Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 29 July 2025

Sec. Mood Disorders

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1624776

Cognitive and motor disturbances in depression: insights from comprehensive behavioral assessments

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Huntsman Mental Health Institute, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, United States
  • 2Department of Health, Medical and Neuropsychology, Leiden University, Leiden, Netherlands
  • 3Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden, Netherlands
  • 4McLean Hospital, Harvard University, Belmont, MA, United States
  • 5Department of Neuroscience, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, Netherlands

Background: Depression affects not only mood and reward processing, but also motor and cognitive functioning, leading to psychomotor disturbances crucial for diagnosis, prognosis, and treatment. Patients with severe psychomotor retardation often respond poorly to SSRIs but benefit from neurostimulation like ECT. However, comprehensive assessments of cognitive and motor domains in the same depression sample are rare.

Methods: This pilot study compared 20 depressed patients and 22 controls across multiple tests of cognitive and motor functions. We examined executive function and processing speed (Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Test), verbal and visual learning and memory (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT-R), and Brief Visuospatial Test (BVMT-R), gait (2-minute walking, 4-meter walking and walking while talking (WWT) tests), sarcopenia (grip strength ftest, knee extension test), and fine motor function (Archimedes Spiral Test, 9 Hole Peg Test). Associations between depression severity and behavioral performance were also explored.

Results: Depressed participants performed significantly worse on the color naming and interference conditions within the D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Test and on the HVLT's delayed recall. They were slower on the 9 Hole Peg Test with both their dominant and nondominant hands, while no differences were noted in gait or sarcopenia. Greater depression severity correlated with poorer performance on the WWT dual cognitive-motor task and quicker movement on the Archimedes Spiral task.

Conclusions: These findings reveal decrements in cognitive and motor domains in depressed individuals, which could impact daily functioning. Overall, results from this pilot study suggest that examining motor disturbances alongside cognitive disturbances could serve as a marker of disease progression and a potential target for intervention.

1 Introduction

A depressive episode is defined by low mood or anhedonia (i.e., loss of interest in normally enjoyable activities), and can be accompanied by decreased energy, concentration problems, psychomotor retardation or agitation, changes in appetite or sleep, increased feelings of worthlessness, guilt, or thoughts of death and suicide (1). For a clinical diagnosis, the constellation of the above symptoms has to be linked to substantial functional impairment and be present for at least two weeks (2). Functional impairment as a result of depression is apparent in daily living activities, interpersonal relationships, work, and scholarly productivity (3, 4). Notably, depression is one of the leading causes of disability worldwide (5, 6). The economic burden of US adults with depression was calculated to be $326 billion in 2018 (7). The high prevalence of this severely disabling disease in combination with the large economic impact warrants the development of novel treatments. In an effort to improve the understanding and treatment of mood disorders, the National Institute of Mental Health developed Research Domain Criteria (RDoC), aiming to identify dimensional models of psychopathology instead of using hard symptom cutoffs for the diagnosis of mood disorders (8, 9). More recently, psychomotor abnormalities were added as a novel RDoC domain (10), highlighting the value of studying psychomotor disturbances as predictor and outcome measures in depression.

Psychomotor abnormalities are known to present in depression as psychomotor retardation or agitation, and their presence typically indicates a more severe disease phenotype (11). Psychomotor retardation in depression can present as slower speed of movement, reduced speaking rate, lack of motor initiation, body immobility, postural abnormalities, loss of facial expressions, and cognitive ideation (12). Psychomotor agitation can present as increased activity, motor restlessness or irritability, characterized by aimless, non-purposeful movements (13, 14). These impairments reflect both a neuromotor and a higher-level cognitive component. Motor deficits that have been observed in depression that can affect both treatment outcomes and daily functioning, independent of motivational factors, include gait impairments, sarcopenia (e.g., grip strength), and poorer fine motor skills (15). Although the motor component of psychomotor disturbances has been recognized as a potential fundamental dimension of depression, prior studies lack comprehensive assessments across all motor domains and, to our knowledge, objective and quantifiable motor profiles are not currently being developed for depression. Motor deficits in depression may, to a certain degree, reflect impairments in cognitive functioning. It has already been established that successful motor performance does not only require the direct, physical control of muscles, but also involves cognitive processes that allow us to engage in goal-directed behavior (16, 17). Especially higher order psychomotor functions that depend on intact cognition, such as visuospatial function, attention, working memory, and executive functioning (18). Dual-task studies show that increased cognitive load worsens motor performance, supporting this interaction (19, 20). Motor and cognitive functions are closely linked via shared neural circuits, and disruptions in one can impact the other, such as driving a car (21, 22). Cognitive impairment also plays a key role in the overall functional impairment associated with depression. More importantly, cognitive dysfunction and thus functional impairment to some degree persist even after improvement of depressive symptoms (23, 24). This is independent from disease severity and can be long-lasting (3). Additionally, cognitive dysfunction not only prevents a full recovery from depression, but it also increases the risk for recurrence of episodes (25, 26).

Over the years, several subtypes of depression have been proposed and identified for patients exhibiting more significant psychomotor impairment (e.g., atypical, melancholic depression) (27). Patients presenting with these psychomotor-related phenotypes respond better to specific antidepressant pharmacotherapy (e.g. tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs), monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs)) (28), which are different from what is usually used as a first line treatment in a major depressive episode (i.e., SSRIs) (29). Also, evidence suggests that patients with this depressed phenotype respond better to neurostimulation interventions (e.g., electroconvulsive therapy) (30). Moreover, studies support that the presence of severe psychomotor impairment in a depressive episode might indicate the presence of bipolar disorder rather than major depressive disorder, suggesting potential utility for differential diagnosis (31). Overall better characterization and understanding of psychomotor abnormalities has great diagnostic, prognostic, and treatment importance for depression.

To identify which cognitive and motor functions are most strongly affected by depression, performance across the breadth of domains should be evaluated. The present pilot study explores the impact of depression on cognitive and motor functions by comparing performance between adults with a current depressive episodes and control subjects across a series of objective cognitive and motor tasks, including those evaluating executive function, processing speed, verbal and visual learning and memory, gait, sarcopenia, and fine motor function. These domains, rarely examined together within a single sample of people with depression, could reveal a pattern of subtle cognitive and motor deficits, which could directly be linked to daily functioning. Relative to clinician-rated tools, our approach offers ecologically more valid measures to capture such a broad motor profile in this population, potentially supporting earlier detection and intervention.

