ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 16 May 2025

Sec. Psychopathology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1597268

Emotion recognition deficits in patients with skin picking disorder: the role of alexithymia while controlling depression, and anxiety levels

  • 1Istanbul Erenkoy Training and Research Hospital for Psychiatric and Neurological Diseases, Istanbul, Türkiye
  • 2Faculty of Economics, Administrative and Social Sciences, Department of Psychology, Bahçeşehir University, Istanbul, Türkiye
  • 3School of Medicine, Marmara University, Istanbul, Türkiye

Objective: Skin Picking Disorder (SPD) has been associated with higher levels of alexithymia, a condition that predicts self-injurious behaviors, a core feature of SPD. Recent studies have expanded the understanding of alexithymia beyond deficits in emotional awareness, highlighting its role on the ability to recognize and process others’ emotions. This study aimed to explore how emotion recognition abilities differ in individuals with varying levels of alexithymia and the presence of SPD.

Methods: This cross-sectional case-control study included 45 individuals diagnosed with SPD and 47 controls. Participants were assessed through the Toronto Alexithymia Scale, the Facial Emotion Recognition Test, the Beck Depression Inventory, and the Beck Anxiety Inventory. Two-way analysis of covariance tests were conducted to evaluate the effects of SPD diagnosis and alexithymia levels on facial emotion recognition tasks, while controlling for anxiety and depression.

Results: Individuals with Skin Picking Disorder (SPD) showed significant impairments in recognizing fear, neutral expressions, and surprise compared to controls. SPD was linked to lower fear recognition accuracy, while both SPD and higher alexithymia were associated with poorer recognition of neutral expressions and surprise. No significant differences were found for happiness, sadness, anger, or disgust. No interaction effects were observed between SPD and alexithymia for any emotion.

Conclusions: This study enhances the understanding of emotion recognition in SPD and alexithymia by highlighting their shared and unique challenges. The absence of a significant interaction effect suggests that these conditions independently contribute to emotion recognition deficits without compounding effects, underscoring the need for targeted interventions.

1 Introduction

Skin-picking disorder (SPD) is a body-focused repetitive behavior characterized by recurrent and compulsive picking of the skin, which results in skin lesions and can lead to significant distress or impairment in daily functioning. Originally, SPD was classified under impulse control disorders not otherwise specified in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders- Text Revision, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV-TR) due to its association with impulsive behaviors (1). However, with the release of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5), SPD was reclassified under obsessive-compulsive and related disorders (2). This shift in classification highlights the compulsive nature of SPD, aligning it more closely with disorders such as trichotillomania, hoarding disorder, body dysmorphic disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. These conditions share a common theme of repetitive behaviors and mental preoccupations that are often driven by an underlying urge or compulsion, rather than an impulsive act alone. For instance, the act of skin-picking in patients with SPD is often driven by an overwhelming urge that arises from an internal sense of tension or emotional discomfort. The compulsive behavior serves as a temporary relief from these negative feelings, yet it simultaneously leads to non-suicidal self-injury, manifesting as skin lesions and scarring (3).

Research indicates that SPD is associated with negative emotions, low self-esteem, reduced subjective physical well-being, and a loss of control (4). A significant proportion of individuals with SPD have at least one lifetime comorbid psychiatric disorder, with anxiety and depression being the most prevalent ones. The prevalence of depression among individuals with SPD ranges from 12.5% to 48%, while anxiety disorders are reported in 8% to 23% of cases (5). Additionally, in a study of 92 individuals diagnosed with SPD, 85.9% reported experiencing anxiety, and 66.3% reported depression as a result of their skin picking behaviors (6). In Turkish clinical samples, the psychiatric comorbidity rate among SPD patients was found to be 78.9% (7).

Alexithymia is a psychological construct characterized by difficulty in identifying and describing emotions, distinguishing emotions from bodily sensations, and a tendency toward externally oriented thinking (8). First introduced by Sifneos (9), the concept emerged within the field of psychosomatic medicine and was initially viewed as a personality trait that increased vulnerability to psychosomatic illness (10). Beyond its established role in psychosomatic conditions (e.g., 11, 12), alexithymia has been widely studied across various psychiatric conditions. It has been linked to anxiety (13), obsessive-compulsive disorders (14, 15), eating disorders (16), personality disorders (17), depressive disorders in psychiatric patients (18).

Recent evidence has also emphasized its role as a significant vulnerability factor in the onset and progression of chronic medical conditions and increasingly been studied in relation to diseases such as asthma, inflammatory bowel disease and type 2 diabetes (19, 20) and psychodermatological disorders such as alopecia areata, psoriasis and chronic urticaria (21). Research indicates that individuals with high levels of alexithymia often exhibit impairments in emotion regulation, increased psychological distress, and reduced adherence to treatment—factors that may contribute to poorer health outcomes and increased disease severity (22, 23). It has also been associated with avoidant coping strategies and immature defense mechanisms, such as denial, particularly in response to emotional stress and trauma (24, 25). These defensive processes may offer short-term protection but can impair the integration of memory, perception, and emotion into conscious awareness (26, 27). Alexithymia is also a significant predictor of non-suicidal self-injury (28), with evidence suggesting that individuals with alexithymia may engage in self-injury to manage their emotional states (29). This connection extends to body-focused repetitive behaviors, which are considered maladaptive strategies for emotion regulation. According to the emotion regulation model, body-focused repetitive behaviors are triggered by negative emotions and provide short-term relief by alleviating unpleasant feelings, though they ultimately lead to increased emotional and physiological distress (30). Additionally, individuals with skin-picking disorder (SPD) exhibit altered neural sensitivity in touch processing, which has been linked to emotional regulation deficits. This disrupted sensory experience may reinforce maladaptive self-soothing behaviors, further contributing to SPD symptoms (31).

