Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 26 August 2025

Sec. Psychopathology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1600165

This article is part of the Research TopicFrom Learned Helplessness to Learned Controllability: Cognitive and Neurobiological Aspects Involved in the Development of Trauma or ResilienceView all 5 articles

Learned helplessness and learned controllability: from neurobiology to cognitive, emotional and behavioral neurosciences

  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, United States
  • 2Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain

The sustained and prolonged exposure to environmental stressors may provoke different reactions, depending on the subjective perception of control. Repeated perception of lack of control may lead to learned helplessness, which has been linked to the development of depression. It has been shown that learned helplessness is associated with increased activation of the serotonergic dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) and the amygdala, as well as the decreased activation in certain regions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC), such as the dorsolateral (DL-PFC) and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VM-PFC). Perception of control has been associated with increased activation of the DL-PFC and the VM-PFC, decreased activation of the DRN and the amygdala, as well as increased activation of the serotonergic medial raphe nucleus (MRN). It is also associated with decreased activation of the ventral hippocampus and increased activation of the ventral striatum, including the nucleus accumbens (NAc). Functionally, perception of control promotes the implementation of active coping strategies, characterized by efficient cognitive-emotional processing, effective decision making, and goal directed actions, that in turn may lead to lower anxiety, greater tolerance of adverse events, emotional stability, and increased resilience. Just as it has been demonstrated that helplessness may result from a learning process, it could be hypothesized that controllability may also be trained and learned. If so, learned controllability could be taught to enhance resilience and provide invaluable resources ultimately reducing depressive symptoms and improving overall well-being. By fostering a sense of controllability, where individuals learn to associate their actions with desired outcomes, it may be possible to counteract the consequences of “learned helplessness” aspects of depression. This could provide direction in development of novel interventions aimed at promoting skill acquisition, problem-solving strategies, and adaptive decision-making, thereby restoring a sense of agency and resilience in the face of adversity.

Introduction

Stress represents a normal aspect of life, where perceived environmental stimuli, known as stressors, provoke an array of adaptive responses aimed at protecting or restoring psycho-physiological equilibrium of the organism. The role of stress in the origin and the development of clinical conditions, such as depression and anxiety disorders, has been extensively investigated (14), focusing on both psychological and neurobiological processes involved. In this context, the long-lasting effects of early life trauma, and detrimental effects of sustained and prolonged environmental stressors, have been universally acknowledged (47). Thus, environmental stressors can trigger different responses, which depend on the concerted activation of diverse neural structures and circuits that support both adaptive and maladaptive physiological and psychological processes.

It has been shown that sustained and prolonged exposure to uncontrollable stressors in rodents can lead to the development of “helplessness”. Since these changes in animal behavior were learned, through exposure to repeated uncontrollable stressful stimuli, it was termed “learned helplessness”, a cognitive/emotional response suggesting a loss of ability to exert behavioral control over environmental stressors, even when this control may be possible. (8). It has been suggested that this may lead to decreased motivation to exert any effort to cope with perceived stressors, which has been hypothesized, in turn, to lead to the development of depression in humans (9). In contrast, animal studies have shown that exerting control in response to stressful situations can lead to the development of adaptive and effective behavioral responses (10, 11). Moreover, human studies suggest that perception of control contributes to the development of effective cognitive and emotional processing, leading to the development of more effective coping strategies, and goal-oriented behavioral responses (1214). In this regard, subjective perception of control may depend on different factors, some of them associated with characteristics of environmental stressors, others related to the available resources to cope with them, and some on individual characteristics, like temperament. In this regard, previous experiences associated with successful responses, based on effective coping strategies and positive outcomes, may provide elements to shape a subjective perception of control, which may lead to the development of “learned controllability”. If so, learned controllability could be potentially taught to enhance resilience and provide invaluable resources as a potential treatment for learned helplessness, counteracting its cognitive and emotional consequences, ultimately reducing depressive symptoms and improving overall well-being. By fostering a sense of controllability, where individuals learn to associate their actions with desired outcomes, it may be possible to counteract the consequences of “learned helplessness” aspects of depression. This approach could provide novel directions in developing interventions aimed at promoting skill acquisition, problem-solving strategies, and more adaptive decision-making, thereby restoring the sense of agency and resilience in the face of adversity. Moreover, reinforcing controllability may provide individuals with the psychological resilience needed to cope with future stressors, thereby preventing relapses and promoting long-term recovery. In summary, understanding the neurobiological mechanisms and psychological processes implicated in the origin and the development of learned helplessness, and of learned controllability, may provide important information for the development of innovative approaches for the treatment and prevention of depression and anxiety disorders.

Physiological responses to stress

Early response to acute environmental stressors involves activation of corticotrophin-releasing factor (CRF, also known as corticotrophin-releasing hormone or CRH) neurons in the amygdala, which participate in fear and anger reactions, as well as activation of noradrenergic neurons in the locus coeruleus (LC), which participate in arousal regulation and enhancement of cognitive functions, that further assists in the implementation of adaptive responses (15). This leads to the activation of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) and the subsequent stimulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) system, both regulated by dedicated neural structures within the central nervous system (CNS) (16, 17). Thus, stimulatory projections from the amygdala, mainly from its central nucleus (CNA), activate the LC, which is a key noradrenergic nucleus reciprocally connected with the amygdala (18), the nucleus of the solitary tract (NTS), the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) of the hypothalamus, and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), which in turn also sends stimulatory projections to the PVN.

The hypothalamic PVN further participates in the activation of the ANS also through stimulatory projections to the NTS, as well as playing a critical role in the activation of the HPA. In sum, the early response to environmental threats involves stimulatory projections from the amygdala, the LC, the NTS, and the PVN activating the sympathetic branch of the ANS, followed by compensatory activation of the para-sympathetic system, and further activation of HPA axis.

Activation of the HPA system starts, as described previously, with stimulatory projections from limbic structures, mainly the amygdala, and indirect projections through the BNST, leading to synthesis and release of CRH, which stimulates the synthesis and release of adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) in the anterior pituitary, which is released to systemic circulation to reach the adrenal cortex, where in turn glucocorticoids, such as cortisol, are produced and released (19, 20). The HPA axis is normally regulated by multiple negative feedback loops, which exert an inhibitory effect on the synthesis and release of CRH and ACTH in the hypothalamic PVN and the pituitary respectively. In addition, cortisol also activates glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampus, which sends direct inhibitory projections to the PVN, and indirectly via the BNST (21). Repeated exposure to environmental stressors, as it occurs during chronic stress, has been associated with increased activation of the HPA system, which may lead to alteration of physiological negative feedback mechanisms. This may be translated into chronic activation of the HPA system, leading to increased levels of cortisol (4, 22). In this regard, evidence supports the association between chronic stress and depression at the molecular level, where hyperactivity of the HPA axis, with the consequent increase in cortisol levels, represents one of the most consistent findings in both conditions (4, 19, 23, 24).