We hypothesized that depressed individuals would perform worse on a series of cognitive tasks as well as a variety of motor measures than controls, specifically on tasks that measure motor speed. We further examined the association between depression symptom severity and behavioral performance within the depressed sample, hypothesizing that more severe depression is related to poorer psychomotor performance.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

A total of 20 individuals with a major depressive episode and 22 control subjects were enrolled in this pilot study. They were recruited through the University of Utah clinics and surrounding communities via referrals and flyers, the University of Utah study Locator Website, the University of Utah-Center on Aging Participant Registry, and researchmatch.org. The pilot study was approved by the University of Utah Institutional Review Board (#00139658, 05/07/2021) and it was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Inclusion criteria included age of 18 years and above for all participants. For the depressed sample, a current major depressive episode as identified through the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) 7.0 (32), and of at least moderate severity based on the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) (33), was required. Participants were also required to be planning or currently receiving treatment for depression. Presence of psychotic features was not considered an exclusion criterion. We did not differentiate between unipolar and bipolar depression; however, we screened participants for past manic and hypomanic symptomatology. None of our participants met criteria for current or past manic/hypomanic episodes, and only one participant reported past hypomanic symptoms without meeting full criteria. Study exclusion criteria included poorly controlled medical conditions, pregnancy, a diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, history of traumatic brain injury or other neurological signs during the past year, MRI contraindications, substance use disorder during the past 3 months, neurocognitive disorder diagnosis during the past year, and diagnosis of autism or intellectual disability. Additionally, specifically to the control group we excluded participants with bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder (lifetime diagnosis), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), eating disorder (current) and daily use of psychotropic medication.

All participants were screened with the MINI in order to confirm that inclusion/exclusion criteria were met. Individuals with depression underwent a subsequent phone/video conference interview with a psychiatrist or psychiatry resident, during which the 17 and 24 item HAM-D; were administered to validate the severity of depression. After participant screening, but prior to their in-person visit, participants completed the Patient Health Questionnaire-9, a self-reported tool designed to screen for and measure the severity of depression, online (34). Out of the 20 depressed participants, only 17 completed the HAM-D scales due to scheduling conflicts and withdrawing prior to their appointment. Through the MINI, we gathered information on past and recurrent depressive episodes. Of the 20 depressed participants, 18 met criteria for at least one past major episode of depression, and 16 met criteria for recurrent major depressive episodes. On average, depressed participants reported 15.9 depressive episodes over their lifetime. (Supplementary Table S7).

2.2 Behavioral measures

2.2.1 Gait

The 4 meter walk test (35) was used to measure gait speed. For this test, participants walked over a straight, 8-meter course with markers at 2m, 6m, and 8m that were placed on the floor. Participants started at the 0m end of the course and were instructed to walk 8 meters overall. Walking duration was recorded once the subject’s leading foot crossed over the 2m marker and ended when the subject’s leading foot crossed the 6m marker. The initial two meters (0m - 2m) allowed subjects to reach a steady walking pace before recording. The last two meters (6m - 8m) were included to prevent a premature decline in speed while recording. Participants completed three trials at normal walking pace and three trials at maximum gait speed without running. The average velocity (m/s) across all three trials was calculated as the outcome measure.

We also used the 2-minute walk test to measure distance traveled at a casual gait speed. For this test, participants were asked to walk at their regular walking pace over a straight walking path of 40 feet of which the beginning and the end were marked. Participants were asked to make a 180 degree turn and continue toward the other end each time to reach the end of the path, repeating this cycle once for a duration of two minutes. The outcome measure was total distance traveled in meters.

Finally, we used the walking-while-talking (WWT) test (36) to measure motor-cognitive dual-tasking. The WWT test involved reciting alternate letters of the alphabet. Subjects were requested to walk a 16-meter course with a turn in the middle. The course was set out with markers at 2m and 8m. Subjects were requested to start walking at 0m and at the 2m mark, the timer was started. From there, subjects were instructed to recite every other letter from the alphabet, while continuing to walk to the 8m mark, make a turn, and then walk back over the 2m mark where the timer was stopped. Participants were instructed to pay equal attention to both walking and reciting. They completed three trials that differed in the starting letter of the alphabet, which was A, B, or N for trials 1, 2, and 3, respectively. Performance was calculated based on the total number of letters recited, the number of correct letters, and the ratios of total transitions per second and correct transitions per second. Velocity (m/s) during the WWT task was also calculated as distance covered in each trial divided by walking time for each respective trial. Dual-task cost was determined as the difference in velocity in meters per second between the normal pace walking on the 4-meter walking test and the dual-task. The average measure across the three trials was calculated for all outcomes of the WWT task. Because one depressed subject did not adhere to the task instructions, data for this participant were removed from analyses.

2.2.2 Sarcopenia

Hand grip strength was obtained in pounds using a hydraulic Jamar Digital Hand Dynamometer. Participants were asked to sit comfortably in a standard chair with legs, back support and fixed armrests. They were instructed to rest their forearms on the arms of the chair with their wrist over the edge of the armrest and their thumb facing upwards. During each trial, participants were required to grip the dynamometer as long and as tightly as possible until the number on the display did not increase further (this generally happened after ~5 sec). Participants completed three trials per hand, alternating between them after each recorded trial. All participants began with their left hand. The maximum performance for participants’ dominant and non-dominant hand across the three trials was used as the outcome measure for this analysis.

Knee extension strength was collected in pounds with the Lafayette Instrument Hand-Held Dynamometer (Model 01165). Participants were instructed to lie supine on a padded mat, with a bolster positioned under the knee completing the trial. They had their tested knee with the bolster fixed at an angle of 35° and their untested knee flat on the mat. Before each trial, the dynamometer was placed on the tibia, proximal to the medial malleolus. During recording, participants were informed that once the trial began, they would hear a tone from the device, then a second tone to start extending their knee with as much force as possible until they heard two tones in rapid succession, indicating the end of the trial. All participants started with their left knee and alternated between left and right for six trials in total. The amount of maximum force generated during each trial was recorded in pounds. The maximum performance of participants’ dominant and non-dominant leg across the three trials was used as the outcome measure.

2.2.3 Fine motor skills

The 9 Hole Peg Test was administered to record dexterity and fine motor skills by recording the time for subjects to place and remove nine pegs within nine holes. For this task, the JAMAR pegboard was used during data collection. This consisted of a design with a 3x3 grid of holes with a receptacle holding nine pegs adjacent to it. Participants were instructed to place their hand next to the board and to move each peg, one at a time to each hole once the trial began. Once all pegs were placed, the participant was required to remove each peg one at a time back to the receptacle. Participants were allowed to place and remove the pegs in any order of their choosing. Participants began by using their dominant hand and were given one practice trial before the recorded time trial began. Six-time trials were then completed, alternating between dominant and non-dominant hands. The time taken by each participant to place and remove each peg was recorded in seconds and used as an outcome measure.

An in-house developed computerized Archimedes Spiral Test was conducted to measure speed and accuracy functions of fine motor skills. The testing materials consisted of a spiral template that was printed on a piece of paper attached to an electronic drawing board (WACOM Graphire Wireless Pen Tablet, model CTE630BT). Participants were instructed to place the pen in the middle of the spiral before the tracing started. They were not allowed to lean on the drawing board with their hand or arm and were asked to trace the spiral as accurately and as fast as possible over three trials using their dominant hand. Automatic quantitative analyses were performed using custom-made software written in MATLAB (version 8.1; The Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA) (3739). This yielded the following outcome measures: movement time (s), defined by the time it took the participant to trace the spiral; length of drawing (cm), defined as the length of the drawn spiral; average speed, defined by the ratio of length of drawing and movement time; speed variability (cm/s), defined as the standard deviation of the instantaneous speed; deviation from template (in pixels), defined as the area between the template and the drawn spiral; number of crossings, defined as the number of times the drawn spiral crossed the template; and return movements, defined as the absence or presence of one or more return movements (i.e., brief movements in the opposite direction). For the analysis, the minimum value across the three trials was calculated for movement time, return movements, and the number of times participants crossed the template. Similarly, for the deviation from the template outcome, the minimum value of the average, sum, and standard deviation of the absolute radial distance between the template and the spiral (in pixels) across the trials was used. The maximum value of the average speed across the three trials was also calculated for the analysis.

