- 1Children Nutrition Research Center, Children’s Hospital of Chongqing Medical University, Chongqing Key Laboratory of Child Neurodevelopment and Cognitive Disorders, Ministry of Education Key Laboratory of Child Development and Disorders, National Clinical Research Center for Child Health and Disorders, Chongqing, China
- 2Department of Primary Child Health Care, Children’s Hospital of Chongqing Medical University, Chongqing, China
Background: The study delves into the intricate task of differentiating intellectual structures among children diagnosed with the high-functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder (HF-ASD), Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), or comorbidity (ASD+ADHD), aiming to assist in their clinical differentiation, with the goal of refining clinical diagnoses and developing targeted therapeutic interventions.
Methods: The study included 200 outpatients aged 6.5–13.0 years (total Intelligence Quotient (IQ) 70–130) at the Children’s Hospital of Chongqing Medical University, and categorized into HF-ASD (n=91), ADHD (n=47), and comorbidity ASD+ADHD (n=62) groups. We utilized the Chinese Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (C-WISC) as the primary assessment tool, supplemented by additional diagnostic measures. Besides, we used SPSS 25.0 to assess the subtest scores and differences.
Results: The comorbidity group had lower total IQ than the other two groups (p<0.001). The verbal IQ(VIQ) were lower than the performance IQ(PIQ) in HF-ASD (p=0.017) and comorbidity (p=0.007) groups. They also scored higher on perceptual organization subtests particularly in Block Design and Object Assembly than the ADHD group. The ADHD group showed higher VIQ than PIQ (p=0.020). The ADHD group’s scores for working memory subtests were lower than in the HF-ASD group. The respective peak scores for the HF-ASD and comorbidity groups were in Block Design (45%,43%) and Object Assembly (30%,37%) and valleys in Picture Completion (52%,24%), Information (HF-ASD 24%), and Arithmetic (comorbidity 42%).
Conclusion: The peak-valley difference in the ADHD group (~2 standard deviations) was smaller than in the HF-ASD and comorbidity groups (~3 standard deviations), and this characteristic could help differentiate between HF-ASD, ADHD, and both together.
1 Introduction
Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) is a complex neurodevelopmental condition marked by two primary symptoms: deficient social communication skills and limited interests accompanied by repetitive, stereotyped behaviors. The prevalence of the disease globally is approximately 1% (1) and it has been increasing in recent years (2). Because ASD is clinically heterogeneous and can be divided into High-Functioning ASD (HF-ASD) (IQ≥70) and Low-Functioning ASD(LF-ASD) (IQ<70) based on intelligence quotient (IQ) (3, 4). Among individuals with HF-ASD exhibit normal intelligence and milder social challenges, often displaying highly selective focus (5).Previous studies have shown that they often show symptoms of attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (6). ADHD, one of the most prevalent neurodevelopmental disorder, is characterized by inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, affecting approximately 7% of children globally (7, 8). ADHD is a common comorbidity of ASD, with coexistence rates ranging from 40% to 70% (9–11). Both disorders often present with inattention as the primary concern in clinical settings, and their overlapping symptoms can easily lead to confusion, complicating their distinction (12, 13). A survey of delayed ASD diagnosis among children and adolescents found that a significant proportion of ASD cases were misdiagnosed or underdiagnosed, with ADHD emerging as the most common misdiagnosis (14, 15). Therefore, failing to accurately differentiate between the two disorders may result in inappropriate treatment, delayed intervention, and worsened social functioning impairment (16, 17).
At present, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale (WISC) have emerged as critical tools for distinguishing these disorders (18, 19). Previous studies have shown that the difference between verbal Intelligence Quotient (VIQ) and Performance Intelligence Quotient (PIQ) helps distinguish ASD from ADHD individuals. Among them, the VIQ of children with ASD was significantly lower than that of PIQ (20, 21) Conversely, some studies of intellectual ability reported no VIQ and PIQ differences in ASD (22). Besides, A comparison study between children with ASD and ADHD found that those with HF-ASD scored significantly lower in verbal comprehension, vocabulary, and understanding, but higher in block design compared to children with ADHD (23). This unbalanced pattern of intelligence is attributed to the neurodiversity of its development. As outlined in the British Medical Bulletin, the psychological definition of neurodiversity reflects the diversity of individual cognitive abilities, in which there are large, significant differences between the peaks and valleys of the profile. Individuals with neurodiversity exhibit cognitive profiles where abilities span ≥2 standard deviations(SD) within a normal distribution (24). The scores of WISC provided clear guidance on the level of difference between strengths and weaknesses that is typical or of clinical significance, and were used to support the diagnosis of neurodevelopmental disorders (25). Emerging evidence suggests such cognitive development patterns may better differentiate neurodevelopmental profiles than conventional metrics (21, 26). This was confirmed by the study of Koyama et al. (23).