Recent research has expanded beyond understanding alexithymia as merely an emotion awareness deficit, showing its relationship with the ability to recognize and process others’ emotions. Clinical studies have demonstrated that individuals with alexithymia also experience deficits in recognizing facially expressed emotions of others (32). Even among non-clinical populations, those with high alexithymia show impairments in recognizing facially expressed emotions. During facial emotion recognition tests, alexithymic individuals exhibit lower activation in brain areas associated with emotional awareness (such as the anterior cingulate cortex) and regions involved in facial emotion recognition (including the amygdala, insula, striatum, inferior frontal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, thalamus, parahippocampal gyrus, and middle frontal gyrus) compared to non-alexithymic individuals (33). A recent neuroimaging study highlighted that the cognitive dimension of alexithymia is particularly associated with lower activation in emotional attention and recognition networks, leading to deficits in emotion processing (34).

Also, SPD has been increasingly linked to deficits in facial emotion recognition (35), a fundamental aspect of social cognition essential for effective interpersonal interactions and emotional regulation. These impairments may contribute to increased emotional distress by limiting an individual’s ability to interpret and respond to social cues, reducing the effectiveness of interpersonal interactions as a means of emotional regulation. Consequently, individuals with SPD may rely more heavily on maladaptive coping mechanisms, such as compulsive skin-picking, to manage distress (36). Furthermore, the lower capacity for emotional self-awareness and insight, further impairing individuals’ ability to recognize and address triggers underlying their compulsive behaviors (34). As a result, alexithymia may play a significant role in the persistence of SPD symptoms by reinforcing the cycle of emotion dysregulation and self-injurious behavior. However, the extent to which emotion recognition impairments in SPD are linked to alexithymia, or whether they occur independently, remains an open question that has not been adequately explored in the literature.

1.1 Objectives of the study

Despite the high prevalence of alexithymia in many psychiatric disorders (37) there is a limited research specifically examining the interplay between alexithymia and facial emotion recognition in SPD. While previous studies have investigated emotion recognition and alexithymia separately across different psychiatric populations, few have explored their interaction in individuals with SPD. Aydin et al. (35) demonstrated that individuals with SPD and Trichotillomania exhibit increased alexithymia. In addition, Kłosowska et al. (38) found that individuals with SPD reported reduced emotional awareness and lower interoceptive accuracy—both traits associated with alexithymia—suggesting difficulties in associating internal bodily cues with emotional states. Similarly, Ciuluvica et al. (39) found that patients with chronic skin conditions exhibited higher levels of emotion suppression and alexithymic traits compared to healthy controls, with significant associations between emotion regulation deficits and poorer quality of life.

In light of these research, this study aims to fill the critical gap by comprehensively assessing facial emotion recognition abilities in individuals with SPD, comparing their performance with that of controls while accounting for varying levels of alexithymia. Importantly, by controlling for depression and anxiety—two conditions highly comorbid with SPD—we aim to disentangle the specific contribution of alexithymia to emotion recognition deficits and determine whether such impairments reflect an intrinsic feature of SPD or are secondary to broader affective symptoms

By addressing these questions, our study may provide novel insights into the underlying cognitive and emotional mechanisms contributing to SPD. A clearer understanding of these factors could have significant clinical implications, guiding the development of targeted interventions aimed at improving emotion recognition and regulation in individuals with SPD. If facial emotion recognition deficits are found to be closely tied to alexithymia, treatment approaches incorporating emotional awareness training may be particularly beneficial. Conversely, if such deficits exist independently of alexithymia, interventions may need to focus more directly on enhancing social cognition skills in SPD patients.

2 Method

2.1 Sampling

This cross-sectional case-control study, conducted between February and August 2024, included 45 patients with SPD diagnosis from dermatology outpatient units of Marmara University Medical Faculty and Erenköy Training and Research Hospital for Mental and Neurological Diseases. A dermatology specialist recorded medical histories. Eligible participants were adults with SPD, without comorbid psychotic disorders or cognitive deficits, and not receiving psychiatric treatment in the past year due to its impact on emotion recognition (40). Psychiatric examinations were conducted at Erenköy Training and Research Hospital for Mental and Neurological Diseases. The control group comprised age- and gender-matched volunteers from the hospital’s administrative staff, excluding those with skin diseases, chronic illnesses, psychosis, cognitive deficits, or recent psychiatric treatment. Ethical approval was granted by Marmara University, Faculty of Medicine (09.02.2024/219), and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

2.2 Measures

All participants filled out the sociodemographic form, the Toronto Alexithymia Scale, the Ekman Emotion Recognition Scale, the Beck Anxiety Inventory, and the Beck Depression Inventory.

Sociodemographic Form: This form, prepared by the researchers, contained questions regarding the participants’ age, gender, education, and marital status.

Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20): The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (8) consists of 20 items in three subscales, namely the Difficulty Identifying Feelings (DIF), Difficulty Describing Feelings (DDF), and Externally-Oriented Thinking (EOT). Each item is rated on a five-point Likert scale (1-5), with total scores ranging from 20 to 100. The validation study of its Turkish form was conducted by Güleç et al. (41), where a score of 60 or above on the scale indicates the presence of severe alexithymia. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the scale was.77 in the present study.

