Hayes.

The bootstrap method-mediated effects test revealed that adolescents’ hypermentalization had significant mediating effects on EN, PA, EA, PN, CTQ, and NSSI, with the ratios of indirect effects to total effects being 14.04%, 9.66%, 7.6%, 8.18%, 14.18%, and 9.50%, respectively. The mediating effect between adolescent hypomentalization and CTQ was significant for all dimensions and NSSI, with mediating effect sizes ranging from 18.73% to 31.03%; none of their confidence intervals were 0.03%. In addition, the mediating effect of hypermentalization between SA and NSSI was not significant, with bootstrap intervals including 0 (Table 5).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Test of the mediating effect of mentalizing between childhood trauma and NSSI.

Structural equation model testing

Using structural equation modeling (SEM) tests can make the results of bootstrap tests a more convincing and scientific exercise. To test the mediating model that exists between childhood trauma, mentalizing, and NSSI, we used SEM.

Hypomentalization and hypermentalization are inversely correlated with childhood trauma and NSSI, respectively. In this study, the two dimensions of mentalizing are modeled separately as mediating variables, and the hypotheses that they mediate the relationship between childhood trauma and NSSI were tested individually.

Model 1: Structural equation modeling was used to verify the mediating role of mentalizing deficits in the association between childhood trauma and NSSI. The model fit well after model correction. All of the childhood trauma → hypomentalization → NSSI pathways were statistically significant. Childhood trauma positively predicted hypomentalization (β = 0.395, P < 0.001), hypomentalization positively predicted NSSI frequency (β = 0.231, P < 0.001), and childhood trauma positively predicted NSSI frequency (β = 0.475, P < 0.001), as shown in Figure 1a. The results of the bootstrap method test indicated that the indirect effect of childhood traumatic experiences on NSSI was 0.091 (95% CI: 0.066–0.120), the total effect was 0.566 (95% CI: 0.501–0.631), the mediating effect accounted for 16.08% of the total effect, and hypomentalization was significantly and represents a partial mediating effect.

Figure 1
Diagram (a) and diagram (b) depict structural equation models of childhood trauma factors influencing hypomentalization or hypermentalization and non-suicidal self-injury frequency. Both diagrams show connections between physical neglect, sexual abuse, emotional abuse, physical abuse, emotional neglect, and childhood trauma. Diagram (a) highlights positive paths with hypomentalization, while diagram (b) shows negative paths with hypermentalization. Both illustrate paths to non-suicidal self-injury frequency. Arrows with coefficients indicate the strength and direction of relationships.

Figure 1. Pathways related to childhood trauma, hypomentalization, and NSSI frequency. (a) Modified model 1. (b) Modified model 2. (Note: ***P < 0.001).

Model 2: Structural equation modeling was applied to verify the mediating role of hypermentalization between childhood trauma and NSSI. After model correction, when 3 < CMIN/Df value < 5, the model did not fit well (Table 6). The pathway of childhood trauma → hypermentalization → NSSI was statistically significant. Childhood trauma negatively predicted overmentalization (β = −0.286, P < 0.001), hypermentalization negatively predicted NSSI frequency (β = 0.528, P < 0.001), and hypermentalization could be used as a protective factor against NSSI. Childhood trauma positively predicted NSSI frequency (β = -0.128, P < 0.001) (Figure 1b). The bootstrap method test revealed that the indirect effect of childhood traumatic experience on NSSI was 0.037 (95% CI: 0.025–0.049), the total effect was 0.564 (95% CI: 0.499–0.629), and the mediating effect accounted for 6.56% of the total effect. The mediating effect was significant and partially mediated, represents a partial mediating effect. However, the model fit was average (3 < CMIN/DF < 5).

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Model fit superiority index.

The above analysis verified that childhood trauma can influence NSSI behavior through mentalizing, and although the model of hypermentalization exhibited a fair fit, the model of hypomentalization demonstrated a good fit. Furthermore, hypermentalization can influence NSSI risk partially through childhood trauma.

