Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 17 September 2025

Sec. Anxiety and Stress Disorders

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1613322

This article is part of the Research TopicGenetic and Epigenetic Influences on PTSD Susceptibility and ExpressionView all articles

Influence of genetic polymorphism and trauma on cortical structures and PTSD severity: Imaging genetics generalized structured component analysis

  • 1Division of Liberal Arts, Kongju National University, Gongju, Republic of Korea
  • 2Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, United States
  • 3Clinical Emotion and Cognition Research Laboratory, Inje University, Goyang, Republic of Korea
  • 4Department of Psychology, Ajou University, Suwon, Republic of Korea
  • 5Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 6Institute for Hope Research, Sogang University, Seoul, Republic of Korea
  • 7Department of Psychiatry, Inje University, Ilsan Paik Hospital, Goyang, Republic of Korea

Objective: The changes in brain structures affected by potentially traumatic events (PTEs) and polymorphisms of various genes are associated with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Our study investigated the pathophysiology of PTSD along with PTEs, genes, and brain regions of interest (ROIs) via imaging genetics generalized structured component analysis (IG-GSCA).

Methods: A total of 231 participants (137 healthy volunteers and 94 PTSD patients) were enrolled. We performed T1-weighted structural magnetic resonance imaging, genotyping for nine genes (SLC6A4, FKBP5, ADCYAP1R1, BDNF, COMT, HTR3A, DRD2, NR3C1, and OXTR), and psychological assessments measuring PTEs, PTSD symptoms, and alcohol problems. Genes, PTEs, and their interactions were set as predictors for volumes of 60 brain ROIs, and volumes of the 60 ROIs were set as predictors for the PTSD severity, implying that volumes of brain ROIs were set to mediate the effects of genes and PTEs on the PTSD severity.

Results: Our results suggested that HTR3A was related to the volume of the anterior cingulate gyrus and NR3C1 was related to the volume of the central operculum. Also, volumes of the central operculum, occipital fusiform gyrus, and anterior cingulate gyrus were negatively associated with the severity of PTSD, while PTEs were positively associated with PTSD severity.

Conclusions: This study is one of the few that examined the relationships between various factors related to PTSD symptom severity, including genetics, environment, gene-environment interactions, and brain regions of interest (ROIs), all within a single model. The findings indicated mediating pathways from the HTR3A gene polymorphism to PTSD symptom severity through the volume of the anterior cingulate gyrus, and from the NR3C1 gene polymorphism to PTSD symptom severity via the volume of the central operculum. However, only the indirect effect involving NR3C1 was statistically significant. Additionally, the study found a significant association between the occipital fusiform gyrus and PTSD symptom severity.

Introduction

Researchers studying mental illness have focused on discovering the pathological mechanisms or biological pathways that could lead to the onset of a mental illness. A recent study reported that differences in the brain seem to be associated with the general risk for mental illness, and detecting these brain differences could help clinicians evaluate an individual’s general risk for developing a mental illness (1). In particular, there is abundant evidence for decreased brain volume associated with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), including the amygdala (2), insula (3, 4), anterior cingulate cortex (5), medial prefrontal cortex (4), and hippocampus (6).

People may develop PTSD after experiencing potentially traumatic events (PTEs) (7). Since PTEs may occur at different developmental stages of life, including the early childhood period in which brain development could happen, it is inevitable that those PTEs will presumably have differential impacts on the brain changes as well as the psychological traits of an individual (8). This could explain why not all individuals exposed to PTEs were diagnosed with lifetime PTSD; in a United States sample of 2,953 respondents, 82.7% were exposed to PTEs, whereas only 8.3% of those exposed to the trauma were diagnosed with lifetime PTSD (9). Therefore, the brain’s change might be a significant mediator between PTEs and PTSD symptoms. Notably, recent theoretical models propose that PTSD symptoms, such as hypervigilance, intrusive recollection, and exaggerated startle, may have evolved as adaptive responses to extreme threat, enhancing immediate survival but becoming maladaptive when prolonged or context-inappropriate (10, 11).

The change of brain structures seems to mediate not only the effects of environmental factors (e.g., PTEs) but also those of genes and their interactions on PTSD. Recent studies (12, 13) suggest that the change of brain structures could mediate the structural pathways from genes (14, 15) and environments (16, 17) to psychiatric disorders, including PTSD. For example, genetic predisposition and early environmental insults affect brain changes, which later may influence the onset of schizophrenia (18) and bipolar disorder (19, 20). Also, one study revealed that childhood PTEs and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) polymorphism interacted to affect the cortical thickness of the left fusiform gyrus and the left transverse temporal, which in turn, were negatively associated with PTSD symptoms (21).

The gene–environment (G×E) framework has become central to contemporary PTSD research, particularly as investigations into the role of genetics and epigenetics in PTSD pathophysiology have advanced. Two recent comprehensive reviews (22, 23) synthesize findings from genome‐wide and candidate‐gene studies, highlighting nine loci that consistently demonstrate replication across independent cohorts and functional relevance to stress‐response pathways. In addition to those comprehensive review studies, PTSD is found to be associated with many genes and their genotypes (21, 24), including serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) (25, 26), FK506 binding protein 51 (FKBP5) (27, 28), adenylate-cyclase activating polypeptide 1 (ADCYAP1R1) (29, 30), brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (31, 32), catechol-O-methyltransferase gene (COMT) (33, 34), 5-hydroxytryptamine receptor 3A (HTR3A) (35, 36), dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) (37, 38), nuclear receptor subfamily 3 group C member 1 (NR3C1) (39, 40), and the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) (41, 42). Although genetic predispositions are expected to affect volumetric changes in the brain, leading to changes in PTSD symptoms, few studies have investigated which brain regions mediate the effects of genes on PTSD symptoms. Similarly, there is a lack of studies examining the brain regions that could mediate the pathways from environmental factors to PTSD symptoms.

Taken together, there are many individual findings about the relationships between PTEs, genes, decreased brain volume, and PTSD symptoms. To our knowledge, however, no studies have examined the pathophysiology of PTSD in a single model that encompasses all of those antecedent variables for PTSD. Therefore, this study aimed to specify and test the relationships among various genes, environmental factors, decreased volume of brain regions of interest (ROIs), and PTSD symptoms in an integrative manner via imaging genetics generalized structured component analysis (IG-GSCA) (12, 43) that could influence brain volumes simultaneously for explaining the variations of PTSD.

Methods

Participants

A total of 255 Korean volunteers were included in this study. Twenty-four participants were excluded due to missing values for some psychological measures and brain images, which left a final sample of 231. Healthy participants numbered 137 (59.3%), recruited from community advertisements, and patients with PTSD numbered 94 (40.7%) and recruited from notices on the bulletin board in the hospital. The PTSD patients reported their traumatic events as follows: 63 (67.02%) severe motor vehicle accidents, 9 (9.57%) defections from North Korea, 8 (8.51%) physical or sexual violence, 8 (8.51%) relationship issues, 5 (5.32%) death of a family member, and 1 (1.06%) fire. We mixed PTSD patients and healthy participants with PTE experiences in the final sample to make data on PTEs and PTSD symptoms normally distributed and to investigate the phenomenon at the level of the general population.

