Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Commun., 05 October 2023
Sec. Health Communication
Volume 8 - 2023 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2023.1244471

Masculinity and veganism: the effect of linking vegan dishes with masculinity on men's attitudes toward vegan food

  • 1Department of Psychology IV, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Bamberg, Bamberg, Germany

Introduction: Vegetarian or vegan diets are not yet popular with most men, though they are beneficial for both health and the environment. Men's low preference for such diets might stem from the prevalent association of meat with masculinity, and of veganism with femininity. Accordingly, linking vegan nutrition to masculinity might help to develop a favorable attitude toward plant-based diets in men. The present study examined the effects of a masculine framing of vegan dishes on men's attitudes toward vegan food and veganism, and explored whether adherence to traditional forms of masculinity might increase the effects of masculine framing.

Methods: We conducted an online experiment with a 2 (man vs. woman) x 2 (conventional vs. masculine framing) design, in which 593 adults participated. We measured participants' attitudes toward vegan dishes (dish ratings, hunger ratings) and veganism (veganism ratings, vegan scenarios ratings), the perceived suitability of the dishes for men and women as well as men's identification with new forms of masculinity.

Results: We found that a masculine framing of vegan dishes influenced the gender association of the dishes, weakening the link to femininity. However, masculine framing did not influence men's or women's attitudes toward the presented vegan dishes or veganism in general. Although the extent to which men identified with new forms of masculinity correlated positively with favorable attitudes toward veganism, new masculinity identification did - with an exception for the rating of the presented dishes - not moderate the effect of the experimental condition.

Discussion: Our findings suggest that the potency of a short-term intervention might not be sufficient to counterbalance the prevailing feminine connotations associated with veganism. Thus, we encourage further exploration of masculine framing to improve men's perception of vegan food and the vegan concept, but with stronger stimuli and/or longer intervention duration.

1. Introduction

Meat and other animal products are considered being the centerpieces of a meal in many cultures (Twigg, 1983). Due to the high demand, global production of animal products increased considerably from 2000 to 2014 (meat by 39%, milk by 38%; FAO, 2018). With an annual meat consumption of about 80 kg per capita, the amount consumed in Europe is twice as high as recommended from a health perspective (WCRF, 2007; Ritchie and Roser, 2017). Negative consequences of excessive meat consumption such as cardiovascular problems, oncological diseases, and type 2 diabetes have been shown in a number of studies (e.g., McAfee et al., 2010; Micha et al., 2010). Recent findings also argue against the widespread belief of a disadvantage of a vegetarian (renouncing meat and fish) or a vegan (renouncing all animal products and thus meat, fish, milk, eggs, and honey) diet (Leitzmann, 2014) because both types have been shown to be associated with lower risk of chronic diseases (Appleby and Key, 2016; Dinu et al., 2017; e.g., obesity, type 2 diabetes mellitus, cardiovascular disease, and cancer). Aside from the health aspect, high consumption of meat and animal products is a burden on the environment (Leip et al., 2015). For example, globally, 83% of agricultural land is used for animal agriculture, which accounts for around 56-58% of dietary greenhouse gas emissions but only for 37% of the protein and 18% of the caloric requirements (Poore and Nemecek, 2018). However, to date, only around 5% of the German population follow a vegetarian diet and only around 1% are vegan (FORSA, 2020). In addition, a vegetarian diet is decidedly more widespread among women (6.1%) than among men (2.5%; Mensink et al., 2016), and among vegans only 19% are men (SKOPOS, 2020). The association between meat consumption and masculinity, especially in Western societies (Adams, 1990; Thomas, 2016) might explain these gender differences. Following a vegan diet, and excluding all animal products, would therefore be the strongest possible antagonist to meat eating and thus to masculinity (Greenebaum and Dexter, 2018).

Given that meat reduction can be beneficial for both environment and health, and men are underrepresented among vegetarians and, in particular, among vegans, it is important to develop favorable attitudes in men toward plant-based diets. Accordingly, the current study examines (1) whether men's attitudes toward a vegan diet can be improved through an intervention, in which vegan food is associated with masculinity, and (2) whether individual differences in masculinity moderate the effect of this intervention.

In the following section, we will first discuss gender, gender stereotypes, and their association with food. Then, we will discuss a masculine framing of vegan dishes as an intervention that might improve the attitude toward vegan dishes and veganism among men. Finally, we will discuss the role of individual differences in identification with masculinity on the effectiveness of such an intervention.

The distinction between (innate) biological sex and socially constructed gender is important. In contrast to biological sex, gender is constructed through culturally produced gender differences between men and women (Athenstaedt and Alfermann, 2011), which are internalized in the process of socialization. This means that family, school, media and others convey social norms, values and rules, leading to gender stereotypes (Steins, 2010). Gender stereotypes are therefore shared assumptions about how men and women differ from one another in their roles and characteristics (descriptive) or how they should differ (prescriptive; Eckes, 2008). These descriptive and prescriptive assumptions about men and women are consistent across many Western cultures (e.g., women are said or expected to be caring and emotionally expressive, while men are said or expected to be dominant and autonomous in their behavior; Williams et al., 1999).