Comparison of OT concentration and mentalizing ability between the NSSI and non-NSSI groups and correlation analysis

The differences in OT concentration between the NSSI and non-NSSI groups were not significant, and the differences in hypermentalization, hypomentalization, and childhood trauma scores between the two groups were statistically significant (P > 0.05, Table 7). Correlation analysis revealed that OT concentration was not significantly correlated with childhood trauma, NSSI frequency, hypermentalization, or hypomentalization (P > 0.05, Table 8).

Table 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Comparison between the two groups.

Table 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Correlation analysis with OT.

Predicted NSSI risk via binary logistic regression analysis

In terms of age and sex, binary logistic regression analysis revealed that the CTQ score positively predicted NSSI risk (OR = 1.094, 95% CI: 1.054–1.135, P < 0.001), the RFQU score positively predicted NSSI risk (OR = 1.182, 95% CI: 1.060–1.318, P = 0.003), and the OT concentration had no significant effect on NSSI risk (P < 0.05) (Table 9).

Table 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. The impact of childhood trauma, mentalizing, and OT on NSSI.

Discussion

Since COVID-19 and its countermeasures in worldwide have caused widespread mental health problems. Lots of data shew that psychological, economic, behavioral, and psychosocial problems associated with the COVID-19 pandemic may lead to a growth in self-harm (27, 28). In the screening sample, this study revealed an elevated rate of NSSI detection compared with that before the onset of COVID-19, which aligned with the results of a longitudinal study of NSSI in an Italian adolescent sample (29). These findings indicate that NSSI is prevalent in the adolescent population and deserves the attention of schools and professionals. However, the detection rate of NSSI in adolescents was higher by Xu et al. (30), which may be different from the original source of our sample. This study focused primarily on secondary schools in the city of Dongguan. The investigation revealed that the occurrence of NSSI nationally was influenced by urban culture (31).

As a rapidly emerging first-tier city, Dongguan has attracted a large number of non-local workers. Pressure from the economic downturn caused by the pandemic has had a significant impact on the families of migrant workers, increasing economic pressures and raising parents’ expectations for their children’s academic performance while simultaneously disrupting parent-child communication. As a result, the prevalence of non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) among the adolescent group in this study was relatively high. In addition, the samples for this study were collected 18 months after the pandemic, during a period when the country had entered a phase of regular control. Schools had shifted to online learning, the range of activities available to students was restricted, and peer interactions were limited. These challenges led to higher levels of negative emotions, such as depression and anxiety, among the adolescents in this study compared to those in previous studies, thereby increasing the risk of developing NSSI (32), particularly in those with preexisting vulnerabilities (e.g., a history of NSSI or high levels of internalization) (29).

We also found significant gender differences in childhood trauma, mentalizing, and NSSI behaviors in this study. Compared with boys, girls had higher levels of childhood trauma, hypomentalization, and NSSI behaviors, a result that aligned with previous views (13, 33, 34). A study that investigated the prevalence of NSSI among 16,853 secondary school students in three Chinese cities reported that female adolescents were at greater risk of NSSI than were male adolescents, whereas one study reported up to three times greater risk (35, 36). A recent longitudinal study of NSSI behavior in adolescents revealed that females not only had a greater risk of NSSI than males but also had a greater frequency of NSSI and a longer duration of NSSI (37). This might be related to the biological functioning of women, who are susceptible to negative emotions due to differences in the brain structure of emotions. Emotional attribution theory suggests that when faced with a negative event, men identify the cause from the objective environment, attribute unpleasant feelings to external sources, and are prone to externalizing negative emotions, whereas women tend to criticize themselves, attribute negative experiences to their own inadequacies, are prone to ruminative thinking, accumulate negative emotions, and are inclined to resort to inappropriate emotional regulation (NSSI) to vent (38, 39).