Psychological responses to stress

Adaptive responses to stressors involve diverse coping strategies, which require an adequate assessment of perceived stimuli, as well as their contextual features, and the consequent appraisal of available resources to cope with them. This also requires certain perception of control and ability to predict outcomes, which in turn is based on diverse learning processes (25). Subjective perception of ability to predict implies the possibility to anticipate the potential impact of environmental stressors, the assessment of available resources to cope with them, the ability to detect their potential signals in advance, and the capacity to learn from these experiences. At the basic level, this learning process may involve Pavlovian or “classical conditioning”, where perceived signals, previously associated with the occurrence of a reinforcer, may lead to a predictive association between both, with the consequent elicitation of anticipatory responses (25). Subjective perception of “control”, or the lack of it, on the other hand implies the ability to learn about potential associations between certain actions and their consequent results. This learning is based on “instrumental conditioning”, where an association is established between an action and an outcome, which becomes a “reinforcer”. Together, “classical conditioning” allows anticipation, or prediction, of the occurrence of potentially relevant events, whereas “instrumental conditioning” allows to exert certain control over these events, providing the bases for the subjective experience of controllability. The learned association between actions and outcomes plays a critical role in the development of behavioral responses aimed at the satisfaction of personal needs or the accomplishment of desired goals. Hence, instrumental learning may depend on two different mechanisms, which involve different neural pathways: a stimulus-response habit, which implies instrumental learning about an action, which constitutes a behavioral response followed by an expected reward, and a goal-directed process, which implies the acquisition of incentive value by the obtained reward.

Successful adaptive behaviors depend on the ability to detect causal associations between actions and outcomes. This allows the organism to distinguish between environmental events that may be directly associated with these behaviors, and those that may happen independently, and may not be associated with any particular action (26). Successful behavioral strategies may lead to the implementation of actions that have been associated with increased probabilities to generate reinforcing outcomes, and avoidance of those that have been associated with decreased probabilities of these outcomes (27). A goal-directed behavior may depend, not only on the value assigned to associations between actions and outcomes, but also on the probability that outcomes will occur in the presence or absence of these actions (28). This causal association constitutes a “contingency”, which represents the difference between the contingent probability to generate an outcome in response to an action, and the non-contingent probability that the expected outcome may occur independently of that action (26, 29). This suggests, that in response to stressful situations, efficient cognitive and emotional processing is important for the development of a subjective perception of control, which in turn may lead to more efficient behavioral choices. Hence, a subjective sense of predictability and controllability may also contribute to downregulation of excessive physiological responses, including those mediated by the ANS and the HPA system, therefore preventing the development of clinical conditions associated with the chronic impact of environmental stressors, and the consequent maladaptive responses.

Learned helplessness

As stated earlier, after prolonged exposure to uncontrollable stressful situations, followed by repeated unsuccessful attempts to cope with them, an animal may learn that any effort to avoid or escape similar situations will be in vain. As a result, it may develop “helplessness”, which represents a failure to attempt escape or prevention. This perception of lack of control, despite the fact that control might still be possible, coupled with recurrent experiences of being unable to escape or avoid aversive situations, may lead to “learned helplessness” (30). This phenomenon was originally described in experimental settings, where animals exposed to a series of unavoidable electric shocks later developed an inability to escape or avoid new shocks in a different environment, where escape was possible through behavioral responses. Therefore, this learned condition, not observed in naive animals, but induced by the impact of unsuccessful attempts to escape uncontrollable stressful stimuli, came to be known as “learned helplessness” (8). Hence, the perception that efforts to control a stressful situation are ineffective, learned from previous unsuccessful experiences, may bias against objective assessment, suggesting that efforts would be worthwhile. In humans, it implies that individuals who have learned that outcomes are independent of their actions might feel that their efforts are ineffective and irrelevant. It has been postulated that the critical factor here was the lack of control the organism experienced over stressful events, which in turn may lead to learned helplessness (31). Hence, learned helplessness may arise from exposure to uncontrollable stressors, depending on the extent to which these stressors are perceived as uncontrollable (32).

The concept of learned helplessness, initially described in rodents, was later tested also in human studies (3335). The initial studies reported that exposure to inescapable threat can elicit some of the analogous responses to those seen in animals (3436) and it has been hypothesized that this mechanism might be contributing to the development of psychopathological conditions (9, 37). However, it was also recognized that adapting the concept of learned helplessness from animals to humans required taking into account unique aspects related to human cognition and attributional styles (37, 38). In humans, helplessness involves causal attributions, which can be perceived as either internal or external, relating to personal or environmental factors. These attributions can be seen as stable or unstable, meaning they might be seen as enduring conditions or factors that could change with experiences. Additionally, attributions can be global or specific, affecting either all aspects of life or only particular areas. (37, 39). In this regard, subsequent research in humans demonstrated that learned helplessness may occur if individuals perceive a lack of contingency between actions and outcomes in a certain context, which not necessarily will apply in a different context (38). In addition, it has been shown that learned helplessness may lead to decreased voluntary response initiation and decreased interest in pleasurable stimuli, or anhedonia, which are both symptoms typically observed in depression.

Learned controllability

It has been suggested that controllability may be developed through a learning process, which involves information processing about stressful situations, behavioral responses, and the associations between them. These associations may play a critical role in conferring controllability and its protective effect when coping with stressful situations (14, 40). Various preclinical studies have demonstrated different behavioral patterns in response to aversive events, depending on the presumed perception of control (8, 10, 4144). In this regard, animal studies have shown that this suggests the ability to successfully respond to environmental challenges, exerting desired actions in the environment through behavioral responses (14).

In humans, subjective perception of control involves both cognitive and emotional processes, where individuals develop a “belief” in their ability to successfully respond to stressful situations. An individual might perceive a reward, or “reinforcer”, as contingent upon their own behavior, or it may be attributed to external factors. If an individual perceives a causal relationship between their actions and a desired outcome, this reflects a belief in internal control. Conversely, if the individual believes that outcomes are unrelated to their own actions and are mainly influenced by external factors beyond their control, this indicates a belief in external control (45). In line with these observations, it has been proposed that expectations of control may lead to the belief in personal ability to succeed by influencing various aspects of coping behaviors. This includes initiating behavioral responses, mobilizing necessary resources, and sustaining efforts over time in the face of perceived aversive situations (46). It has been suggested that, in contrast to what is observed in “learned helplessness”, the perception of control arises when individuals recognize that their behavioral responses can improve the likelihood of achieving desired outcomes, such as obtaining rewards or terminating adversities, compared to the likelihood of achieving similar outcomes in the absence of these behaviors (9).