Previous studies have primarily focused on the analysis of intellectual characteristics in children diagnosed solely with HF-ASD and ADHD, while research on children with both ASD and ADHD is relatively scarce. Additionally, it has been noted that there are differences in the WISC scale scores between the two conditions. However, few studies have conducted in-depth analyses of the intellectual patterns combining these three disorders. This methodological gap impedes the identification of these disorder cognitive traits, which are essential for refining diagnostic precision and developing targeted interventions. Therefore, resolving this diagnostic ambiguity holds urgent clinical significance. The present study seeks to investigate the intellectual structure imbalance in children with ASD and ADHD, as measured by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale. By analyzing cognitive paradigms linked to neurodiversity, this research aims to establish a foundation for clinical differential diagnosis and targeted interventions.
2 Methods
2.1 Participants and procedure
The study is a single-center case-control analysis conducted at the Children’s Hospital Affiliated with Chongqing Medical University from 2023 to 2024.This cross-sectional study recruited 421 children and adolescents aged 6.5–13.0 years, with primary clinical presentations of attentional deficits. Participants underwent a multi-phase diagnostic evaluation to stratify neurodevelopmental profiles. Inclusion criteria mandated comprehensive medical documentation and baseline cognitive assessment using the Chinese Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (C-WISC)(total IQ>70) (27, 28), Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS),Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS)(excluding the ADHD group), Infant-Junior High School Student Social Life Ability Scale (S-M Scale), and Vanderbilt ADHD Rating scales. The final analytical cohort comprised 200 participants after exclusions for missing diagnostic information (22%, n=49), incomplete diagnostic scale(21%, n=47), uncertain diagnosis (5%, n=10),diagnosed with other diseases(10%,n=22), IQ < 70 (17%, n=38), age < 6 years (10%, n=22), and coexisting other neurodevelopmental disorders (15%, n=33). Diagnosed by two developmental-behavioral pediatric experts following the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) criteria for ASD, ADHD, and ASD + ADHD (7, 29, 30). Inter-rater reliability was evaluated using Bland-Altman analysis to quantify agreement between ADOS and SRS scores, the scatters are basically within the 95% agreement interval (within 1.96 SDs) (31), indicating that the consistency was considered acceptable. Discrepancies were resolved through panel review. Strict data integrity checks ensured exclusion of ambiguous or incomplete cases, enhancing diagnostic homogeneity. In addition, the inclusion method of this study is included retrospectively. And the detailed schematic diagram for specific clinical diagnosis groupings is provided in Appendix Table 1. This study has been approved by the Ethics Committee of the Children’s Hospital Affiliated with Chongqing Medical University (2024305).
2.2 Assessment tools
Standardized assessment procedures, including the same set of evaluations, were followed for all participants.
2.2.1 Chinese Wechsler intelligence scale for children
Devised by Hunan Medical University, the C-WISC is suitable for children aged 6 to 16 years. This scale has undergone rigorous cultural adaptation including linguistic modifications to reflect Mandarin semantic structures and norming processes specific to urban Chinese populations. Its norming procedure includes stratified sampling based on age, gender, urban-rural distribution, and parents’ educational level, strictly following the WISC-R’s operational and scoring standards, with IQ scores based on a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. Its reliability and validity are close to those of the original version (WISC-R) and meet statistical requirements (28). It consists of a Verbal Scale (Vs) comprising Information, Similarities, Arithmetic, Vocabulary, and Digit Span subtests, and a Performance Scale (Ps) comprising Picture Completion, Picture Arrangement, Block Design, Object Assembly, and Coding subtests. Verbal IQ (VIQ) is derived from the Vs scores, and performance IQ (PIQ) is derived from the Ps scores. The full-scale IQ is based on the combined Vs and Ps. Subtests were grouped into Verbal Comprehension (Information, Similarities, Vocabulary), Perceptual Organization (Picture Completion, Picture Arrangement, Block Design, Object Assembly), and Working Memory (Arithmetic, Digit Span, Coding) factors. The IQ range for normal children is 70-130. An IQ below 70 is considered intellectual disability. If a child has both an IQ below 70 and ASD, they are diagnosed with low-functioning ASD. In addition to that, a VIQ-PIQ difference > 15 indicated clinically significant intellectual imbalance. Internally, this refers to the difference between high and low scores within operational or linguistic subdomains. Overalls, it encompasses the variation between the highest and lowest scores across the entire battery of tests.
2.2.2 Autism diagnostic observation schedule
The Chinese version of ADOS was used. This version has been validated as having good reliability and practical validity (32).It is considered the gold standard for assessing ASD symptoms in one-year-old individuals and older (33–35). It evaluates social interaction, communication, stereotypic behaviors, interests, and imagination. Scores indicate the severity of the autistic symptoms. The scale consists of different modules and differs in terms of diagnostic threshold scores for autism and autism spectrum disorders (ASD).
