- School of Psychology, Speech and Hearing, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand
Introduction: A diagnosis of ADHD has been repeatedly associated with low self-esteem in university students. Low self-esteem is associated with a range of negative outcomes including poor social function, increased psychopathology, and low academic self-efficacy. This study examined associations between ADHD, self-esteem, and daily activities in university students.
Methods: A total of n =125 university students from New Zealand (50 meeting diagnostic criteria for ADHD) completed a survey measuring ADHD symptoms, self-esteem, and variables associated with self-esteem as well as a seven-day ecological momentary assessment survey measuring momentary self-esteem, mood, and current activity.
Results: ADHD was associated with a range of negative outcomes including low self-esteem, low general and academic self-efficacy, and high levels of mental distress. Global self-esteem was associated with momentary self-esteem. Differences between students with and without ADHD were observed both in momentary self-esteem ratings and the frequencies of endorsed activities. In both groups, decreased momentary self-esteem was associated with being alone and procrastinating. For other activities, a differential pattern of momentary self-esteem increases was observed across groups.
Discussion: This study provides support for the view that self-esteem is best conceptualised as both a stable trait and a state, fluctuating in response to daily events. Notably, this study also provides evidence that engagement in specific activities is associated with dynamic elevations in self-esteem among university students meeting diagnostic criteria for ADHD.
Introduction
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that has been shown to persist across the lifespan (1) with a worldwide prevalence of 5.9% in youth and 2.5% in adulthood (2). Characterised by symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, the diagnosis is associated with a range of negative outcomes including impairments in social, emotional, academic, health, and functional domains (1, 3, 4). Among university-aged students, research shows that, relative to non-ADHD university students, individuals diagnosed with ADHD have higher levels of emotional dysregulation, reduced social skills, and higher rates of psychological distress (5, 6). Additionally, these students exhibit lower academic performance (7) and academic coping skills (6), greater procrastination, and lower self-esteem (5).
Self-esteem can be broadly defined as an individual’s subjective evaluation of themselves (8); it refers to an individual’s conceptualization of who they are and their value as a person (9). Self-esteem is distinct from self-concept, which refers to an individual’s understanding of themselves including both their qualities and appearance (10), and self-worth, which can be understood as an individual’s sense of whether they meet their own standards regarding their personality and achievements (11). Self-esteem can be examined globally, providing a summative conceptualization of an individual’s self-evaluation or it can be examined across specific domains (e.g. academic self-esteem) (12). As such, global self-esteem can be conceptualized as the of sum of domain-specific self-esteem conceptualizations. Global self-esteem has been shown to be correlated with domain specific self-esteem, suggesting a general consistency within the construct (12).
Self-esteem is not necessarily reflective of an individual’s objective skills and traits (8). However, high self-esteem has been associated with self-worth and self-acceptance (8) and high rates of global self-esteem have been linked to positive outcomes in social, occupational, and health domains (13). Conversely, lower global self-esteem has been associated with a wide range of difficulties in inter- (14) and intrapersonal function (15). In university students, low self-esteem has been shown to be predictive of difficulties in social function in general (16) as well as among individuals diagnosed with ADHD (17). Additionally, low self-esteem in the first two years of university has been found to predict low academic self-efficacy in subsequent years (18).
Individuals with a diagnosis of ADHD have been found to have lower self-esteem compared to their non-ADHD peers in both adolescence (19) and adulthood (20). A recent meta-analysis of 127 studies revealed that when compared to individuals not diagnosed with ADHD, ADHD individuals had worse self-esteem and self-esteem related outcomes (3). Additionally, dimensional comparisons have revealed that individuals displaying more severe ADHD symptoms have lower self-esteem relative to those with milder symptoms (21). These relationships have also been observed in university students with studies finding that students with ADHD have lower self-esteem than students without ADHD and that self-esteem is negatively correlated with ADHD symptomatology (17, 22).
Low self-esteem among individuals with ADHD has been proposed to be an outcome of ADHD symptomatology where enduring negative consequences associated with ADHD symptoms negatively affect self-esteem (23). Such theoretical conceptualizations have been supported by previous research which has shown that individuals with ADHD tend to have poorer academic performance (24), experience more peer rejection (25), and experience more negative parent-child interactions in childhood (26), all of which have been associated with low self-esteem in individuals with ADHD (20). In addition to the relation between low self-esteem and the sequalae of consequences as a function of ADHD symptoms, there appears to be a direct effect of symptoms on self-esteem, as recent studies have shown that adolescents meeting criteria for ADHD describe their experiences of the core symptoms of ADHD negatively (24, 27) with qualitative studies indicating that adolescents with ADHD feel different and marginalized by these experiences (24, 25). While the cognitive and neuropsychological mechanisms supporting the association between ADHD and self-esteem are underexamined, a recent study found that across subtypes, working memory, which has repeatedly been associated with ADHD diagnosis and symptom severity (26), was negatively correlated with self-esteem (28). Lastly, as the initiation of treatment for ADHD symptoms has been shown to result in significant improvement in self-esteem (3), it appears that there is a robust link between ADHD symptoms and consequences to self-esteem.