In terms of learning theory and “instrumental learning”, underlying both “learned helplessness and “learned controllability”, this process involves the assessment of potential goals, such as obtaining rewards or avoiding punishments, identifying the necessary actions to achieve these goals, and understanding the associations between them. As a result, goal-directed actions emerge as behavioral responses, based on the assessment of action-outcomes contingencies, characterized by the expectation of achieving desired outcomes through these actions (47). Subjective perception of control in humans requires the ability to detect potential changes in action-outcome contingencies, certain flexibility and sensitivity to perceive these changes, and the capability to integrate this information to continue implementing adaptive behavioral responses to successfully act and interact in the face of changing environments (48, 49). Furthermore, learned ability to exert behavioral control over environmental stressors creates a perception of stressors as controllable, which in turn reduces associated negative effects and moderates the potential negative consequences of exposure to subsequent uncontrollable stressors (50).

Neurobiological mechanisms

Adaptive responses to stress, including defense behaviors such as fight or flight, depend on the concerted activation of different neural structures, such as the PFC, the amygdala, the BNST, the hypothalamus, and the periaqueductal gray (PAG). These structures are integrated into neural circuits, where they are reciprocally connected, and are also regulated by different neurotransmitter systems, including serotonergic, noradrenergic, and dopaminergic projections. In response to stressful situations, the medial PFC, the amygdala, particularly the CNA, the BNST, and the hypothalamus, project stimulatory input to the PAG, which integrates sensory information and participates in the initiation of “active” responses (51, 52). Concurrently, the amygdala and the BNST promote fear and anxiety, which are typical emotional reactions elicited in response to certain environmental stressors (9, 53), particularly if these are perceived as uncontrollable. It has been shown that prolonged and sustained exposure to uncontrollable stressors may lead to altered functioning of these structures, and these alterations have been linked to the development of learned helplessness. In this regard, research has demonstrated that learned helplessness can result in various behavioral consequences, including impaired fight-or-flight responses and heightened levels of fear and anxiety (9). Research has demonstrated that the suppression of “active” responses, such as fight or flight, may lead to “passive” reactions, also known as learned “passivity”. In this regard, activation of the PAG has been associated with “active” responses (54), particularly the dorsal part of the PAG (dPAG), which has been shown to participate in the activation of motor and autonomic components of active defensive responses (55). It has been shown that the PAG, the BNST and the amygdala receive significant serotonergic projections. Serotonin (5-hydroxitryptamine, 5HT) is mainly produced in the CNS by serotonergic neurons located in the dorsal RN (DRN) and the medial RN (MRN) (5658). The DRN sends projections to the amygdala, particularly to the baso-lateral nucleus (BLA) and the CNA, the BNST, the PAG, some areas of the PFC, and to a lesser extent the ventral hippocampus (5961) Activation of the DRN may lead to increased anticipatory anxiety, which plays an adaptive role in response to stressful situations (9, 32, 62), associated with increased arousal and alertness. Pre-clinical studies have shown that exposure to shock may lead to the immediate activation of the DRN, with the consequent release of 5-HT, in subjects exposed to both escapable or inescapable conditions (9). Activation of the DRN, with the consequent release of 5-HT in the amygdala, the BNST, and the PAG, has been associated with exposure to uncontrollable stressors (63, 64). Hence, in response to environmental stressors, the DRN receives excitatory input from the amygdala and other neural structures (65). It has been shown that the amygdala, particularly the CNA, sends projections to the DRN and to the locus coeruleus (LC) (66). Hence, in response to environmental stressors, the DRN receives excitatory input from the amygdala, primarily mediated by CRF, which stimulates the release of 5-HT in various neural structures (66, 67). Similarly, the LC receives excitatory input from the amygdala, also mediated by CRF, leading to the release of norepinephrine (NE) in different neural targets (66, 68), also associated with increased arousal and alertness. Noteworthy, the DRN receives noradrenergic projections from the LC (69), which exert a tonic excitatory control mediated by α1-adrenoceptors (70). The activity of serotonergic neurons in the DRN is self-regulated by inhibitory somato-dendritic 5-HT1A auto-receptors, which are activated by released 5-HT, thereby reducing further 5-HT release. However, high concentrations of 5-HT, resulting from intense DRN activation during inescapable stress, may down-regulate these auto-receptors, potentially neutralizing their normal inhibitory effect. This, in turn, may lead to increased 5-HT release by the DRN (71). Therefore, it has been proposed that inescapable stressful conditions may lead to the desensitization of 5-HT1A auto-receptors in the DRN, which could result in the sensitization of these serotonergic neurons, subsequently increasing 5-HT release (9). Hence, in response to uncontrollable stressors, serotonergic projections from the DRN to the PAG may result in the inhibition of active responses, while serotonergic projections from the DRN to the amygdala and BNST may provoke increased fear and anxiety.

Regarding neurobiological mechanisms involved in controllability, it has been shown that certain areas of the PFC, the hippocampus and the striatum participate in the assessment of perceived stressors and the detection of control. If stressors are perceived as controllable, activation of these neural structures may be translated into goal-directed behaviors, which differ from habitual behaviors (72). Specific areas in the ventro-medial PFC (VM-PFC), such as the pre-limbic PFC (PL-PFC), which has been proposed to be analogous to BA 32 in the human brain, participates in the acquisition of goal-directed associations (73), whereas the infra-limbic PFC (IL-PFC), which has been proposed to be analogous to BA 25 in the human brain, participates in habitual reward seeking (49). The PL-PFC sends projections to the dorso-medial striatum (DM-ST), which participates in the acquisition and expression of goal-directed actions (74), while the dorso-lateral striatum (DL-ST) participates in habitual reward seeking (75, 76). Hence, the PL-PFC and the DM-ST, which are involved in goal-directed associations, participate in control detection (9). The ventral striatum (V-ST), including the nucleus accumbens (NAc), plays a critical role in reward expectation, as well as goal-directed and motivated behaviors, also facilitating reward seeking (77, 78). It has been shown that activation of the VM-PFC may lead to inhibition of stress reactions, particularly mediated by activation of the amygdala and the DRN (79). Further research performed with humans showed that the VM-PFC expressed a differential response in controllable situations (39). Therefore, activation of the VM-PFC was associated with perceived control in humans and down regulation of stress responses, mediated by the activation of the amygdala and the DRN (39). The DRN receives projections from the VM-PFC, particularly from the IL and PL areas (80). Activation of these regulatory projections from the VM-PFC may lead to decreased activation of the DRN (81), as well as decreased activation of the amygdala (79), which in turn has been associated with decreased learned helplessness. In contrast, inhibition of the VM-PFC may increase symptoms of learned helplessness (81, 82). Hence, activation of the VM-PFC has been associated with perception of control and resilient behavior (79). It has been shown that the inhibitory effect exerted by the PL area of the VM-PFC on the DRN is mediated by glutamatergic projections, which exert stimulatory effect on inhibitory GABAergic interneurons (80). Hence, activation of these interneurons may be translated into an inhibitory effect on serotonergic neurons in the DRN. Likewise, glutamatergic projections from the VM-PFC may stimulate GABAergic interneurons in the amygdala, with the consequent inhibitory effect. Therefore, excessive activation of the amygdala may be counteracted by activation of the VM-PFC, hence decreasing feelings of fear and anxiety and improving subjective feelings of increased control. The amygdala also shares reciprocal connections with the DRN (83). It sends direct stimulatory projections from the CNA to the DRN mediated by CRF, or indirectly through projections from the BNST. The effects of CRF may vary depending on the specific receptor it binds to, whether it is released into serotonergic or GABAergic neurons, and the available ligand concentrations. Lower concentrations of CRF, associated with acute stressful situations, may bind mainly to CRF-R1, which has an inhibitory effect on 5-HT release, while higher concentrations, associated with chronic stressful conditions, may bind mainly to CRF-R2, which have a stimulatory effect on 5-HT release from the DRN (67). Hence, serotonergic projections from the DRN to the amygdala, particularly to the BLA, may lead to chronic anxiogenic states, which may be counteracted by the VM-PFC, which exerts inhibitory effects on the amygdala and the DRN.