2.2.3 Vanderbilt ADHD rating scale
The scale includes parent rating scales, teacher rating scales and Rating scale-IV(RS-IV). It is used to assess ADHD symptoms, functional impairment and treatment response in children and adolescents (36, 37). It also aids in differentiating ASD + ADHD from other neurodevelopmental disorders. A positive diagnosis criterion may encompass the following medical and professional aspects: a. Number of Symptoms: A minimum of six symptoms must meet the positive criteria within either the inattentive or hyperactive-impulsive domains. b. Duration of Symptoms: These symptoms must persist for at least six months and be more pronounced than what is typically expected for the child’s developmental level. c. Occurrence of Symptoms: The symptoms must manifest in at least two settings (e.g., home and school). d. Functional Impairment (functional impairment scales): These symptoms must interfere with the child’s normal functioning in family, school, or work environments. Although different studies have reported varying results regarding the validity and reliability analysis, they generally indicate that the scale has high consistency and practicality for the diagnosis of ADHD (38).
2.2.4 Social responsiveness scale
This scale assesses ASD features across age groups, quantifying social awareness, cognition, communication, motivation, and restricted interests/repetitive behaviors (39, 40). Scores ≥65 indicate positive ASD features, with higher scores reflecting greater social impairment (41). The reliability and validity of this scale in the ASD population aged >4 years are 0.946 and 0.958, respectively, demonstrating good applicability (42).
2.2.5 Infant-junior high school student social life ability scale
This scale evaluates self-reliance skills, motor skills, academic performance, interpersonal skills, collaborative tasks, and self-management, aiding in identifying social interaction impairments in children with ASD. In our country, the reliability of this scale is 0.98, and its validity is over 95% (43). Scores ≤ 9 (standard score) indicate that the adaptive ability may be suspiciously abnormal or deficient (44).
2.3 Statistical analysis
Data were analyzed using SPSS Statistics for Macintosh, Version 25.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). In our study, normally distributed continuous variables are reported as the mean ± standard deviation (`x± s). To compare multiple groups of these variables, we conducted Repeated-measures ANOVA, with post-hoc Least Significant Difference (LSD) tests multiple comparisons, which were employed to evaluate cognitive strengths and weaknesses within each diagnostic group. For non-normally distributed continuous variables, data are presented as the median and range, and group comparisons were carried out using the Kruskal-Wallis H test, with Dunn’s test utilized for pairwise analyses, and applied Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons to minimize the risk of Type I errors (false positives). Furthermore, we conducted paired t-tests to compare Verbal Intelligence Quotient (VIQ) and Performance Intelligence Quotient (PIQ) scores between children diagnosed with different conditions. Categorical variables are expressed in terms of the number of cases (N) and percentages (%), and within-group comparisons of these data were analyzed using the chi-square test. Across all analyses, a p-value of less than 0.05 (p< 0.05) was deemed statistically significant.
3 Results
3.1 Demographics
The study included 200 children aged 6.5–13.0 (mean: 7.499 ± 1.064) years treated at the specialist outpatient department of the Children’s Hospital of Chongqing Medical University between 2023 and 2024. These included the HF-ASD (n = 91), ADHD (n = 47), and ASD + ADHD (n = 62) groups (Table 1).Additionally, We also analyzed the results of relevant scales used for auxiliary diagnosis and observed significant differences among children with different diagnoses in terms of Infant-Junior High School Student Social Life Ability Scale (S-M Scale)(p<0.001), Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS)(p<0.001), Vanderbilt ADHD Diagnostic Rating Scale(p<0.001). Furthermore, it is evident that children with ASD + ADHD scored lower on the S-M scale, while their scores on the SRS were higher than in the HF-ASD group.
3.2 Comparison of full-scale intellectual quotient
Significant differences were observed between groups (p<0.001), with the ASD + ADHD group having the lowest total IQ and the HF-ASD group the highest (Table 1).
3.3 Comparison of VIQ and PIQ
Paired samples t-tests showed that the ADHD group had significantly higher Verbal Intelligence Quotient (VIQ) than Performance Intelligence Quotient (PIQ) (t = 2.413, p= 0.020), differing by about 5 points (0.3 SDs). In contrast, the HF-ASD and ASD + ADHD groups had significantly lower VIQ than PIQ, differing by about 4 points (Appendix Figure 1).