Self-esteem has been shown to be a stable construct, such that individuals who experience low self-esteem relative to their peers at one point in their life are more likely to have low self-esteem relative to their peers at other points in their life (8). Traditionally, the measurement of self-esteem has been measured using survey-based methodologies and has often been assessed at singular time points. These singular assessments reflect global self-esteem (29); however, self-esteem has been shown to be both stable over time as well as fluctuate across moments (30, 31). Such discrepancies have resulted in an ongoing debate regarding whether self-esteem is best categorised along state or trait dimensions (32). Some researchers argue that the stability of global self-esteem over the lifespan indicates that self-esteem is best conceptualised as a trait (33) while others emphasise the differential influence of situational factors on self-esteem (34, 35). Results of a study examining self-esteem using a latent variable trait-state model indicated that self-esteem contains both trait and state components, indicating that there is utility in examining self-esteem as both fixed and fluctuating (32). Research that has examined state changes in self-esteem through the assessment of multiple daily ratings of self-esteem has found that higher self-esteem variability has been associated with negative outcomes such as increased internalising symptoms (36), maladaptive reactions to negative feedback (35), and social anxiety and avoidance (37). It has been proposed that such fluctuations in self-esteem are a result of recently experienced self-relevant experiences (35) and research has linked negative interpersonal events, daily challenges, negative generalisation, and depressive symptoms to increased self-esteem variability in young people (35, 36, 38). Conversely, a study by Sowislo et al. (39) found no effect of self-esteem contingency (defined as fluctuations in self-esteem based on self-relevant events) and only a small effect of self-esteem instability (defined as short-term variability in self-esteem) on depressive symptoms in adults. In a study of daily self-esteem fluctuations in young adults, DeHart & Pelham (40) found that individuals with reported low trait self-esteem were particularly vulnerable for state implicit self-esteem fluctuations following negative events. This suggests that trait self-esteem may act as a buffer to reduce the effects of negative events on state self-esteem fluctuations. Overall, findings indicate that while self-esteem may remain generally stable across time, it varies within the day in response to contextual fluctuations which may differentially influence individuals with low and high, levels of global self-esteem. These findings have specific relevance to individuals diagnosed with ADHD. In addition to reporting low self-esteem (20), individuals diagnosed with ADHD experience significantly more negative daily life events compared to their non-ADHD peers (41). Further, among individuals diagnosed with a psychiatric disorder, lower self-esteem has been identified as a risk and perpetuating factor (42, 43). As such, understanding the influence of the construct among individuals with ADHD warrants continued consideration.
To our knowledge, no research to date has specifically examined daily self-esteem patterns and consequences among individuals diagnosed with ADHD. Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) is a research assessment protocol that allows for the repeated sampling of behaviours occurring in real time, conducted in the natural environment (44). The advantage of EMA over traditional methods is that it allows for the measurement of variables both repeatedly and in real time. The use of EMA methodologies has been successfully used in studies with children, adolescents, and adults diagnosed with ADHD (45), however, none have been used to specifically assess self-esteem as an outcome variable.
The present study aimed to examine self-esteem across seven days in young adult university students with and without ADHD. In line with previous research, it was hypothesised that when compared to their non-ADHD peers, individuals meeting criteria for ADHD would report lower rates of global self-esteem and that these differences would be seen across daily assessments. As individuals with ADHD have been found to experience more negative daily life events, it was also hypothesised that individuals meeting criteria for ADHD would report experiencing a greater number of potentially stress-inducing activities (e.g. procrastinating) and that these activities would be associated with relatively greater changes in reported self-esteem.
Materials and methods
Participants
A total of n=185 late adolescent and young adult university students residing in New Zealand signed up to participate in this study. Of those, 21 did not meet study criteria and 39 did not engage in the study after providing consent. The final sample of n = 125 participants included 18 males, 106 females, and one participant who reported being intersex. Age ranged from 17 to 21 (M=18.2, SD = 0.85). Participants were initially recruited through their participation in another study (20.8%).To increase sample size, additional participants were recruited through a first-year psychology course (79.2%). All participants were students at a regional university in New Zealand.
Materials
ADHD symptom severity was measured using the Adult ADHD Self Report Scale (ASRS; 46). The ASRS is an 18-item self-report scales that measures ADHD symptoms on a 5-point Likert scales with 1 being “never” and 5 being “very often”. This measure is frequently used in samples of individuals with ADHD, and in this sample, this scale had excellent internal consistency (α = 0.92). Item responses were averaged to create mean scores for total ADHD.
Global self-esteem was measured using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RES; 32). The RES is a 10-item self-report scale that measures self-esteem (e.g. I take a positive attitude toward myself) on a 4-point Likert scale with 1 being “strongly agree” and 4 being “strongly disagree” (47). Items are averaged to create an overall mean score. In the present sample, the RES demonstrated high internal consistency (α = .89).
The New General Self-Efficacy Scale (NGSES (48) is an 8-item scale measuring the extent to which individuals believe they can achieve their goals. Items (e.g. Even when things are tough, I can perform quite well) are assessed using a 5-point Likert scale with 1 being “strongly disagree” and 5 being “strongly agree”. The NGSES scale has been found to have high reliability and validity (48) and was found to have high internal consistency in the present sample (α =.89).
The belief individuals have in their ability to achieve academic goals was measured using the 5-item General Academic Self-Efficacy Scale (GASES (49). Items (e.g. I generally manage to solve difficult academic problems if I try hard enough) are measured on a 5-point Likert scale with 1 being “strongly disagree” and 5 being “strongly agree” and averaged to create a single measure of academic self-efficacy. The GASES has been found to be both reliable and valid (49) and in the present sample, the GASES demonstrated high internal consistency (α = .81).
The Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS-21) is a 21-item scale measuring depression, anxiety, and stress on separate subscales using a 4-point Likert scale with 0 being “did not apply to me at all” and 3 being “applied to me very much, or most of the time” (50). In the present sample, the DASS-21 had high-acceptable internal consistency with α = .87 for the depression subscale, α = .80 for the anxiety subscale, and α = .80 for the stress subscale.
Emotional reactivity was measured using The Perth Emotional Reactivity Scale short form (PERS-S (51),). The PERS-S is an 18-item scale that measures emotional reactivity on a 5-point Likert scale with 1 being “very unlike me” and 5 being “very like me”. This scale consists of two nine item subscales measuring positive emotional reactivity (e.g. I tend to get happy very easily, α = .78) and negative emotional reactivity (e.g. I tend to get upset very easily, α = .86). For each subscale, items are averaged to create an overall mean score.