Facial Emotion Recognition Test (FERT): The photographic set utilized in this research, compiled by Ekman and Friesen (42), comprises images depicting six distinct emotional expressions—happiness, sadness, fear, anger, surprise, and disgust—alongside a neutral expression. The set includes a total of 56 photographs representing 8 models, four females and four males. Initially, patients were introduced to the first seven trial photographs; subsequently, they were requested to identify the remaining 49 images. The photographs were displayed to the patients on a computer in a slideshow format (43), and they were required to respond within a timeframe of 5 seconds.

Beck Depression Inventory (BDI): BDI (44, 45) is used to measure the level of depressive symptoms in adults. It consists of 21 items, each scored from 0 to 3, allowing for a total score range of 0 to 63. A cutoff score of 17 has been established for severe depression. The Turkish validity and reliability of this scale have been conducted by Hisli (46).

Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI): BAI (47) consists of twenty-one items, each scored on a Likert scale from 0 to 3. This scale is specifically designed to evaluate the severity of both subjective and somatic symptoms of anxiety in individuals. The Turkish adaptation of this scale has been validated and tested by Ulusoy et al. (48), confirming its suitability and efficacy for use within the Turkish population.

2.3 Statistical analysis

Data analysis was conducted using SPSS for Windows version 24.0. The normality of the Difficulty Identifying Feelings (D(92) = .058, p = .900), Difficulty Describing Feelings (D(92) = .088, p = .073), and Externally-Oriented Thinking (D(92) = .083, p = .140) subscales of the TAS-20 was assessed using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Additionally, the normality of the Happiness (D(92) = .117, p = .151), Sadness (D(92) = .077, p = .614), Fear (D(92) = .070, p = .724), Surprise (D(92) = .120, p = .131), Disgust (D(92) = .076, p = .629), and Neutral (D(92) = .069, p = .753) subscales of the Facial Emotion Recognition Test, as well as scores from the BDI (D(92) = .121, p = .127) and the BAI (D(92) = .069, p = .744), were also examined.

Between-group differences in continuous variables (e.g., age, TAS-20, FERT, BDI, and BAI scores) were analyzed using independent samples t-tests. Pearson correlation analyses were performed to examine relationships between alexithymia, depression, anxiety, and emotion recognition performance. A series of two-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) tests were performed to investigate the main and interaction effects of SPD diagnosis (SPD vs. control) and alexithymia levels (low vs. high) on emotion recognition performance as measured by the Facial Emotion Recognition Test (FERT). Given the significant differences in anxiety and depression levels between the groups, these variables were included as covariates in all ANCOVA models to control for their potential confounding effects. Separate ANCOVA tests were conducted for each emotion category (happiness, sadness, fear, anger, surprise, disgust, and neutral expressions).

A priori power analysis for ANCOVA was conducted using G*Power 3.1 software to determine the required sample size for detecting medium effect sizes (f = 0.25) with a power of 0.80 and an alpha level of 0.05. The results indicated that a minimum total sample size of 88 participants was necessary to achieve sufficient statistical power. The current study’s sample size (N = 92) met this requirement, ensuring adequate power to detect meaningful effects.

3 Results

3.1 Demographic variables of participants

The study included 92 participants, consisting of 45 individuals diagnosed with SPD and 47 individuals in the control group. The SPD group consisted of 29 females (64.4%) and 16 males (35.6%), while the control group included 29 females (61.7%) and 18 males (38.3%). An independent sample t-test was performed to determine whether there was a significant difference between the mean age of participants diagnosed with SPD and those in the control group. The results indicated that participants with SPD (M = 42.18, SD = 11.82) and control group participants (M= 38.49, SD = 10.85), did not significantly differ from each other, t(90) = 1.56, p = .122.

Furthermore, 15 participants (31.91%) of the SPD group had comorbid depressive disorders, 4 participants (8.51%) had anxiety disorders, and 11 participants (23.40%) had both depressive and anxiety disorders. Additionally, 1 participant (2.13%) had obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), and 2 participants (4.4%) had dissociative disorders. Meanwhile, 12 participants (25.53%) of the SPD group had no comorbid psychiatric diagnosis history, whereas all participants in the control group reported no history of psychiatric diagnosis within the past year. All demographic characteristics of the sample can be found in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the sample.

When the groups were compared regarding their scale scores (Table 2), the SPD group displayed a significantly higher level of BAI (Mdiff = 10.561, t(50.91) = 6.072, p <.001), BDI (Mdiff = 13.696, t(55.58) = 5.374, p <.001) and TAS -20 (Mdiff = 5.192, t(90) 2.353, p = .021) levels than the control group (Table 3). Moreover, with respect to the cut-off score of the TAS-20 scale, 15 (33.3%) participants in the SPD group, and 9 (19.1%) participants in the control group were identified as having severe alexithymic traits.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptives of variables.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Group differences in scale scores.

3.2 Correlational analyses

Pearson correlation analyses were conducted to explore the associations between participants’ levels of alexithymia, depression, and anxiety, and their ability to facial emotion recognition.

The total score of Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) exhibited negative correlations with the Facial Emotion Recognition Test (FERT) subscales, namely the sadness (r =-.327, p=.001), anger (r = -.267, p= .010), and surprise (r = -.422, p<.001). However, there were no significant correlations with fear (r =-.198, p= .059), happiness (r =-.078, p= .459), disgust (r =-.127, p= .228) and neutral (r =-.150, p= .154) state. Distinctly, the level of difficulties in identifying emotions subscale of the TAS-20 was negatively correlated with the fear subscale of the FERT (r =-.277, p=.007).

BDI (r = .495, p <.001) and BAI (r = .515, p <.001) scores of the participants found to be in a positive correlation with their TAS-20 scores. And therefore, added to further analysis as control variables.