This was the first study to investigate the mediating role of mentalizing in the relationship between childhood maltreatment and NSSI in a nonclinical sample of teenagers, aiming to examine the underlying mechanisms of the effects of childhood maltreatment on NSSI. In this study, NSSI was found to be associated with mentalizing through a survey of 1313 junior high school students, and a pathway analysis was conducted. The results showed that mentalizing had a mediating effect on the occurrence of NSSI in the mediating pathway through which childhood trauma affected NSSI. Consistent with the findings of a recent study of a mediating model of mentalizing in clinical and community-based self-harm samples (40), attachment and mentalizing were found to play unique mediating roles between childhood trauma and psychopathology in this study of self-harm patients, which included clinical samples. We extended this model to a community NSSI sample and tested the mentalizing model of NSSI via the bootstrap method of the SPSS Process macro and structural equation modeling models, with community adolescent NSSI occurrence associated with mentalizing impairment. These studies reveal the mechanisms underlying the associations between childhood trauma and BPD and NSSI, where early adverse experiences may cause recent mental distress. In the face of negative events, early traumatic experiences are often revisited as intense emotional experiences, and people go on to avoid normal related psychological experiences and instead regulate emotional distress through maladaptive behaviors (41). NSSI is considered a maladaptive emotion regulation strategy that addresses negative emotions and stress in the short term but impairs interpersonal functioning in the long term, and it is not recognized as such (42). Mentalization deficits could lead to adolescents lacking socially beneficial qualities such as trust and empathy in interpersonal relationships and easily distort the true intentions of others, thereby increasing emotional distress and increasing the risk of NSSI, which is consistent with the cognitive–emotional model view (43). When we are unable to properly read through the expressions displayed by others during social interactions or even fail to read such cues to empathize with others’ internal experiences, leading to increased interpersonal distress, this often directly contributes to the failure of their self-emotional regulation strategies, prompting them to adopt NSSI to alleviate negative emotions. Individuals who suffer early trauma, particularly psychological trauma, may be more likely to lose this protective factor to maintain social relationships in the face of sudden stress, as confirmed by Germine et al. (44). Childhood trauma is not conducive to the effective development of mentalizing, and impaired mentalizing capacity is associated with many self-harm-related psychiatric disorders (45). Impaired mental health in the face of stress increases the risk of various types of psychiatric distress, and psychiatric distress and symptoms are positively associated with, for example, NSSI and depressed mood (46). Therefore, professionals need to focus on adolescent childhood trauma reduce the incidence of NSSI in adolescents. However, exploring the mechanisms of NSSI in terms of only psychological factors does not fully capture its underlying complexity, and this study also examines OT as a potential biological factor contributing to NSSI.

To explore the associations of social hormones such as OT with NSSI and mentalizing, this study included 109 NSSI and 113 non-NSSI individuals in the primary screening sample for OT testing. The results revealed that there was no significant difference in salivary OT levels between NSSI and non-NSSI individuals. In addition, OT levels did not correlate with measures of hypermindfulness, mentalizing deficits, or childhood trauma. Although OT is associated with mental illness, its potential association with NSSI remains underexplored. Bertsch reported an inverse relationship between blood OT levels and aggression in a study focusing on blood OT levels (47), and although the main focus was on personality disorders, the results suggested that OT could reduce aggressive behavior. A recent review demonstrated that OT could be an important mediator in the intergenerational transmission of early adversity in BPD (48). These findings indicate that OT may mediate the effects of childhood trauma on mental disorders. In contrast, our findings found no difference in salivary OT levels between the NSSI group and the control group. There may be multiple reasons for these inconsistent results. First, in clinical samples, OT levels in BPD patients have also been found to be related to the severity of the illness (49), and there may be no difference in OT concentrations between patients with milder psychiatric illnesses and healthy individuals. The sample selected for this study was a nonclinical sample, so the course of the disease was not as severe as that of the inpatients, and the damage to the OT system took some time to appear at the peripheral level. Second, the OT peaks might have shown a certain circadian rhythm of secretion, and the time selected did not show peak values (50). Second, this study selected saliva samples, whereas changes in the OT concentration in cerebrospinal fluid are much more sensitive and better reflect facial perception but are not related to peripheral OT (51, 52). Third, lower levels of OT were observed in patients hospitalized for suicide than in the normal group (53), a change that was not observed in patients with NSSI. Childhood trauma may not be linked to oxytocin (OT) in patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD). In addition, these findings suggest that the neurobiological mechanisms of suicide differ from those of NSSI and that the occurrence of NSSI may not involve changes in the OT system. In patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD), childhood trauma shows no association with oxytocin (OT) levels. This study produced results that align with these findings. However, patients with BPD are more likely to interact with experiences of childhood trauma, often exhibiting abnormal OT responses in social situations, thereby influencing changes in OT levels (54). Childhood trauma might not be sufficient to impair the function of OT but is also influenced by other factors, such as genes and personality traits, that jointly influence OT alterations.