Gender stereotypes also include food and food choices. For example, McPhail et al. (2012) conducted semi-structured interviews in which most Canadian adults and teenagers indicated gender associations for different types of nutrition and reported eating accordingly. Smaller servings, healthier foods, and even certain dishes (e.g., sushi, couscous, and green salad) were perceived as more appropriate for women. Food for men, in contrast, was defined by large portions, and by being hearty and filling (e.g., hamburgers, hot dogs, and pizza). Cavazza et al. (2015) confirmed that the dish itself, the portion size and the presentation of the dish play a role in gender perception and found that this perception as gender appropriate or inappropriate affects the willingness to eat specific types of food. Specifically, this meant that women preferred Caprese salad to a hamburger, rated smaller portions better than large portions, and preferred an elegant presentation of food to a messy arrangement. In addition, people who eat smaller portions are perceived to be more feminine and the consumption of larger portions is associated with masculinity (Bock and Kanarek, 1995).

In particular, meat consumption is linked to gender stereotypes of masculinity (Rozin et al., 2012), probably explaining men's lower willingness to adopt plant-based diets. Meat is associated with strength and potency, which makes meat appear more suitable for men: “men are strong, men need to be strong, thus men need meat” (Adams, 1990, p. 33). Men could identify themselves as men by eating meat, hence they consume more than women. Other studies confirm the connection between high meat consumption and a positive attitude toward meat, especially in men (Rothgerber, 2013). Men are more committed to consuming meat than women and are stronger supporters of the four arguments put forward in favor of meat consumption (Piazza et al., 2015). These are listed under the 4Ns and describe the assumption that meat consumption is natural, normal, necessary, and nice. Conversely, Adams (1990) argues that vegetables and other meat-free foods tend to be perceived as appropriate food for women, making them uninteresting and inappropriate for men. Building on Brillat-Savarin's statement from 1862 that one is what one eats (c.f. Adams, 1990, p. 36), Adams concludes: “to eat a vegetable is to become a vegetable, and by extension, to become woman-like“.

As Niederle and Schubert (2020) point out, veganism can be discussed as a diet, philosophy, movement or (social) practice. As an example of gender differences regarding philosophy, one can look at the empirical finding of women having a more negative attitude toward livestock farming and being more likely to support animal welfare organizations (Eldridge and Gluck, 1996; Knight et al., 2004). Further, men in Rothgerber's (2013) study believed that caring for animals is a sign of weakness and femininity. The vegan diet, which does not contain any animal products and is, therefore, more intense than vegetarianism, is thus a strong contrast to meat consumption (Greenebaum and Dexter, 2018). This was also shown in studies that examined the perception of vegetarians and vegans by other people. Vegetarians were perceived to be less masculine than people who eat meat (Ruby and Heine, 2011). However, a recent study Thomas (2016) found this effect only for the perception of vegans but not of vegetarians, indicating that vegetarianism might become more mainstream and appropriate for men. Interestingly, Thomas' study also showed that men in particular attribute lower masculinity to other vegan men, regardless of their motivation for vegan nutrition (e.g., health, caring about animals). Bogueva et al. (2022) reported on Australian men's experience of visiting and eating at a vegan restaurant. The men's statements showed concerns linked to masculinity, dietary identity and social perception by others. They perceived their visit to the restaurant as a discrepancy with masculinity. Further, the way they spoke about their experience, it becomes obvious that their eating behavior served the management of the impression others have of them, and that this was more important than their actual liking of the plant-based burger. Thus, (not) eating meat is a (social) practice with a symbolic value for impression management regarding gender, and what Malitska (2021) reports about organized vegetarianism in the early twentieth-century Russian Empire applies nowadays, too: food is not an end in itself. Instead, food might be best described as a social marker and can demonstrate belonging to a group, but also contribute to discrimination (De Garine, 2001). The symbolic value of a product plays an important role in consumers' decision to buy this product (Levy, 1959). For example, products may be chosen to strengthen a person's own gender identity and to make sure that others perceive us accordingly (Grubb and Grathwohl, 1967). Indeed, advertising, cookbooks and blogs have recognized this relationship and have already tried to link vegan food to masculinity. Hart (2018), for example, examined the relationship between gender and veganism in the depiction of vegan food on vegan blogs and found that masculine attributes were often used to promote vegan food as being suitable for men. Fortifying food that is easy to prepare, very filling, hearty and meat-like is particularly often used in connection with suitability for men (Rogers, 2008; Gal and Wilkie, 2010; McPhail et al., 2012; Cavazza et al., 2015; Hart, 2018; Love and Sulikowski, 2018; Sexton et al., 2019; Contois, 2020). Such framing might be beneficial since Bogueva et al. (2022) found indications of strict enforcements of vegan food to lead to perceived limited freedom and thus psychological reactance in men. The same conclusion is drawn by Szczebyło et al. (2022), who argue that changing dietary behavior at a deep level of belief is very difficult and thus the willingness for transitions in dietary behaviors needs to be facilitated, for instance with new value creation.