The Baruth Protective Factors Inventory (BPFI) is a 16-item scale measuring factors that contribute to resilience, such as social support, adaptive personality, and compensating experiences (52). A score on the BFPI serves as a unitary measure of protective factors across all the measured domains. The BFPI uses a 5-point Likert scale with 1 being “strongly agree” and 5 being “strongly disagree” (52). In the present study the BFPI has been found to have high internal consistency (α = .83).
The Smartphone Ecological Momentary Assessment (SEMA3) digital application was used to deliver the ecological momentary assessment survey. This application was developed by the Melbourne eResearch group (53) and allows researchers to send surveys to participants’ iOS or Android mobile devices to be answered in real time. The survey consisted of the following five questions:
1. “How are you feeling?”
Participants selected relevant emotions from the following list: “content,” “happy,” “energetic,” “excited,” “neutral,” “bored,” “tired,” “anxious,” “scared,” “stressed,” “angry,” “irritated,” “jealous,” “sad,” “upset,” and “disappointed”.
2. “How intense is your feeling?” measured using a 10-point Likert scale from 1 (low intensity) to 10 (high intensity).
3. “Rate your current self-esteem,” measured using a 10-point Likert scale from 1 (very low) to 10 (very high).
4. “What are you doing?”
“Participants were able to select activities from the following list: “spending time with family,” “spending time with friends,” “spending time alone,” “at university,” “doing homework or studying,” “working,” “volunteering,” “exercising,” “participating in an extracurricular activity,” “at a sports game or practice,” “attending a social event,” “relaxing,” “doing a hobby/something fun,” “using social media,” “getting organised for something (e.g. university, bed, socialising),” “procrastinating a task (e.g. homework, chores, getting ready),” “eating,” and “having an argument”.
5. The final question was: “Are your feelings related to what you’re currently doing?”. Participants were able to choose “yes,” “no,” or “not sure”.
Procedure
Data was collected in between September 2023 and June 2024. At the start of this study, individuals provided informed consent, completed the questionnaire battery which was hosted on Qualtrics, and were given instructions to download the SEMA3 application onto their mobile device. Starting the following day, participants were sent notifications to their mobile device with instructions to complete the EMA questions. Participants received five notifications per day for seven consecutive days. Notifications were sent in the early morning (8am), late morning (11am), early afternoon (2pm), early evening (5pm), and late evening (8pm). Each survey was available for completion until the next time point notification was sent out. Upon completion of the study, participants were compensated with either course credit or a grocery voucher. The current study was reviewed and approved by the Human Ethics Committee at the University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand.
Data analysis
Group membership (ADHD/non-ADHD) was determined following DSM-5-TR (54) guidelines requiring the presence of 5 or more symptoms of inattention and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity that have persisted for at least 6 months and reported as maladaptive and impairing. Symptoms were considered present if rated “often” or “very often”. Within the ADHD group, a Kruskal-Wallis test was conducted to assess possible subgroup differences in reported ADHD symptoms.
Pearson correlations and independent samples t-tests were conducted in order to examine the relations and group differences (ADHD/non-ADHD) between global self-esteem, total ADHD symptom severity, and the constructs measured in the questionnaire battery.
Assessment of EMA data involved the use of two-way mixed model ANOVAs to examine group differences in self-esteem across and within days. Overall mean EMA reported self-esteem and self-esteem variability, as assessed as overall mean self-esteem SD, was examined as a function of group status and in relation to reported global self-esteem and ADHD symptoms. Chi-square tests of independence were calculated to assess differences in activity engagement.
Independent samples t-tests were conducted to examine self-esteem as a function of activity engagement. Mean self-esteem ratings were compiled for each reported activity. Group comparisons were conducted for each activity. Activity self-esteem means were also compared against the overall self-esteem mean score for each group. All analyses were conducted using Jamovi.version 2.6.44.
Results
As can be seen in Table 1, correlation analyses revealed significant negative correlations between ADHD symptomatology and self-esteem, self-efficacy, academic self-efficacy, and protective factors. ADHD symptoms were negatively correlated with ratings of depression, anxiety, stress, and negative emotional reactivity. Self-esteem was positively correlated with measures of academic and self-efficacy and positive emotional reactivity and negatively associated with depression, stress, anxiety, and negative emotional reactivity.
Based upon endorsement of ADHD diagnostic criteria, a total of 50 participants (40%) were assigned to the ADHD group and 75 participants (60%) were assigned to the non-ADHD group. Of those assigned to the ADHD group 21 (42%) had previously been formally assessed for ADHD, 10 (48%) of these had preexisting diagnoses and 11 (52%) were assessed as part of participation in a previous study. The remaining 29 (58%) endorsed symptoms meeting or exceeding the diagnostic ADHD symptom criteria. Within the ADHD group there was no difference in ADHD symptoms between participants who had previously received a diagnosis of ADHD (M = 68.8, SD = 8.39), participants who completed a clinical interview assessing them for ADHD as part of another study (M = 72.9, SD = 8.35), and participants who were assigned to the ADHD group based on their endorsement of symptoms meeting criteria for ADHD (M = 66.3, SD = 9.01), χ2 (2) = 5.10, p = .08. There was no difference between the ADHD group and the non-ADHD group in age (t = -1.69, p = .09), sex (χ2 = 1.10, p = .58), or gender (χ2 = 1.49, p = .48).
Group comparisons, as seen in Table 2, revealed that the ADHD group reported significantly lower levels of self-esteem, self-efficacy, academic self-efficacy, positive emotional reactivity and significantly higher depression, anxiety, stress, and negative emotional reactivity. The magnitude of effect in these differences was large (d ranging from 0.79 to 1.07) for all variables except positive emotional reactivity which was small.