3.4 Subtest scores and peak-valley differences
Kruskal-Wallis tests revealed significant differences in all subtests across groups (p < 0.001, SD=3). Post hoc comparisons showed that the HF-ASD (12.39 and 9.98 points) and ASD + ADHD (11.89 and 10.48 points) groups scored significantly higher on Block Design and Object Assembly than the ADHD group (7.67 and 9.31points). The ADHD group(7.41 points) scored significantly lower on Coding than the HF-ASD group and ASD + ADHD group(9.44 and 7.65 points)(Figure 1). The HF-ASD and ASD + ADHD groups showed greater score variability, with peaks in Block Design and Object Assembly and valleys in Picture Completion and Information. In contrast, the ADHD group showed more stable scores, with peaks in Object Assembly and Classification and a valley in Coding. Then, an analysis of the top three highest and lowest proportions of subtest scores for children with different diagnoses revealed that the highest scores (peaks) of 45% of children with HF-ASD and 43% of those with ASD + ADHD were in Block Design. The respective peak rates in Object Assembly were 30 and 37%. Conversely, their respective lowest score (valleys) rates were >50% and 24% in Picture Completion. Furthermore, 42% of the ASD +ADHD group scored lowest on Arithmetic. In contrast, among children with ADHD, only 6% had their peaks in Block Design, while the peaks of 45% were in Object Assembly and 37% in Similarities. For valleys, 33% of children with ADHD scored lowest on Coding (Table 2). Finally, we used the subtest scores of the C-WISC as the x-axis and the SRS scores as the y-axis, we analyzed their correlation. The results revealed a negative correlation between SRS scores and most C-WISC subtests, indicating that higher subtest scores were associated with lower SRS scores (i.e., reduced social impairment). In contrast, Block Design and Object Assembly subtests demonstrated a positive correlation with SRS scores, meaning higher scores on these subtests corresponded to higher SRS scores (i.e., greater social deficits).Subsequently, we used partial correlation analysis to analyze the correlation between SRS scores and block design, with total IQ, verbal IQ, and age as control variables. The results showed that after excluding confounding factors, there was still a correlation between block design and SRS scores(p=0.237). (Appendix Figure 3).

Figure 1. Comparison of score between Children with HF-ASD, ADHD, ASD+ADHD. HF-ASD High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder, ADHD Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, ASD+ADHD comorbidity.
3.5 Quantification of the peak-valley differences
Variance and quantitative analysis of subtest differences revealed significant differences between groups in intra- and inter-subtest scores (Table 3). The ASD + ADHD group had a mean peak-valley difference of 2.62 SDs in the intra - subtest and 2.94 SDs in the inter - subtest. The HF-ASD group had a mean difference of 2.69 SDs in the intra - subtest and 3.05 SDs in the inter - subtest. In contrast, the ADHD group had a mean difference of 1.79 SDs in the intra - subtest subtests and 2.07 SDs in the inter - subtest (Table 3). The ROC curve to validate 2.6SDs showed an AUC of 0.824.
4 Discussion
Previous studies have documented the cognitive strengths and deficits in the intellectual architecture of children diagnosed with ASD (26, 45, 46). However, no studies have systematically leveraged intellectual traits of ASD to differentiate it from ADHD. Thus, this study represents the first systematic investigation of intellectual constructs in differentiating ASD, ADHD and comorbid ASD+ADHD. Consistent with established epidemiological trends (47), our cohort exhibited a male predominance (76.6%–90.48%). Children with ASD+ADHD demonstrated significantly lower total IQ compared to pure ADHD or HF-ASD groups. We proposed that functional impairment is more severe in children with ASD+ADHD. These findings suggest that the comorbid group’s hybrid profile suggests additive neurodevelopmental disruptions, where ASD-related strengths coexist with ADHD-related inhibitory deficits, exacerbating functional imbalances (9). Research results revealed significant imbalance between VIQ and PIQ across groups, and further found that the VIQ of children with HF-ASD and comorbid ASD and ADHD was significantly lower than their PIQ by about 0.3 SDs corroborating prior findings (21, 26). VIQ reflects crystallized intelligence—a domain influenced by cultural and educational backgrounds—encompassing knowledge, vocabulary, language comprehension, and academic skills (48). However, PIQ represents fluid intelligence, relying on innate factors like perception, memory, processing speed, and reasoning abilities (49). These findings may be related to the fact that children with ASD develop later in life with impaired social participation and communication, which hinders the development of crystallized intelligence and leads to low VIQ (50, 51). ADHD group are more likely to be affected by deficits in innate abilities (fluid intelligence deficits) and less likely to be affected by later-developing abilities such as language (52, 53). Therefore, differences between VIQ and PIQ may help distinguish between ASD.
Children with HF-ASD or ASD+ADHD exhibited greater subtest score fluctuations compared to those with ADHD alone, with pronounced peak-valley discrepancies (54). These findings collectively suggest a markedly imbalanced intellectual structure in children with ASD, featuring distinct strengths and weaknesses. The results of the analysis of the proportion indicate that the peaks of the HF-ASD group and the Comorbidity group are mostly concentrated in the Block Design, whereas the ADHD group is more focused on the Object Assembly. This is similar to previous research reports (55). This difference may stem from the fact that while Block Design and Object Assembly both assess visuospatial abilities, the latter additionally demands adaptive visuomotor integration—a skill contrasting with the rigid, stereotypical mode of Block Design (20). Elevated visuospatial abilities in ASD (such as peak performance on Block Design) may reflect atypical neural specialization in right-hemispheric regions, particularly the dorsal visual stream responsible for spatial processing (15). Besides, the valleys of the HF-ASD group and the Comorbidity group are mostly focused on the Picture Completion, whereas the ADHD group showed significantly lower scores on Coding subtests (19). This phenomenon may be attributable to the notion that coding is indicative of working memory capacity. The findings that children diagnosed with ADHD exhibit impairments in working memory are consistent with the conclusions of previous studies (56). The Picture Completion has been demonstrated to reflect visuomotor dexterity and visual recognition of basic details of objects. Previous studies have shown that children diagnosed with ASD have deficits in this area (57). Consequently, the study proposed the cognitive strengths and weaknesses of HF-ASD,ADHD and comorbid ASD+ADHD can be differentiated through the C-WISC.