All correlations between the TAS-20, FERT, BDI, and BDA scores, as well as the mean and standard deviations of all the scales and subscales are provided in Table 4.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlation coefficients of variables.

3.3 Facial emotion recognition in SPD and alexithymia: group comparisons

To examine differences in facial emotion recognition between individuals with Skin Picking Disorder (SPD) and controls, as well as the impact of alexithymia, a series of ANCOVA tests were conducted. Each analysis tested for the main effects of diagnosis (SPD vs. control controls) and alexithymia levels (low vs. high) on emotion recognition performance while controlling for anxiety and depression. Mean and standard deviations of each factor group can be seen in Table 5.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Means and standard deviations by factor groups.

Although the TAS-20 evaluates three separate cognitive aspects of alexithymia, research has shown that the composite TAS-20 score offers a more complete depiction (8). As a result, the total TAS-20 score was used as a grouping variable in the following analyses. Additionally, since participants with SPD often have co-occurring psychiatric conditions and their scores were significantly higher than those of the control group, the analyses controlled for the levels of anxiety and depression among the participants.

The assumption of homogeneity of regression slopes was tested by including interaction terms between the covariates and grouping variables in the ANCOVA models. None of the interaction effects were statistically significant (all p-values >.05), indicating that the relationship between the covariates and the dependent variables did not differ across groups, thus satisfying the assumption.

The analyses did not reveal significant group differences in recognizing happiness, sadness, anger, or disgust. For happiness, Levene’s test was significant, F(3, 88) = 9.33, p <.001, indicating a violation of the homogeneity of variances assumption. Neither SPD diagnosis (F(1,86) = .512, p= .476, partial η²= .006) nor alexithymia (F(1,86) = 1.505, p = .223, partial η²= .017) had a significant effect, and their interaction was also not significant (F(1,86) = 1.015, p= .317, partial η²=.012). Similarly, for sadness, Levene’s test was not significant, F(3, 88) = 0.57, p = .638. No significant main effects were found for SPD (F(1,86) = 1.250, p= .267, partial η²= .014) or alexithymia (F(1,86) = .737, p= .393, partial η²= .008), nor was there a significant interaction (F(1,86) = .010, p = .922, partial η²= .000).

For anger recognition, Levene’s test was significant, F(3, 88) = 8.61, p <.001. SPD (F(1,86) = 1.093, p= .299, partial η²=.013) and alexithymia (F(1,86) = 0.168, p= .683, partial η²=.002) did not show significant effects, nor did their interaction (F(1,86) = 1.037, p= .311, partial η²=.012). Similarly, recognition of disgust was unaffected by SPD (F(1,86) = .348, p= .557, partial η²= .004), alexithymia (F(1,86) = .525, p= .471, partial η²= .006), or their interaction (F(1,86) = 0.480, p = .490, partial η²= .006).

While SPD and alexithymia did not significantly influence the recognition of these emotions, anxiety significantly predicted sadness recognition (F(1,86) = 8.283, p = .005, partial η²= .088) and anger recognition (F(1,86) = 5.702, p = .019, partial η²= .062). However, depression did not significantly predict recognition of sadness (F(1,86) = 1.095, p = .298, partial η²= .013), anger (F(1,86) = 0.065, p= .799, partial η²= .001), or disgust (F(1,86) = 2.134, p = .148, partial η²= .024).

The SPD group exhibited significant deficits in recognizing fear and neutral expressions compared to controls. Levene’s test indicated that the assumption of homogeneity of variances was met for fear expressions (F(3, 88) = 2.71, p = .050), but violated for neutral expressions (F(3, 88) = 16.87, p <.001). A significant main effect of diagnosis was found for fear recognition (F(1,86) = 5.159, p= .026, partial η²= .057), indicating that individuals with SPD had more difficulty recognizing fear than those in the control group. However, alexithymia (F(1,86) = 1.649, p= .202, partial η²= .019) and the interaction between SPD and alexithymia (F(1,86) = 2.856, p= .095, partial η²= .032) were not significant. For neutral expressions, both SPD (F(1,86) = 21.488, p <.001, partial η²= .200) and alexithymia (F(1,86) = 5.317, p= .024, partial η²= .058) significantly influenced recognition performance, suggesting that individuals with SPD and those with higher alexithymia levels had greater difficulty in recognizing neutral expressions. However, the interaction between SPD and alexithymia was not significant (F(1,86) = 3.283, p= .073, partial η²= .037). Neither anxiety (F(1,86) = 0.651, p= .422, partial η²= .008) nor depression (F(1,86) = .340, p= .561, partial η²= .004) significantly predicted fear recognition, and neither predicted neutral expression recognition (F(1,86) = 1.705, p= .195 partial η²= .019; F(1,86) = .034, p = .853, partial η²= .000).

The ability to recognize surprise was significantly impaired in both SPD and alexithymia groups. Levene’s test was significant for surprise expressions, F(3, 88) = 4.36, p = .007. A main effect of SPD was found (F(1,86) = 6.390, p= .013), indicating that individuals with SPD had greater difficulty recognizing surprise compared to controls. Additionally, alexithymia significantly influenced surprise recognition (F(1,86) = 10.413, p= .002), suggesting that individuals with higher alexithymia levels were also impaired in recognizing surprise. However, the interaction between SPD and alexithymia was not significant (F(1,86) = 2.311, p= .132). Neither anxiety (F(1,86) = .676, p= .413) nor depression (F(1,86) = .297, p= .587) significantly predicted recognition of surprise.