Ecological momentary assessment
Engagement with the EMA protocol was high, with 96% of surveys completed. As depicted in Table 3, frequency analyses revealed that significantly fewer surveys were completed in the morning compared to the rest of the daily assessment periods (χ2 = 74.4, p <.001, Cramer’s V = 0.13) and fewer surveys were completed later in the assessment period (χ2 = 13.0, p = .043, Cramer’s V = 0.05).
The overall mean of momentary self-esteem was significantly positively correlated with reported global self-esteem (r = .61, p <.001) and academic self-efficacy (r = .43, p <.001) and negatively correlated with ADHD symptomatology (r = -.46, p <.001). The ADHD group had significantly lower overall mean momentary self-esteem compared to the non-ADHD group (t = 4.65, p <.001, d = 0.85). Both groups completed a similar number of surveys (t = 0.86, p = .390, d = 0.16) and although ADHD symptomatology was negatively correlated with the number of EMA surveys completed, such that higher ADHD symptomatology was associated with completing fewer surveys, this correlation was not significant (r = -.17, p = .06).
Figure 1 presents group differences in momentary self-esteem across days. The overall model for the two-way mixed model ANOVA examining the effects of group and day was significant, F (13, 4183) = 26.87, p <.001. There was a significant main effect for group, F(1, 4183) = 336.70, p <.001, but not for day, (F(6, 4183) = 0.75, p = .61, or the group x day interaction, F(6, 4183) = 1.41, p = .21. These results demonstrate that self-esteem for individuals meeting criteria for ADHD is lower and stable across days.

Figure 1. Group differences in mean momentary self-esteem across days. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.
Figure 2 depicts mean self-esteem ratings across the daily assessment intervals. The overall model for the two-way mixed model ANOVA was significant, F(9, 4187) = 39.91, p <.001) with significant main effects for both group, F(1, 4187) = 336.76, p <.001 and time, F(4, 4187) = 4.22, p <.01. Non-ADHD individuals reported higher levels of self-esteem across all assessment intervals. Post-hoc analyses examining differences in assessment intervals revealed that self-esteem reported in the early morning (M = 5.19, SD = 2.07) was significantly lower when compared to late morning (M = 5.52, SD = 2.14), early afternoon (M = 5.50, SD = 2.23), and late evening (M = 5.62, SD = 2.36). The Group x Time interaction was not significant, F(4, 4187) = 0.51, p = .73 indicating that differences in daily self-esteem ratings were similar for both groups.

Figure 2. Mean momentary self-esteem across daily assessment intervals. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.
Variability in self-esteem, as measured by overall momentary self-esteem standard deviation was not significantly associated with ADHD symptoms (r = .12, p = .19) and was not found to significantly differ as a function of group status (t = -1.09, p = .28, d = 0.20).
Activity engagement and momentary self-esteem
As shown in Table 4, examination of activity engagement frequencies revealed that studying, relaxing, and spending time alone and with friends were the activities most reported by both groups. While effect sizes were small, compared to the non-ADHD group, the ADHD group reported higher rates of procrastinating, being at university, working, getting organised, and lower rates of being with friends.
In order to understand the relation between activity engagement on self-esteem, mean momentary self-esteem scores were calculated for each endorsed activity. These scores were then compared against the calculated mean momentary self-esteem for all activities. These comparisons were done independently for each group. Differences were observed in the activities associated with increasing and decreasing momentary self-esteem across the two groups and are presented in Tables 5 and 6. For the ADHD group, being alone and procrastinating were associated with significantly lower momentary self-esteem, while being with friends, at university, working, socialising, or engaging in hobbies were associated with significantly higher momentary self-esteem. For the non-ADHD group, being alone, procrastinating, studying, and arguing were associated with significantly lower reported self-esteem while being with family, friends, relaxing, doing hobbies, and eating were associated with significantly higher momentary self-esteem.

Table 5. Significant differences in ADHD group momentary self-esteem by activity relative to overall ADHD group momentary self-esteem.

Table 6. Significant differences in Non-ADHD group momentary self-esteem by activity relative to overall non-ADHD group momentary self-esteem.
Discussion
This study sought to better understand the relation between self-esteem and ADHD symptomatology in a sample of young adult university students living in New Zealand. Low self-esteem, a feature frequently found in studies on individuals diagnosed with ADHD, has been associated with a number of negative outcomes and has been posited to be a contributing factor in the maintenance of psychiatric disorders. Among university students, high rates of ADHD symptoms and low self-esteem have been associated with factors, such as procrastination (5), that are directly relevant to success in academic contexts. To date, studies examining self-esteem outcomes in individuals diagnosed with ADHD have considered self-esteem as a global construct, reflective of general trait like processes, and often measured at one point in time. However, self-esteem fluctuations have been associated with increased negative life events and shown to be more prominent among individuals with low, compared to high, self-esteem (40). The current study measured self-esteem multiple times a day across seven days. To better understand the contextual factors that contribute to daily self-esteem ratings, this study recorded activity engagement at each self-esteem measurement. It was expected that compared to their non-ADHD peers, individuals meeting criteria for ADHD would report lower rates of global self-esteem and that these differences would be seen across daily assessments. It was also hypothesised that individuals meeting criteria for ADHD would report engaging in a greater number of potentially stress-inducing activities and that these events would be associated with changes in reported self-esteem.
Consistent with previous literature demonstrating that self-esteem is associated with wellbeing (13), global self-esteem measured at the start of this study was associated with higher self-and academic-efficacy and positive emotional reactivity and negatively associated with depression, anxiety, stress, and negative emotional reactivity. Self-esteem was negatively associated with ADHD symptomatology. Initial group comparisons revealed that, compared to their non-ADHD peers, those who met or exceeded the criteria for a diagnosis of ADHD reported significantly lower self-esteem, self-and academic-efficacy, positive emotional reactivity and higher depression, anxiety, stress, and negative emotional reactivity. These comparisons generated large effect sizes. These results support study hypotheses and are consistent with a large body of research linking ADHD to a range of negative outcomes including lower self-esteem (19), lower self-efficacy (21) and academic self-efficacy (55, 56), poor mental health (57), and poor emotion regulation (58).
