EDITORIAL article
Front. Psychiatry
Sec. Social Neuroscience
Volume 16 - 2025 | doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1686354
This article is part of the Research TopicNeurological Mechanisms of Empathy for DistressView all 7 articles
Editorial: Neurological Mechanisms of Empathy for Distress
Provisionally accepted- 1Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), Beijing, China
- 2School of Public Health, University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong, SAR China
- 3Chongqing Normal University, Chongqing, China
Select one of your emails
You have multiple emails registered with Frontiers:
Notify me on publication
Please enter your email address:
If you already have an account, please login
You don't have a Frontiers account ? You can register here
Editorial on the Research Topic Neurological Mechanisms of Empathy for Distress Neurological Mechanisms of Empathy for Distress (July 2022 to July 2025) was a special project proposed to investigate the neurological mechanisms underlying empathy for distress. Empathy is the sharing and understanding of others' feelings (Meng et al., 2024; Zhao et al., 2023). It is a multidimensional concept, consisting of empathy for others' physical sensations (e.g., empathy for pain), inner feelings (e.g., empathy for emotions), and spiritual thoughts (e.g., empathy for music) (Sobrak-Seaton, 2024; Zhao et al., 2024). With a collaborative effort by 27 authors, in tandem with 17 reviewers and editors, the current project was finalized, featuring six articles that illuminate the nature of empathy for distress. As introduced in the following, the topics covered by these articles went broader than the initial design of the project. However, intriguingly, the theme of these studies converged with the concept of multidimensional empathy: PHYSICAL EMPATHY Li et al. studied the event-related potentials (ERPs) of empathy for pain, based on the Empathy for Pain Stimuli System (EPSS; Meng et al., 2024). They observed that empathy for pain was modulated both by participant traits (e.g., autistic traits) and stimulus traits (e.g., static vs. dynamic stimuli). Essentially, they verified that the ERP components of empathy for pain were two-fold: early emotional components (N1, P2, and N2, which arise above the frontoparietal lobe) and late cognitive components (P3 and LPP, which occur above the parieto-occipital lobe). The former could represent an automatic emotional contagion of others' pain, and the latter might represent a cognitive evaluation of the pain per se, as well as its potential consequences. EMOTIONAL EMPATHY Arioli et al. investigated the neurostructural bases of empathy for emotions. By focusing on grey matter volume through univariate and multivariate brain morphometry, they observed robust neural correlates of emotional empathy, yet not for cognitive empathy. Moreover, distinct brain structures were identified between self-oriented (i.e., personal distress) and other-oriented emotional empathy (i.e., empathic concern). The former involved the insula and amygdala, which might underpin one's tendency to experience an automatic resonance with others' pain, as well as inclination to withdraw from it for self-protection. Instead, empathic concern was associated with the medial precuneus and inferior parietal cortex, likely reflecting one's ability to shift attention from oneself to others, thereby enabling prosocial behaviour. Lavin et al. studied the impact of the presence of a "third individual" on the prosocial reactions of young adults using the electroencephalogram (EEG) technique. They observed that when presented with a "third person" in need (i.e., a child potentially benefiting from the donation), participants were more likely to accept altruistic yet unfair monetary distribution. Using EEG, Lavin et al. further identified that the presence of the "third person" in need enhanced participants' frontal theta oscillations. They pondered that the increased oscillations might be due to an inclusive empathic concern for others in general. In other words, being concerned about one individual may have a Ripple Effect, triggering empathy concerns for others' well-being in a more general context. SPIRITUAL EMPATHY Obando Yar et al. conducted a systematic review to explore the neurological basis of individuals' perception of fictional characters. They reported that the neural activation related to the fictional resonance was qualified by the role of the characters (i.e., protagonists vs. antagonists): the appreciation of protagonists was followed by a stronger synchronization among empathy-related brain regions, such as the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). In contrast, engaging with antagonists may lead to enhanced activation of brain regions involved in subjective value integration and comparison, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Overall, their findings indicated that moral resonance might serve as an amplifier of spiritual empathy. CROSS-DIMENSIONAL EMPATHY Pan et al. studied the impacts of death attitude and inadequate personal traits (e.g., antisocial and narcissistic personalities) on clinical empathy with trainee nurses. Clinical empathy stresses spiritual resonance with patients' physical, emotional, and mental suffering due to illness per se, clinical treatments, and approaching death. They found that nurses with more serious personality inadequacies reported a lower level of clinical empathy. Furthermore, via mediation and moderation analyses, they found that these nurses' adverse attitudes to death (i.e., fear, avoidance, and unrealistic beliefs) could underpin the above impairment. Ergo, Pan et al. suggested that maintaining a neutral and realistic attitude towards life and death is essential for promoting empathy among clinical professionals. Vickers et al. innovatively studied a primeval–humane empathy towards charities. They presented participants with brief written introductions to charities and asked about their decisions on monetary donations. Behavioral results revealed that the donation was biased towards neonates with immediate nurturant needs over other aids for adults. Neural imaging results further indicated that the above donation bias was relevant to motor-related brain regions (e.g., primary and supplementary motor areas). Vickers et al. deemed that care for neonates—closely related to species survival—predominantly are physical activities (e.g., feeding and cuddling). Hence, the above neurological evidence-based altruism bias towards neonates might have been prioritized by primitive communities, encoded in our brains, and passed on to our modern societies. FOR THE FUTURE Empathy is a multidimensional unity (Sobrak-Seaton, 2024; Zhao et al., 2024). It ranges from the primary type of empathy for others' externalized physical feelings (e.g., pain), to inner feelings (e.g., emotions), and ultimately to the innermost thoughts embodied in the forms of art, music, literature, and similar media (e.g., artistic expressions). The associations among these empathy unity elements, especially empathy for distress in the physical, emotional, and spiritual domains, warrant in-depth investigation. An understanding of their associations and potentially overlapped neural bases may eventually reveal the secrets underpinning the overall concept of empathy. Remarkably, with the development of artificial intelligence (Kuai et al., 2025; Wang et al., 2024), the above spectrum of empathy is anticipated to be expanded into AI-mediated empathy.
Keywords: empathy for distress, Empathy for pain, empathy for emotions, spiritual empathy, Neurological mechanisms
Received: 15 Aug 2025; Accepted: 01 Sep 2025.
Copyright: © 2025 Zhao, Chen and Meng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
* Correspondence:
Shanquan Chen, School of Public Health, University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong, SAR China
Jing Meng, Chongqing Normal University, Chongqing, China
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.