From an empirical perspective, framing products and behaviors as appropriate for men and women seems to work. Worth et al. (1992) conducted two experiments in which different products (beer or jeans) were presented and framed with either masculine or feminine adjectives. In line with the framing, the participants not only judged the products as being more suitable for the respective gender but also liked them better if the framing matched their own gender. In addition, studies have examined how products or behaviors that are already associated with one gender can be made more interesting for the opposite gender (e.g., Gal and Wilkie, 2010; Brough et al., 2016). Brough et al. (2016), for example, examined the willingness to behave in an environmentally friendly manner (willingness to donate to an environmental organization and attitude toward electric cars), which is associated with femininity and hence not similarly popular with men. They found that the reactions were affected by the importance of maintaining gender identity (i.e., individual factors) and the gender-related presentation of the relevant stimulus (i.e., situational factors). Men were influenced in their behavior by presenting the products in a more masculine light by using gender-appropriate colors, fonts, words, and symbols. Interestingly, the effects of the framing manipulations on women were much smaller. Gal and Wilkie (2010) conducted a similar study on food choices by presenting different dishes in a masculine or feminine way. The gender association of the dishes was manipulated by altering the ingredients, the verbal descriptions, and the names of the dishes. Concerning ingredients, the masculine dish, for example, contained gravy, while the feminine dish contained red wine sauce. With regard to verbal description, the masculine dish was described as “hearty” while the feminine dish was described as “luscious”. Finally, regarding the name, the masculine dish was called “Western Salad”, whereas the feminine dish was called “Nature Salad”. Gal and Wilkie (2010) found that product presentation had an effect on men's food choices, with men preferring the dishes described in a masculine manner. For women, in contrast, the effect of the gender-related manipulations was again much smaller. This finding agrees with the notion that men, compared to women, face greater disadvantages for behavior that is inconsistent with their gender (Martin, 1990). Men should therefore be more receptive to gender-related information. Supporting this, White and Dahl (2006) found that products with feminine associations are more likely to be avoided by men than masculine-associated products are avoided by women.

Although gender stereotypes represent culturally shared assumptions about how men and women (should) differ from one another (Eckes, 2008), there are individual differences regarding the personal importance and identification with these stereotypes. In addition, stereotypes are open to change. Kaplan et al. (2017), for example, argue that an increasing number of men develop alternative “new” ideas of masculinity, which challenge traditional male norms. These so-called forms of new masculinity are associated with values such as authenticity, emotional expressivity, and holistic self-awareness. Accordingly, they might influence men's attitudes and behaviors in traditionally “male” and “female” contexts.

With regard to nutrition, De Backer et al. (2020) investigated whether identification with new masculinity influences attitudes, intentions and behavior toward meat. They found that the men who identified with new forms of masculinity ate less meat, had less attachment to meat, were more likely to reduce their meat consumption, and were more positive about vegetarians. Accordingly, men who identify strongly with new masculinity might need less nudging toward vegan nutrition and they might be less influenced by (traditional) masculine framings of vegan dishes.

The present study pursues three aims. First, we investigate whether vegan food, which has a strong feminine connotation, can be rendered more attractive for men through the association of vegan food with masculine attributes. Second, we examine whether any effects of such an association transfer to attitudes toward veganism in general. Third, individual differences in men are addressed by including identification with new masculinity as a moderator variable in the study, which might affect the effectiveness of pairing vegan food with (traditional) masculine attributes. The resulting hypotheses are presented below.

Based on the findings of previous experiments in which gender association was shifted by using gender-associated attributes (e.g., Gal and Wilkie, 2010; Brough et al., 2016) we assume that a description of vegan food with masculine attributes influences the gender-stereotypical symbolism of the vegan dish, resulting in a weakening of its feminine connotation and a movement toward perceived suitability for men.

Hypothesis 1: An association of vegan dishes with masculine attributes weakens the association of vegan dishes with femininity.

If this holds true, then this change in the gender stereotypical symbolism of the vegan dish might have the potential to reduce the perceived inconsistency with men's gender identity. Accordingly, we expect that men are going to rate a vegan dish better when described with masculine attributes.

Hypothesis 2: Men rate vegan dishes better if they contain masculine instead of conventional attributes.