Building on prior research, this study extends the quantification of cognitive peak-valley discrepancies (intra- and inter-subtest score variations) in children with HF-ASD and ADHD, while exploring their neurocognitive underpinnings. Children with HF-ASD exhibited significantly greater intra- and inter-subtest variability, with mean peak-valley differences exceeding 3 SD—nearly 50% higher than the <2 SD observed in the ADHD group. These findings confirm that children with HF-ASD exhibited more pronounced peak-valley pattern—reflecting both exceptional strengths and marked deficits—relative to the weaker heterogeneity performance patterns of children with ADHD. Although not all individuals with HF-ASD exhibit significant peaks and troughs in their intellectual structure due to individual differences, this may be a subtype of ASD. However, from a clinical perspective, this phenomenon is quite common. While our findings align with earlier reports of uneven cognitive profiles in ASD (58, 59), they extend this work by bridging neuropsychological observations to clinical applications—a critical gap in prior research.
In the study, we wanted to look for a clinical link between this cognitive trait and further clues for identifying and diagnosing children with ASD and ADHD. In clinical practice, by calculating the difference between the highest and lowest scores on each subtest, if the difference exceeds 2.6 SD (SD = 3), it may indicate potential social issues, necessitating further evaluation using social-related scales and medical history for diagnosis. Of course, the possibility of underlying social difficulties cannot be ruled out. Certainly, due to the limited sample size of this study, this threshold requires further validation.
The cognitive strengths and weaknesses described above are specific manifestations of neurodiversity (24, 60). Both help doctors to take a medical history for early diagnosis, while enabling parents to better understand their children’s intellectual structure beyond academic scores (61). More importantly, we can also suggest different interventions for different cognitive characteristics. Leveraging the strengths of children with ASD can enhance their weaknesses, increase interaction through cultivating advantageous abilities (such as programming or chess), and foster social engagement and vocational readiness (62). Therefore, Paradigms for Identifying Cognitive Strengths and Weaknesses in children with ASD, ADHD and Comorbidity, especially those currently viewed as weaknesses but with potential for transformation, is significant. Thus, analysis of the structural imbalance features of intelligence, it can be demonstrated that children with ASD and ADHD have different cognitive paradigms, and combining this paradigm with clinical history can help physicians and families recognize children’s strengths and weaknesses in order to identify children with ADHD from those who have both autism and ADHD.
5 Limitations
Several limitations should be acknowledged. First, C-WISC may be affected by differences in cultural contexts and its application in other languages needs to be further validated, and it is not the newest scale, so may cause score bias and reduce sample representativeness compared to the WISC-IV/V. Besides, the sample was relatively small, the underrepresentation of females and ADHD children may limit the generalizability of cognitive findings, thereby leading to sample bias. And healthy populations and low-functioning cases (IQ<70) were not included in this study, which may limit applicability. The next step is to enrich the sample size in order to further refine the application of the cognitive paradigm in different populations. Finally, this study relied exclusively on clinical scales, which may introduce subjectivity and error, and could be combined with objective screening or diagnostic techniques (e.g., eye-tracking technology, near-infrared functional brain imaging, magnetic resonance, etc.) in the future.
6 Conclusion
This study used the C-WISC to analyze the intellectual structure of children between HF-ASD, ADHD, and ASD + ADHD. Notable imbalances were identified in the intellectual profiles of children with ASD and those with ASD + ADHD, characterized by a distinct peak-and-valley pattern with differences exceeding 2.6 SDs. Specifically, peaks were predominantly observed in the Block Design and Object Assembly subtests, whereas valleys were evident in the Picture Completion subtest. Although children with ADHD also exhibited peaks and valleys, the differences were smaller, typically under 2 SDs, with peaks mostly in Object Assembly and Similarities and valleys primarily in Coding.
These findings could help differentiate between HF-ASD, ADHD, and ASD + ADHD, and offer clinicians novel insights for diagnosing and intervening in ASD. By recognizing the cognitive strengths and weaknesses of children with ASD, clinicians and educators could devise personalized intervention strategies that cater to the unique needs of these children, providing a robust foundation for improving their outcomes.
Data availability statement
The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to Tingyu Li, dHlsaUB2aXAuc2luYS5jb20=.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by the Children’s Hospital of Chongqing Medical University Institutional Review Board, with approval number (2024) IRB (study) 305. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s), and minor(s)’ legal guardian/next of kin, for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.