The study of controllability in humans includes additional aspects, such as the ability to anticipate the impact of negative events, especially threatening situations, which are necessary for survival in an adverse environment. In this regard, the concept of predictability, which means the ability to predict the attainment of beneficial goals, as well as the capacity to avoid aversive outcomes, plays a critical role in the development of a sense of controllability. In human studies, the ability to anticipate future events may be highly adaptive. However, excessive anticipation of negative events may provoke feelings of anticipatory anxiety, which can be maladaptive (8486). It has been proposed that anticipatory anxiety is linked to a subjective perception of anticipated lack of control over future events (39). In this regard, research on human subjects has also shown that the VM-PFC plays a crucial role in the subjective perception of control, particularly in anticipating aversive events (39). Activation of the VM-PFC has been also associated with decreased activity in the amygdala (87, 88), supporting the role of this topdown regulatory mechanism in humans (79). These findings reinforce the importance of these neural structures, as previously demonstrated in studies with non-human animals. Hence, in response to stressful situations, activation of the VM-PFC, with the subsequent inhibition of the amygdala and the DRN, is necessary to sustain the effect of subjective perception of control on emotional responses, which in turn may affect cognitive processing (39). Other areas of the PFC have been shown to participate in the regulation and activation of the VM-PFC. In this regard, the DL-PFC may induce activation of the VM-PFC (89, 90), which in turn may be translated into inhibition of the DRN and the amygdala. The DL-PFC plays a critical role in cognitive functions, such as working memory (91, 92, and decision making (93), while the VM-PFC has been associated with emotional functions, such as emotional regulation (94, 95), and it has been shown to participate in value-based decision making (96). These interactions between both areas in the PFC highlight the importance of the interplay between cognitive processing and emotional processing, particularly in stressful situations that require rapid and effective implementation of adaptive responses. The DL PFC is also reciprocally connected with the dorsomedial PFC (DM-PFC) and the ventrolateral PFC (VL-PFC), forming a circuit involved in the regulation of the amygdala, which in turn may be further translated into emotion regulation (97).

Therapeutic strategies and future developments

Learned helplessness has been associated with alterations of the serotonergic system, particularly increased activation of the DRN, associated with passive behavioral responses, and decreased activation of the MRN, associated with decreased tolerance over adverse events. Conversely, the perception of control over adverse events has been associated with increased activation of the VMPFC, the DLPFC, and the MRN. It has been shown that the VMPFC contributes to emotional regulation and the inhibition of subcortical structures, such as the amygdala and the DRN, while the DLPFC supports goal-directed behavior and instrumental learning. Both areas are also innervated by serotonergic projections. Hence, under conditions of adequate serotonergic tone, their function is enhanced, allowing for improved evaluation of action and the restoration of perceived control. In contrast, dysregulation of these circuits may underlie the generalized passivity observed in states of learned helplessness.

Numerous studies have demonstrated that antidepressants, particularly selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), can effectively reverse passive behavioral responses induced by uncontrollable stress in animals, hence recovering the ability to elicit active responses. The therapeutic effects of SSRIs involve the modulation of multiple mechanisms associated with certain neural structures and specific receptors, mainly mediated by different 5-HT receptor subtypes. In the amygdala, SSRIs downregulate excitatory 5-HT2A receptors and enhance the function of inhibitory post-synaptic 5-HT1A receptors, leading to reduced emotional reactivity and attenuation of fear responses (98, Hensler et al, 2006, 99). This receptor-level modulation contributes to diminished limbic hyper-reactivity and facilitates prefrontal top-down regulation. In the hippocampus, SSRIs promote BDNF-dependent neuroplasticity and neurogenesis primarily through activation of post-synaptic 5-HT1A receptors, supporting contextual processing of stress and emotional memory integration (82, 100102). Within the VMPFC, increased serotonergic neurotransmission, mediated mainly by post-synaptic 5-HT1A receptors, contributes to the suppression of subcortical stress responses by enhancing prefrontal inhibition over the amygdala and the DRN, thereby reducing passive, generalized stress responses and enabling context-sensitive appraisal of controllability (9, 103105). This mechanism is critical to the transition from more reflexive, emotion-driven responses, to more cognitive-driven responses, including goal-directed coping behavior. In the DLPFC, SSRIs modulate executive function and goal-directed behavior through binding to post-synaptic 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptors, improving executive control, cognitive flexibility and instrumental learning, which are necessary to detect and consolidate action-outcome contingencies (98, 104, 106). Together, these receptor-specific effects promote a neurobiological setting in which adaptive coping strategies and the perception of control may be recovered and strengthened, which in turn may lead to recover the capacity to distinguish between controllable and uncontrollable stressors, thereby reversing learned helplessness and promoting learned controllability. (9, 82, 98, 103).

Although the effects of SSRIs have been widely studied in relation to mood and anxiety, further research is needed to examine their potential role in restoring the perception of control. Investigating their effects within paradigms that explicitly assess controllability could offer new insights into the biological mechanisms underlying recovery from learned helplessness and the development of learned controllability.

In addition to psychopharmacological approaches, future research should further investigate the potential of neuromodulatory interventions, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), to enhance perceived control. Considering the central role of prefrontal regions in instrumental learning, goal-directed behavior, and emotion regulation, TMS may serve as a valuable tool to strengthen the neural circuits underlying the subjective experience of control. This effect may be particularly effective when TMS is combined with behavioral tasks specifically designed to reinforce the sense of agency and facilitate the learning of action–outcome contingencies. Such integrated neurocognitive approaches could open new avenues for mechanism-based treatments targeting disorders characterized by learned helplessness, maladaptive coping, and impaired stress resilience.