Typically, men show a strong tendency to behave according to gender roles and to avoid products and behavior associated with women. Women, on the other hand, seem less concerned about maintaining their gender identity (e.g., White and Dahl, 2006; Gal and Wilkie, 2010; Brough et al., 2016; e.g., they are more likely to choose masculine-associated products than men choosing feminine-associated products). Therefore, we do not expect the description of vegan dishes with masculine attributes to affect women's ratings of vegan food as much as men's ratings. Indeed, for women, there might not be a meaningful effect at all.

Hypothesis 3: For women, the rating of vegan food does not differ for conventional descriptions or descriptions with masculine attributes.

In addition, it is investigated whether the representation of vegan dishes with masculine attributes also affects men's general ratings of veganism. This is hypothesized because by associating a vegan dish with masculine attributes, not only the dish itself but also the underlying concept of veganism could be influenced. For reasons mentioned above for Hypothesis 3, we expected no such effect or a much weaker effect in women.

Hypothesis 4: The masculine description of vegan dishes leads to a better attitude toward veganism in men.

Hypothesis 5: The masculine description of vegan dishes does not influence the attitude toward veganism in women.

Finally, men's identification with new forms of masculinity (which are challenging traditional male norms and are associated with values such as authenticity, emotional expressivity, and holistic self-awareness; Kaplan et al., 2017) could influence the amount to which an association of vegan food with (traditional) masculinity affects men's attitude toward vegan food and veganism in general. Specifically, men who identify more strongly with new masculinity, should show weaker identification with traditional masculine stereotypes and should thus be less influenced by a masculine framing.

Hypothesis 6: Men who identify less strongly with new forms of masculinity are more positively influenced in their rating of vegan food that is described with masculine attributes than men who identify more strongly with new forms of masculinity.

Hypothesis 7: Men who identify less strongly with new forms of masculinity are more positively influenced in their attitudes toward veganism through the masculine description of vegan dishes than men who identify more strongly with new forms of masculinity.

2. Methods

2.1. Data collection and sample

The data for the study was collected online via SoSci Survey (Leiner, 2019) in two waves, namely at the end of 2020 and at the end of 2021. The study was advertised as being about the rating of recipes. Participants learned about the study privately, through the internet (e.g., Facebook groups, Instagram, LinkedIn, SurveyCircle, SurveySwap), or through the University of Würzburg's recruiting system. There was no monetary reward, but on SurveyCircle it was indicated that 30 cents per participant would be donated to UNICEF and participants in the University's recruiting system could receive credits for course participation.

The questionnaire was completed by 631 participants. Of these, 48 participants were excluded based on the following criteria: other gender than men or women, younger than 18 years old, an incorrect answer to any of the first control questions (i.e., recognition of the name of the dish) in combination with a 50% threshold for the second control questions (i.e., recognition of main ingredients of the dish), and less time than 12s spent on any of the dish descriptions, which would indicate scanning (Brysbaert, 2019). Thus, the final sample comprised 593 participants, but hypotheses were tested only on the 382 omnivore participants (see Table 1). Of the final sample, most were in their mid-twenties, had completed A-levels, were university students, and identified as omnivores.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics of the sample.

Given the results reported by Brough and colleagues (2016) on the effects of masculine branding on men's attitudes toward green behaviors, we expected effect sizes to be small to medium in size. A power analysis with G*power (Faul et al., 2009) indicated that a sample of 395 participants was required to detect a small effect (f2 = 0.02) in multiple regression analysis (single regression coefficients; 1-β = 0.80, α = 0.05, two-tailed test). An ethics approval was not required for this study because according to the ethical guidelines of the German Society for Psychology (DGPs) and the regulations of the Ethics Committee of the Institute for Psychology of the Faculty for Human Sciences of the Julius-Maximilians-University of Würzburg, prior review is not mandatory for research that provides signed informed consent from study participants, collects data anonymously, and has no foreseeable negative impact on participants. However, the study was reviewed post-hoc by the Ethics Committee of the University of Bamberg, Germany, and approval was granted.

2.2. Study design and procedure

The study represents a between-subjects design. The description of the dishes (conventional vs. masculine framing) was randomly assigned to the participants. Gender (man vs. woman) was assessed via self-report. Identification with new masculinity was assessed as a continuous covariate.

Participants were informed about the study's general structure and received information on voluntariness, anonymity, data protection, and the retention period for the anonymized data (in accordance with the European General Data Protection Regulation). Further participation in the study was only possible after consent. After that, the participants provided information about their demographics as well as information on eating behavior (diet, frequency of meat consumption, restrictions, and allergies).