Author contributions
D-YZ: Formal Analysis, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Data curation, Project administration, Resources, Validation, Methodology, Investigation, Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. Q-HW: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. B-YH: Writing – original draft, Resources. DA: Writing – original draft, Resources. YZ: Resources, Writing – original draft. YD: Writing – review & editing, Resources. TY: Supervision, Writing – review & editing, Resources, Project administration. JC: Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. LC: Writing – review & editing, Resources. YW: Writing – original draft, Resources. H-YC: Resources, Writing – original draft. X-LX: Writing – original draft, Resources. Q-HM: Resources, Writing – original draft. T-YL: Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Data curation, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization.
Funding
The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No.81771223), and Chief Medical Expert Studio of Chongqing (No.YWBF[2018]263) for providing financial support for this research.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to all the participants and their caregivers who gave their time and efforts to participate in this research.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The handling editor DY declared a past co-authorship with the authors Q-HW, T-YL, TY.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1610278/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Hirota T and King BH. Autism spectrum disorder. Jama. (2023) 329:157–68. doi: 10.1001/jama.2022.23661
2. Zeidan J, Fombonne E, Scorah J, Ibrahim A, Durkin MS, Saxena S, et al. Global prevalence of autism: A systematic review update. Autism Res. (2022) 15:778–90. doi: 10.1002/aur.2696
3. Bartak L and Rutter M. Differences between mentally retarded and normally intelligent autistic children. J Autism Child Schizophr. (1976) 6:109–20. doi: 10.1007/BF01538054
4. Chiang HL, Kao WC, Chou MC, Chou WJ, Chiu YN, Wu YY, et al. School dysfunction in youth with autistic spectrum disorder in Taiwan: The effect of subtype and ADHD. Autism Res. (2018) 11:857–69. doi: 10.1002/aur.1923
5. Rau S, Skapek MF, Tiplady K, Seese S, Burns A, Armour AC, et al. Identifying comorbid ADHD in autism: Attending to the inattentive presentation. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2020) 69:101468. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2019.101468
6. Lee J and Lee SI. Unrecognized comorbid autism spectrum disorder in children initially diagnosed with only attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Asian J Psychiatr. (2023) 85:103629. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2023.103629
7. Barnett R. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet. (2016) 387:737. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)00332-9
8. Salari N, Ghasemi H, Abdoli N, Rahmani A, Shiri MH, Hashemian AH, et al. The global prevalence of ADHD in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ital J Pediatrics. (2023) 49:48. doi: 10.1186/s13052-023-01456-1
9. Antshel KM, Zhang-James Y, Wagner KE, Ledesma A, and Faraone SV. An update on the comorbidity of ADHD and ASD: a focus on clinical management. Expert Rev Neurotherapeutics. (2016) 16:279–93. doi: 10.1586/14737175.2016.1146591
10. Joshi G, Faraone SV, Wozniak J, Tarko L, Fried R, Galdo M, et al. Symptom profile of ADHD in youth with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder: A comparative study in psychiatrically referred populations. J Attention Disord. (2014) 21:846–55. doi: 10.1177/1087054714543368
11. Leitner Y. The co-occurrence of autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children - what do we know? Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:268. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00268
12. Matsuura N, Ishitobi M, Arai S, Kawamura K, Asano M, Inohara K, et al. Distinguishing between autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder by using behavioral checklists, cognitive assessments, and neuropsychological test battery. Asian J Psychiatry. (2014) 12:50–7. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2014.06.011
13. Craig F, Lamanna AL, Margari F, Matera E, Simone M, and Margari L. Overlap between autism spectrum disorders and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: searching for distinctive/common clinical features. Autism Res. (2015) 8:328–37. doi: 10.1002/aur.1449
14. Kentrou V, de Veld DMJ, Mataw KJK, and Begeer S. Delayed autism spectrum disorder recognition in children and adolescents previously diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Autism. (2018) 23:1065–72. doi: 10.1177/1362361318785171
15. Miodovnik A, Harstad E, Sideridis G, and Huntington N. Timing of the diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics. (2015) 136:e830–e7. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-1502
16. Visser JC, Rommelse NNJ, Greven CU, and Buitelaar JK. Autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in early childhood: A review of unique and shared characteristics and developmental antecedents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 65:229–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.019
17. Rong Y, Yang C-J, Jin Y, and Wang Y. Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2021) 83:101759. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2021.101759
18. Stephenson KG, Beck JS, South M, Norris M, and Butter E. Validity of the WISC-V in youth with autism spectrum disorder: factor structure and measurement invariance. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2021) 50:669–81. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2020.1846543
19. Kanai C, Hashimoto R, Itahashi T, Tani M, Yamada T, Ota H, et al. Cognitive profiles of adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder and those with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder based on the WAIS-III. Res Dev Disabil. (2017) 61:108–15. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.12.008
20. Kim H and Song DH. Comparison of the K-WISC-IV profiles of boys with autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Res Dev Disabil. (2020) 97:103539. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2019.103539
21. Mouga S, Café C, Almeida J, Marques C, Duque F, and Oliveira G. Intellectual profiles in the autism spectrum and other neurodevelopmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2016) 46:2940–55. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2838-x
22. Scheirs JGM and Timmers EA. Differentiating among children with PDD-NOS, ADHD, and those with a combined diagnosis on the basis of WISC-III profiles. J Autism Dev Disord. (2009) 39:549–56. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0657-4
23. Koyama T, Tachimori H, Osada H, and Kurita H. Cognitive and symptom profiles in high-functioning pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2006) 36:373–80. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0075-4
24. Doyle N. Neurodiversity at work: a biopsychosocial model and the impact on working adults. Br Med Bull. (2020) 135:108–25. doi: 10.1093/bmb/ldaa021
25. Doyle N. Defining Neurodiversity and Identifying Neurominorities. In: Patton E and Santuzzi AM, editors. Neurodiversity and Work: Employment, Identity, and Support Networks for Neurominorities. Springer Nature Switzerland, Cham (2024). p. 13–38.