Self-esteem measured by the EMA protocol was significantly associated with a gold standard measure of global self-esteem, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (59). Additionally, group differences seen in initial group comparisons of global self-esteem were consistently reflected in measured momentary self-esteem both within and across days. These findings support the validity of the assessment of self-esteem used in the EMA protocol and the view that self-esteem is stable and trait like (8). However, as the frequency graphs of this study reveal, self-esteem is best conceptualised both as a state as well as a trait (30) where self-esteem is generally stable with fluctuations likely manifesting as a function of contextual factors (60) and individual differences (30).
As individuals diagnosed with ADHD have been shown to exhibit greater response variability across multiple areas of assessment (61) and that self-esteem variability has been associated with low rated self-esteem (40), it was hypothesised that the individuals meeting symptom criteria for ADHD would demonstrate greater fluctuations in EMA assessed self-esteem variability. Interestingly, group differences were not seen in self-esteem fluctuations. It is possible that our measure of variability, overall mean standard deviation, although a frequently used measure of performance variability in studies examining individuals diagnosed with ADHD (61), was not nuanced enough and more specific parameters may be better suited to examine the nature of state self-esteem variability.
Previous research has established an association between activity engagement and self-esteem such that engagement in activities that are physical in nature (62) or valued (63) have been associated with increased self-esteem, while other activities, such as excessive social media use (64), have been associated with lower self-esteem. Individuals diagnosed with ADHD have been shown to experience more frequent daily negative events (41) and greater impairment in daily functions (65) compared to non-ADHD individuals. While Whalen et al. (66) found that ADHD severity was positively associated with time in non-productive activities, to our knowledge, no studies have examined frequency differences in daily activities between those with and without ADHD.
Similarities and differences were seen in reported activities over the seven days. Both groups reported frequently being alone, with friends, studying, and working, activities which generally comport with other studies examining daily activities in university students (67). Of the few group differences seen in activity engagement, the strongest effect was seen in frequency of procrastination.
While not part of the diagnostic framework, procrastination, the tendency to delay or an activity that has a deadline, has been associated with symptoms of ADHD in university students (68). Recently, Bodalski et al. (5) found that self-esteem partially explained the relation between ADHD symptoms and procrastination in a sample of undergraduate university students. While this study did not consider self-esteem as a predictor of outcomes such as procrastination, the lower self-esteem and increased occurrence of procrastination among individual meeting criteria for ADHD in this study support such proposed relations. However, it was surprising to see that although individuals meeting criteria for ADHD procrastinated more, the effect of procrastination on self-esteem was similar across the two groups.
Individuals meeting criteria for ADHD also reported more frequently getting organised relative to their non-ADHD peers. Given that difficulties in organizational tasks is one of the symptom criteria of the diagnosis, it is not surprising that university students with ADHD would endorse more time spent getting organised. Despite this, it was notable that self-esteem was not found to be lower getting organized when compared to overall reported self-esteem.
Interestingly, students with ADHD reported spending more time at university than their non-ADHD peers and this was associated with increased self-esteem for this group. It was found that university students diagnosed with ADHD reported greater environmental mastery relative to their non-ADHD peers (69), suggesting that this group of individuals may be particularly adept at managing their environment and effectively using available opportunities. University campuses often offer opportunities for social engagement and past research has shown that students who are engaged in extracurricular clubs and organizations benefit in their psychosocial development (70). As such, it may be that university students diagnosed with ADHD who are adept at managing social environments, benefit from university campus offerings and this is reflected in relative increases in self-esteem. Future studies may wish to explore how spending time on campus could be used as a mechanism to improve self-esteem and academic self-efficacy in students with ADHD.
Relatedly, given the consistent findings, supported by this study, that self-esteem is lower among individuals diagnosed with ADHD and that individuals reporting lower self-esteem are at risk for increased negative outcomes, social factors associated with improved self-esteem have clinical relevance for this population. For individuals reporting clinically high levels of ADHD symptoms, engaging in hobbies, and social activities such as spending time with friends, socialising, being at university, and working were particularly notable for reports of elevated self-esteem.
Engagement in hobbies (71) and social interactions (72, 73) have been associated with improved self-esteem and future studies may wish to examine these social factors as possible mechanisms for raising self-esteem and improving mental health outcomes in individuals diagnosed with ADHD.
There were some limitations to this study and results should be interpreted with caution. While all individuals in the ADHD group endorsed meeting symptom criteria for ADHD, not all individuals competed a formal assessment process. Future research may wish to examine the relations identified in this study in a sample of formally diagnosed individuals. Further, this study only considered total ADHD symptoms, and it is possible that the findings seen in this study differ as a function of symptom cluster. In their review of long-term outcomes, Harpin et al. (3) noted that self-esteem outcomes were worse for non-medicated ADHD individuals and indicated that engagemnt with evidence-based interventions for ADHD resulted in improved self-esteem. This study did not assess history or use of pharmacologic or behavioural treatments. Lastly, female individuals were overrepresented in this study and findings may not generalise to young adult males with ADHD.
In their systematic review of self-esteem among adults with ADHD, Pedersen et al. (20) noted that there was a lack of studies examining the repeated finding of lower self-esteem in this population. This study highlights both the stability and fluctuating nature of self-esteem and reveals that while global self-esteem is stable, self-esteem can vary across the day and is affected by the activities individuals choose to engage in. Although there were activities, such as procrastination and getting organised, that individuals meeting criteria for ADHD engaged in more frequently than their non-ADHD peers, effect size differences were such that contextual differences alone would not be enough to account for stable low self-esteem. Psychiatric history has been shown to predict low self-esteem (74), and it is possible that the low self-esteem seen in this study is, in part, a function of enduring complications with difficulties in attention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity.