4 Discussion

The primary aim of this study was to investigate how emotion recognition abilities vary based on levels of alexithymia and the presence of SPD, both independently and in interaction. To achieve a more comprehensive understanding, we also accounted for the potential confounding effects of anxiety and depression, given their high prevalence in patients with SPD (49) and their known association with emotion recognition performance (50). The findings reveal distinct patterns in emotion recognition among individuals with SPD and those with varying levels of alexithymia, highlighting both shared and unique challenges associated with these conditions.

For the emotion of fear, a significant difference was observed between individuals with SPD and controls, indicating that those with SPD have more difficulty recognizing fear. On the other hand, alexithymia levels alone did not show a significant relationship with the recognition of fear, nor was there a significant interaction between SPD and alexithymia levels. These findings indicate that challenges in recognizing fear might be more specifically associated with the presence of SPD rather than a deficit in emotional processing linked to alexithymia. The difficulties in fear recognition observed in individuals with SPD could stem from broader social cognitive deficits beyond just emotion recognition. For instance, impairments in understanding others’ mental states, such as recognizing and interpreting the intentions, beliefs, and emotions of others, could contribute to these challenges (51). This broader perspective suggests that focusing solely on alexithymia might overlook other critical aspects of social cognition that are affected in SPD.

When evaluating the recognition of surprise, significant differences were found for both SPD diagnosis and levels of alexithymia, indicating that each condition is independently linked to difficulties in recognizing this emotion. However, no significant interaction effect was observed between SPD and alexithymia, suggesting that these factors do not combine to increase difficulties in recognizing surprise. Beyond difficulties in processing their own emotions, individuals with SPD may face additional challenges in recognizing surprise due to specific factors associated with the disorder, such as attentional biases (52), or more generalized deficits in theory of mind and the recognition of nonaffective social cues (53) which are also observed in OCD-related disorders.

Similarly, for neutral expressions, significant differences were observed for both SPD diagnosis and alexithymia levels, implying that both factors are independently associated with difficulties in interpreting these expressions. Again, no significant interaction effect between SPD and alexithymia was detected. Similar to surprise expressions, which can convey a range of emotions like happiness or fear (54), neutral expressions may be especially challenging to interpret due to their ambiguity. This ambiguity can result in biases, leading individuals to perceive emotions that are not present or struggle to detect subtle facial cues (32). Research has shown that higher levels of self-reported childhood trauma, including sexual and emotional abuse and physical neglect, which are linked to increased alexithymia (55), are also associated with greater misinterpretations of neutral facial expressions, often interpreting them as anger, sadness, or contempt (56). Furthermore, research suggests that individuals with body-focused repetitive behavior disorders may exhibit a tendency to disengage from emotional threat cues (57). If these individuals are prone to interpreting neutral stimuli negatively, as indicated by Passardi et al. (56), they may similarly disengage from neutral stimuli. This tendency could result in inadequate processing, leading to inaccuracies in accurately interpreting these expressions. This tendency to disengage from perceived threat cues may reflect underlying avoidant defense mechanisms activated in response to emotional stress (24, 25), and may function as an strategy to minimize the psychological impact of distressing stimuli (58).

In contrast to the findings for fear, surprise, and neutral expressions, no significant differences were observed for the recognition of other emotions, such as happiness, sadness, disgust, and anger, in relation to SPD, alexithymia, or their interaction. This is consistent with studies in the literature, such as Starita et al. (59), which found no differences between high and low alexithymia groups when identifying happy or disgusted facial expressions. On the other hand, Prkachin et al. (60) found that individuals with high levels of alexithymia were less accurate in matching sad, angry, and fearful faces with their corresponding facial expressions, but showed no significant difference compared to individuals with low alexithymia when it came to matching happy, disgusted, or surprised facial expressions. Additionally, those with high alexithymia were capable of correctly identifying all facial expressions in a non-timed setting and rated the intensity of happy and disgusted expressions similarly to the low alexithymia group. It is also noteworthy that among healthy individuals, alexithymia has been linked to difficulties in identifying others’ emotional expressions, particularly when the stimuli are presented for longer durations and with high intensity (61). These contradictory findings suggest that the relationship between alexithymia and emotion recognition is not straightforward and may depend on multiple factors, including the specific emotions being assessed, the methods used to measure emotion recognition (e.g., speeded vs. non-speeded tasks), and the context in which the emotions are presented. Additionally, the absence of SPD-related differences in our sample might be influenced by variables such as age; for example, research indicates that the association between alexithymia and non-suicidal self-injury behavior is more pronounced in younger populations, whereas the link between alexithymia and risky drinking—a different form of maladaptive coping—is stronger in older individuals (62). Future research should explore these variables further to clarify these relationships.

One limitation of the study is that it concentrated mainly on static emotional expressions, which do not frequently occur in everyday situations. In daily life, people typically need to recognize emotions that change dynamically and are expressed at different levels of intensity, making real-world emotional interpretation more complex (59). Additionally, the study’s cross-sectional design prevents any conclusions about causality between variables, limiting our understanding of how these factors may influence each other over time. Another limitation is that our sample consisted of participants who were seeking medical help, which suggests varying levels of awareness regarding their condition. As a result, the findings may not be representative of all individuals with SPD, particularly those who are not needing any medical treatment. Furthermore, reliance on self-report measures introduces potential biases, such as inaccurate self-assessment, which could affect the accuracy of the findings (63). Last but not least, the violation of the homogeneity of variances assumption in several ANCOVA models, as indicated by significant Levene’s test results, should be acknowledged as a limitation. Although the groups in the design were approximately equal in size—an aspect known to reduce the impact of variance heterogeneity on Type I error rates (64), this violation may still compromise the accuracy and robustness of the results.