Although OT cannot yet be considered as a candidate biomarker for NSSI, further evaluation in the NSSI population is needed in the future.

Conclusion

This study developed a mentalizing model of NSSI in a community adolescent sample, extending the mentalizing model of BPD to community adolescents with NSSI and providing new ideas for mechanistic research and psychological interventions for NSSI. The case-control study revealed that baseline salivary OT concentrations did not serve as a biomarker for NSSI in community samples. This study was also the first to explore the association between OT concentrations and NSSI in a community-based sample of adolescents. Although salivary OT may not serve as a peripheral biomarker for NSSI, further evaluation is needed in adolescents with inpatient NSSI. Future studies can be exploratory and validated with a focus on the severity of NSSI.

Limitation

The sample selected for this research consists of cross-sectional data, and future studies should include additional longitudinal data to enhance the credibility of the causal relationship between mentalizing and NSSI. With respect to the exploration of the biological factors of NSSI, although salivary OT is not associated with NSSI, the connection between central OT and NSSI cannot be eliminated, and future studies should investigate this further.

Data availability statement

The raw data are publicly available in the Figshare repository under doi: 10.6084/m9.figshare.29941013.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by This study received approval from the Ethics Committee of the Affiliated Hospital of Guangdong Medical University (Approval No: PJKT2022-051). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

CC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Project administration, Resources, Supervision. YZ: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Methodology, Validation, Visualization. ZL: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. ZW: Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. JZ: Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. YL: Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The authors declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by National Nature Science Foundation of China (Grant No.81101021) and Graduate Education Innovation Program of Guangdong Province (Grant No. 2022JGXM107).

Acknowledgments

We thank LetPub (www.letpub.com.cn) for its linguistic assistance during the preparation of this manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

NSSI, Nonsuicidal self-injurious; BPD, Borderline Personality Disorder ; DSM-5, Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th edition; RF, Reflective functioning; OT, Oxytocin; ANSAQ, Adolescent nonsuicidal self-injury assessment questionnaire; RFQ-8, Reflective functioning questionnaire-8; RFQC, Certainty about mental states; RFQU, Uncertainty about mental states; CTQ, Childhood trauma questionnaire; EA, Emotional abuse; PA, Physical abuse; SA, Sexual abuse; EN, Emotional neglect; PN, Physical neglect; CI, Confidence interval; GFI, Goodness-of-fit index; AGFI, Adjusted goodness-of-fit index; CFI, Comparative fit index; IFI, Incremental fit index; TFI, Tucker–Lewis coefficient; SRMR, Standardized root mean square residual; RMSEA, Root mean square error of approximation; Z, Mann–Whitney U test; SE, Standard error; SEM, Structural equation modeling; OR, Odds ratio; CMIN, The Chi-square value of the maximum likelihood ratio; DF, Degrees of Freedom.

References

1. Ghinea D, Edinger A, Parzer P, Koenig J, Resch F, and Kaess M. Non-suicidal self-injury disorder as a stand-alone diagnosis in a consecutive help-seeking sample of adolescents. J Affect Disord. (2020) 274:1122–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.06.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Reichl C and Kaess M. Self-harm in the context of borderline personality disorder. Curr Opin Psychol. (2021) 37:139–44. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.12.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Selby EA, Harnedy LE, Hiner M, and Kim J. Developmental and momentary dynamics in the onset and maintenance of nonsuicidal self-injurious behavior and borderline personality disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2022) 24:897–909. doi: 10.1007/s11920-022-01396-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Whitlock J, Muehlenkamp J, Eckenrode J, Purington A, Baral Abrams G, Barreira P, et al. Nonsuicidal self-injury as a gateway to suicide in young adults. J Adolesc Health. (2013) 52:486–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.09.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Asarnow JR, Porta G, Spirito A, Emslie G, Clarke G, Wagner KD, et al. Suicide attempts and nonsuicidal self-injury in the treatment of resistant depression in adolescents: findings from the TORDIA study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2011) 50:772–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.04.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Mannekote Thippaiah S, Shankarapura Nanjappa M, Gude JG, Voyiaziakis E, Patwa S, Birur B, et al. Non-suicidal self-injury in developing countries: a review. Int J Soc Psychiatry. (2020) 67:472–82. doi: 10.1177/0020764020943627