Regarding psychotherapeutic strategies, cognitive therapy contributes to restoring the perception of control by restructuring maladaptive beliefs related to self-efficacy and contingency. By modifying cognitive appraisals of stressors, such interventions engage prefrontal regions, such as the DL-PFC, involved in top-down regulation, thereby attenuating limbic overactivation. Moreover, therapeutic paradigms based on experiential re-exposure to controllable stressors can reactivate instrumental learning circuits and strengthen the encoding of action-outcome contingencies. These experiences facilitate the development of adaptive coping strategies and may serve to reverse helplessness by enhancing functional connectivity between the DL-PFC, the VM-PFC, the striatum, and limbic areas, such as the amygdala. Thus, therapies that restore the subjective sense of control may have significant neurobiological and clinical impact in the treatment of stress-related disorders.

More recently, we have initiated the development of therapeutic strategies based on virtual reality (VR). Given the central role of perceived control in modulating stress responses and promoting adaptive coping mechanisms, VR represents a promising avenue for intervention. VR environments enable the creation of immersive and interactive scenarios in which individuals can actively engage in goal-directed behaviors and experience clear cause–effect relationships between their actions and outcomes, thereby facilitating the development and consolidation of a subjective sense of controllability. Moreover, the adaptability of VR-based interventions to individual needs supports the implementation of personalized therapeutic strategies that not only aim to reverse learned helplessness, but also foster long-term psychological resources to enhance perceived control, resilience, and emotional well-being.

Conclusions

It has been shown that prolonged and sustained exposure to uncontrollable stressors may lead to learned helplessness, i.e. inability to escape or avoid aversive situations even when escape/avoidance are possible, also interpreted as a perception of lack of control. In “learned helplessness” an organism learns that outcomes are independent of their actions, and it was suggested that in humans this process involves, in addition to a subjective perception of helplessness, a learned attitude of decreased motivation to exert any effort or coping with perceived stressors.

Conversely the sense of control offers the ability to effectively respond to environmental stressors by taking purposeful actions through goal-oriented behaviors. This ability, which may be stimulated, trained or learned, constitutes a process referred to as “learned controllability”. Learned controllability thus might play a role in the development of both psychological and neurobiological aspects of resilience. (11, 79). If so, the ability to perceive control may have a regulatory effect on emotional reactions triggered by adverse situations, whereby a subjective perception of control plays a crucial role in developing more effective coping strategies and achieving more successful behavioral outcomes. This may lead to improved decision-making and goal-directed actions, ultimately reinforcing the subjective perception of controllability.

Neurobiologically learned helplessness had been linked to increased activity in the DRN and decreased activity in the MRN, as well as hyperactivation of the amygdala. These changes are accompanied in the long run by alterations in the HPA axis and reduced activation of the VMPFC (9, 107, 108). Heightened perception of control, on the other hand, was linked to increased activation of the MRN, with the consequent regulatory effect on the ventral hippocampus and, subsequently, the NAc. In humans it has been associated with increased activation of the DL-PFC and the VM-PFC, which may play a role in decreasing activation in the amygdala and the DRN. These changes have been associated with improved tolerance to adversity, decreased anxiety, and the development of resilience.

Controllability, similarly to learned helplessness, is associated with instrumental learning, where an individual learns to associate a voluntary action with a desired result (11), where associations between behavioral responses and effective outcomes, are learned. Behavioral responses are directed toward achieving desired outcomes, aimed at effectively coping with perceived stressors, such as avoiding aversive situations or approaching favorable stimuli. Establishing a causal link between voluntary actions and desired outcomes enhances subjective perception of control, which may be perceived as rewarding, hence providing positive feedback to the learning process. In this regard, it has been observed that striatal activity can be influenced by the perceived relationship between a voluntary action and its outcome (109). Moreover, the emotional experience of perceiving control, manifested through decision-making behavior, has been associated with activity in cortico-striatal systems involved in reward processing (110). Hence, the perceived efficacy of voluntary actions in achieving desired goals provides essential feedback for developing a subjective sense of controllability. On a cognitive level, the goal-directed system assesses potential changes in outcomes, enabling the modification of behavioral strategies as needed (111). It has been demonstrated that the goal-directed system also participates in the detection of control over adverse situations, which plays a critical role in the development of more effective coping strategies and therapeutic resources. In this regard, it has been shown that previous experiences of control over adverse situations may reduce the susceptibility to helplessness, hence providing “immunization” against certain stressful conditions (9, 112), which then may be recognized as “controllable”. This is important because the therapeutic focus stemming from learned helplessness has been on reversing the harmful impacts caused by a sense of uncontrollability (11). It has been shown that animals initially exposed to escapable shocks may learn to react with an active response and not become passive, even when faced with inescapable shocks later in life (113). Since “controllability” in humans may be understood as feeling that emerges from instrumental learning and higher-level cognitive processing, it is possible to conceive therapeutic strategies based on this learning process. Hence, individuals exposed to chronic stressful situations may be able to develop a subjective sense of control, which may be translated into more adaptive responses rather than the development of learned helplessness. In addition, it could be possible to conceive the development of preventive strategies, aimed at teaching and training vulnerable individuals, mostly during early periods of life, to develop a subjective feeling of control.

Further delineating the psychological and neurobiological mechanisms underlying cognitive and emotional processes involved in learned helplessness and learned controllability might be an important step for advancing modern psychiatry. Therefore, ongoing research and further advances in understanding these mechanisms may lead to the development of more effective biological and therapeutic approaches for the prevention and treatment of depression, as well as more efficient strategies for fostering resilience.

Literature search approach

The literature reviewed in this manuscript was selected to support the central hypothesis that learned helplessness and learned controllability represent opposing processes with implications for therapeutic intervention. Rather than following a systematic review protocol, we adopted a targeted narrative approach, identifying relevant publications across a broad temporal range through searches in PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Science. Search terms included combinations of learned helplessness, learned controllability, perceived control, serotonin, stress, prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and antidepressants. Additional sources were identified via manual screening of reference lists from key conceptual and empirical articles. Priority was given to studies that informed the neurobiological and therapeutic dimensions of the argument.