On the other hand, the study’s methodological rigor was strengthened by the exclusion of participants in the SPD group with a history of psychiatric treatment within the previous year, ensuring that the potential confounding effects of psychotropic medications on emotion recognition were minimized. Moreover, the a priori power analysis confirmed that the sample size met the required threshold for detecting medium effect sizes with adequate statistical power, further reinforcing the reliability of the findings. These methodological considerations enhanced the validity of the study and ensured that the observed effects are not driven by sample size limitations or uncontrolled confounding factors.

Overall, this study advances the field by filling gaps related to the complexities of emotion recognition in SPD and alexithymia, offering a more detailed understanding of how these conditions are associated with emotional and social cognitive functions. By highlighting both the shared and distinct challenges of these conditions and accounting for various confounding factors, the study provides a strong foundation for future research to explore these relationships further, especially in more naturalistic and dynamic settings. Understanding that SPD and alexithymia impact emotion recognition through different mechanisms suggests that treatments should be tailored accordingly. As the field continues to grow, building on these findings will be crucial in creating comprehensive, individualized treatment strategies that better address the complexities of emotional and social cognitive functioning in clinical populations. Individuals with SPD who also exhibit elevated alexithymia may benefit from interventions that go beyond symptom management and target foundational deficits in emotion processing and social cognition. For example, mentalization-based therapies (65), which aim to improve individuals’ capacity to understand their own and others’ mental states, may be particularly effective in enhancing emotional awareness. Similarly, computer-based emotion recognition training programs, which have been used in populations with social cognition deficits, could offer accessible and targeted ways to improve facial emotion recognition abilities (e.g. 66, 67). Integrating such approaches into standard therapeutic protocols—such as cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) for body-focused repetitive behaviors—may enhance treatment outcomes by addressing underlying emotional and cognitive mechanisms. Further clinical research is needed to examine the effectiveness of these interventions and to assess whether enhancing social-emotional functioning contributes to reductions in symptom severity and improvements in quality of life.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Marmara University, Faculty of Medicine (09.02.2024/219). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

ME: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Methodology, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AT: Data curation, Formal Analysis, Methodology, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. MÇ: Data curation, Resources, Writing – review & editing. MY: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. (4th ed., text rev., Vol. 1). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2000). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890423349

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Fifth Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Gupta MA, Gupta AK, and Haberman HF. Neurotic excoriations: A review and some new perspectives. Compr Psychiatry. (1986) 27:381–6. doi: 10.1016/0010-440X(86)90014-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Gallinat C, Stürmlinger LL, Schaber S, and Bauer S. Pathological skin picking: phenomenology and associations with emotions, self-esteem, body image, and subjective physical well-being. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:732717. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.732717

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Grant JE, Odlaug BL, Chamberlain SR, Keuthen NJ, Lochner C, and Stein DJ. Skin picking disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2012) 169:1143–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12040508

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Flessner CA and Woods DW. Phenomenological characteristics, social problems, and the economic impact associated with chronic skin picking. Behav Modification. (2006) 30:944–63. doi: 10.1177/0145445506294083

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Yalcin M, Tellioglu E, Yildirim DU, Savrun BM, Ozmen M, and Aydemir EH. Psychiatric features in neurotic excoriation patients: the role of childhood trauma. Noro Psikiyatri Arsivi. (2015) 52:336–41. doi: 10.5152/npa.2015.9902

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Bagby RM, Taylor GJ, and Parker JDA. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia scale—II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. J Psychosomatic Res. (1994) 38:33–40. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90006-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Sifneos PE. The prevalence of “alexithymic” characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosomatics. (1973) 22:255–62. doi: 10.1159/000286529

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Nemiah JC and Sifneos PE. Psychosomatic illness: A problem in communication. Psychother Psychosomatics. (1970) 18:154–60. doi: 10.1159/000286074

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kano M and Fukudo S. The alexithymic brain: The neural pathways linking alexithymia to physical disorders. BioPsychoSocial Med. (2013) 7:1. doi: 10.1186/1751-0759-7-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Panayiotou G. Alexithymia as a core trait in psychosomatic and other psychological disorders. In: Charis C and Panayiotou G, editors. Somatoform and Other Psychosomatic Disorders. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing (2018). p. 89–106. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-89360-0_5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Ogłodek EA, Szota AM, Just MJ, Araszkiewicz A, and Szromek AR. Sense of alexithymia in patients with anxiety disorders comorbid with recurrent urticaria. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2016) 12:995–1004. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S94600

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Khosravani V, Samimi Ardestani M, Sharifi Bastan F, and Kamali Z. The relationship between alexithymia and symptom dimensions in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Obsessive-Compulsive Related Disord. (2017) 14:127–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2017.04.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Pozza A, Giaquinta N, and Dèttore D. The contribution of alexithymia to obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms dimensions: an investigation in a large community sample in Italy. Psychiatry J. (2015) 2015:1–6. doi: 10.1155/2015/707850

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Saure E, Raevuori A, Laasonen M, and Lepistö-Paisley T. Emotion recognition, alexithymia, empathy, and emotion regulation in women with anorexia nervosa. Eating Weight Disord - Stud Anorexia Bulimia Obesity. (2022) 27:3587–97. doi: 10.1007/s40519-022-01496-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Chaim CH, Almeida TM, De Vries Albertin P, Santana GL, Siu ER, and Andrade LH. The implication of alexithymia in personality disorders: A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry. (2024) 24:647. doi: 10.1186/s12888-024-06083-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Honkalampi K, Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Lehto SM, Hintikka J, Haatainen K, Rissanen T, et al. Is alexithymia a risk factor for major depression, personality disorder, or alcohol use disorders? A prospective population-based study. J Psychosomatic Res. (2010) 68:269–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.05.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Martino G, Caputo A, Schwarz P, Bellone F, Fries W, Quattropani MC, et al. Alexithymia and inflammatory bowel disease: A systematic review. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:1763. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01763