The total participants consisted of 75 (32.5%) men and 156 (67.5%) women, with a mean age of 46.13 years (standard deviation, SD = 13.51). The mean years of education were 13.04 (SD = 3.27). Each participant signed a written informed consent form before participating in the study. All the experimental protocols were approved by the Institutional Review Board at Inje University Ilsan Paik Hospital (IRB no. 2015 - 07-025). All measurements and experiments were carried out in accordance with the guidelines and regulations of the board and the Declaration of Helsinki.

Psychological measures

The Korean-validated version of the Life Events Checklist (LEC) was used to assess the experience of PTEs (44). The LEC comprises 17 items about PTEs, and the responses include experiencing, witnessing, and learning about a PTE. This study analyzed responses to experiencing PTE, since recalling witnessing or learning about it throughout their lifetime seemed difficult and confusing to some respondents. For each of the 17 items, responses were coded as 1 if the event was experienced and 0 if not, and the sum of these scores was used in subsequent analyses.

To examine the severity of PTSD symptoms, we administered the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM - 5 (CAPS - 5) (45). It consists of 30 items and provides information about the frequency and severity of PTSD symptoms. The severity of PTSD symptoms is rated from 0 (“absent”) to 4 (“extreme/incapacitating”). The total severity score was obtained by adding up all the item scores for each individual. A licensed psychiatrist measured PTEs using LEC and PTSD symptom severity using CAPS through a 1:1 interview, and licensed nurses or a clinical psychologist observed the entire process to secure reliability.

To adjust for the possible confounding effect of alcohol problems from other variables, the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) was used to assess alcohol consumption, drinking behaviors, and alcohol-related problems as a covariate. The AUDIT is a 10-item screening tool developed by the World Health Organization and is well-validated in the Korean population (46). The AUDIT is assessed on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (“never”) to 4 (“4 or more times a week”).

DNA genotyping

We used the same DNA genotyping process as in previous studies (12, 21). Participants had their blood sampled to extract DNA using a NanoDrop® ND - 1000 UV-Vis Spectrophotometer. Then, genomic DNA was diluted to a 5 ng/µl concentration on 96-well polymerase chain reaction (PCR) plates. TaqMan SNP genotyping assays were obtained from Applied Biosystems. Afterward, the probes were labeled with either the FAM or the VIC dye at the 5’ end and with a minor-groove binder and a non-fluorescent quencher at the 3’ end. PCR was performed in 5 μl of the mixture containing 2 μl of a DNA sample, 0.125 μl of each TaqMan™ SNP Genotyping Assay (Thermo Fisher Scientific, USA), 2.5 μl of the TaqMan™ Genotyping Master Mix (Thermo Fisher Scientific, USA), and 0.375 μl of distilled water. Amplification and detection were carried out with a detection system (QuantStudio 12K Flex Real-Time PCR System, Thermo Fisher Scientific, USA), using the following profile: 50 °C for 2 min, 95 °C for 10 min, followed by 60 cycles of 95 °C for 15 sec, and 60 °C for 1 min. After the PCR amplification, allelic discrimination (an endpoint plate read) was performed on the same machines (the QuantStudio 12K Flex Real-Time PCR System). The QuantStudio 12K Flex Software calculated the fluorescence measured during the plate read and plotted Rn values based on the signals coming from each well. Subsequently, automatic or manual allele calls were performed on the analyzed plates. Three positive samples and one negative control sample were present on each plate. We confirmed the clustering image with positive controls. Intra-genomic DNA (gDNA) samples of known genotypes were used for the positive control. Guided by two recent comprehensive studies (22, 23) and other multiple empirical reports, we therefore selected the following nine genes (18 SNPs) for inclusion in our IG‐GSCA model: SLC6A4, FKBP5, ADCYAP1R1, BDNF, COMT, HTR3A, DRD2, NR3C1, and OXTR. A total of nine genes with 18 SNPs were analyzed and are described in Table 1. For further analyses, the number of minor alleles was coded for each genotype: 0 for wild types with zero minor alleles, 1 for hetero types with one minor allele, and 2 for mutant types with two minor alleles.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. List of genes and their SNP indicators included in analyses.

MRI acquisition and processing and voxel-based morphometry

MRI was performed using a 1.5 T scanner (Magneton Avanto, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). Head motion was minimized with restraining foam pads provided by the manufacturer. High-resolution T1-weighted MRI images were acquired with the acquisition parameters of a 227 × 384 acquisition matrix, a 210 × 250 field-of-view, 0.9 × 0.7 × 1.2 voxel size, a total of 87,168 voxels, a TE of 3.42 ms, a TR of 1,900 ms, 1.2-mm slice thickness, and a flip angle of 15°.

We used the same MRI process as the one used in the previous study (47). Images were inspected visually for motion or other artifacts before and after preprocessing. The voxel-based volumetry (VBM) was conducted using CAT12 (http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat/) implemented in SPM12 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK). SPM12 tissue probability maps were used for the initial spatial registration. The structural T1 images were regularized with an ICBM East Asian template and normalized using the DARTEL algorithm (48). The images were then segmented into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid (49). Jacobian-transformed tissue probability maps were used to modulate images. The volume of the brain parts was extracted using the Neuromorphometric atlas (http://neuromorphometrics.com/) from 142 parts from 67 ROIs. Divided by a subject’s total brain volume, the extracted volumes of the brain parts were transformed into the relative sizes of volumes to adjust for individual differences in overall brain volume (50). Among the 67 ROIs in total, we excluded seven subcortical ROIs that were far from cortical function related to PTSD; cerebral exterior for it is considered residual non‐brain tissue from skull‐stripping (51), cerebellar vermis, brain stem, and optic chiasm since they have not shown consistent volumetric abnormalities in PTSD voxel‐based morphometry meta‐analyses (52), cerebellar and cerebral white matter for they are conceptually distinct from the gray‐matter volumetric framework (53), and cerebrospinal fluid since it is considered as an indirect marker of brain atrophy rather than as a mediator in structural models (54). A total of 60 remaining ROIs as potential mediators for the pathways from the nine genes to PTSD in the model. The list of the 60 ROIs and the means and SDs of their volumes are provided in Table 2.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean and standard deviation of volumes (cm3) of ROIs and their indicators included in the analysis.