2.2.4 Cognitive function

The Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) (40) was used to measure processing speed. In this symbol/number coding task, participants are presented with a key containing nine unique geometric shapes paired with numbers ranging from 1-9. They are instructed to use this key to match symbols that are presented in a sequence to their corresponding numbers, without skipping any items. After completing a practice run of 10 items, participants have 90 seconds to complete as many items in consecutive order as possible. The outcome measure for the SDMT is the total number of correctly completed items.

We used the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised (HVLT-R) (41, 42) to test short- and long- term verbal memory and learning. Across three trials, participants were presented with a list of 12 words that were read to them and instructed to verbally recall as many words as they could. Without prior notification, after 25 minutes elapsed, participants were asked to report how many words they can remember to test delayed recall. The outcome measures for the HVLT-R were age adjusted T-scores (scaled scores) for the total number of immediate and delayed correctly recalled words. Scaled scores were derived from the HVLT-R professional manual.

The Brief Visuospatial Memory Test Revised (BVMT-R) (43) was used to test visual memory and learning. For this task, participants were asked to carefully study a sheet with six figures. They have 10 seconds and then are asked to draw each figure exactly as it appeared (shape) and place it in the same area as it was presented on the sheet (location). This was repeated for three trials. Without prior notification, after 25 minutes elapsed, participants were asked to try to draw as many figures as they could remember in the correct location and shape. The outcome measures for the BVMT-R were age adjusted T-scores (scaled scores) for the total number of immediate and delayed correctly drawn designs. Scaled scores were derived from the BVMT-R professional manual.

Finally, we used the D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Test (44) to measure cognitive interference. This task consists of four trials, beginning with the color naming trial, during which participants are presented with a page containing a series of red, green, and blue squares. Participants are asked to say the names of the colors as quickly as they can without making mistakes. This is followed by the word reading trial, where they are presented with a page containing the words “red,” “green,” and “blue” printed in black ink and are asked to read the words aloud as quickly as they can without making mistakes. Subsequently, participants complete the inhibition trial, where they are shown a page containing the words “red,” “green,” and “blue” printed incongruently in red, green, or blue ink. They are asked to say the color of the ink in which each word is printed as quickly as they can without making mistakes. Last is the inhibition/switching trial, where a page containing the words “red,” “green,” and “blue” written in red, green, or blue ink is provided to participants. Half of these words are enclosed within boxes, while the other half are presented without. Participants are asked to say the color of the ink in which each word is printed (if unboxed), but to read the word aloud (and not name the ink color) when a word appears inside a box, as quickly as they can without making mistakes. As outcome measures, we used the time to complete each different trial and the number of self-corrected and uncorrected responses at each trial. Each timed trial results were converted to scaled scores based on participants’ age and trial duration, following the normative tables provided within the D-KEFS Examiner manual.

2.3 Hand dominance

Hand dominance was determined using the Dutch handedness questionnaire (45). This is a 16-item self-assessment handedness questionnaire asking which hand is being used for certain operations (e.g., opening a jar). Scores of -1, 0, and 1 were assigned to items for which the subject selected left hand, ambidextrous, and right hand, respectively. Final scores were binarized (left-handed vs. right-handed) by averaging the scores on the 16 items and selecting left-handedness for a negative mean score and right-handedness for a positive mean score.

2.4 Procedure

Once participants were determined eligible for participation, electronic informed consent was completed. On the day of their visit, participants completed the following behavioral assessments: 2-minute walk test, 4-meter walk test, WWT test, SDMT, D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Test, 9-Hole Peg Test, HVLT-R, Archimedes Spiral Test, BVMT-R, grip strength, and knee extension strength. Subjects received $90 compensation for completing the entire study.

2.5 Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed in R version 4.2.2 (2022-10-31). Prior to statistical analysis, all data were evaluated for outliers, but this did not result in any exclusions. Group differences in potentially confounding continuous and categorical variables were compared using t-tests and chi-square tests, respectively. Both the t-tests and chi-square tests were conducted using functions from base R (R core tools). Group differences in behavioral measures were tested using linear regression models adjusted for age, sex, and education. The η2 value for the group factor, i.e., the proportion of variance in the model explained by group, is reported as a measure of effect size for the group comparisons. Association between depression severity and behavioral measures was tested using linear regression models, also adjusted for age, sex, education. For these regression models we used behavioral measures as the outcome and depression severity (HAMD-24 scores) as the predictor. The η2 value for the depression severity, i.e., the proportion of variance in the model explained by the HAMD-24 score, is reported as a measure of effect size for the HAMD-24 score. An alpha threshold of 0.05 (two-tailed) was used for all hypotheses. Models were adjusted for multiple comparisons using false discovery rate (FDR) correction (46).

3 Results

3.1 Demographic and clinical characteristics

An overview of the demographic information is presented in Table 1. There were no significant differences in mean age, years of education completed, nor sex distribution differences between the control and depressed participants. Participants in the depression group showed significantly more symptoms of depression on the PHQ-9 than controls. For three individuals from the depression group, the HAM-D interviews were not completed due to scheduling conflicts and withdrawing prior to their appointment.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the present sample.

3.2 Group differences in cognitive and motor behavioral outcome measures

3.2.1 Cognition

Group differences in behavioral outcome measures in cognitive domains are displayed in Table 2. Significant differences in performance were observed in the color naming and cognitive interference conditions of the D-KEFS Color-Word Interference test, as well as in the delayed recall of the HVLT-R. In all above instances, the depressed participants performed worse than the controls; however, these results did not survive FDR correction. No significant differences were observed in the remaining cognitive function measures.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Group differences in cognitive performance.

3.2.2 Gait

Group differences in the gait domain outcome measures are displayed in Table 3. Results showed no significant differences between depressed participants and controls for any of the gait measures.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Group differences in the gait domain.

3.2.3 Sarcopenia

Group differences in the sarcopenia domain outcome measures are displayed in Table 4. There were no significant differences observed between depressed participants and controls in the sarcopenia outcome measures.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Group differences in the sarcopenia domain.

3.2.4 Fine motor function

Group differences in the fine motor function domain outcome measures are displayed in Table 5. Compared to control participants, those with depression were significantly slower at the 9 Hole Peg Test both for the dominant and nondominant hand. These differences survived FDR correction for the nondominant hand. No group differences in Archimedes spiral tracing performance were observed.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Group differences in the fine motor function domain.