In the main part of the study, the manipulated descriptions of four dishes were presented. There were two randomizations between the participants. Firstly, it was randomized whether the descriptions contained conventional or masculine attributes, resulting in two test groups. Secondly, the order of the dishes was randomized for each participant, since previous studies showed that certain dishes can already be associated with a gender (McPhail et al., 2012). The dishes were rated one after another by the participants in the following order: control questions, the rating of the dishes, the feeling of hunger, and the assignment to a gender. After the presentation of all dishes, the participants were asked what attitude they have toward veganism and how likely they would choose a vegan dish in different given situations.

In the last part of the study, some of the used stimuli were validated with a questionnaire in which adjectives were rated with regard to their masculinity/femininity. In addition, participants filled out the New Masculinity Inventory (Kaplan et al., 2017). The final questions were whether the participant knew vegetarians or vegans personally, whether they disliked a dish for a specific reason, whether there were technical or understanding difficulties, and whether there was any assumption about the purpose of the study. After having thanked the participants for taking part in the study, they were debriefed and asked to keep the study content confidential. The wording of the questions and options for answers can be found in the Supplementary Tables S1S9.

2.3. Stimuli

For the stimuli (see Supplementary Graphics S1S6), recipes from the vegan food blog Eat this! (https://www.eat-this.org/) were taken as inspiration and adapted for the study purpose. There were descriptions of four different dishes: salad, spaghetti carbonara, goulash, and burger. Several dishes were selected because dishes can already be associated with gender (McPhail et al., 2012). It was assumed that the salad and partly also the spaghetti carbonara have a more feminine connotation, while goulash and burgers tend to have a more masculine connotation. No photos were presented, and the colors and design were kept constant and in simple neutral colors. The text itself differed between a masculine and a conventional framing in the two conditions and was divided into a slogan at the beginning of the description, followed by a short text about the ingredients and preparation, and a short section praising the dish. Throughout the text, plenty of conventional or masculine attributes in the form of adjectives and phrases were used to create a distinctly framed dish. Examples of the wording in the conventional framing condition are “special touch”, “colorful”, “creative”, and “delicious”, and examples of the wording in the masculine framing condition are “beast”, “meaty”, “protein-rich”, and “filling” (see Supplementary Tables S10, S11 for a full overview). Most of the attributes were taken from other studies in which they were named and/or used because of their association with masculinity. The conventional attributes were primarily taken from the Edgy Veggies Toolkit (Turnwald et al., 2020). For testing whether the attributes contribute to a conventional or masculine description, a selection of adjectives was checked for their validity at the end of the study by rating them with regard to their masculinity/femininity (see Supplementary Table S12).

2.4. Instruments

2.4.1. Gender association of the vegan dishes

After the description of each dish, participants answered whether the food was more suitable for women or for men (from 1 = more likely for women to 7 = more likely for men).

2.4.2. Attitudes toward the dishes

After the description of each dish, participants answered five questions about how they liked the dish (e.g., “I would like to eat the dish”; scroll bar from 1 = disagree to 101 = agree). The ratings on these five items were averaged per dish. The internal consistency was high (α = 0.98). For the analysis the mean value of the four dishes was used (possible value range from 1 to 101). In addition, the feeling of hunger after every dish was collected (scroll bar from 1 = not hungry to 101 = very hungry).

2.4.3. Attitudes toward veganism

For the rating of veganism, two scales were implemented that were based on previous work of Bryant (2019), Jennings et al. (2019), and Silva Souza et al. (2020). The participants indicated their opinion on eight items that consisted of opposing word pairs to cover many different facets related to veganism (bad/good, harmful/beneficial, unpleasant/pleasant, inedible/enjoyable, negative/positive, repulsive/appealing, boring/exciting, and awkward/comfortable) on a 7-point scale. The internal consistency was high (α = 0.95). The answer to the eight items was averaged. In addition to that, participants indicated on 6 items how likely they would choose a vegan dish in certain situations (e.g., “when out and about with friends” or “in a restaurant”) on a 5-point-scale (from 1 = definitely not to 5 = definitely yes; with an option not to answer the question). The internal consistency was high (α = 0.92). In case of missing values, the available information was divided by the number of answers given and averaged.

2.4.4. New masculinity

The New Masculinity Inventory (NMI; Kaplan et al., 2017) was developed to record the identification with non-traditional forms of masculinity. The 17 items of the original inventory were translated to German by the authors of this paper. As in the study by De Backer et al. (2020), participants indicated on a 5-point scale to what extent they agreed with the statements (from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = fully agree). The items were averaged to obtain a scale score. We also included an option to omit items, which differs from the original version (I cannot judge). In case of missing values, the available information was divided by the number of answers given and averaged. The internal consistency was high (α = 0.87).