26. Lin L, Zhang Z, Dai M, Luo M, Liang J, Li X, et al. Test analysis of intelligence structure and characteristics in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Chin J Child Health Care. (2018) 26:352–6. doi: 10.11852/zgetbjzz2018-26-04-02
27. Minshew NJ, Turner CA, and Goldstein G. The application of short forms of the Wechsler Intelligence scales in adults and children with high functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2005) 35:45–52. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-1030-x
28. Yaoxuan Gong TC. Chinese revised wechsler intelligence scale for children (C-WISC). Chin J Clin Psychol. (1994) 2:1–6. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.1994.01.001
29. Ousley O and Cermak T. Autism spectrum disorder: defining dimensions and subgroups. Curr Dev Disord Rep. (2013) 1:20–8. doi: 10.1007/s40474-013-0003-1
30. Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5: American psychiatric association;. American Psychiatric Publishing Company. (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
31. Chen H. Application of Bland-Altman analysis in the evaluation of consistency of clinical measurement methods. China Health Statistics. (2007) 24:308–9. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1002-3674.2007.03.029
32. Bing-rui Z, Qiong X, Ping L, and Xiu X. Evaluation on reliability and validity of Chinese version Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Module-1 and clinical application. Chin J evidence based pediatrics. (2013) 8:257–61. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1673-5501.2013.04.004
33. Chlebowski C, Green JA, Barton ML, and Fein D. Using the childhood autism rating scale to diagnose autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2010) 40:787–99. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0926-x
34. Sun X, Allison C, Auyeung B, Zhang Z, Matthews FE, Baron-Cohen S, et al. Validation of existing diagnosis of autism in mainland China using standardised diagnostic instruments. Autism. (2015) 19:1010–7. doi: 10.1177/1362361314556785
35. Barbaresi W, Cacia J, Friedman S, Fussell J, Hansen R, Hofer J, et al. Clinician diagnostic certainty and the role of the autism diagnostic observation schedule in autism spectrum disorder diagnosis in young children. JAMA Pediatrics. (2022) 176:1233–41. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2022.3605
36. Langberg JM, Vaughn AJ, Brinkman WB, Froehlich T, and Epstein JN. Clinical utility of the vanderbilt ADHD rating scale for ruling out comorbid learning disorders. Pediatrics. (2010) 126:e1033–e8. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1267
37. Becker SP, Langberg JM, Vaughn AJ, and Epstein JN. Clinical utility of the vanderbilt ADHD diagnostic parent rating scale comorbidity screening scales. J Dev Behav Pediatrics. (2012) 33:221–8. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e318245615b
38. Jun G, Dan X, Yunyun X, Shipu Z, Weijun L, Xia Z, et al. Reliability and validity of the vanderbilt attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder parent rating scale. Capital Food Med. (2024) 31:33–5. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1005-8257.2024.14.012
39. Aldridge FJ, Gibbs VM, Schmidhofer K, and Williams M. Investigating the clinical usefulness of the social responsiveness scale (SRS) in a tertiary level, autism spectrum disorder specific assessment clinic. J Autism Dev Disord. (2011) 42:294–300. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1242-9
40. Chen K-L, Lin C-H, Yu T-Y, Huang C-Y, and Chen Y-D. Differences between the childhood autism rating scale and the social responsiveness scale in assessing symptoms of children with autistic spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2018) 48:3191–8. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3585-y
41. Constantino JN. Social Responsiveness Scale. In: Volkmar FR, editor. Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Springer International Publishing, Cham (2021). p. 4457–67.