3.3 Association between depression severity and behavioral measures

Significant associations between depression severity and behavioral performance within the depressed sample are displayed in Figure 1. Greater severity of depression is associated with poorer performance on the WWT dual cognitive-motor task within the gait domain and quicker movement times in the Archimedes Spiral task within the fine motor function domain; however, these associations did not survive FDR correction. No significant associations were observed for the remaining behavioral measures. Detailed results are shown in Supplementary Tables S1-S4.

Figure 1
Two scatter plots show relationships with HAMD 24 Total Score. The left plot depicts Total Movement Time, revealing a negative correlation with a b-value of -0.301 and p-value of 0.040. The right plot illustrates Total Transition Ratio, also negatively correlated with a b-value of -0.009 and p-value of 0.045. Both plots include regression lines and data points. Blue curves frame the plots' top and right sides, with scores ranging from fifteen to thirty-five on the x-axes.

Figure 1. Association between depression severity and behavioral measures. Scatterplots with distribution density curves for the HAMD-24 total score (top) and behavioral measure (right-hand side). Left Scatter plot with a regression line showing the association between depression severity as indicated by HAM-D 24 scores and total movement time in the Archimedes Spiral Test. Right Scatter plot with a regression line showing the association between depression severity and total transitions ratio in the walking while talking test. Note that the movement time reflects the minimum movement time across the three trials, and the ratio reflects the average across three trials.

4 Discussion

This pilot study aimed to examine how motor and cognitive abilities are affected in depression. To this end, a group of depressed individuals with and a group of healthy controls performed several objective motor and cognitive tests. The present results show that, in comparison to controls, depressed participants performed worse on measures of executive function, verbal memory, and fine motor abilities, but not in terms of gait or sarcopenia. Within the depressed group, it was observed that greater depression severity was associated with poorer cognitive-motor dual-tasking performance and faster movement time on the Archimedes spiral task assessing fine motor function.

4.1 Depressed participants show significant fine motor impairments, but no deficits in other motor domains

Depressed participants performed worse on the 9 Hole Peg Test reflecting poorer fine motor abilities. Fine motor alterations have been established as a feature of depressive symptomatology (47), yet few studies have assessed them using objective, standardized measures. Fine motor problems can substantially affect daily functioning, impacting routine tasks, work productivity, and social and recreational activities (48). Although a clear relationship between psychomotor slowing and poor functional outcomes has been established in schizophrenia, this connection has not been specifically investigated in depression. Studies that examine specific domains of motor function, such as fine motor skills, and the degree of functional disability they account for could help us better understand the contribution of motor function to the disease burden of depression. Furthermore, fine motor alterations may also interfere with specific treatments for depression. For example, an evidence-based treatment called behavioral activation encourages individuals to engage more consistently in enjoyable activities and fulfill meaningful life roles. This approach aims to disrupt patterns of avoidance, withdrawal, and inactivity that are common in depression (49). However, individuals with fine motor disturbances may find it challenging to participate in certain activities (e.g., drawing, playing video games), thus potentially hindering the effectiveness of behavioral activation treatment. Moreover, theories on social functioning in depression suggest that a lack of skills in a particular domain may lead to low self-acceptance (50) and potentially interact negatively with emotional processing (51). The ways in which motor disturbances interact with other domains affected in depression—such as positive valence, social functioning, and arousal—remain largely unexplored (52). However, investigating these relationships could deepen our understanding of the role of motor function in depression and inform the development of targeted treatments to address these specific impairments.

There are several potential explanations for the lack of a statistical group difference in gait and sarcopenia motor domains. First, previous literature reports that increased gait variability during dual-tasking has been demonstrated in older depressed patients (20, 36) with depression also being an independent risk factor for falls in elderly patients. This suggests that motor brain reserves are probably affected more significantly later in life (67.2 ± 3.6 years) (53). Because with a mean age of 32.8 years, our sample was much younger on average these motor deficits may not have fully emerged yet. Another explanation could be that the motor task was too simple. Walking is considered to be a relatively automatic process (54). This is supported by the fact that during earlier developmental stages (childhood), where this automatization is under development, more profound variability in gait measures has been found (55, 56). Interestingly, a study examining the motor characteristics of various phases of bipolar disorder found significantly faster gait during the hypomanic phase. However, no significant differences were observed during the depressed phase. The authors suggest that both psychomotor agitation and retardation may co-occur during depression, potentially exerting opposing effects (57). Additionally, sarcopenia has been linked to depression, primarily because depressive symptoms such as fatigue, poor appetite, and low motivation can result in decreased physical activity and increased periods of sitting or lying down. Over time, these behaviors may lead to sarcopenia (58). However, our study found no significant differences in this domain, which may be explained by the characteristics of our sample. Since our participants were community-dwelling patients and not hospitalized, their depression may not have been severe enough for the effects leading to sarcopenia to become apparent.

4.2 Depressed participants exhibit significant decrements in processing speed, cognitive interference, and memory

It was also observed that depressed participants exhibited poorer performance in the color-naming and cognitive interference (inhibition) trials of the D-KEFS Color-Word Interference task, but their performance was not significantly different in the word-naming and inhibition/switching trials. It is widely accepted that color naming is more time-consuming than word naming, as it is thought to involve more complex cognitive processes (59). Moreover, although the inhibition/switching trial is generally considered more challenging, there is anecdotal evidence suggesting otherwise. The inhibition trial, which heavily involves color naming and is more time-consuming, precedes the inhibition/switching trial. Here, participants might have already gained practice with the inhibition process. These findings suggest that the cognitive interference (inhibition) trial also requires more cognitive resources (60). This shows that under conditions of greater cognitive demand, participants with depression underperformed compared to controls. Our results of reduced processing speed and cognitive interference in depression corroborate the existing literature on neurocognitive impairment in depression. Prior studies indicate that cognitive difficulties not only contribute to psychosocial deficits, but that they also decrease the probability of remission and recovery from a depressive episode (25, 61). Moreover, a recent study using a letter sorting test with different levels of difficulty showed that increased cognitive effort was associated with lower momentary mood (62). Collectively, these findings underscore how cognitive impairments compound the functional impairments caused by motor deficits in depression. Studies on other disorders with psychomotor changes, such as Parkinson’s disease and Huntington’s disease (63, 64), which often co-occur with depression, suggest that psychomotor disturbances may reflect specific biological underpinnings involving neural circuits like the basal ganglia. A recent study demonstrated that individuals with depression show distinct connectivity patterns within the motor network, even in remission (65). These findings indicate that both cognitive and motor dysfunctions persist in remitting states, underscoring the importance of developing interventions targeting both cognitive and motor functions in depression.

Notably, depressed participants exhibited poorer performance on the delayed recall portion of the HVLT task. While verbal memory deficits are typically associated with late-life onset of depression (66), findings in younger patients have been less consistent. However, such decrements in late-life depression have been linked to an increased risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease (67, 68). Given that the mean age of our sample is 32.8 years, our results suggest that memory difficulties can also manifest in younger individuals with depression. This highlights the importance of early interventions in the treatment of depression to potentially mitigate these cognitive impairments.