Statistical analysis–IG-GSCA

We used a component-based structural equation modeling technique specifically developed for imaging genetics research, called Imaging Genetics Generalized Structured Component Analysis (IG-GSCA) (12, 43). IG-GSCA enables researchers to statistically model and test path-analytic relationships among various constructs, representing those constructs as weighted composites of observed variables—termed components. For instance, a gene can be viewed a genetic construct composed of multiple SNPs located within the gene (55), whereas a brain ROI can be seen as an imaging construct comprising the voxels within the ROI (56). IG-GSCA generates a component for each gene and ROI from the relevant SNPs and brain voxels, respectively, and specifies biologically plausible pathways between genes, ROIs, and behavioral phenotypes within a single modeling framework. It also accommodates various well-documented interactions between constructs (57) in imaging genetics studies, such as gene-gene interactions (epistasis; G×G) (58) or gene-environment interaction (G×E) (59). The observed genetic or imaging variables used to define are referred to as indicators of their respective constructs.

Unlike univariate approaches—which estimate parameters for each equation separately and thus require post hoc corrections (e.g., Bonferroni, Šidák, Benjamini–Hochberg) to control the family‐wise error—IG-GSCA jointly estimates all parameters within a unified multivariate model. This simultaneous estimation accounts for shared variance and inherently mitigates Type I error inflation without the need for external corrections. Moreover, it enables IG-GSCA to statistically test mediation effects, such as whether ROIs mediate the relationship between genes and behavioral phenotypes. These features make IG-GSCA especially well-suited for imaging genetics studies that examine pathways from genetic variations to changes in brain structure to behavioral outcomes (60).

Lastly, IG-GSCA incorporates regularization techniques (61) to address multicollinearity, a common issue in imaging genetics (62) For example, neighboring genes often exhibit high correlations, which may cause multicollinearity when included simultaneously as predictors for the same outcome. In general, multicollinearity leads to unstable parameter estimates with inflated standard errors, especially in studies with small sample sizes. Regularization helps prevents overfitting to sample-specific noise, yielding more stable estimates and better generalizability. Given our hypothesized model includes a large number of genetic and brain-imaging variables as well as their interaction terms —relative to a modest sample size (N = 231), we employed IG-GSCA with regularization. A simulation study using a model of comparable complexity (e.g., nine-gene components, nine G×E interactions, 60 ROI components, and one outcome) demonstrates IGSCA performs as expected with a sample size of N = 250 (12).

Model specification

Since recent studies suggested that the change of brain structures could mediate the pathways from genes and environmental factors to psychiatric disorders (12), we hypothesized a structural model where brain ROIs with cortical structural features mediate the effects of genes and PTEs on PTSD symptom severity. A total of nine genes with 18 SNPs were selected based on their relevance to PTSD (25, 27, 29, 31, 33, 36, 37, 39, 41). The number of minor alleles was coded for each genotype: 0 for wild types with zero minor alleles, 1 for hetero types with one minor allele, and 2 for mutant types with two minor alleles for further analyses. Also, the interaction effects of genes and PTEs were additionally considered based on the previous findings (63, 64). Among the 67 ROIs from the whole brain, we excluded seven subcortical ROIs that are far from cortical function related to PTSD (i.e., cerebral exterior, cerebellar vermis, brain stem, optic chiasm, cerebellar and cerebral white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid) from the model, leaving a total of 60 ROIs as mediating variables in the model. In addition, we set age, gender, and alcohol-related problems measured with AUDIT as covariates to control for possible contaminating effects (6568). In all, we contemplated the structural model involving the pathways from nine genes and PTEs to 60 brain ROIs, the paths from 60 brain ROIs and PTEs to PTSD symptom severity, and the paths from three covariates (gender, age, and AUDIT score) to 60 brain ROIs and PTSD symptom severity. The total number of pathways specified in the structural model was 1,384. The structural model is displayed in Figure 1, in which each individual pathway between two constructs, termed a path coefficient parameter, is symbolized by an arrow between the two constructs.

Figure 1
Diagram illustrating the interaction between genes, brain regions (ROIs), and factors like age, gender, and alcohol on PTSD. Genes and gene-environment interactions influence multiple ROIs, which further connect to the PTSD outcome. Factors, including potential traumatic events (PTE), modify these interactions.

Figure 1. Hypothesized model specification. Hexagons represent constructs, whereas squares are observed variables. PTE, potentially traumatic event; ROI, regions of interest; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder.

Results

Descriptive statistics

The demographic and psychological characteristics of healthy and PTSD participants are presented in Table 3. The two groups did not show statistically significant gender differences (t = 1.642, p = 0.252), age difference (t = 1.309, p = 0.192), or AUDIT (t = -1.144, p = 0.254), whereas their PTE and PTSD scores were significantly different (t = -4.823, p < 0.001 for PTEs; t = -13.348, p < 0.001 for PTSD). Meanwhile, all variables, including both LEC scores and PTSD symptom severity scores, were normally distributed even for the total pool of participants since each skewness score was below 2.0 and each kurtosis score was below 7.0.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparison of demographic, psychological, and behavioral characteristics in participants.

Model analysis

We tested an IG-GSCA model in which nine candidate genes (18 SNPs), their interactions with PTEs, and three covariates (age, gender, AUDIT score) were linked to PTSD symptom severity via 60 cortical ROIs. In total, the model specified 1,384 pathways, and its structure is shown in Figure 1.

Each set of indicators for genes and ROIs is described in Tables 1 and 2. The number of SNP indicators per gene ranged from 1 to 8. Each ROI construct was composed of the corresponding ROIs on both brain hemispheres except the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). The three sub-parts (e.g., opercular, orbital, and triangular) of IFG on each side of the brain were used as indicators of IFG, as indicated in the Neuromorphometric Atlas. The constructs for G×E interaction did not have indicators since they were defined as products of a pair of genes and PTE.

IG-GSCA results

We fitted our hypothesized IG-GSCA model to data via GSCA Pro (69). We adopted ridge-regularization for the structural part of our model and considered values between 0 and 1,000 as candidate values of the ridge tuning parameter λ. We selected λ = 62 based on 10 × 5 fold cross-validation. We employed 5,000 bootstrap samples to estimate standard errors and 95% confidence intervals for the parameter estimates.

We calculated the values of three goodness-of-fit measures: FIT, the goodness-of-fit index (GFI), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). FIT is computed based on the residuals for components and indicators, whereas GFI and SRMR are calculated based on the discrepancy between the sample and reproduced covariance matrices of indicators. A higher value of FIT or GFI indicates a better fit, whereas a lower value of SRMR suggests a better fit. IG-GSCA provided the FIT value of 0.640, indicating that the model explained 64.0% of the total variance of all the components and indicators in the model. It also yielded a GFI value of 0.989, which is far greater than the recommended cutoff criterion (0.93) (70). Lastly, it produced the SRMR value of 0.04, which is much smaller than the recommended cutoff criterion (0.08) (70).