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Moulton-Perkins A, Lee Y-W, Warren F, Howard S, et al. Development and validation of a self-report measure of mentalizing: the reflective functioning questionnaire. PloS One. (2016) 11:e0158678. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0158678

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Ochsner KN. The social-emotional processing stream: five core constructs and their translational potential for schizophrenia and beyond. Biol Psychiatry. (2008) 64:48–61. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Fonagy P, Gergely G, Jurist EL, and Target M. Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self, 1st ed. London: Routledge (2002).

Google Scholar

10. Fonagy P and Luyten P. A multilevel perspective on the development of borderline personality disorder. In: Cicchetti D, editor. Developmental psychopathology, 3rd ed, vol. 3 . Wiley, New York (2016). p. 726–92.

Google Scholar

11. Kristiansen VR, Handeland TB, Lau B, Søderstrøm K, Håkansson U, and Øie MG. Trauma in childhood and adolescence and impaired executive functions are associated with uncertain reflective functioning in mothers with substance use disorder. Addict Behav Rep. (2020) 11:100245. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2019.100245

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Beilharz JE, Paterson M, Fatt S, Wilson C, Burton A, Cvejic E, et al. The impact of childhood trauma on psychosocial functioning and physical health in a non-clinical community sample of young adults. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. (2020) 54:185–94. doi: 10.1177/0004867419881206

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Müller LE, Bertsch K, Bülau K, Herpertz SC, and Buchheim A. Emotional neglect in childhood shapes social dysfunctioning in adults by influencing the oxytocin and the attachment system: results from a population-based study. Int J Psychophysiol. (2019) 136:73–80. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.05.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Cropp C, Alexandrowicz RW, and Taubner S. Reflective functioning in an adolescent community sample. Ment Health Prev. (2019) 14:200156. doi: 10.1016/j.mph.2019.200156

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bateman A and Fonagy P. Borderline personality disorder and mood disorders: mentalizing as a framework for integrated treatment. J Clin Psychol. (2015) 71:792–804. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22206

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Parris MS, Grunebaum MF, Galfalvy HC, Andronikashvili A, Burke AK, Yin H, et al. Attempted suicide and oxytocin-related gene polymorphisms. J Affect Disord. (2018) 238:62–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.05.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Engel S, Laufer S, Knaevelsrud C, and Schumacher S. The endogenous oxytocin system in depressive disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2019) 101:138–49. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.11.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Ditzen B, Schaer M, Gabriel B, Bodenmann G, Ehlert U, and Heinrichs M. Intranasal oxytocin increases positive communication and reduces cortisol levels during couple conflict. Biol Psychiatry. (2009) 65:728–31. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Aspesi D and Choleris E. Neuroendocrine underpinning of social recognition in males and females. J Neuroendocrinol. (2022) 34:e13070. doi: 10.1111/jne.13070

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Servan A, Brunelin J, and Poulet E. The effects of oxytocin on social cognition in borderline personality disorder. Encephale. (2018) 44:46–51. doi: 10.1016/j.encep.2017.11.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Bertsch K, Gamer M, Schmidt B, Schmidinger I, Walther S, Kästel T, et al. Oxytocin and reduction of social threat hypersensitivity in women with borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2013) 170:1169–77. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13020263

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Carrasco JL, Buenache E, MacDowell KS, de la Vega I, López-Villatoro JM, Moreno B, et al. Decreased oxytocin plasma levels and oxytocin receptor expression in borderline personality disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2020) 142:319–25. doi: 10.1111/acps.13222

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Liu W, Wan Y, Tao F, and Hao J. Mediating effect of social support on relationship between childhood abuses and non-suicidal self-injuries in adolescents. Chin Ment Health J. (2017) 31:230–4. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2017.03.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Xu L. Reliability and validity of Chinese version of reflective functioning questionary-8. China Medical University, Shenyang (2018).