2.4.5. Eating behavior

Participants indicated their diet and could choose between omnivorous (meat, dairy products, and vegetables), pescatarian (no meat, but fish), vegetarian (neither meat nor fish), and vegan (neither meat, fish, eggs nor dairy products). Based on the study by De Backer et al. (2020), this study asked about the frequency of meat and fish consumption. Participants stated how often they eat meat/fish at a meal (breakfast, lunch, dinner, in-between; 5-point scale from 1 = never to 5 = every day). The distinction between the various options was made to account for the entire consumption and not just the main meals. The internal consistency was α = 0.89. For an overview of general consumption, the mean value was used. Some additional questions were implemented to check for an influence or correlations with the rating of the dishes: whether meat consumption was restricted for various reasons (religion, health, diet, ethics, intolerance, disgust); food allergies; knowing vegetarians and/or vegans (Thomas, 2016); whether participants did not like a dish for a specific reason (because of the dish itself, the ingredients, an allergy, because it did not contain any animal products or for other reasons).

2.4.6. Validation of stimuli

At the end of the study, we checked whether the conventional and masculine attributes used for the experimental manipulation were perceived as neutral or masculine. We also included feminine items that had not been used in the study to avoid a focus on masculinity (see Supplementary Table S3). For each adjective, the participants indicated whether they perceived it to be neutral, feminine, or more masculine (from 1 = more feminine to 7 = more masculine; with an option not to answer the question).

2.4.7. Control questions

To control for attention and proper understanding two control questions followed each presented dish description. People were asked to indicate which dish they had read about, and which ingredient was not included (from a selection of four dishes or four ingredients). The final sample only considers participants who answered the question about the dishes correctly and who answered at least two of the four questions about the ingredients correctly. At the end of the study, participants were asked whether there were any difficulties in understanding or technical issues during the study (few to none) and whether there were any guesses as to what the study was about (a few participants made correct guesses).

2.5. Data analysis

Data preparation and statistical analysis were carried out with R (R Core Team, 2021; version: 4.2.2). To test the hypotheses, we used linear modeling (lm) provided in the stats package (version: 4.2.2). Categorical variables were effect-coded (gender: −1 = female; 1 = male; experimental manipulation: −1 = conventional; 1 = masculine) and the continuous moderator variable (New Masculinity Inventory) was mean centered. For pairwise comparisons, we also used the package emmeans (version: 1.8.4.1). All tests were conducted two-tailed and the alpha level was set to 0.05. Hypotheses were tested with omnivore participants only because we were interested in the effects on attitude change toward vegan dishes and veganism in persons who do not follow a vegan diet. Hypotheses were specified before the data were collected, and the analytic plan was pre-specified. Any data-driven analyses are clearly identified and discussed appropriately.

3. Results

3.1. Preliminary analyses

3.1.1. Participants' eating behavior and its relation to new masculinity

Most participants had an omnivorous diet, but there were clear gender differences in the number of vegans and vegetarians compared to omnivores, with women being more likely to be vegan and vegetarian than men (see Figure 1). Omnivore men also ate more meat than omnivore women at different meals (see Table 2). Furthermore, among all participants, the major meat reduction reasons were ethics (men = 10.6%; women = 41.7%), health (8.8%; 26.0%), disgust (2.7%; 18.7%), diet (2.4%; 3.2%), intolerance (0.2%; 2.4%), and religion (0%; 1.52%). More reasons for reducing meat were associated with a better rating of the vegan dishes (rs = 0.22, p < 0.001). In addition, among carnivores, more contact with veg*an people was associated with a better rating of the vegan dishes (rs = 0.17, p = 0.001), with most participants (n = 284) in the sample reporting knowing both vegetarians and vegans. Food allergies did not occur often (n = 50 omnivore participants) and did not affect food rating significantly (allergies: M = 68.42, SD = 21.56; no allergies: M = 71.05, SD = 20.22; B = −0.00, p = 0.396). Finally, a stronger identification with new masculinity in men was associated with lower meat consumption in general (rs = −0.25, p = 0.002), at breakfast (rs = −0.18, p = 0.028), at lunch (rs = −0.19, p = 0.015), and meat snacking (rs = −0.35, p < 0.001), but not with meat consumption at dinner (rs = −0.11, p = 0.158).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Gender differences in diet.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparison between omnivore men and women regarding the amount of meat consumed.

3.1.2. Validation of the stimuli

A selection of the adjectives used in the experimental versions to frame the dishes as masculine or conventional were tested for gender associations with masculinity/femininity (see Supplementary Table S3). The twelve adjectives that were intended to be perceived as masculine were significantly different from a neutral rating and rated in the masculine direction (d = 0.21 to 1.48). Six of the seven conventional adjectives were significantly different from a neutral rating and rated toward femininity, and one adjective did not significantly differ from a neutral rating (d = 0.00 to −0.84). The other five adjectives that were expected to be perceived as feminine and were used as filler items were significantly different from the mean toward femininity (d = −0.45 to −1.69). Thus, the ratings of the adjectives indicated that the experimental conditions (masculine vs. conventional framing) were distinguishable, with the masculine adjectives being perceived as more masculine and the conventional ones as neutral or even more on the feminine side.