Table 4 presents standardized loading estimates, which can be interpreted as the correlations between constructs and their indicators. All the loading estimates were statistically significant, and their effect sizes were large (i.e., at least larger than 0.7), indicating that components were constructed to well explain the variations of their indicators. The average R2 for all the indicators was 0.87.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The estimates of loadings and their standard errors and 95% confidence intervals.

Among 187 significant path coefficients, we paid most attention to reporting and interpreting the statistically significant path coefficient estimates from ROIs to the severity of PTSD symptoms and the ones from genes or PTEs to ROIs, since this study aimed to identify the mediating pathways from genes or PTEs to ROIs to the severity of PTSD symptoms. Figure 2 presents the mediating model, and Table 5 provides the standardized estimates for those path coefficients. The estimates of the entire path coefficients are presented in Supplementary Material.

Figure 2
Diagram showing connections between genes NR3C1 and HTR3A, brain regions, and PTSD. Arrows indicate pathways with values: NR3C1 to central operculum, occipital fusiform, anterior cingulate gyri; and PTSD with connections to PTE. Values include .09, -.11, -.13, .10, and .35.

Figure 2. The estimates for the path coefficients that form statistically significant linkages from genes and PTE to ROIs to PTSD symptom severity. PTE, potentially traumatic event; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. The estimates for the path coefficients that form statistically significant linkages from genes and PTE to ROIs to PTSD symptom severity, and their standard errors and 95% confidence intervals (direct effects only).

For the relationships between brain ROIs and the severity of PTSD, the severity of PTSD turned out to be negatively associated with three brain ROIs—central operculum (b = -0.11, SE = 0.04, 95% CI = [-0.18, -0.00]), occipital fusiform gyrus (b = -0.13, SE = 0.05, 95% CI = [-0.21, -0.03]), and anterior cingulate gyrus (b = -0.10, SE = 0.05, 95% CI = [-0.19, -0.00])1, suggesting that people with smaller volume in those ROIs had more susceptible to the severity of PTSD symptoms. The PTEs also had a negative association with the severity of PTSD symptoms (b = 0.35, SE = 0.04, 95% CI = [0.26, 0.41]) even after the effects of ROIs and covariates were manipulated. It would imply that the traumatic experiences had a direct effect on the severity of PTSD symptoms even after adjusting the effects of covariates and decreasing the volume of ROIs. Figure 3 shows three brain areas significantly mediated between genes and PTSD symptom severity.

Figure 3
Illustration of a human brain highlighting specific regions. The left image shows the anterior cingulate gyrus and occipital fusiform gyrus. The right image highlights the central operculum.

Figure 3. Three brain areas significantly mediated between genes and PTSD symptom severity.

In the pathways from genes to the three brain ROIs above, HTR3A had a negative influence on the anterior cingulate gyrus (b = -0.11, SE = 0.04, 95% CI = [-0.18, -0.03]), indicating that people with a more minor allele in HTR3A were likely to have a smaller anterior cingulate gyrus. However, the indirect effect of HTR3A on the severity of PTSD symptoms through the anterior cingulate gyrus was not statistically significant (b = 0.01, SE = 0.01, 95% CI = [-0.00, 0.02]). On the other hand, the effect of NR3C1 on the central operculum was positive (b = 0.09, SE = 0.03, 95% CI = [0.02, 0.15]), implying that people with a mutant allele in NR3C1 had a larger central operculum. Moreover, NR3C1 had an indirect effect on the severity of PTSD symptoms mediated through the central operculum (b = -0.01, SE = 0.01, 95% CI = [-0.02, -0.00]), which means that the mutation of the NR3C1 led people to be less vulnerable to PTSD, though enlarging their central operculum. None of the interaction effects of genes and PTE on the three ROIs were statistically significant. The ratios of the amount of explained variance for each ROI or the severity of PTSD to their total variances are in Table 6 (average R2 = 0.25).

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. R2 for all the dependent constructs, regions of interest (ROIs) and PTSD symptom severity.

Discussion

This study conducted a comprehensive examination of the path-analytic relationships between posttraumatic events, genes, their interactions, brain volumes, and PTSD symptom severity in a single modeling framework via IG-GSCA while controlling for the effects of possibly contaminating variables such as gender, age, and alcohol problems. In grounding these path‐analytic tests, we drew on the classic diathesis–stress model, which holds that genetic risk and environmental exposures (PTEs) jointly influence psychopathology. More recent extensions (7173) further underscore how gene-environment interactions can shape brain structure and function. By embedding our IG‐GSCA interaction terms within this established G×E paradigm, we both structure our hypotheses and sharpen the interpretation of indirect pathways from genes, via brain structural changes, to PTSD symptom severity. Our major findings are threefold, as follows.

First, although the indirect effect of HTR3A on PTSD symptom severity via anterior cingulate volume was not statistically significant, we observed significant individual associations along the pathway. Specifically, the number of minor alleles (T) in the HTR3A gene polymorphism was negatively associated with the anterior cingulate volume, which was also negatively associated with the severity of PTSD symptoms. The HTR3A gene encodes subunit A of the type 3 receptor for serotonin, and the type 3 receptors are largely reported in the anterior cingulate cortex (7476). While some studies suggest the relationship between the CC genotype (the wild type) of HTR3A and increased PTSD symptom severity (77), other opposite studies suggest the relationship between the number of minor alleles T of HTR3A and a higher pain score, which is related to higher distress (78). These contradictory results could be due to other contaminating variables, such as age, gender, ethnicity, and other covariates. In our Korean sample, controlling for age, gender, and alcohol-related problems, we found a significant negative relationship between the number of minor alleles and anterior cingulate volume. The decreased volume of the anterior cingulate, which involves fear-conditioning (79) and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (80), is observed in people with PTSD (8083). Therefore, while the mediation effect was not significant, the directionality of the observed associations is consistent with prior findings and may suggest a potential trend that warrants further investigation in larger samples.

Second, this study also revealed that the central operculum volume acted as a statistical mediator of the association between NR3C1 genotype and PTSD symptom severity. The number of minor alleles (G) in the NR3C1 gene polymorphism was positively associated with the volume of the central operculum, and the central operculum volume was negatively associated with the severity of PTSD symptoms. The NR3C1 gene codes for the glucocorticoid receptor gene, which is known as a key element involved in several steps of HPA axis modulation (84, 85). A previous study found that the presence of the G allele (the minor allele) in rs258747 significantly reduced the risk of PTSD (40). This is in line with our finding that the number of minor alleles in NR3C1 was associated with a decrease in PTSD symptom severity. The central operculum volume mediated this relation; the number of minor alleles was related to an increase in the operculum volume, which was negatively associated with PTSD symptom severity. A study suggested that the operculum region might be related to PTSD (86). From the Neuromorphometric atlas, the central operculum refers to the middle part of the operculum between the anterior limit of the insula and the posterior limit of the ventral bank of the ascending ramus of the posterior lateral sulcus above the circular sulcus of the insula. In other words, the central operculum is the region near the insula that is functionally connected to other regions such as the insula, anterior cingulate cortex, and thalamus. This functional network of brain regions is called the “cingulo-opercular network.” (87, 88) Since the cingulo-opercular network is known to be engaged in dealing with alertness, it could be related to the NR3C1 gene involved in HPA axis modulation, as in our result.