Google Scholar

25. Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D, Ahluvalia T, et al. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl. (2003) 27:169–90. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00541-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Wen ZL and Ye BJ. Analysis of mediating effects: Development of methods and models. Adv psychol Sci. (2014) 22:731–745. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.00731

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Sher L. The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on suicide rates. QJM. (2020) 113:707–12. doi: 10.1093/qjmed/hcaa202/

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. World Health Organization. WHO Coronavirus (COVID-19) dashboard (2021). Available online at: https://covid19.who.int/ (Accessed 28 Dec 2021).

Google Scholar

29. De Luca L, Giletta M, Nocentini A, and Menesini E. Non-Suicidal Self-Injury in adolescence: the role of pre-existing vulnerabilities and COVID-19-related stress. J Youth Adolesc. (2022) 51:2383–95. doi: 10.1007/s10964-022-01669-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Xu H, Wang R, Li R, Jin Z, Wan Y, and Tao F. Associations of non-suicidal self-injury and psychological symptoms with suicide attempt in adolescents: are there any gender differences? Front Psychiatry. (2022) 13:894218. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.894218

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. hao Y and Liu J. Research progress on physiological pain, social pain and their influencing factors in patients with non - suicidal self - injury. Chin J Nervous Ment Dis. (2024) 50:49–53. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1002-0152.2024.01.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Schwartz-Mette RA, Duell N, Lawrence HR, and Balkind EG. COVID-19 distress impacts adolescents’ depressive symptoms, NSSI, and suicide risk in the rural, northeast US. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2022) 52:702–15. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2022.2042697

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Charpentier Mora S, Bastianoni C, Cavanna D, and Bizzi F. Emerging adults facing the COVID-19 pandemic: emotion dysregulation, mentalizing, and psychological symptoms. Curr Psychol. (2024) 43:1–6. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03322-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Chiesa M and Fonagy P. Reflective function as a mediator between childhood adversity, personality disorder and symptom distress. Pers Ment Health. (2014) 8:52–66. doi: 10.1002/pmh.1245

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Yates TM. The developmental psychopathology of self-injurious behavior: compensatory regulation in posttraumatic adaptation. Clin Psychol Rev. (2004) 24:35–74. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2003.10.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Xiao W, Rong F, Li S, Xu H, Jin Z, Li R, et al. Co-occurrence patterns of adverse childhood experiences and their associations with non-suicidal self-injury and suicidal behaviors and gender difference among middle school students: a three-city survey in China. J Affect Disord. (2023) 320:140–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.09.124

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Wang B, Zhang M, Zhang J, Hu J, You J, and Liang Y. Latent transition analysis of the self-injury in adolescents: a longitudinal study. J Psychol Sci. (2015) 38:1368–76. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20150613

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Azhu H, Geng Xu, and Puyu Su. Meta-analysis on the prevalence characteristics of non-suicidal self-injury among middle school students in mainland China. Chin J School Health. (2017) 38:1665–70. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2017.11.019

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Hafsa S, Aqeel M, and Shuja KH. The moderating role of emotional intelligence between inter-parental conflicts and loneliness in male and female adolescents. Nature-Nurture J Psychol. (2021) 1:38–48. doi: 10.47391/NNJP.5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Stagaki M, Nolte T, Feigenbaum J, King-Casas B, Lohrenz T, Fonagy P, et al. The mediating role of attachment and mentalising in the relationship between childhood maltreatment, self-harm and suicidality. Child Abuse Negl. (2022) 128:105576. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2022.105576

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Allen JG. Mentalizing suicidal states. In: Michel K and Jobes DA, editors. Building a therapeutic alliance with the suicidal patient. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, US (2011). p. 81–91.