Self-esteem, while stable, is mutable (75). As such, consideration of activities, such as engagement with leisure, vocational, and social activities, that raise self-esteem merits further attention as possible intervention targets for this at-risk group. Clinicians may wish to focus on engaging university student clients with ADHD in activities such as spending time on campus that set them up for success and improve self-esteem. A recent study examining the challenges facing of university support services indicated a need for specialized interventions and support services tailored to meet the needs of students diagnosed with ADHD (76). The results of the current study, which identify specific situations and contexts that influence self-esteem, has the possibility to directly inform novel interventions targeting this vulnerable group of university students.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Human Research Ethics Committee, University of Canterbury. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
ET: Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization. SH: Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Supervision.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Sciberras E, Roos LE, and Efron D. Review of prospective longitudinal studies of children with ADHD: Mental health, educational, and social outcomes. Curr Attention Disord Rep. (2009) 1:171–7. doi: 10.1007/s12618-009-0024-1
2. Faraone SV, Banaschewski T, Coghill D, Zheng Y, Biederman J, Bellgrove MA, et al. The world federation of ADHD international consensus statement: 208 evidence-based conclusions about the disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2021) 128:789–818. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.01.022
3. Harpin V, Mazzone L, Raynaud J, Kahle J, and Hodgkins P. Long-term outcomes of ADHD: a systematic review of self-esteem and social function. J attention Disord. (2016) 20:295–305. doi: 10.1177/1087054713486516
4. Fleming M, Fitton CA, Steiner MF, McLay JS, Clark D, King A, et al. Educational and health outcomes of children treated for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA pediatrics. (2017) 171:e170691–e. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2017.0691
5. Bodalski EA, Flory K, Canu WH, Willcutt EG, and Hartung CM. ADHD symptoms and procrastination in college students: The roles of emotion dysregulation and self-esteem. J Psychopathol Behav Assessment. (2023) 45:48–57. doi: 10.1007/s10862-022-09996-2
6. Weyandt LL and DuPaul GJ. ADHD in college students: Developmental findings. Dev Disabil Res Rev. (2008) 14:311–9. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.38
7. Pagespetit È, Pagerols M, Barrés N, Prat R, Martínez L, Andreu M, et al. ADHD and academic performance in college students: A systematic review. J Attention Disord. (2025) 29:281–97. doi: 10.1177/10870547241306554
8. Orth U and Robins RW. The development of self-esteem. Curr Dir psychol Science. (2014) 23:381–7. doi: 10.1177/0963721414547414
9. Birkeland MS, Melkevik O, Holsen I, and Wold B. Trajectories of global self-esteem development during adolescence. J adolescence. (2012) 35:43–54. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.06.006
10. Bailey 2JA. Self-image, self-concept, and self-identity revisited. J Natl Med Assoc. (2003) 95:383.
11. Bırni G and Eryılmaz A. Conceptual and theoretical review of self-worth. Psikiyatride Güncel Yaklaşımlar. (2024) 16:327–46. doi: 10.18863/pgy.1336880
12. von Soest T, Wichstrøm L, and Kvalem IL. The development of global and domain-specific self-esteem from age 13 to 31. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2016) 110:592. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000060
13. Orth U and Robins RW. Is high self-esteem beneficial? Revisiting a classic question. Am Psychol. (2022) 77:5. doi: 10.1037/amp0000922
14. Cameron JJ and Granger S. Does self-esteem have an interpersonal imprint beyond self-reports? A meta-analysis of self-esteem and objective interpersonal indicators. Pers Soc Psychol Review. (2019) 23:73–102. doi: 10.1177/1088868318756532
15. Sowislo JF and Orth U. Does low self-esteem predict depression and anxiety? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. psychol bulletin. (2013) 139:213. doi: 10.1037/a0028931
16. Crocker J and Luhtanen RK. Level of self-esteem and contingencies of self-worth: Unique effects on academic, social, and financial problems in college students. Pers Soc Psychol Bulletin. (2003) 29:701–12. doi: 10.1177/0146167203029006003
17. Dooling-Litfin JK and Rosen LA. Self-esteem in college students with a childhood history of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Coll Student Psychotherapy. (1997) 11:69–82. doi: 10.1300/J035v11n04_06
18. Luo Y, Gao W, and Liu X. Longitudinal relationship between self-esteem and academic self-efficacy among college students in China: evidence from a cross-lagged model. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:877343. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.877343
19. Slomkowski C, Klein RG, and Mannuzza S. Is self-esteem an important outcome in hyperactive children? J Abnormal Child Psychol. (1995) 23:303–15. doi: 10.1007/BF01447559
20. Pedersen AB, Edvardsen BV, Messina SM, Volden MR, Weyandt LL, and Lundervold AJ. Self-esteem in adults with ADHD using the rosenberg self-esteem scale: A systematic review. J Attention Disord. (2024) 28:1124–38. doi: 10.1177/10870547241237245
21. Newark PE, Elsässer M, and Stieglitz R-D. Self-esteem, self-efficacy, and resources in adults with ADHD. J Attention Disord. (2016) 20:279–90. doi: 10.1177/1087054712459561
22. Shaw-Zirt B, Popali-Lehane L, Chaplin W, and Bergman A. Adjustment, social skills, and self-esteem in college students with symptoms of ADHD. J attention Disord. (2005) 8:109–20. doi: 10.1177/1087054705277775
23. Young S and Bramham J. ADHD in adults: A psychological guide to practice. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons (2006).