4.3 Severity of depression predicts poorer dual-task performance but unexpected patterns in spiral drawing performance

Within the depressed sample, we observed that greater severity of depression correlated with poorer performance on the WWT dual cognitive-motor task within the gait domain and quicker movement times in the Archimedes Spiral task within the fine motor function domain. More specifically, higher depression severity was linked to fewer transitions per second in the WWT aspect of the dual-task. This part of the task predominantly measures the cognitive component of movement (20), indicating that decrements in this area may reflect not only restricted cognitive resources but also a diminished capacity to allocate these resources effectively during multitasking (69, 70). These findings suggest diminished cognitive control in individuals with more severe depression and support the idea that psychomotor difficulties reflect a more severe depression phenotype. Additionally, this could also reflect the relationship between cognitive and motor functions, potentially demonstrating how cognitive decrements underlie motor ones in depression. Interestingly, despite significant associations with the cognitive component of the dual task, we did not observe a significant relationship between depression severity and dual-task cost, calculated as the difference in walking speed between the 4-meter walk and the dual-task condition. A more nuanced method proposed by Brauner et al., the P-index (71), weighs dual task performance by cognitive accuracy and may thus offer a more sensitive metric. This promising approach could be applied in future studies.

Interestingly, in contrast to our expectations, faster movement times on the Archimedes Spiral task were associated with greater severity of depression. This finding could have several possible explanations. One potential factor is poor motivation and effort, which are often observed in depressed individuals and could interfere with their ability to fully engage in the task (72). This phenomenon suggests that the apparent speed in task completion may not necessarily reflect enhanced motor function, but rather a reduced quality of task engagement driven by the underlying symptoms of depression. This could also explain why no group differences in performance on the Archimedes spiral test were observed, as the group of depressed participants comprises both mildly and moderately severely depressed.

4.4 The need for precision in defining psychomotor disturbances in psychiatric research

Taken together, our findings underscore the need to clarify what constitutes psychomotor disturbance in depression. Although psychomotor symptoms are frequently referenced in both clinical and research settings, the term itself remains poorly defined and is often used to describe a wide range of motor and cognitive symptoms without clear distinction (18, 73). Some researchers argue that the term should focus only on the motor aspects of psychomotor retardation. However, others believe this view is too narrow, as cognitive factors are closely linked to motor symptoms (74). Moreover, psychomotor symptoms are often intertwined with other aspects of depression, such as fatigue, motivation, and impaired attentional processes, with no clear boundaries (75, 76). For example, some clinical measures of psychomotor disturbances evaluate the cognitive aspect, including attentional tests [e.g. Trail Making Test (77), Posner’s covert orientation of visual attention test (COVA) (78)], while others do not [e.g. Nufferno Speed test (79)] (80, 81). By using objective measures of cognitive and motor domains, this study contributes to the process of disentangling these overlapping functions. Future studies should also include measures that assess cognitive processes underlying motor functioning, to further elucidate psychomotor functioning in depression.

4.5 Strengths and limitations

A notable strength of our study is that we used a series of objective cognitive and motor behavioral tasks, spanning multiple domains. While prior studies have investigated different cognitive domains within a single depressed sample [e.g., (8284)], and some studies report on multiple motor functions in depression (85, 86), to our knowledge this study is the first to investigate different motor domains in a depressed adult sample using objective behavioral tasks. Moreover, the tasks utilized are reflective of many daily activities and daily functioning and can directly serve as a functional outcome measure (87).

There are certain limitations to this study. Cognitive and motor function was studied in participants with a currently active depressive episode. An open question therefore remains whether our findings generalize to other depression states (e.g., remitted). Previous studies indicated that cognitive deficits in depression are long-lasting and could persist even if patients return to the euthymic state. We recommend that future studies examine cognitive and motor outcomes in patients with depression in remission or use longitudinal designs to further investigate whether cognitive and motor outcomes in depression change as the depressive symptoms improve.

Similarly, our current sample size did not allow us to differentiate between potential subtypes of depressive symptomatology. It has been well-established that psychomotor changes are present in a depressive episode, yet not all patients with depression will experience those symptoms. Additionally, psychomotor changes can encompass both agitation and retardation, which have opposite effects. Therefore, distinguishing between depressed patients with psychomotor changes versus those without would also be crucial. We acknowledge that the sample size in this study is small; however, this was a pilot investigation specifically designed to explore preliminary signals of cognitive and motor dysfunction in depression. We believe these early findings provide valuable insights that support the need for further, larger-scale research in this area.

Another limitation of this work is that we did not examine premorbid functioning of the depressed participants. Education level (Supplementary Table S8) was included as a covariate in our analysis; however, we cannot rule out that some of the observed performance differences may be explained by other factors unrelated to depression. However, we cannot rule out the possibility that some observed performance differences may be influenced by factors unrelated to depression. Additional sociodemographic variables, such as income and occupation, could provide further insight into premorbid functioning and may prove valuable for future research. An additional confounding factor may be the presence of moderate anxiety in the depressed group, which was not observed in the control group. Given that anxiety can contribute to motor alterations (15), future studies should control for its potential effects. Another significant contributing factor could have been medication effects (88). Studies with longitudinal designs where cognitive and motor performance is measured prior to and during an active depressive episode could resolve this issue. A list of medications used by the depressed and control groups can be found in Supplementary Tables S5 and S6.

A final suggestion for future research relates to better understanding the impact of cognitive and motor decrements on functioning in daily life. While neurocognitive decrements have been documented more extensively in older patients (36, 89) our study and growing evidence suggest that this may start earlier in life (90, 91) when individuals are still actively involved in the workforce community, may have a young family and other social obligations. Therefore, understanding the progression of cognitive and motor function across the lifespan in patients with depression can help us further determine the neurocognitive and motor impact of depression. As suggested by others, including functional outcome measures in different domains [e.g., physical health, psychological health, social relationships, and environment as measured via the WHOQOL (92)] could be very meaningful to this end.

5 Conclusions

Taken together, the current study indicates that depressed participants experience cognitive and motor decrements, specifically in processing speed, cognitive inhibition, delayed recall, and fine motor function, but not in episodic memory or gross motor function. These findings align with indications that depression significantly impacts executive functioning and memory and demonstrate a decline in fine motor function. These results are directly related to overall daily functioning and quality of life. Therefore, the current findings highlight the need for more specific interventions targeting cognitive and motor dysfunctions in depression that can improve overall quality of life. Future longitudinal studies are essential to track these disturbances over time and across different stages of symptom improvement, while also assessing potential differences in cognitive and motor outcomes among different psychomotor subtypes of depression.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by University of Utah Institutional Review Board, #00139658, 05/07/2021. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

ID: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Investigation. MR: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Methodology. KW: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. CP: Writing – review & editing. SW: Writing – review & editing. Jv: Writing – review & editing. BM: Writing – review & editing, Investigation, Conceptualization. VK: Data curation, Conceptualization, Project administration, Supervision, Methodology, Writing – review & editing, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by a grant from the Ben B. and Iris M. Margolis Foundation awarded to VK and a NARSAD Young Investigator Grant from the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation, grant number 30026, awarded to MR. ID was supported by the University of Utah Research Track Program for Psychiatry Residents. The funders were not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the participants for their willingness to take part in this study. We thank Benjamin Silvester, Isaac Cress, Jasmine Jacobo, Samuel Garaycochea, Tobi Yoon, and William Seppanen for their assistance with data collection.