Google Scholar

42. Taylor PJ, Jomar K, Dhingra K, Forrester R, Shahmalak U, and Dickson JM. A meta-analysis of the prevalence of different functions of non-suicidal self-injury. J Affect Disord. (2018) 227:759–69. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.073

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Hasking P, Whitlock J, Voon D, and Rose A. A cognitive-emotional model of NSSI: using emotion regulation and cognitive processes to explain why people self-injure. Cognit Emot. (2016) 31:1543–56. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1241219

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Germine L, Dunn EC, McLaughlin KA, and Smoller JW. Childhood adversity is associated with adult theory of mind and social affiliation, but not face processing. PloS One. (2015) 10:e0129612. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129612

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Ensink K, Bégin M, Normandin L, and Fonagy P. Parental reflective functioning as a moderator of child internalizing difficulties in the context of child sexual abuse. Psychiatry Res. (2017) 257:361–6. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.07.051

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Plener PL, Kaess M, Schmahl C, Pollak S, Fegert JM, and Brown RC. Nonsuicidal self-injury in adolescents. Dtsch Arztebl Int. (2018) 115:23–30. doi: 10.3238/arztebl.2018.0023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Bertsch K, Schmidinger I, Neumann ID, and Herpertz SC. Reduced plasma oxytocin levels in female patients with borderline personality disorder. Horm Behav. (2013) 63:424–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Steele H and Siever L. An attachment perspective on borderline personality disorder: advances in gene–environment considerations. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2010) 12:61–7. doi: 10.1007/s11920-009-0091-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Fang A, Jacoby RJ, Beatty C, Germine L, Plessow F, Wilhelm S, et al. Serum oxytocin levels are elevated in body dysmorphic disorder and related to severity of psychopathology. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2020) 113:104541. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104541

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Van Dam JM, Garrett AJ, Schneider LA, Buisman-Pijlman FTA, Short MA, Hodyl NA, et al. Variability of the cortisol awakening response and morning salivary oxytocin in late adolescence. J Neuroendocrinol. (2018) 30:e12645. doi: 10.1111/jne.12645

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Martin J, Kagerbauer SM, Gempt J, Podtschaske A, Hapfelmeier A, and Schneider G. Oxytocin levels in saliva correlate better than plasma levels with concentrations in the cerebrospinal fluid of patients in neurocritical care. J Neuroendocrinol. (2018) 30:e12596. doi: 10.1111/jne.12596

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Madrid JE, Oztan O, Sclafani V, Del Rosso LA, Calonder LA, Chun K, et al. Preference for novel faces in male infant monkeys predicts cerebrospinal fluid oxytocin concentrations later in life. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:12935. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13109-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Jahangard L, Shayganfard M, Ghiasi F, Salehi I, Haghighi M, Ahmadpanah M, et al. Serum oxytocin concentrations in current and recent suicide survivors are lower than in healthy controls. J Psychiatr Res. (2020) 128:75–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.05.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Kartal F, Uğur K, Mete B, Demirkol ME, and Tamam L. The relationship between the oxytocin level and rejection sensitivity, childhood traumas, and attachment styles in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Investig. (2022) 19:239–46. doi: 10.30773/pi.2021.0358

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mentalizing, childhood trauma, salivary oxytocin, nonsuicidal self-injury, structural equation modeling

Citation: Chu C, Zhou Y, Lin Z, Wang Z, Zhang J and Lu Y (2025) The first study of a mentalizing mediator model in a community sample of nonsuicidal self-injured adolescents and its association with salivary oxytocin. Front. Psychiatry 16:1590441. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1590441

Received: 09 March 2025; Accepted: 08 August 2025;
Published: 29 August 2025.

Edited by:

Ming Ai, First Affiliated Hospital of Chongqing Medical University, China

Reviewed by:

Liliane Vilete, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
Muhammad Aqeel, Foundation University Rawalpindi, Pakistan

Copyright © 2025 Chu, Zhou, Lin, Wang, Zhang and Lu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chengjing Chu, Y2hjaGoyMDE1QGdkbXUuZWR1LmNu

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.