24. Jones S and Hesse M. Adolescents with ADHD: Experiences of having an ADHD diagnosis and negotiations of self-image and identity. J Attention Disord. (2014) 22:92–102. doi: 10.1177/1087054714522513
25. Eccleston L, Williams J, Knowles S, and Soulsby L. Adolescent experiences of living with a diagnosis of ADHD: a systematic review and thematic synthesis. Emotional Behav Difficulties. (2019) 24:119–35. doi: 10.1080/13632752.2019.1582762
26. Alderson RM, Kasper LJ, Hudec KL, and Patros CH. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and working memory in adults: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. (2013) 27:287. doi: 10.1037/a0032371
27. Løhre A. Identity formation in adolescents with concentration problems, high levels of activity or impulsiveness: A pragmatic qualitative study. Int J Educ Psychol. (2020) 9:1–23. doi: 10.17583/ijep.2020.4315
28. Molavi P, Nadermohammadi M, Salvat Ghojehbeiglou H, Vicario CM, Nitsche MA, and Salehinejad MA. ADHD subtype-specific cognitive correlates and association with self-esteem: A quantitative difference. BMC Psychiatry. (2020) 20:1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-02887-4
29. Rosenberg M, Schooler C, Schoenbach C, and Rosenberg F. Global self-esteem and specific self-esteem: Different concepts, different outcomes. Am sociological Rev. (1995), 141–56. doi: 10.2307/2096350
30. Heatherton TF and Polivy J. Development and validation of a scale for measuring state self-esteem. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1991) 60:895. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.60.6.895
31. Geukes K, Nestler S, Hutteman R, Dufner M, Küfner AC, Egloff B, et al. Puffed-up but shaky selves: State self-esteem level and variability in narcissists. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2017) 112:769. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000093
32. Donnellan MB, Kenny DA, Trzesniewski KH, Lucas RE, and Conger RD. Using trait–state models to evaluate the longitudinal consistency of global self-esteem from adolescence to adulthood. J Res personality. (2012) 46:634–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.07.005
33. Trzesniewski KH, Donnellan MB, and Robins RW. Stability of self-esteem across the life span. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2003) 84:205. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.205
34. Jones EE, Rhodewalt F, Berglas S, and Skelton JA. Effects of strategic self-presentation on subsequent self-esteem. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1981) 41:407. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.41.3.407
35. Kernis MH. Measuring self-esteem in context: The importance of stability of self-esteem in psychological functioning. J personality. (2005) 73:1569–605. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00359.x
36. Mlawer F, Hubbard JA, Bookhout MK, and Moore CC. Levels and instability of daily self-esteem in adolescents: relations to depressive and anxious symptoms. Res Child Adolesc Psychopathology. (2021) 49:1083–95. doi: 10.1007/s10802-021-00802-3
37. Oosterwegel A, Field N, Hart D, and Anderson K. The relation of self-esteem variability to emotion variability, mood, personality traits, and depressive tendencies. J Personality. (2001) 69:689–708. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.695160
38. Hayes AM, Harris MS, and Carver CS. Predictors of self-esteem variability. Cogn Ther Res. (2004) 28:369–85. doi: 10.1023/B:COTR.0000031807.64718.b9
39. Sowislo JF, Orth U, and Meier LL. What constitutes vulnerable self-esteem? Comparing the prospective effects of low, unsta ble, and contingent self-esteem on depressive symptoms. J Abnormal Psychol. (2014) 123:737. doi: 10.1037/a0037770
40. DeHart T and Pelham BW. Fluctuations in state implicit self-esteem in response to daily negative events. J Exp Soc Psychol. (2007) 43:157–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.01.002
41. Skirrow C, Ebner-Priemer U, Reinhard I, Malliaris Y, Kuntsi J, and Asherson P. Everyday emotional experience of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: evidence for reactive and endogenous emotional lability. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:3571–83. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714001032
42. Colmsee I-SO, Hank P, and Bošnjak M. Low self-esteem as a risk factor for eating disorders. Zeitschrift für Psychologie. (2010). doi: 10.1027/2151-2604/a000433
43. van Zelst C, Van Nierop M, Oorschot M, Myin-Germeys I, van Os J, Delespaul P, et al. Stereotype awareness, self-esteem and psychopathology in people with psychosis. Plos one. (2014) 9:e88586. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088586
44. Shiffman S, Stone AA, and Hufford MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2008) 4:1–32. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415
45. Miguelez-Fernandez C, de Leon SJ, Baltasar-Tello I, Peñuelas-Calvo I, Barrigon ML, Capdevila AS, et al. Evaluating attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using ecological momentary assessment: a systematic review. ADHD Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disord. (2018) 10:247–65. doi: 10.1007/s12402-018-0261-1
46. Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone S, Hiripi E, et al. The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. psychol Med. (2005) 35:245–56. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002892
47. Sinclair SJ, Blais MA, Gansler DA, Sandberg E, Bistis K, LoCicero AJE, et al. Psychometric properties of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale: Overall and across demographic groups living within the United States. Eval Health professions. (2010) 33:56–80. doi: 10.1177/0163278709356187
48. Chen G, Gully SM, and Eden D. Validation of a new general self-efficacy scale. Organizational Res Methods. (2001) 4:62–83. doi: 10.1177/109442810141004
49. van Zyl LE, Klibert J, Shankland R, See-To EW, and Rothmann S. The general academic self-efficacy scale: psychometric properties, longitudinal invariance, and criterion validity. J psychoeducational assessment. (2022) 40:777–89. doi: 10.1177/07342829221097174
50. Norton PJ. Depression Anxiety and Stress Scales (DASS-21): Psychometric analysis across four racial groups. Anxiety stress coping. (2007) 20:253–65. doi: 10.1080/10615800701309279
51. Becerra R, Preece D, Campitelli G, and Scott-Pillow G. The assessment of emotional reactivity across negative and positive emotions: Development and validation of the Perth Emotional Reactivity Scale (PERS). Assessment. (2019) 26:867–79. doi: 10.1177/1073191117694455
52. Baruth KE and Caroll JJ. A formal assessment of resilience: the Baruth Protective Factors Inventory. J Individ Psychol. (2002) 58:235–44.