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Silvestro O, Ricciardi L, Catalano A, Vicario CM, Tomaiuolo F, Pioggia G, et al. Alexithymia and asthma: A systematic review. Front Psychol. (2023) 14:1221648. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1221648

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Yadav S, Narang T, and Kumaran MS. Psychodermatology: A comprehensive review. Indian J Dermatology Venereology Leprology. (2013) 79:176. doi: 10.4103/0378-6323.107632

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Martino G, Viola A, Vicario CM, Bellone F, Silvestro O, Squadrito G, et al. Psychological impairment in inflammatory bowel diseases: The key role of coping and defense mechanisms. Res Psychother (Milano). (2023) 26(3):731. doi: 10.4081/ripppo.2023.731

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Di Giuseppe M and Conversano C. Psychological components of chronic diseases: The link between defense mechanisms and alexithymia. Mediterranean J Clin Psychol. (2022) 10. doi: 10.13129/2282-1619/mjcp-3602

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Helmes E, McNeill PD, Holden RR, and Jackson C. The construct of alexithymia: Associations with defense mechanisms. J Clin Psychol. (2008) 64(3):318–31. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20461

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Parker JDA, Taylor GJ, and Bagby RM. Alexithymia: Relationship with ego defense and coping styles. Compr Psychiatry. (1998) 39:91–8. doi: 10.1016/S0010-440X(98)90084-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Elzinga BM, Bermond B, and Van Dyck R. The relationship between dissociative proneness and alexithymia. Psychother Psychosomatics. (2002) 71:104–11. doi: 10.1159/000049353

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Modestin J, Lötscher K, and Erni T. Dissociative experiences and their correlates in young non-patients. Psychol Psychotherapy: Theory Res Pract. (2002) 75:53–64. doi: 10.1348/147608302169544

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Gatta M, Dal Santo F, Rago A, Spoto A, and Battistella PA. Alexithymia, impulsiveness, and psychopathology in nonsuicidal self-injured adolescents. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2016) 12:2307–17. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S106433

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Norman H, Oskis A, Marzano L, and Coulson M. The relationship between self-harm and alexithymia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Scandinavian J Psychol. (2020) 61:855–76. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12668

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Roberts S, O’Connor K, Aardema F, and Bélanger C. The impact of emotions on body-Focused repetitive behaviors: Evidence from a non-treatment-seeking sample. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2015) 46:189–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.10.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Schienle A, Schlintl C, and Wabnegger A. A neurobiological evaluation of soft touch training for patients with skin-picking disorder. NeuroImage: Clin. (2022) 36:103254. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2022.103254

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Grynberg D, Chang B, Corneille O, Maurage P, Vermeulen N, Berthoz S, et al. Alexithymia and the processing of emotional facial expressions (EFEs): systematic review, unanswered questions and further perspectives. PloS One. (2012) 7:e42429. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042429

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Jongen S, Axmacher N, Kremers NAW, Hoffmann H, Limbrecht-Ecklundt K, Traue HC, et al. An investigation of facial emotion recognition impairments in alexithymia and its neural correlates. Behav Brain Res. (2014) 271:129–39. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.05.069

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Van Der Velde J, Gromann PM, Swart M, Wiersma D, De Haan L, Bruggeman R, et al. Alexithymia influences brain activation during emotion perception but not regulation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2015) 10:285–93. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu056

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Aydin EP, Demirci H, Begenen AG, Kenar JG, Altunay IK, Ozer OA, et al. Facial emotion recognition difficulties may be specific to skin picking disorder, but could also be related to the presence of alexithymia in trichotillomania. Eur J Psychiatry. (2022) 36(2):130–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpsy.2021.11.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Chamberlain SR, Lochner C, Stein DJ, Goudriaan AE, Van Holst RJ, Zohar J, et al. Behavioural addiction—A rising tide? Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2016) 26:841–55. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.08.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Wang X, Li X, Guo C, Hu Y, Xia L, Geng F, et al. Prevalence and correlates of alexithymia and its relationship with life events in Chinese adolescents with depression during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:774952. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.774952

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Kłosowska J, Cieśla A, Szymańska D, Jankowska A, and Prochwicz K. Interoception, emotion regulation strategies and skin-picking behaviors: Results of an intensive longitudinal study. J Psychiatr Res. (2024) 180:47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2024.07.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Ciuluvica C, Amerio P, Battista B, Di Nardo M, and Fulcheri M. Emotional regulation strategies related to coping strategies and alexithymic traits in dermatologic patients. Psychother Psychosomatics. (2013) 82(Suppl 1):1–134. doi: 10.1159/000354142

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Haime Z, Watson AJ, Crellin N, Marston L, Joyce E, and Moncrieff J. A systematic review of the effects of psychiatric medications on social cognition. BMC Psychiatry. (2021) 21:597. doi: 10.1186/s12888-021-03545-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Güleç H, Köse S, Güleç YM, Evren C, Borckardt J, and Sayar K. Reliability and factorial validity of the Turkish version of the 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale (TAS-20). Klinik Psikofarmakoloji Bülteni-Bulletin Clin Psychopharmacol. (2009) 19:214–20.