42. Gong J, Zou S, Liu D, Li W, Zhong Y, QIU X, et al. Reliability and validity of social responsiveness scale. Med Innovation China. (2019) 16:19–23. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1674-4985.2019.01.005
43. Zhang Z-X and Lei Z-W. Re-standardization of the “Infant-junior high school student social life ability scale. J Chin Clin Psychol. (1995) 1):12–5. doi: CNKI:SUN:ZLCY.0.1995-01-002
44. Yuan J, Song J, Zhu D, Sun E, Xia L, Zhang X, et al. Lithium treatment is safe in children with intellectual disability. Front Mol Neurosci. (2018) 11:425. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2018.00425
45. Lin L, Dai M, Xiao Q, Liang J, Liu B, and Jing J. Test analysis of intelligence structure and characteristics in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Chin J Child Health Care. (2016) 24:906–9. doi: 10.11852/zgetbjzz2016-24-09-04
46. Oliveras-Rentas RE, Kenworthy L, Roberson RB, Martin A, and Wallace GL. WISC-IV profile in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: impaired processing speed is associated with increased autism communication symptoms and decreased adaptive communication abilities. J Autism Dev Disord. (2011) 42:655–64. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1289-7
47. Werling DM and Geschwind DH. Sex differences in autism spectrum disorders. Curr Opin neurol. (2013) 26:146–53. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32835ee548
48. Esposito S, Tong F, and Fu T. Meta-analysis of fluid intelligence tests of children from the Chinese mainland with learning difficulties. PloS One. (2013) 8:e78311. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078311.g003
49. Planche P and Lemonnier E. Children with high-functioning autism and Asperger's syndrome: Can we differentiate their cognitive profiles? Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2012) 6:939–48. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.12.009
50. Kuschner ES, Bennetto L, and Yost K. Patterns of nonverbal cognitive functioning in young children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2006) 37:795–807. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0209-8
51. Joseph RM, Tager-Flusberg H, and Lord C. Cognitive profiles and social-communicative functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2002) 43:807–21. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00092
52. Tang HL and Shan YF. Differences in intelligence level and structure in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Appl Pediatrics. (2001) 16:140–1. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1003-515X.2001.03.008
53. Berenguer-Forner C, Miranda-Casas A, Pastor-Cerezuela G, and Rosello-Miranda R. Comorbidity of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit with hyperactivity. A review study. Rev neurologia. (2015) 60:S37–43. doi: 10.33588/rn.60S01.2014559
54. Minshew NJ, Muenz LR, Goldstein G, and Payton JB. Neuropsychological functioning in nonmentally retarded autistic individuals. J Clin Exp neuropsychol. (1992) 14:749–61. doi: 10.1080/01688639208402860
55. Christoforou M, Jones EJ, White P, and Charman T. Executive function profiles of preschool children with autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review. JCPP advances. (2023) 3:e12123. doi: 10.1002/jcv2.12123
56. Hervey AS, Epstein JN, and Curry JF. Neuropsychology of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. (2004) 18:485–503. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.485
57. Nydén A, Niklasson L, Stahlberg O, Anckarsater H, Wentz E, Rastam M, et al. Adults with autism spectrum disorders and ADHD neuropsychological aspects. Res Dev Disabil. (2010) 31:1659–68. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.04.010
58. Charman T, Pickles A, Simonoff E, Chandler S, Loucas T, and Baird G. IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders: data from the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). psychol Med. (2010) 41:619–27. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710000991
59. Wilson AC. Cognitive profile in autism and ADHD: A meta-analysis of performance on the WAIS-IV and WISC-V. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2024) 39:498–515. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acad073
60. Sonuga-Barke E and Thapar A. The neurodiversity concept: is it helpful for clinicians and scientists? Lancet Psychiatry. (2021) 8:559–61. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00167-X
61. Warren N, Eatchel B, Kirby A, Diener M, Wright C, and D'Astous V. Parent-identified strengths of autistic youth. Autism: Int J Res Practice. (2020) 25:79–89. doi: 10.1177/1362361320945556
Keywords: autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, Wechsler intelligence scale of children (C-WISC), intellectual structures, comorbidity
Citation: Zhang D-Y, Wei Q-H, Hu B-Y, Ai D, Zhang Y, Ding Y, Yang T, Chen J, Chen L, Wu Y, Chen H-Y, Xiang X-L, Mou Q-H and Li T-Y (2025) The value of intellectual structural imbalance in the differentiation of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Front. Psychiatry 16:1610278. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1610278
Received: 11 April 2025; Accepted: 28 July 2025;
Published: 14 August 2025.
Edited by:
Dongchuan Yu, Southeast University, ChinaReviewed by:
Xia Chi, Nanjing Medical University, ChinaHanyu Dong, First Affiliated Hospital of Jilin University, China
Javier Pérez-Flores, University of La Laguna, Spain
Copyright © 2025 Zhang, Wei, Hu, Ai, Zhang, Ding, Yang, Chen, Chen, Wu, Chen, Xiang, Mou and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Ting-Yu Li, dHlsaUB2aXAuc2luYS5jb20=