53. O’Brien ST, Dozo N, Hinton JD, Moeck EK, Susanto R, Jayaputera GT, et al. SEMA3: A free smartphone platform for daily life surveys. Behav Res Methods. (2024) 56:7691–706. doi: 10.3758/s13428-024-02445-w
54. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5-TR. Fifth, text revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing (2022).
55. Cvitkovic D, Pisonic SS, and Radošević I. Predictors of academic self-efficacy of university students: grades, learning disabilities and ADHD. Int J Special Education. (2024) 39:44–52. doi: 10.52291/ijse.2024.39.5
56. Sarid M and Lipka O. The relationship between academic self-efficacy and class engagement of self-reported LD and ADHD in Israeli undergraduate students during COVID-19. Eur J Psychol Education. (2024) 39:253–74. doi: 10.1007/s10212-023-00677-6
57. Rucklidge JJ, Downs-Woolley M, Taylor M, Brown JA, and Harrow S-E. Psychiatric comorbidities in a New Zealand sample of adults with ADHD. J Attention Disord. (2016) 20:1030–8. doi: 10.1177/1087054714529457
58. Soler-Gutiérrez A-M, Pérez-González J-C, and Mayas J. Evidence of emotion dysregulation as a core symptom of adult ADHD: A systematic review. PloS One. (2023) 18:e0280131. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0280131
59. Rosenberg M. Rosenberg self-esteem scale (RSE). Acceptance commitment Ther Measures package. (1965) 61:18. doi: 10.1037/t01038-000
60. Onorato RS and Turner JC. Fluidity in the self-concept: the shift from personal to social identity. Eur J Soc Psychol. (2004) 34:257–78. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.195
61. Kuntsi J and Klein C. Intraindividual variability in ADHD and its implications for research of causal links. In: Behavioral neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and its treatment. New York: Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. (2011). p. 67–91.
62. Mazereel V, Vansteelandt K, Menne-Lothmann C, Decoster J, Derom C, Thiery E, et al. The complex and dynamic interplay between self-esteem, belongingness and physical activity in daily life: An experience sampling study in adolescence and young adulthood. Ment Health Phys activity. (2021) 21:100413. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2021.100413
63. DesRoches A and Willoughby T. Bidirectional associations between valued activities and adolescent positive adjustment in a longitudinal study: Positive mood as a mediator. J Youth adolescence. (2014) 43:208–20. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-9957-x
64. Tazghini S and Siedlecki KL. A mixed method approach to examining Facebook use and its relationship to self-esteem. Comput Hum Behavior. (2013) 29:827–32. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.010
65. Caci H, Asherson P, DonFrancesco R, Faraone SV, Hervas A, Fitzgerald M, et al. Daily life impairments associated with childhood/adolescent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder as recalled by adults: results from the European Lifetime Impairment Survey. CNS spectrums. (2015) 20:112–21. doi: 10.1017/S1092852914000078
66. Whalen CK, Jamner LD, Henker B, Delfino RJ, and Lozano JM. The ADHD spectrum and everyday life: Experience sampling of adolescent moods, activities, smoking, and drinking. Child Dev. (2002) 73:209–27. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00401
67. Forgasz HJ and Leder GC. Academic life: Monitoring work patterns and daily activities. Aust Educ researcher. (2006) 33:1–22. doi: 10.1007/BF03246278
68. Stevens AE, Abu-Ramadan TM, and Hartung CM. Promoting academic success in college students with ADHD and LD: A systematic literature review to identify intervention targets. J Am Coll Health. (2022) 70:2342–55. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2020.1862127
69. Wilmshurst L, Peele M, and Wilmshurst L. Resilience and well-being in college students with and without a diagnosis of ADHD. J Attention Disord. (2011) 15:11–7. doi: 10.1177/1087054709347261
70. Foubert JD and Urbanski LA. Effects of involvement in clubs and organizations on the psychosocial development of first-year and senior college students. NASPA J. (2006) 43:166–82. doi: 10.2202/1949-6605.1576
71. Cagirtekin A and Tanriverdi O. Social hobbies can increase self-esteem and quality of life in female breast cancer patients with type A personality trait: KRATOS study. Med Oncol. (2022) 40:50. doi: 10.1007/s12032-022-01925-3
72. Bishop JA and Inderbitzen HM. Peer acceptance and friendship: An investigation of their relation to self-esteem. J Early Adolescence. (1995) 15:476–89. doi: 10.1177/0272431695015004005
73. Han SS and Kim KM. Influencing factors on self-esteem in adolescents. J Korean Acad Nursing. (2006) 36:37–44. doi: 10.4040/jkan.2006.36.1.37
74. Akdemir D, Çak T, Aslan C, Aydos B, Nalbant K, and Cuhadaroglu-Cetin F. Predictors of self-esteem in adolescents with a psychiatric referral. Turkish J pediatrics. (2016) 58:69–78. doi: 10.24953/turkjped.2016.01.010
75. Cook J, Knight E, Hume I, and Qureshi A. The self-esteem of adults diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a systematic review of the literature. ADHD Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disord. (2014) 6:249–68. doi: 10.1007/s12402-014-0133-2
Keywords: ADHD, young adults, university students, self-esteem, daily activities
Citation: Turner EH and Harty SC (2025) Daily activities and self-esteem among university students with and without ADHD. Front. Psychiatry 16:1622354. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1622354
Received: 03 May 2025; Accepted: 04 August 2025;
Published: 26 August 2025.
Edited by:
Anthony Rostain, Cooper Medical School of Rowan University, United StatesReviewed by:
Miriam Sarid, Western Galilee College, IsraelKhushmand Rajendran, Borough of Manhattan Community College, United States
Copyright © 2025 Turner and Harty. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Seth C. Harty, c2V0aC5oYXJ0eUBjYW50ZXJidXJ5LmFjLm56
†These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship
‡ORCID: Seth C. Harty, orcid.org/0000-0002-5571-4487