Third, in addition to the volume of the central operculum and anterior cingulate gyrus, the volume of the occipital fusiform gyrus showed significantly negative relations with the severity of PTSD as well. According to the Neuromorphometric Atlas used in this study, the region from the occipitotemporal and collateral sulci anterior to the antero-medial limit of the ventral bank of the parietooccipital sulcus is labeled the fusiform gyrus, and the region from these sulci that lies posterior to the antero-medial limit of the parietooccipital sulcus is labeled the occipital fusiform gyrus. The occipital fusiform gyrus is known to be associated with the recall of traumatic autobiographical episodes (89), and PTSD patients had a lower volume of occipital fusiform gyrus compared to healthy controls (90). Our result is in line with those of previous studies. However, none of the genes showed significant relationships with the occipital fusiform gyrus volume. Thus, the occipital fusiform gyrus would not mediate between gene or PTE expressions and PTSD symptom severity.

However, this study has some limitations. First, our IG-GSCA model explained only approximately 28% of the variance in PTSD symptom severity. This suggests that important variables may underlie the observed variation, such as other psychiatric comorbidities like anxiety, depression, bipolar disorder (91, 92). Future studies should consider incorporating these additional variables to improve the model’s explanatory power.

Second, the number of participants in our study was relatively small (N = 231) compared to the number of parameters in the structural model (number of paths = 1,384). Although IG-GSCA has been shown to recover parameters well even with small sample sizes (12), the statistical power to detect all true effects remains limited. For instance, only a few G×E interaction terms reached statistical significance (e.g., PTE × BDNF on MPrG). This pattern may reflect insufficient power to detect such interaction effects, rather than a lack of true underlying associations. Studies with larger sample sizes would be valuable for cross-validating our findings.

Third, our findings are insufficient to make strong causal claims for the hypothesized pathways between variables. While this study proposed a model in which brain structures mediate the effects of genes and an environmental factor on PTSD symptom—and found empirical support for this structure—it does not rule out alternative causal models. For example, PTSD symptom severity could plausibly influence brain structure rather than the reverse. Additionally, potential biases introduced by the regularization applied in this study may obscure true causal relationships, despite improving the model’s predictive generalizability. Therefore, researchers should be cautious in interpreting the observed associations as causal. Further studies that also model reverse pathways from PTSD symptom severity to genetic and neurobiological markers may help clarify the complex relationships among PTSD, genes, and brain structure.

Fourth, the participants of this study consisted of mixed samples of PTSD patients (n = 94) and healthy controls (n = 137). Although this mixing was intended to assemble a cohort broadly representative of the general population, it likely diluted group‐specific differences in brain structure and may have reduced power to detect some gene–environment–brain pathways that are unique to PTSD. Future studies should recruit larger, more homogeneous PTSD cohorts for primary analyses, and when mixed samples are unavoidable, conduct pre-specified subgroup or interaction tests (e.g., including diagnostic status as a moderator) to uncover disorder‐specific effects of genes and PTEs on brain structure.

Fifth, the PTEs score was assessed only for those who experienced traumatic events, excluding those they witnessed or learned about, because witnessing or learning scores seemed unreliable for some participants in this study. However, exposure through witnessing or learning can also contribute to PTSD symptomatology. Future studies should consider developing or utilizing more reliable assessment tools for indirect trauma exposure to examine their distinct effects on brain structure and symptom severity.

Lastly, we lacked detailed clinical-status data, such as standardized diagnostic procedures, time since PTSD onset, and treatment history, which may influence both brain structures and symptom severity. Future studies should include comprehensive clinical assessments to determine how diagnostic method, illness duration, and treatment history modulate the associations among genes, brain structure, and PTSD symptoms.

Nonetheless, this study is among the few that attempted to simultaneously examine the relationships among a wide range of variables related to PTSD symptom severity—genetic variants, an environmental exposure, gene-environment interactions, and brain ROIs, simultaneously—within a single integrative model. Our findings revealed potential mediating pathways from HTR3A polymorphism to the anterior cingulate gyrus volume to PTSD symptom severity and from NR3C1 polymorphism to the central operculum volume to PTSD symptom severity, though only the latter indirect effect reached statistical significance. In addition, we identified a direct association between the occipital fusiform gyrus with PTSD symptom severity. These results emphasized the importance of holistically investigating the biological pathways underlying PTSD symptoms. In particular, detecting stress-related brain changes may inform clinicians’ evaluation of both risk and compensatory adaptation across the PTSD trajectory. We believe that our findings could serve as a stepping stone for future studies aiming to elucidate the broader pathophysiological mechanisms of PTSD.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because of the confidentiality regulations outlined in the Bioethics & Safety Act and the Personal Information Protection Act. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Institutional Review Board at Inje University Ilsan Paik Hospital (IRB no. 2015-07-025). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

MJ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft. GC: Writing – original draft, Software, Methodology, Writing – review & editing, Visualization, Formal Analysis. HJ: Writing – review & editing, Data curation, Methodology. YC: Methodology, Writing – review & editing, Validation, Formal Analysis. HH: Validation, Formal Analysis, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. SL: Supervision, Software, Project administration, Conceptualization, Resources, Investigation, Funding acquisition, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This research was supported by the National Research Foundation (NRF) funded by the Korean government (MSIT) (No. RS-2024-00455484) and the [K-Brain Project] of the National Research Foundation (NRF) funded by the Korean government (MSIT) (RS-2023-00262568).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1613322/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ The estimate b, SE, and 95% CI denote the estimate of path coefficient, its standard error, and its 95% confidence interval, respectively.