Conflict of interest

BM reports consulting for Inside Edge, VML, and Atheneum, and research support from Health Rhythms, LivaNova, Compass Pathways, and Abbott for projects unrelated to this work.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1624776/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Otte C, Gold SM, Penninx BW, Pariante CM, Etkin A, Fava M, et al. Major depressive disorder. Nat Rev Dis Primers. (2016) 2:16065. doi: 10.1038/nrdp.2016.65

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. American Psychiatric Association and American Psychiatric Association eds. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association (2013). 947 p.

Google Scholar

3. Hammer-Helmich L, Haro JM, Jönsson B, Tanguy Melac A, Di Nicola S, Chollet J, et al. Functional impairment in patients with major depressive disorder: the 2-year PERFORM study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2018) 14:239–49. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S146098

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Zhou J, Zhou J, Feng L, Feng Y, Xiao L, Chen X, et al. The associations between depressive symptoms, functional impairment, and quality of life, in patients with major depression: undirected and Bayesian network analyses. psychol Med. (2023) 53:6446–58. doi: 10.1017/S0033291722003385

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Liu J, Liu Y, Ma W, Tong Y, and Zheng J. Temporal and spatial trend analysis of all-cause depression burden based on Global Burden of Disease (GBD) 2019 study. Sci Rep. (2024) 14:12346. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-62381-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Reddy MS. Depression: the disorder and the burden. Indian J Psychol Med. (2010) 32:1–2. doi: 10.4103/0253-7176.70510

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Greenberg PE, Fournier AA, Sisitsky T, Simes M, Berman R, Koenigsberg SH, et al. The economic burden of adults with major depressive disorder in the United States (2010 and 2018). Pharmacoeconomics. (2021) 39:653–65. doi: 10.1007/s40273-021-01019-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Morris SE and Cuthbert BN. Research Domain Criteria: cognitive systems, neural circuits, and dimensions of behavior. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2012) 14:29–37. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2012.14.1/smorris

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kim JU, Bessette KL, Westlund-Schreiner M, Pocius S, Dillahunt AK, Frandsen S, et al. Relations of gray matter volume to dimensional measures of cognition and affect in mood disorders. Cortex. (2022) 156:57–70. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.06.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Walther S, Bernard JA, Mittal VA, and Shankman SA. The utility of an RDoC motor domain to understand psychomotor symptoms in depression. Psychol Med. (2019) 49:212–6. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718003033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Damme KSF, Park JS, Walther S, Vargas T, Shankman SA, and Mittal VA. Depression and psychosis risk shared vulnerability for motor signs across development, symptom dimensions, and familial risk. Schizophr Bull. (2022) 48:752–62. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbab133

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Buyukdura JS, McClintock SM, and Croarkin PE. Psychomotor retardation in depression: Biological underpinnings, measurement, and treatment. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2011) 35:395–409. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.10.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Day RK. Psychomotor agitation: poorly defined and badly measured. J Affect Disord. (1999) 55:89–98. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(99)00010-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Pompili M, Ducci G, Galluzzo A, Rosso G, Palumbo C, and De Berardis D. The management of psychomotor agitation associated with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder: A brief review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:4368. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18084368

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Elkjær E, Mikkelsen MB, Michalak J, Mennin DS, and O’Toole MS. Motor alterations in depression and anxiety disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2022) 317:373–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.08.060

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Abrahamse EL, Ruitenberg MFL, de Kleine E, and Verwey WB. Control of automated behavior: insights from the discrete sequence production task. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:82. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00082

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Ruitenberg MFL and Koppelmans V. Cognition in motion: evidence for intact action control with healthy aging. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2020) 76:252–61. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbaa184

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Paquet A, Lacroix A, Calvet B, and Girard M. Psychomotor semiology in depression: a standardized clinical psychomotor approach. BMC Psychiatry. (2022) 22:474. doi: 10.1186/s12888-022-04086-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Bayot M, Dujardin K, Tard C, Defebvre L, Bonnet CT, Allart E, et al. The interaction between cognition and motor control: A theoretical framework for dual-task interference effects on posture, gait initiation, gait and turning. Neurophysiol Clin. (2018) 48:361–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2018.10.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Doumas M, Smolders C, Brunfaut E, Bouckaert F, and Krampe RTH. Dual task performance of working memory and postural control in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychology. (2012) 26:110–8. doi: 10.1037/a0026181

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Leisman G, Moustafa AA, and Shafir T. Thinking, walking, talking: integratory motor and cognitive brain function. Front Public Health. (2016) 4:94. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00094

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Tozzi L, Bertrand C, Hack LM, Lyons T, Olmsted AM, Rajasekharan D, et al. A cognitive neural circuit biotype of depression showing functional and behavioral improvement after transcranial magnetic stimulation in the B-SMART-fMRI trial. Nat Ment Health. (2024) 2:987–98. doi: 10.1038/s44220-024-00271-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. McIntyre RS, Xiao HX, Syeda K, Vinberg M, Carvalho AF, Mansur RB, et al. The prevalence, measurement, and treatment of the cognitive dimension/domain in major depressive disorder. CNS Drugs. (2015) 29:577–89. doi: 10.1007/s40263-015-0263-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Bora E, Harrison BJ, Yücel M, and Pantelis C. Cognitive impairment in euthymic major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. psychol Med. (2013) 43:2017–26. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002085

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Hasselbalch BJ, Knorr U, Hasselbalch SG, Gade A, and Kessing LV. Cognitive deficits in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder. Neuropsychology. (2012) 26:642–51. doi: 10.1037/a0029301

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Conradi HJ, Ormel J, and de Jonge P. Presence of individual (residual) symptoms during depressive episodes and periods of remission: a 3-year prospective study. Psychol Med. (2011) 41:1165–74. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001911

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Parker G and McCraw S. The properties and utility of the CORE measure of melancholia. J Affect Disord. (2017) 207:128–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.09.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Janzing JGE, Birkenhäger TK, van den Broek WW, Breteler LMT, Nolen WA, and Verkes RJ. Psychomotor Retardation and the prognosis of antidepressant treatment in patients with unipolar Psychotic Depression. J Psychiatr Res. (2020) 130:321–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.07.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Kendrick T, Taylor D, and Johnson CF. Which first-line antidepressant? Br J Gen Pract. (2019) 69:114–5. doi: 10.3399/bjgp19X701405

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. van Diermen L, Vanmarcke S, Walther S, Moens H, Veltman E, Fransen E, et al. Can psychomotor disturbance predict ect outcome in depression? J Psychiatr Res. (2019) 117:122–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2019.07.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Robin A, Sauvaget A, Deschamps T, Bulteau S, and Thomas-Ollivier V. Combined measures of psychomotor and cognitive alterations as a potential hallmark for bipolar depression. Psychiatry Investig. (2019) 16:954–7. doi: 10.30773/pi.2019.0116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. (1998) 59 Suppl 20:22–33;quiz 34-57.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