Google Scholar

42. Ekman P and Friesen WV. Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: onsulting Psychologists Press. (1976).

Google Scholar

43. Lawrence K, Campbell R, and Skuse D. Ekman-friesen pictures of facial affect test—Computerized version. (2016). doi: 10.1037/t49272-000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Beck AT. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1961) 4:561. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Beck AT, Steer RA, and Carbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev. (1988) 8:77–100. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hisli N. Beck depresyon envanterinin geçerliği üzerine bir çalışma. Psikoloji Dergisi. (1988) 22:118–26.

Google Scholar

47. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, and Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. J Consulting Clin Psychol. (1988) 56:893–7. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ulusoy M, Sahin N, and Erkmen H. Turkish version of the Beck Anxiety Inventory: Psychometric properties. J Cogn Psychother. (1997) 12:163–72.

Google Scholar

49. Wabnegger A, Übel S, Suchar G, and Schienle A. Increased emotional reactivity to affective pictures in patients with skin-picking disorder: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Behav Brain Res. (2018) 336:151–5. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.08.040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Demenescu LR, Kortekaas R, Den Boer JA, and Aleman A. Impaired attribution of emotion to facial expressions in anxiety and major depression. PloS One. (2010) 5:e15058. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015058

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Di Tella M, Adenzato M, Catmur C, Miti F, Castelli L, and Ardito RB. The role of alexithymia in social cognition: Evidence from a non-clinical population. J Affect Disord. (2020) 273:482–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.05.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Mullen M, Hanna D, Bradley M, Rogers D, Jordan J-A, and Dyer KFW. Attentional bias in individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder: A preliminary eye-tracking study. J Behav Cogn Ther. (2021) 31:199–204. doi: 10.1016/j.jbct.2020.10.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Jansen M, Overgaauw S, and De Bruijn ERA. Social cognition and obsessive-compulsive disorder: A review of subdomains of social functioning. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:118. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Kim H, Somerville LH, Johnstone T, Alexander AL, and Whalen PJ. Inverse amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to surprised faces. NeuroReport. (2003) 14:2317–22. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200312190-00006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Feyzioğlu A, Taşlıoğlu Sayıner AC, Özçelik D, Tarımtay Altun F, and Budak EN. The mediating role of early maladaptive schemas in the relationship between early childhood trauma and alexithymia. Curr Psychol. (2023) 42:17851–61. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-02988-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Passardi S, Peyk P, Rufer M, Wingenbach TSH, and Pfaltz MC. Facial mimicry, facial emotion recognition and alexithymia in post-traumatic stress disorder. Behav Res Ther. (2019) 122:103436. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2019.103436

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Diefenbach GJ, Mouton-Odum S, and Stanley MA. Affective correlates of trichotillomania. Behav Res Ther. (2002) 40:1305–15. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00006-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Prochwicz K, Antosz-Rekucka R, Kałużna-Wielobób A, Sznajder D, and Kłosowska J. Negative affectivity moderates the relationship between attentional control and focused skin picking. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:6636. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19116636

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Starita F, Borhani K, Bertini C, and Scarpazza C. Alexithymia is related to the need for more emotional intensity to identify static fearful facial expressions. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:929. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00929

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Prkachin GC, Casey C, and Prkachin KM. Alexithymia and perception of facial expressions of emotion. Pers Individ Dif. (2009) 46:412–7. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.11.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Parker JDA, Taylor GJ, and Bagby M. Alexithymia and the recognition of facial expressions of emotion. Psychother Psychosomatics. (1993) 59:197–202. doi: 10.1159/000288664

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Greene D, Boyes M, and Hasking P. The associations between alexithymia and both non-suicidal self-injury and risky drinking: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2020) 260:140–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.08.088

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Muzi S, Di Trani M, Renzi A, and Pace CS. Can alexithymia be assessed through an interview in adolescents? The Toronto Structured Interview for Alexithymia: Reliability, concurrent validity, discriminant validity, and relationships with emotional-behavioral symptoms. Front Psychiatry. (2023) 13:1055946. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1055946

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Field AP. Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics (5th ed.). London, UK: Sage Publications (2018).

Google Scholar

65. Löf J, Clinton D, Kaldo V, and Rydén G. Symptom, alexithymia and self-image outcomes of Mentalisation-based treatment for borderline personality disorder: a naturalistic study. BMC Psychiatry. (2018) 18:185. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1699-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Schoeneman Patel SE, Haut KM, Guty E, Dodell-Feder D, Saxena A, Nahum M, et al. Social cognition training improves recognition of distinct facial emotions and decreases misattribution errors in healthy individuals. Front Psychiatry. (2022) 13:1026418. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1026418

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Thonse U, Behere RV, Frommann N, Praharaj SK, and Sharma PSVN. Efficacy of a culturally adapted emotion recognition training program in improving facial emotion recognition in persons with schizophrenia. Indian J psychol Med. (2024), 02537176241281451. doi: 10.1177/02537176241281451

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: skin picking disorder, alexithymia, emotion recognition, body-focused repetitive behavior disorder, depression, anxiety

Citation: Ergelen M, Taşlıoğlu Sayıner AC, Çırakoğlu M and Yalçın M (2025) Emotion recognition deficits in patients with skin picking disorder: the role of alexithymia while controlling depression, and anxiety levels. Front. Psychiatry 16:1597268. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1597268

Received: 20 March 2025; Accepted: 23 April 2025;
Published: 16 May 2025.

Edited by:

Salvatore Campanella, Université libre de Bruxelles, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Roberta Vecchiotti, Hermanas Hospitalarias, Italy
Orlando Silvestro, University Magna Graecia of Catanzaro, Italy

Copyright © 2025 Ergelen, Taşlıoğlu Sayıner, Çırakoğlu and Yalçın. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Murat Yalçın, bXVyYXR5YWxjaW5AZ21haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.