References

1. Elliott ML, Romer A, Knodt AR, and Hariri AR. A connectome-wide functional signature of transdiagnostic risk for mental illness. Biol Psychiatry. (2018) 84:452–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.03.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Kuo JR, Kaloupek DG, and Woodward SH. Amygdala volume in combat-exposed veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder: A cross-sectional study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2012) 69:1080–6. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.73

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Ahmed F, Spottiswoode BS, Carey PD, Stein DJ, and Seedat S. Relationship between neurocognition and regional brain volumes in traumatized adolescents with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychobiology. (2012) 66:174–84. doi: 10.1159/000339558

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Nardo D, Högberg G, Jonsson C, Jacobsson H, Hällström T, and Pagani M. Neurobiology of sleep disturbances in ptsd patients and traumatized controls: mri and spect findings. Front Psychiatry. (2015) 6:134. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00134

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Kühn S and Gallinat J. Gray matter correlates of posttraumatic stress disorder: A quantitative meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. (2013) 73:70–4. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.06.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Woon FL, Sood S, and Hedges DW. Hippocampal volume deficits associated with exposure to psychological trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in adults: A meta-analysis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 34:1181–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.06.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Association AP. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Dsm-5. American Psychiatric A and American Psychiatric Association DSMTF, editors. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

8. Bremner JD. Traumatic stress: effects on the brain. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2006) 8:445. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2006.8.4/jbremner

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kilpatrick DG, Resnick HS, Milanak ME, Miller MW, Keyes KM, and Friedman MJ. National estimates of exposure to traumatic events and ptsd prevalence using dsm-Iv and dsm-5 criteria. J Trauma Stress. (2013) 26:537–47. doi: 10.1002/jts.21848

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. de Kloet ER, Joëls M, and Holsboer F. Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2005) 6:463–75. doi: 10.1038/nrn1683

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Rudzki S. Is ptsd an evolutionary survival adaptation initiated by unrestrained cytokine signaling and maintained by epigenetic change? Military Med. (2022) 188:e1625–e36. doi: 10.1093/milmed/usac095

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hwang H, Cho G, Jin MJ, Ryoo JH, Choi Y, and Lee SH. A knowledge-based multivariate statistical method for examining gene-brain-behavioral/cognitive relationships: imaging genetics generalized structured component analysis. PloS One. (2021) 16:e0247592. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0247592

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Yun J-Y, Jin MJ, Kim S, and Lee S-H. Stress-related cognitive style is related to volumetric change of the hippocampus and Fk506 binding protein 5 polymorphism in post-traumatic stress disorder. psychol Med. (2020) 52(7):1–12. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720002949

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Peper JS, Brouwer RM, Boomsma DI, Kahn RS, and Hulshoff Pol HE. Genetic influences on human brain structure: A review of brain imaging studies in twins. Hum Brain Mapp. (2007) 28:464–73. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20398

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Thompson PM, Cannon TD, Narr KL, Van Erp T, Poutanen V-P, Huttunen M, et al. Genetic influences on brain structure. Nat Neurosci. (2001) 4:1253–8. doi: 10.1038/nn758

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Fox SE, Levitt P, and Nelson CA III. How the timing and quality of early experiences influence the development of brain architecture. Child Dev. (2010) 81:28–40. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01380.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Sale A, Berardi N, and Maffei L. Enrich the environment to empower the brain. Trends Neurosci. (2009) 32:233–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.12.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Tsuang MT, Stone WS, and Faraone SV. Genes, environment and schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. (2001) 178:s18–24. doi: 10.1192/bjp.178.40.s18

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. van der Schot AC, Vonk R, Brans RG, van Haren NE, Koolschijn PCM, Nuboer V, et al. Influence of genes and environment on brain volumes in twin pairs concordant and discordant for bipolar disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2009) 66:142–51. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.541

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Van der Schot AC, Vonk R, Brouwer RM, van Baal GCM, Brans RG, van Haren NE, et al. Genetic and environmental influences on focal brain density in bipolar disorder. Brain. (2010) 133:3080–92. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq236

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Jin MJ, Jeon H, Hyun MH, and Lee S-H. Influence of childhood trauma and brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66met polymorphism on posttraumatic stress symptoms and cortical thickness. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-42563-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Bainomugisa C. The Role of Genetics and Epigenetics in Post-Traumatic Stress and Migraine Disorders Doctoral dissertation, Queensland University of Technology. (2025).

Google Scholar

23. Kuznetsov A, Kokorev D, Sustretov A, Kozlov A, Lyamin A, Sheyfer M, et al. Genetic contributors to ptsd: the role of snvs, gene interactions and haplotypes for developing ptsd prevention measures. A comprehensive review. Psychiatr Danub. (2023) 35:141–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

24. Grabe HJ, Wittfeld K, van der Auwera S, Janowitz D, Hegenscheid K, Habes M, et al. Effect of the interaction between childhood abuse and Rs1360780 of the Fkbp5 gene on gray matter volume in a general population sample. Hum Brain Mapp. (2016) 37:1602–13. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23123

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Broekman BF, Olff M, and Boer F. The genetic background to ptsd. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2007) 31:348–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Lee HJ, Lee MS, Kang RH, Kim H, Kim SD, Kee BS, et al. Influence of the serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphism on susceptibility to posttraumatic stress disorder. Depression Anxiety. (2005) 21:135–9. doi: 10.1002/da.20064

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Watkins LE, Han S, Harpaz-Rotem I, Mota NP, Southwick SM, Krystal JH, et al. Fkbp5 polymorphisms, childhood abuse, and ptsd symptoms: results from the national health and resilience in veterans study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2016) 69:98–105. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.04.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Xie P, Kranzler HR, Poling J, Stein MB, Anton RF, Farrer LA, et al. Interaction of Fkbp5 with childhood adversity on risk for post-traumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2010) 35:1684–92. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.37

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Nievergelt CM, Maihofer AX, Mustapic M, Yurgil KA, Schork NJ, Miller MW, et al. Genomic predictors of combat stress vulnerability and resilience in us marines: A genome-wide association study across multiple ancestries implicates Prtfdc1 as a potential ptsd gene. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2015) 51:459–71. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.10.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Rothbaum BO, Kearns MC, Reiser E, Davis JS, Kerley KA, Rothbaum AO, et al. Early intervention following trauma may mitigate genetic risk for ptsd in civilians: A pilot prospective emergency department study. J Clin Psychiatry. (2014) 75:0–. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13m08715

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ferić Bojić E, Kučukalić S, Džubur Kulenović A, Avdibegović E, Babić D, Agani F, et al. Associations of gene variations in neuropeptide Y and brain derived neurotrophic factor genes with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatria Danubina. (2019) 31:227–34. doi: 10.24869/psyd.2019.227

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Guo JC, Yang YJ, Zheng JF, Guo M, Wang XD, Gao YS, et al. Functional Rs6265 Polymorphism in the Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor Gene Confers Protection against Neurocognitive Dysfunction in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder among Chinese Patients with Hepatocellular Carcinoma. J Cell Biochem. (2019) 120:10434–43. doi: 10.1002/jcb.28328

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Valente NLM, Vallada H, Cordeiro Q, Bressan RA, Andreoli SB, Mari JJ, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase (Comt) Val158met polymorphism as a risk factor for ptsd after urban violence. J Mol Neurosci. (2011) 43:516–23. doi: 10.1007/s12031-010-9474-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Winkler EA, Yue JK, Ferguson AR, Temkin NR, Stein MB, Barber J, et al. Comt Val158met polymorphism is associated with post-traumatic stress disorder and functional outcome following mild traumatic brain injury. J Clin Neurosci. (2017) 35:109–16. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2016.09.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Jang K-I, Lee S-H, Huh HJ, and Chae J-H. Influence of the 5-Ht3a receptor gene polymorphism and childhood sexual trauma on central serotonin activity. PloS One. (2015) 10:e0145269. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145269