Author contributions

GT: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft. TO: Writing – review & editing, Supervision.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Gold PW. The organization of the stress system and its dysregulation in depressive illness. Mol Psychiatry. (2015) 20:32–47. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.163

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Nemeroff CB. The preeminent role of early untoward experience on vulnerability to major psychiatric disorders: the nature-nurture controversy revisited and soon to be resolved. Mol Psychiatry. (1999) 4:106–8. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000512

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Heim C and Nemeroff CB. The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorders: preclinical and clinical studies. Biol Psychiatry. (2001) 49:1023–39. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01157-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Tafet GE and Nemeroff CB. The links between stress and depression: psychoneuroendocrinological, genetic, and environmental interactions. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2016) 28:77–88. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.15030053

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Kaufman J, Plotsky PM, Nemeroff CB, and Charney DS. Effects of early adverse experiences on brain structure and function: clinical implications. Biol Psychiatry. (2000) 48:778–90. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00998-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Heim C, Newport DJ, Mletzko T, Miller AH, and Nemeroff CB. The link between childhood trauma and depression: insights from HPA axis studies in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2008) 33:693–710. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Nemeroff CB and Seligman F. The pervasive and persistent neurobiological and clinical aftermath of child abuse and neglect. J Clin Psychiatry. (2013) 74:999–1001. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13com08633

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Overmier JB and Seligman ME. Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance responding. J Comp Physiol Psychol. (1967) 63:28–33. doi: 10.1037/h0024166

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Maier SF and Seligman ME. Learned helplessness at fifty: Insights from neuroscience. Psychol Rev. (2016) 123:349–67. doi: 10.1037/rev0000033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Seligman ME and Maier SF. Failure to escape traumatic shock. J Exp Psychol. (1967) 74:1–9. doi: 10.1037/h0024514

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Baratta MV, Seligman MEP, and Maier SF. From helplessness to controllability: toward a neuroscience of resilience. Front Psychiatry. (2023) 14:1170417. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1170417

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Skinner EA and Zimmer-Gembeck MJ. Perceived control and the development of coping The Oxford handbook of stress, health, and coping. Folkman S, editor. New York: Oxford University Press (2011) p. 35–59.

Google Scholar

13. Ly V, Wang KS, Bhanji J, and Delgado MR. A reward-based framework of perceived control. Front Neurosci. (2019) 13:65. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00065

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Wang KS, Yang Y-Y, and Delgado MR. How perception of control shapes decision making. In: Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, vol. 41. (2021). p. 85–91.

Google Scholar

15. LeDoux JE. Brain mechanisms of emotion and emotional learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (1992) 2:191–7. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(92)90011-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Chrousos GP and Gold PW. The concepts of stress and stress system disorders. Overview of physical and behavioral homeostasis. JAMA. (1992) 267:1244–52. doi: 10.1001/jama.1992.03480090092034

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. López JF, Akil H, and Watson SJ. Neural circuits mediating stress. Biol Psychiatry. (1999) 46:1461–71. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00266-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Samuels ER and Szabadi E. Functional neuroanatomy of the noradrenergic locus coeruleus: its roles in the regulation of arousal and autonomic function part I: principles of functional organisation. Curr Neuropharmacol. (2008) 6:235–53. doi: 10.2174/157015908785777229

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Arborelius L, Owens MJ, Plotsky PM, and Nemeroff CB. The role of corticotropin-releasing factor in depression and anxiety disorders. J Endocrinol. (1999) 160:1–12. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1600001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Tafet GE and Bernardini R. Psychoneuroendocrinological links between chronic stress and depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2003) 27:893–903. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(03)00162-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Holsboer F. The corticosteroid receptor hypothesis of depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2000) 23:477–501. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00159-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Herman JP, McKlveen JM, Ghosal S, Kopp B, Wulsin A, Makinson R, et al. Regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical stress response. Compr Physiol. (2016) 6:603–21. doi: 10.1002/j.2040-4603.2016.tb00694.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Chrousos GP. Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nat Rev Endocrinol. (2009) 5:374–81. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2009.106

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Baumeister D, Lightman SL, and Pariante CM. The interface of stress and the HPA axis in behavioural phenotypes of mental illness. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2014) 18:13–24. doi: 10.1007/7854_2014_304

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Balleine BW and Dickinson A. Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology. (1998) 37:407–19. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00033-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. O’Callaghan C, Vaghi MM, Brummerloh B, Cardinal RN, and Robbins TW. Impaired awareness of action-outcome contingency and causality during healthy ageing and following ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Neuropsychologia. (2019) 128:282–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Balleine BW and O’Doherty JP. Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2010) 35:48–69. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.131

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Dickinson A and Balleine B. Motivational control of goal-directed action. Anim Learn Behavior. (1994) 22:1–18. doi: 10.3758/BF03199951

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Hammond LJ. The effect of contingency upon the appetitive conditioning of free-operant behavior. J Exp Anal Behav. (1980) 34:297–304. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1980.34-297

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Seligman MEP. Helplessness: On Depression, Development, and Death. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman (1975).

Google Scholar

31. Maier SF. Learned helplessness and animal models of depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (1984) 8:435–46. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(84)80032-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Maier SF and Watkins LR. Stressor controllability and learned helplessness: the roles of the dorsal raphe nucleus, serotonin, and corticotropin-releasing factor. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2005) 29:829–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Maier SF and Seligman MEP. Learned helplessness: theory and evidence. J Exp Psychol: Gen. (1976) 105:3–46. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.105.1.3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Hiroto DS and Seligman MEP. Generality of learned helplessness in man. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1975) 31:311–27. doi: 10.1037/h0076270

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Miller WR and Seligman ME. Depression and learned helplessness in man. J Abnorm Psychol. (1975) 84:228–38. doi: 10.1037/h0076720

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Hiroto DS. Locus of control and learned helplessness. J Exp Psychol. (1974) 102:187–93. doi: 10.1037/h0035910

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Abramson LY, Seligman ME, and Teasdale JD. Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. J Abnorm Psychol. (1978) 87:49–74. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.87.1.49

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Song X and Vilares I. Assessing the relationship between the human learned helplessness depression model and anhedonia. PloS One. (2021) 16:e0249056. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249056

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Kerr DL, McLaren DG, Mathy RM, and Nitschke JB. Controllability modulates the anticipatory response in the human ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Front Psychol. (2012) 3:557. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00557

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Salomons TV, Nusslock R, Detloff A, Johnstone T, and Davidson RJ. Neural emotion regulation circuitry underlying anxiolytic effects of perceived control over pain. J Cognit Neurosci. (2015) 27:222–33. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00702

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Seligman ME, Maier SF, and Geer JH. Alleviation of learned helplessness in the dog. J Abnorm Psychol. (1968) 73:256–62. doi: 10.1037/h0025831

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Seligman ME and Beagley G. Learned helplessness in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol. (1975) 88:534–41. doi: 10.1037/h0076430

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Seligman ME, Rosellini RA, and Kozak MJ. Learned helplessness in the rat: time course, immunization, and reversibility. J Comp Physiol Psychol. (1975) 88:542–7. doi: 10.1037/h0076431

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Weiss JM, Goodman PA, Losita BA, Corrigan S, Charry JM, and Bailey WH. Behavioral depression produced by an uncontrollable stressor: relationship to norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin levels in various regions of rat brain. Brain Res Rev. (1981) 3:36–97. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(81)90005-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Rotter JB. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. psychol Monographs: Gen Appl. (1966) 80:1–28. doi: 10.1037/h0092976

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev. (1977) 84:191–215. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Dickinson A. Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy. Phil Trans R Soc Lond. (1985), B30867–78.