3.2. Hypotheses

3.2.1. Influence of masculine attribution on the gender association of the vegan dishes

First, the perceived gender association of the vegan dishes was tested with a linear model with the between-subjects factor being the experimental condition (masculine framing vs. conventional framing; see Table 3). In the masculine framing condition, the perception of suitability for dishes shifted toward “more suitable for men”, but these changes were small and never crossed the neutral threshold toward masculinity. Thus, the dishes were never rated as “more suitable for men” but were still perceived as being more suitable for women, moving closer to a neutral association, which supports Hypothesis 1.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Gender association of framed dishes in the conventional and masculine conditions.

3.2.2. Influence of masculine attribution on the rating of dishes and hunger

Linear models with the between-subjects factors gender (men vs. women) and experimental condition (masculine framing vs. conventional framing) and their interaction were used to test Hypothesis 2 (men in the masculine group rate the vegan dishes better than those in the conventional group), and Hypothesis 3 (women are not affected by the experimental manipulation). Dependent variables were the rating of the dishes and self-reported hunger after reading the description of the dishes.

As expected, women rated vegan dishes on average better than men (women: M = 72.49, SD = 20.42 vs. men: M = 66.94, SD = 19.79; B = −8.99, t = −2.919, p = 0.004). Masculine framing did not significantly predict dish rating across men and women (conventional: M = 69.91, SD = 20.46 vs. masculine: M = 71.36, SD = 20.28; B = −0.92, t = 2.533, p = 0.718) and the interaction between gender and experimental condition was not significant (B = 6.95, t = 1.588, p = 0.113). Simple main effects indicated that women in the two conditions did not differ significantly (conventional: M = 72.95, SD = 21.23 vs. masculine: M = 72.04, SD = 19.65; B = 0.92, t = 0.360, p = 0.984). The same was true for men (conventional: M = 63.97, SD = 17.55 vs. masculine: M = 70.00, SD = 21.58; B = −6.04, t = −1.691, p = 0.330; see Figure 2). Thus, the data did not support Hypothesis 2, but it supported Hypothesis 3.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Rating of vegan dishes. Rating from 1 = disagree to 101 = agree. Error bars show 95% CI.

The same analysis was conducted for reported hunger after reading the recipes. Women and men reported similar hunger (women: M = 33.13, SD = 27.59 vs. men: M = 36.34, SD = 28.25; B = 2.84, t = 0.668, p = 0.505). Masculine framing did not significantly predict hunger (conventional: M = 34.38, SD = 27.90 vs. masculine: M = 34.04, SD = 27.81; B = −0.56, t = −0.159, p = 0.874) and the interaction between gender and experimental condition was not significant (B = 0.74, t = 0.122, p = 0.903). Simple main effects indicated that women in the two conditions did not differ significantly (conventional: M = 33.41, SD = 27.48 vs. masculine: M = 32.86, SD = 27.81; B = 0.56, t = 0.159, p = 0.999). The same was true for men (conventional: M = 36.25, SD = 28.83 vs. masculine: M = 36.43, SD = 27.88; B = −0.18, t = −0.036, p = 1; see Figure 3). Thus, again, the data did not support Hypothesis 2, but it supported Hypothesis 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Rating of reported hunger. Rating from 1 = not hungry to 101 = very hungry. Error bars show 95% CI.

3.2.3. Influence of masculine attribution on the rating of veganism

Linear models with the between-subjects factors gender (men vs. women) and experimental condition (masculine framing vs. neutral framing) and their interaction were used to test Hypothesis 4 (the masculine attribution of vegan dishes leads to a better attitude toward veganism in men) and Hypothesis 5 (the masculine attribution of vegan dishes does not influence attitude toward veganism in women). Dependent variables were the rating of veganism and the rating of vegan scenarios.

As expected, women rated veganism on average better than men (women: M = 4.87, SD = 1.07 vs. men: M = 4.35, SD = 1.12; B = −0.58, t = −3.472, p = 0.001). Masculine framing did not significantly predict the rating of veganism (conventional: M = 4.65, SD = 1.11 vs. masculine: M = 4.74, SD = 1.11; B = 0.52, t = 0.383, p = 0.702) and the interaction was not significant (B = 0.10, t = 0.430, p = 0.667). Simple main effects indicated that women in the two conditions did not differ significantly (conventional: M = 4.85, SD = 1.15 vs. masculine: M = 4.90, SD = 0.99; B = −0.05, t = −0.380, p = 0.981). The same was true for the men (conventional: M = 4.27, SD = 0.93 vs. masculine: M = 4.42, SD = 1.28; B = −0.20, t = −1.023, p = 0.736; see Figure 4). Thus, the data did not support Hypothesis 4, but it supported Hypothesis 5.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Rating of veganism. Rating from 7-point scale of opposites. Error bars show 95% CI.