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Schechter DS, Moser DA, Pointet VC, Aue T, Stenz L, Paoloni-Giacobino A, et al. The association of serotonin receptor 3a methylation with maternal violence exposure, neural activity, and child aggression. Behav Brain Res. (2017) 325:268–77. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.10.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Duan Z, He M, Zhang J, Chen K, Li B, and Wang J. Assessment of functional tag single nucleotide polymorphisms within the Drd2 gene as risk factors for post-traumatic stress disorder in the Han Chinese population. J Affect Disord. (2015) 188:210–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.08.066

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Tian Y, Liu H, Guse L, Wong TK, Li J, Bai Y, et al. Association of genetic factors and gene–environment interactions with risk of developing posttraumatic stress disorder in a case–control study. Biol Res Nurs. (2015) 17:364–72. doi: 10.1177/1099800415588362

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Hauer D, Weis F, Papassotiropoulos A, Schmoeckel M, Beiras-Fernandez A, Lieke J, et al. Relationship of a common polymorphism of the glucocorticoid receptor gene to traumatic memories and posttraumatic stress disorder in patients after intensive care therapy. Crit Care Med. (2011) 39:643–50. doi: 10.1097/CCM.0b013e318206bae6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Lian Y, Xiao J, Wang Q, Ning L, Guan S, Ge H, et al. The relationship between glucocorticoid receptor polymorphisms, stressful life events, social support, and post-traumatic stress disorder. BMC Psychiatry. (2014) 14:1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12888-014-0232-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Kučukalić S, Ferić Bojić E, Babić R, Avdibegović E, Babić D, Agani F, et al. Genetic susceptibility to posttraumatic stress disorder: analyses of the oxytocin receptor, retinoic acid receptor-related orphan receptor a and cannabinoid receptor 1 genes. Psychiatria Danubina. (2019) 31:219–26. doi: 10.24869/psyd.2019.219

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Sippel LM, Han S, Watkins LE, Harpaz-Rotem I, Southwick SM, Krystal JH, et al. Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms, attachment, and ptsd: results from the national health and resilience in veterans study. J Psychiatr Res. (2017) 94:139–47. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.07.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Hwang H, Cho G, Jung K, Falk CF, Flake JK, Jin MJ, et al. An approach to structural equation modeling with both factors and components: integrated generalized structured component analysis. psychol Methods. (2020) 26(3):273–94. doi: 10.1037/met0000336

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bae H, Kim D, Koh H, Kim Y, and Park JS. Psychometric properties of the life events checklist-Korean version. Psychiatry Invest. (2008) 5:163. doi: 10.4306/pi.2008.5.3.163

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Weathers FW, Litz BT, Herman DS, Huska JA, and Keane TM eds. The Ptsd Checklist (Pcl): Reliability, Validity, and Diagnostic Utility. San Antonio, TX: Annual convention of the international society for traumatic stress studies, San Antonio, TX (1993).

Google Scholar

46. Lee B, Lee C, Lee P, Choi M, and Namkoong K. Development of Korean version of alcohol use disorders identification test (Audit-K): its reliability and validity. J Korean Acad Addict Psychiatry. (2000) 4:83–92.

Google Scholar

47. Kim S, Jang S-K, Kim D-W, Shim M, Kim Y-W, Im C-H, et al. Cortical volume and 40-hz auditory-steady-state responses in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. NeuroImage: Clin. (2019) 22:101732. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101732

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage. (2007) 38:95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Ashburner J and Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage. (2005) 26:839–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. O’Brien LM, Ziegler DA, Deutsch CK, Frazier JA, Herbert MR, and Locascio JJ. Statistical adjustments for brain size in volumetric neuroimaging studies: some practical implications in methods. Psychiatry Res: Neuroimaging. (2011) 193:113–22. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.01.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Fischl B and Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2000) 97:11050–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Karl A, Schaefer M, Malta LS, Dörfel D, Rohleder N, and Werner A. A meta-analysis of structural brain abnormalities in ptsd. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2006) 30:1004–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.03.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Basser PJ, Mattiello J, and LeBihan D. Mr diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging. Biophys J. (1994) 66:259–67. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(94)80775-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ashburner J and Friston KJ. Voxel-based morphometry—the methods. NeuroImage. (2000) 11:805–21. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0582

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Romdhani H, Hwang H, Paradis G, Roy-Gagnon MH, and Labbe A. Pathway-based association study of multiple candidate genes and multiple traits using structural equation models. Genet Epidemiol. (2015) 39:101–13. doi: 10.1002/gepi.21872

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Jung K, Cho SS, Lee J, Kim S, and Ryoo JH. An illustrative application of generalized structured component analysis for brain connectivity research. Behaviormetrika. (2020) 47:273–89. doi: 10.1007/s41237-019-00080-w

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Hwang H, Ho M-HR, and Lee J. Generalized structured component analysis with latent interactions. Psychometrika. (2010) 75:228–42. doi: 10.1007/s11336-010-9157-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Cordell HJ. Epistasis: what it means, what it doesn’t mean, and statistical methods to detect it in humans. Hum Mol Genet. (2002) 11:2463–8. doi: 10.1093/hmg/11.20.2463

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Ottman R. Gene–environment interaction: definitions and study design. Prev Med. (1996) 25:764–70. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1996.0117

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Hariri AR and Weinberger DR. Imaging genomics. Br Med Bull. (2003) 65:259–70. doi: 10.1093/bmb/65.1.259

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Hwang H. Regularized generalized structured component analysis. Psychometrika. (2009) 74:517–30. doi: 10.1007/s11336-009-9119-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Arslan A. Genes, brains, and behavior: imaging genetics for neuropsychiatric disorders. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2015) 27:81–92. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.13080185

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Blokland GA, de Zubicaray GI, McMahon KL, and Wright MJ. Genetic and environmental influences on neuroimaging phenotypes: A meta-analytical perspective on twin imaging studies. Twin Res Hum Genet. (2012) 15:351–71. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.11

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Meaney MJ. Epigenetics and the biological definition of gene× Environment interactions. Child Dev. (2010) 81:41–79. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01381.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Haller M and Chassin L. The influence of ptsd symptoms on alcohol and drug problems: internalizing and externalizing pathways. psychol Trauma: Theory Res Pract Policy. (2013) 5:484. doi: 10.1037/a0029335

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Olff M. Sex and gender differences in post-traumatic stress disorder: an update. Eur J Psychotraumatol. (2017) 8:1351204. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2017.1351204

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Squeglia LM and Gray KM. Alcohol and drug use and the developing brain. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2016) 18:46. doi: 10.1007/s11920-016-0689-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Xu J, Kobayashi S, Yamaguchi S, Iijima K-I, Okada K, and Yamashita K. Gender effects on age-related changes in brain structure. Am J Neuroradiol. (2000) 21:112–8.