Google Scholar

48. Barker JM, Glen WB, Linsenbardt DN, Lapish CC, and Chandler LJ. Habitual behavior is mediated by a shift in response-outcome encoding by infralimbic cortex. eNeuro. (2018) 4:ENEURO.0337–17.2017. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0337-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Barker JM, Bryant KG, and Chandler LJ. Inactivation of ventral hippocampus projections promotes sensitivity to changes in contingency. Learn Mem. (2018) 26:1–8. doi: 10.1101/lm.048025.118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Amat J, Aleksejev RM, Paul E, Watkins LR, and Maier SF. Behavioral control over shock blocks behavioral and neurochemical effects of later social defeat. Neuroscience. (2010) 165:1031–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.11.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Lu B, Fan P, Li M, Wang Y, Liang W, Yang G, et al. Detection of neuronal defensive discharge information transmission and characteristics in periaqueductal gray double-subregions using PtNP/PEDOT: PSS modified microelectrode arrays. Microsyst Nanoeng. (2023) 9:70. doi: 10.1038/s41378-023-00546-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Zhang H, Zhu Z, Ma WX, Kong LX, Yuan PC, Bu LF, et al. The contribution of periaqueductal gray in the regulation of physiological and pathological behaviors. Front Neurosci. (2024) 18:1380171. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1380171

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. LeDoux J. The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cell Mol Neurobiol. (2003) 23:727–38. doi: 10.1023/A:1025048802629

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Deng H, Xiao X, and Wang Z. Periaqueductal gray neuronal activities underlie different aspects of defensive behaviors. J Neurosci. (2016) 36:7580–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4425-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Benarroch EE. Periaqueductal gray: an interface for behavioral control. Neurology. (2012) 78:210–7. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31823fcdee

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Azmitia EC. The primate serotonergic system: progression towards a collaborative organization. In: Meltzer H, editor. Psychopharmacology: The Third Generation of Progress. Raven Press, New York (1987). p. 61–74.

Google Scholar

57. Jacobs BL and Azmitia E. Structure and function of the brain serotonin system. Physiol Rev. (1992) 72:165–229. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1992.72.1.165

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Azmitia EC and Whitaker-Azmitia PM. Anatomy, cell biology, and plasticity of the serotonergic system. Neuropsychopharmacological implications for the actions of psychotropic drugs. In: Bloom FE and Kupfer DJ, editors. Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress. Raven Press, New York (1995). p. 443–449.

Google Scholar

59. Hensler JG. Serotonergic modulation of the limbic system. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2006) 30:203–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.06.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Hensler JG. Serotonergic modulation of the limbic system. Receptor plasticity and the pathophysiology of anxiety and depression. Pharmacol Ther. (2006) 109:173–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.06.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Waselus M, Valentino RJ, and Van Bockstaele EJ. Collateralized dorsal raphe nucleus projections: a mechanism for the integration of diverse functions during stress. J Chem Neuroanat. (2011) 41:266–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2011.05.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Deakin JF and Graeff FG. 5-HT and mechanisms of defence. J Psychopharmacol. (1991) 5:305–15. doi: 10.1177/026988119100500414

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Maswood S, Barter JE, Watkins LR, and Maier SF. Exposure to inescapable but not escapable shock increases extracellular levels of 5-HT in the dorsal raphe nucleus of the rat. Brain Res. (1998) 783:115–20. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)01313-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Grahn RE, Will MJ, Hammack SE, Maswood S, McQueen MB, Watkins LR, et al. Activation of serotonin-immunoreactive cells in the dorsal raphe nucleus in rats exposed to an uncontrollable stressor. Brain Res. (1999) 826:35–43. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(99)01208-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Pollak Dorocic I, Fürth D, Xuan Y, Johansson Y, Pozzi L, Silberberg G, et al. A whole-brain atlas of inputs to serotonergic neurons of the dorsal and median raphe nuclei. Neuron. (2014) 83:663–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Retson TA and Van Bockstaele EJ. Coordinate regulation of noradrenergic and serotonergic brain regions by amygdalar neurons. J Chem Neuroanat. (2013) 52:9–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2013.04.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Valentino RJ, Lucki I, and Van Bockstaele E. Corticotropin-releasing factor in the dorsal raphe nucleus: Linking stress coping and addiction. Brain Res. (2010) 1314:29–37. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.100

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Valentino RJ, Page ME, and Curtis AL. Activation of noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons by hemodynamic stress is due to local release of corticotropin-releasing factor. Brain Res. (1991) 555:25–34. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90855-P

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Baraban JM and Aghajanian GK. Noradrenergic innervation of serotonergic neurons in the dorsal raphe: demonstration by electron microscopic autoradiography. Brain Res. (1981) 204:1–11. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(81)90646-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Baraban JM and Aghajanian GK. Suppression of firing activity of 5-HT neurons in the dorsal raphe by alpha-adrenoceptor antagonists. Neuropharmacology. (1980) 19:355–363. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(80)90187-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Rozeske RR, Evans AK, Frank MG, Watkins LR, Lowry CA, and Maier SF. Uncontrollable, but not controllable, stress desensitizes 5-HT1A receptors in the dorsal raphe nucleus. J Neurosci. (2011) 31:14107–15. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3095-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Smith KS and Graybiel AM. A dual operator view of habitual behavior reflecting cortical and striatal dynamics. Neuron. (2013) 79:361–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Killcross S and Coutureau E. Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cereb Cortex. (2003) 13:400–8. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.4.400

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Yin HH, Ostlund SB, Knowlton BJ, and Balleine BW. The role of the dorsomedial striatum in instrumental conditioning. Eur J Neurosci. (2005) 22:513–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04218.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, and Balleine BW. Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. Eur J Neurosci. (2004) 19:181–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03095.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, and Balleine BW. Inactivation of dorsolateral striatum enhances sensitivity to changes in the action–outcome contingency in instrumental conditioning. Behav Brain Res. (2006) 166:189–96. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.07.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Britt JP, Benaliouad F, McDevitt RA, Stuber GD, Wise RA, and Bonci A. Synaptic and behavioral profile of multiple glutamatergic inputs to the nucleus accumbens. Neuron. (2012) 76:790–803. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Friedman DP, Aggleton JP, and Saunders RC. Comparison of hippocampal, amygdala, and perirhinal projections to the nucleus accumbens: combined anterograde and retrograde tracing study in the Macaque brain. J Comp Neurol. (2002) 450:345–65. doi: 10.1002/cne.10336

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Maier SF, Amat J, Baratta MV, Paul E, and Watkins LR. Behavioral control, the medial prefrontal cortex, and resilience. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2006) 8:397–406. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2006.8.4/smaier