33. Helmreich I, Wagner S, Mergl R, Allgaier AK, Hautzinger M, Henkel V, et al. Sensitivity to changes during antidepressant treatment: a comparison of unidimensional subscales of the Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (IDS-C) and the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD) in patients with mild major, minor or subsyndromal depression. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2012) 262:291–304. doi: 10.1007/s00406-011-0263-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, and Williams JB. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J Gen Intern Med. (2001) 16:606–13. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Reuben DB, Magasi S, McCreath HE, Bohannon RW, Wang YC, Bubela DJ, et al. Motor assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology. (2013) 80:S65–75. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872e01

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Gabel NM, Crane NA, Avery ET, Kay RE, Laurent A, Giordani B, et al. Dual-tasking gait variability and cognition in late-life depression. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2015) 30:1120–8. doi: 10.1002/gps.4340

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Koppelmans V, Ruitenberg MFL, Schaefer SY, King JB, Jacobo JM, Silvester BP, et al. Classification of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease using manual motor measures. Neurodegener Dis. (2024) 24:54–70. doi: 10.1159/000539800

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Hoogendam YY, van der Lijn F, Vernooij MW, Hofman A, Niessen WJ, van der Lugt A, et al. Older age relates to worsening of fine motor skills: a population-based study of middle-aged and elderly persons. Front Aging Neurosci. (2014) 6:259. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00259

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Hoogendam YY, Schagen SB, Ikram MA, Boogerd W, Seynaeve C, Seidler RD, et al. Late effects of adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer on fine motor function. Psychooncology. (2015) 24:1799–807. doi: 10.1002/pon.3796

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Ryan J, Woods RL, Britt CJ, Murray AM, Shah RC, Reid CM, et al. Normative data for the symbol digit modalities test in older white Australians and Americans, African-Americans, and Hispanic/Latinos. J Alzheimers Dis Rep. (2020) 4:313–23. doi: 10.3233/ADR-200194

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Benedict RHB, Schretlen D, Groninger L, and Brandt J. Hopkins verbal learning test – revised: normative data and analysis of inter-form and test-retest reliability. Clin Neuropsychol. (1998) 12:43–55. doi: 10.1076/clin.12.1.43.1726

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Brand AN, Jolles J, and Gispen-de Wied C. Recall and recognition memory deficits in depression. J Affect Disord. (1992) 25:77–86. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(92)90095-N

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Benedict RHB, Schretlen D, Groninger L, Dobraski M, and Shpritz B. Revision of the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test: Studies of normal performance, reliability, and validity. psychol Assess. (1996) 8:145–53. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.8.2.145

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Delis DC, Kaplan E, and Kramer JH. Reliability and validity of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: an update J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2004) 10(2):301–3. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704102191.

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Van Strien JW. Dutch handedness questionnaire. FSW, Department of Psychology, Erasmus University Rotterdam December (2002). doi: 10.1037/t00463-000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Benjamini Y and Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc: Ser B (Methodological). (1995) 57:289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Sabbe B, Hulstijn W, Van Hoof J, and Zitman F. Fine motor retardation and depression. J Psychiatr Res. (1996) 30:295–306. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(96)00014-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Burr P and Choudhury P. Fine motor disability. In: StatPearls. StatPearls Publishing, Treasure Island (FL (2024). Available online at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK563266/.

Google Scholar

49. Dobson KS, Hollon SD, Dimidjian S, Schmaling KB, Kohlenberg RJ, Gallop RJ, et al. Randomized trial of behavioral activation, cognitive therapy, and antidepressant medication in the prevention of relapse and recurrence in major depression. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2008) 76:468–77. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.3.468

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Kupferberg A and Hasler G. The social cost of depression: Investigating the impact of impaired social emotion regulation, social cognition, and interpersonal behavior on social functioning. J Affect Disord Rep. (2023) 14:100631. doi: 10.1016/j.jadr.2023.100631

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Young KS, Parsons CE, Stein A, and Kringelbach ML. Motion and emotion: depression reduces psychomotor performance and alters affective movements in caregiving interactions. Front Behav Neurosci. (2015) 9:26. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Schrijvers D, Hulstijn W, and Sabbe BGC. Psychomotor symptoms in depression: a diagnostic, pathophysiological and therapeutic tool. J Affect Disord. (2008) 109:1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.10.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Bock O. Dual-task costs while walking increase in old age for some, but not for other tasks: an experimental study of healthy young and elderly persons. J Neuroeng Rehabil. (2008) 5:27. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-5-27

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Clark DJ. Automaticity of walking: functional significance, mechanisms, measurement and rehabilitation strategies. Front Hum Neurosci. (2015) 9:246. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00246

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Boonyong S, Siu KC, van Donkelaar P, Chou LS, and Woollacott MH. Development of postural control during gait in typically developing children: the effects of dual-task conditions. Gait Posture. (2012) 35:428–34. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.11.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Chauvel G, Palluel E, Brandao A, Barbieri G, Nougier V, and Olivier I. Attentional load of walking in children aged 7–12 and in adults. Gait Posture. (2017) 56:95–9. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.04.034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Kang GE, Mickey BJ, McInnis MG, Krembs BS, and Gross MM. Motor behavior characteristics in various phases of bipolar disorder revealed through biomechanical analysis: Quantitative measures of activity and energy variables during gait and sit-to-walk. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 269:93–101. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.062

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Li Z, Tong X, Ma Y, Bao T, and Yue J. Prevalence of depression in patients with sarcopenia and correlation between the two diseases: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Cachexia Sarcopenia Muscle. (2022) 13:128–44. doi: 10.1002/jcsm.12908

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Swanson J. The Delis-Kaplan executive function system: A review. Can J School Psychol. (2005) 20:117–28. doi: 10.1177/0829573506295469

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Lippa SM and Davis RN. Inhibition/Switching Is not Necessarily Harder than Inhibition: An Analysis of the D-KEFS Color-Word Interference Test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2010) 25:146–52. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acq001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Matcham F, Simblett SK, Leightley D, Dalby M, Siddi S, Haro JM, et al. The association between persistent cognitive difficulties and depression and functional outcomes in people with major depressive disorder. Psychol Med. (2022) 53(13):6334–44. doi: 10.1017/S0033291722003671

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Chen J, Huys Q, Stringaris A, and Nielson D. On the misery of cognitive effort. PsyArXiv (2023). Available online at: https://psyarxiv.com/r9e4j/.