Google Scholar

69. Hwang H, Cho G, and Choo H. Gsca Pro 1 (Version 1.1.8) GSCA Pro 1 (Version 118) [Computer software] (2021). Available online at: http://www.gscapro.com (Accessed August 31, 2025).

Google Scholar

70. Cho G, Hwang H, Sarstedt M, and Ringle CM. Cutoff criteria for overall model fit indexes in generalized structured component analysis. J Mark Anal. (2020) 8:189–202. doi: 10.1057/s41270-020-00089-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Marchese S and Huckins LM. Trauma matters: integrating genetic and environmental components of ptsd. Adv Genet (Hoboken). (2023) 4:2200017 p. doi: 10.1002/ggn2.202200017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Mehta D and Binder EB. Gene × Environment vulnerability factors for ptsd: the hpa-axis. Neuropharmacology. (2012) 62:654–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.03.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Seah C, Breen MS, Rusielewicz T, Bader HN, Xu C, Hunter CJ, et al. Modeling gene × Environment interactions in ptsd using human neurons reveals diagnosis-specific glucocorticoid-induced gene expression. Nat Neurosci. (2022) 25:1434–45. doi: 10.1038/s41593-022-01161-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Barnes JM, Barnes N, Champaneria S, Costall B, and Naylor R. Characterisation and autoradiographic localisation of 5-Ht3 receptor recognition sites identified with [3h]-(S)-zacopride in the forebrain of the rat. Neuropharmacology. (1990) 29:1037–45. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(90)90110-D

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Carrillo M, Ricci LA, Schwartzer JJ, and Melloni RH. Immunohistochemical characterization of 5-Ht3a receptors in the Syrian hamster forebrain. Brain Res. (2010) 1329:67–81. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.02.038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Gehlert DR, Gackenheimer SL, Wong DT, and Robertson DW. Localization of 5-Ht3 receptors in the rat brain using [3h] Ly278584. Brain Res. (1991) 553:149–54. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90242-N

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Olatunji BO, Cox RC, and Blackford JU. Fear reacquisition and symptoms of combat-related ptsd: specificity and preliminary examination of the influence of the 5-Ht3a receptor gene. Behav Res Ther. (2022) 153:104085. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2022.104085

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Jounger SL, Christidis N, Hedenberg-Magnusson B, List T, Svensson P, Schalling M, et al. Polymorphisms in the Htr2a and Htr3a genes contribute to pain in tmd myalgia. Front Oral Health. (2021) 2:647924. doi: 10.3389/froh.2021.647924

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Hamner MB, Lorberbaum JP, and George MS. Potential role of the anterior cingulate cortex in ptsd: review and hypothesis. Depression Anxiety. (1999) 9:1–14. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6394(1999)9:1<1::AID-DA1>3.0.CO;2-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Woodward SH, Kaloupek DG, Streeter CC, Martinez C, Schaer M, and Eliez S. Decreased anterior cingulate volume in combat-related ptsd. Biol Psychiatry. (2006) 59:582–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.07.033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Kitayama N, Quinn S, and Bremner JD. Smaller volume of anterior cingulate cortex in abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder. J Affect Disord. (2006) 90:171–4. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.11.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Rauch SL, Shin LM, Segal E, Pitman RK, Carson MA, McMullin K, et al. Selectively reduced regional cortical volumes in post-traumatic stress disorder. Neuroreport. (2003) 14:913–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

83. Yamasue H, Kasai K, Iwanami A, Ohtani T, Yamada H, Abe O, et al. Voxel-based analysis of mri reveals anterior cingulate gray-matter volume reduction in posttraumatic stress disorder due to terrorism. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2003) 100:9039–43. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1530467100

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Nicolaides NC, Chrousos G, and Kino T. Glucocorticoid receptor. In: Endotext. MDText.com, Inc (2020). Available online at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK279171/

Google Scholar

85. Palma-Gudiel H, Córdova-Palomera A, Leza JC, and Fañanás L. Glucocorticoid receptor gene (Nr3c1) methylation processes as mediators of early adversity in stress-related disorders causality: A critical review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 55:520–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.05.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Kennis M, Van Rooij S, Van Den Heuvel M, Kahn R, and Geuze E. Functional network topology associated with posttraumatic stress disorder in veterans. NeuroImage: Clin. (2016) 10:302–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.12.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Sadaghiani S and D’Esposito M. Functional characterization of the cingulo-opercular network in the maintenance of tonic alertness. Cereb Cortex. (2015) 25:2763–73. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu072

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Touroutoglou A, Hollenbeck M, Dickerson BC, and Barrett LF. Dissociable large-scale networks anchored in the right anterior insula subserve affective experience and attention. Neuroimage. (2012) 60:1947–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Driessen M, Beblo T, Mertens M, Piefke M, Rullkoetter N, Silva-Saavedra A, et al. Posttraumatic stress disorder and fmri activation patterns of traumatic memory in patients with borderline personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2004) 55:603–11. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.08.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Tavanti M, Battaglini M, Borgogni F, Bossini L, Calossi S, Marino D, et al. Evidence of diffuse damage in frontal and occipital cortex in the brain of patients with post-traumatic stress disorder. Neurol Sci. (2012) 33:59–68. doi: 10.1007/s10072-011-0659-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Reddy MK, Meyer TD, Wittlin NM, Miller IW, and Weinstock LM. Bipolar I disorder with comorbid ptsd: demographic and clinical correlates in a sample of hospitalized patients. Compr Psychiatry. (2017) 72:13–7. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.08.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Spinhoven P, Penninx BW, van Hemert AM, de Rooij M, and Elzinga BM. Comorbidity of ptsd in anxiety and depressive disorders: prevalence and shared risk factors. Child Abuse Negl. (2014) 38:1320–30. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2014.01.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: PTSD (posttraumatic stress disorder), genetic polymorphisms, brain structures, potentially traumatic events (PTEs), imaging genetics generalized structured component analysis (IG-GSCA), HTR3A, NR3C1

Citation: Jin MJ, Cho G, Jeon H, Choi Y, Hwang H and Lee S-H (2025) Influence of genetic polymorphism and trauma on cortical structures and PTSD severity: Imaging genetics generalized structured component analysis. Front. Psychiatry 16:1613322. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1613322

Received: 17 April 2025; Accepted: 20 August 2025;
Published: 17 September 2025.

Edited by:

Li Yang, Peking University Sixth Hospital, China

Reviewed by:

Jayne Morriss, University of Southampton, United Kingdom
Binbin Wang, Physical Peking University, China

Copyright © 2025 Jin, Cho, Jeon, Choi, Hwang and Lee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Seung-Hwan Lee, bHNocHNzQHBhaWsuYWMua3I=; bHNocHNzQGhhbm1haWwubmV0

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.