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Jankowski MP and Sesack SR. Prefrontal cortical projections to the rat dorsal raphe nucleus: ultrastructural features and associations with serotonin and gamma-aminobutyric acid neurons. J Comp Neurol. (2004) 468:518–29. doi: 10.1002/cne.10976

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Amat J, Baratta MV, Paul E, Bland ST, Watkins LR, and Maier SF. Medial prefrontal cortex determines how stressor controllability affects behavior and dorsal raphe nucleus. Nat Neurosci. (2005) 8:365–71. doi: 10.1038/nn1399

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Amat J, Paul E, Zarza C, Watkins LR, and Maier SF. Previous experience with behavioral control over stress blocks the behavioral and dorsal raphe nucleus activating effects of later uncontrollable stress: role of the ventral medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. (2006) 26:13264–72. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3630-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Maier SF, Grahn RE, Kalman BA, Sutton LC, Wiertelak EP, and Watkins LR. The role of the amygdala and dorsal raphe nucleus in mediating the behavioral consequences of inescapable shock. Behav Neurosci. (1993) 107:377–88. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.107.2.377

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Nitschke JB, Sarinopoulos I, Mackiewicz KL, Schaefer HS, and Davidson RJ. Functional neuroanatomy of aversion and its anticipation. Neuroimage. (2006) 29:106–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.068

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Nitschke JB, Sarinopoulos I, Oathes DJ, Johnstone T, Whalen PJ, Davidson RJ, et al. Anticipatory activation in the amygdala and anterior cingulate in generalized anxiety disorder and prediction of treatment response. Am J Psychiatry. (2009) 166:302–10. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07101682

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Sarinopoulos I, Grupe DW, Mackiewicz KL, Herrington JD, Lor M, Steege EE, et al. Uncertainty during anticipation modulates neural responses to aversion in human insula and amygdala. Cereb Cortex. (2010) 20:929–40. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp155

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Phelps EA, Delgado MR, Nearing KI, and LeDoux JE. Extinction learning in humans: role of the amygdala and vmPFC. Neuron. (2004) 43:897–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Maier SF and Watkins LR. Role of the medial prefrontal cortex in coping and resilience. Brain Res. (2010) 1355:52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Hare TA, Camerer CF, and Rangel A. Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science. (2009) 324:646–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1168450

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Hare TA, Hakimi S, and Rangel A. Activity in dlPFC and its effective connectivity to vmPFC are associated with temporal discounting. Front Neurosci. (2014) 8:50. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Goldman-Rakic PS. Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron. (1995) 14:477–85. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Barbey AK, Koenigs M, and Grafman J. Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex. (2013) 49:1195–205. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Lee D and Seo H. Mechanisms of reinforcement learning and decision making in the primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2007) 1104:108–22. doi: 10.1196/annals.1390.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Winecoff A, Clithero JA, Carter RM, Bergman SR, Wang L, and Huettel SA. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex encodes emotional value. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:11032–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4317-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Suzuki Y and Tanaka SC. Functions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion regulation under stress. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:18225. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-97751-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Grabenhorst F and Rolls ET. Value, pleasure and choice in the ventral prefrontal cortex. Trends Cognit Sci. (2011) 15:56–67. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Berboth S and Morawetz C. Amygdala-prefrontal connectivity during emotion regulation: A meta-analysis of psychophysiological interactions. Neuropsychologia. (2021) 153:107767. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107767

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Celada P, Puig MV, and Artigas F. Serotonin modulation of cortical neurons and networks. Front Integr Neurosci. (2013) 7:25. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Blier P and de Montigny C. Serotonin and drug-induced therapeutic responses in major depression, obsessive-compulsive and panic disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. (1999) 21:91S–8S. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1395360

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Santarelli L, Saxe M, Gross C, Surget A, Battaglia F, Dulawa S, et al. Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science. (2003) 301:805–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1083328

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Duman RS and Aghajanian GK. Synaptic dysfunction in depression: potential therapeutic targets. Science. (2012) 338:68–72. doi: 10.1126/science.1222939

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Castrén E and Hen R. Neuronal plasticity and antidepressant actions. Trends Neurosci. (2013) 36:259–67. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.12.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Harmer CJ, Goodwin GM, and Cowen PJ. Why do antidepressants take so long to work? A cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant drug action. Br J Psychiatry. (2009) 195:102–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051193

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Drevets WC, Savitz J, and Trimble M. The subgenual anterior cingulate cortex in mood disorders. CNS Spectr. (2008) 13:663–81. doi: 10.1017/S1092852900013754

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Furman DJ, Hamilton JP, and Gotlib IH. Frontostriatal functional connectivity in major depressive disorder. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. (2011) 1:11. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-1-11

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Eshel N and Roiser JP. Reward and punishment processing in depression. Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 68:118–24. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.01.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Pryce CR, Azzinnari D, Spinelli S, Seifritz E, Tegethoff M, and Meinlschmidt G. Helplessness: a systematic translational review of theory and evidence for its relevance to understanding and treating depression. Pharmacol Ther. (2011) 132:242–67. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2011.06.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Maier SF and Watkins LR. Stressor controllability, anxiety, and serotonin. Cog Ther Res. (1998) 6:595–613. doi: 10.1023/A:1018794104325

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Tricomi EM, Delgado MR, and Fiez JA. Modulation of caudate activity by action contingency. Neuron. (2004) 41:281–92. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00848-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Leotti LA and Delgado MR. The inherent reward of choice. Psychol Sci. (2011) 22:1310–8. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Zwosta K, Ruge H, and Wolfensteller U. Neural mechanisms of goal-directed behavior: outcome-based response selection is associated with increased functional coupling of the angular gyrus. Front Hum Neurosci. (2015) 9:180. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00180

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Jones SL, Nation JR, and Massad P. Immunization against learned helplessness in man. J Abnorm Psychol. (1977) 86:75–83. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.86.1.75

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Kubala KH, Christianson JP, Kaufman RD, Watkins LR, and Maier SF. Short- and longterm consequences of stressor controllability in adolescent rats. Behav Brain Res. (2012) 234:278–84. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.06.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stress, depression, trauma, resilience, helplessness, controllability

Citation: Tafet GE and Ortiz Alonso T (2025) Learned helplessness and learned controllability: from neurobiology to cognitive, emotional and behavioral neurosciences. Front. Psychiatry 16:1600165. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1600165

Received: 01 April 2025; Accepted: 06 August 2025;
Published: 26 August 2025.

Edited by:

ChiHye Chung, Konkuk University, Republic of Korea

Reviewed by:

Li Wang, Children’s National Hospital, United States
Sonia Tabares, Institución Universitaria de Envigado, Colombia

Copyright © 2025 Tafet and Ortiz Alonso. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gustavo E. Tafet, Z3VzdGFmZXRAdGFtdS5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.