Google Scholar

63. Cong S, Xiang C, Zhang S, Zhang T, Wang H, and Cong S. Prevalence and clinical aspects of depression in Parkinson’s disease: A systematic review and meta analysis of 129 studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2022) 141:104749. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104749

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Clark ML, Abimanyi-Ochom J, Le H, Long B, Orr C, and Khanh-Dao Le L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of depression and apathy frequency in adult-onset Huntington’s disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2023) 149:105166. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105166

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Wüthrich F, Lefebvre S, Mittal VA, Shankman SA, Alexander N, Brosch K, et al. The neural signature of psychomotor disturbance in depression. Mol Psychiatry. (2024) 29:317–26. doi: 10.1038/s41380-023-02327-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Masse C, Vandel P, Sylvestre G, Noiret N, Bennabi D, Mauny F, et al. Cognitive impairment in late-life depression: A comparative study of healthy older people, late-life depression, and mild Alzheimer’s disease using multivariate base rates of low scores. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:724731/full. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.724731/full

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Linnemann C and Lang UE. Pathways connecting late-life depression and dementia. Front Pharmacol. (2020) 11:279/full. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2020.00279/full

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Elser H, Horváth-Puhó E, Gradus JL, Smith ML, Lash TL, Glymour MM, et al. Association of early-, middle-, and late-life depression with incident dementia in a Danish cohort. JAMA Neurol. (2023) 80:949–58. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2023.2309

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Leone C, Feys P, Moumdjian L, D’Amico E, Zappia M, and Patti F. Cognitive-motor dual-task interference: A systematic review of neural correlates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 75:348–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, and Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol. (2000) 41:49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Brauner FO, Balbinot G, Figueiredo AI, Hausen DO, Schiavo A, and Mestriner RG. The performance index identifies changes across the dual task timed up and go test phases and impacts task-cost estimation in the oldest-old. Front Hum Neurosci. (2021) 15:720719/full. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.720719/full

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Grahek I, Shenhav A, Musslick S, Krebs RM, and Koster EHW. Motivation and cognitive control in depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2019) 102:371–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.04.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Greden JF. Tests of psychomotor function. In: Hall RCW and Beresford TP, editors. Handbook of Psychiatric Diagnostic Procedures: Vol II. Springer Netherlands, Dordrecht (1985). p. 81–96. doi: 10.1007/978-94-011-6728-4_4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Walther S and Morrens M. Editorial: psychomotor symptomatology in psychiatric illnesses. Front Psychiatry. (2015) 6:81/full. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00081/full

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Ghanean H, Ceniti AK, and Kennedy SH. Fatigue in Patients with Major Depressive Disorder: Prevalence, Burden and Pharmacological Approaches to Management. CNS Drugs. 32(1):65–74. doi: 10.1007/s40263-018-0490-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Dantchev N. The measurement of retardation in depression. J Clin Psychiatry. (1998) 59(Suppl 14):19–25.

Google Scholar

77. Llinàs-Reglà J, Vilalta-Franch J, López-Pousa S, Calvó-Perxas L, Torrents Rodas D, and Garre-Olmo J. Trail Making Test - an overview. (2017) 24(2):183–96. doi: 10.1177/1073191115602552

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Smith MJ, Brébion G, Banquet JP, and Cohen L. Retardation of mentation in depressives: Posner’s covert orientation of visual attention test. J Affect Disord. (1995) 35(3):107–15. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(95)00044-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Blackburn IM. Mental and psychomotor speed in depression and mania. Br J Psychiatr. (1975) 126:329–35. doi: 10.1192/bjp.126.4.329

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Keller AS, Leikauf JE, Holt-Gosselin B, Staveland BR, and Williams LM. Paying attention to attention in depression. Transl Psychiatry. (2019) 9:279. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0616-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Gorwood P, Richard-Devantoy S, Baylé F, and Cléry-Melun ML. Psychomotor retardation is a scar of past depressive episodes, revealed by simple cognitive tests. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2014) 24:1630–40. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.07.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Lam RW, Kennedy SH, McIntyre RS, and Khullar A. Cognitive dysfunction in major depressive disorder: effects on psychosocial functioning and implications for treatment. Can J Psychiatry. (2014) 59:649–54. doi: 10.1177/070674371405901206

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Kriesche D, Woll CFJ, Tschentscher N, Engel RR, and Karch S. Neurocognitive deficits in depression: a systematic review of cognitive impairment in the acute and remitted state. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2023) 273:1105–28. doi: 10.1007/s00406-022-01479-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Hack LM, Tozzi L, Zenteno S, Olmsted AM, Hilton R, Jubeir J, et al. A cognitive biotype of depression linking symptoms, behavior measures, neural circuits, and differential treatment outcomes: A prespecified secondary analysis of a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Netw Open. (2023) 6:e2318411. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2023.18411

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Günther W, Günther R, Streck P, Römig H, and Rödel A. Psychomotor disturbances in psychiatric patients as a possible basis for new attempts at differential diagnosis and therapy. Eur Arch Psychiatr Neurol Sci. (1988) 237:65–73. doi: 10.1007/BF00382369

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Günther W and Gruber H. Psychomotorische Störungen bei psychiatrischen Patienten als mögliche Grandlage neuer Ansätze in Differentialdiagnose und Therapie. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr. (1983) 233:187–209.

Google Scholar

87. McDowell CP, Gordon BR, and Herring MP. Sex-related differences in the association between grip strength and depression: Results from the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing. Exp Gerontol. (2018) 104:147–52. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2018.02.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Prado CE, Watt S, and Crowe SF. A meta-analysis of the effects of antidepressants on cognitive functioning in depressed and non-depressed samples. Neuropsychol Rev. (2018) 28:32–72. doi: 10.1007/s11065-018-9369-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Verghese J, Kuslansky G, Holtzer R, Katz M, Xue X, Buschke H, et al. Walking while talking: effect of task prioritization in the elderly. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. (2007) 88:50. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2006.10.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Lee RSC, Hermens DF, Porter MA, and Redoblado-Hodge MA. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode Major Depressive Disorder. J Affect Disord. (2012) 140:113–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.10.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Baune BT, Fuhr M, Air T, and Hering C. Neuropsychological functioning in adolescents and young adults with major depressive disorder–a review. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 218:261–71. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.04.052

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

92. The WHQOL Group. The World Health Organization quality of life assessment (WHOQOL): Development and general psychometric properties. Soc Sci Med. (1998) 46:1569–85. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(98)00009-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognition, depression, motor functioning, psychomotor disturbances, fine motor

Citation: Douka I, Ruitenberg MFL, Weischedel K, Phouthavongsay C, Weisenbach SL, van der Geest JN, Mickey BJ and Koppelmans V (2025) Cognitive and motor disturbances in depression: insights from comprehensive behavioral assessments. Front. Psychiatry 16:1624776. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1624776

Received: 07 May 2025; Accepted: 01 July 2025;
Published: 29 July 2025.

Edited by:

Luca Steardo Jr, University Magna Graecia of Catanzaro, Italy

Reviewed by:

Fabiane de Oliveira Brauner, Gait and Balance Academy (GaBA), United States
Yongqing Hou, Zhejiang University, China
Alison Robin, Université de Nantes, France

Copyright © 2025 Douka, Ruitenberg, Weischedel, Phouthavongsay, Weisenbach, van der Geest, Mickey and Koppelmans. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Vincent Koppelmans, dmluY2VudC5rb3BwZWxtYW5zQHV0YWguZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.