The same analysis was conducted for the rating of the vegan scenarios. As expected, women rated vegan scenarios better than men (women: M = 3.13, SD = 0.78 vs. men: M = 2.82, SD = 0.80; B = −0.29, t = −2.413, p = 0.016). Masculine framing did not significantly predict the rating of vegan scenarios (conventional: M = 2.94, SD = 0.81 vs. masculine: M = 3.11, SD = 0.79; B = 0.17, t = 1.736, p = 0.083) and the interaction was not significant (B = −0.03, t = −0.165, p = 0.869). Simple main effects indicated that women in the two conditions did not differ significantly (conventional: M = 3.04, SD = 0.80 vs. masculine: M = 3.21, SD = 0.76; B = −0.17, t = −1.737, p = 0.306). The same was true for the men (conventional: M = 2.75, SD = 0.80 vs. masculine: M = 3.21, SD = 0.76; B = −0.14, t = −1.030, p = 0.732; see Figure 5). Thus, again, the data did not support Hypothesis 4, but it supported Hypothesis 5.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Rating of vegan scenarios. Rating from 1 = definitely not to 5 = definitely yes. Error bars show 95% CI.

3.2.4. Influence of new masculinity on the relation between framing and rating of the dishes and veganism

The final hypotheses concerned a moderation effect of men's identification with new masculinity on the effect of the experimental manipulation, assuming that a stronger identification with new forms of masculinity weakens the positive effect of a masculine framing on the rating of vegan dishes by men (Hypothesis 6) and the rating of veganism by men (Hypothesis 7). To test these hypotheses, linear models with the categorical variable experimental condition (masculine vs. conventional framing) and new masculinity as (mean-centered) continuous moderator variable were calculated using only participants identifying as omnivore men.

For the rating of vegan dishes, there was no significant effect of identification with new masculinity (B = 4.60, t = 0.929, p = 0.355). However, the interaction between experimental conditions and new masculinity was significant (B = 15.90, t = 2.492, p = 0.014), but the direction was not as expected in Hypothesis 6. As can be seen in Figure 6, the positive effect of the masculine framing increased the more men identified with new masculinity.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Men's rating of vegan dishes in relation to their NM identification. Rating from 1 = disagree to 101 = agree. The new masculinity scores were mean centered. The solid line represents conventional framing, the dotted line masculine framing.

For reported hunger, there was no significant effect of identification with new masculinity (B = −0.91, t = −0.115, p = 0.909) and the interaction between experimental condition and new masculinity was not significant (B = −4.01, t = −0.395, p = 0.693). Thus, again, the data did not support Hypothesis 6.

Concerning the attitude toward veganism, results showed that a higher identification with new masculinity significantly predicted the rating of veganism (B = 0.850, t = 3.197, p = 0.002). The more men identified with new masculinity, the better their attitudes toward veganism. However, there was no significant interaction effect between experimental conditions and new masculinity and thus no support for Hypothesis 7 (B = 0.45, t = 1.311, p = 0.192).

For the rating of vegan scenarios, there was a significant effect of identification with new masculinity (B = 0.60, t = 2.818, p = 0.006). The more men identified with new masculinity, the better they rated the dishes. However, the interaction between experimental conditions and new masculinity was not significant (B = −0.22, t = −0.807, p = 0.421). Thus, again, the data did not support Hypothesis 7.

4. Discussion

This study investigated whether vegan food, which is typically associated with femininity, can be made more attractive for men by describing it with masculine attributes. To do so, we provided participants with a description of vegan dishes with a conventional or a masculine framing. We expected that a description of vegan food with masculine attributes affects the gender association of the vegan dishes, and thus reduce the perceived inconsistency with men's gender identity (Hypothesis 1). In line with our hypothesis, we found that the vegan dishes framed in a masculine way shifted toward a gender-neutral rating, weakening the link to femininity.

We also assumed that men like vegan dishes better when described with masculine attributes (Hypothesis 2), but this hypothesis was not supported for any of the dependent variables (rating of the dishes and reported hunger after reading the recipes). As a control, we also examined women's ratings. Here we also found no meaningful differences between the two framing conditions, which supported Hypothesis 3. We also investigated if any effects of a masculine framing generalized to men's attitudes toward veganism in general (Hypothesis 4). Again, we could not find a meaningful effect for any of the dependent variables (rating of veganism and rating of different vegan scenarios). As a control, we also examined women's ratings. As for the men, there were no differences between the experimental conditions, which aligned with Hypothesis 5.

According to these findings, it worked to influence the gender association of vegan dishes by describing the dishes in a masculine way. This underlines that gender associations of products and behaviors are susceptible to influences (Martin et al., 1990; Worth et al., 1992). However, the masculine framing did not meaningfully affect men's attitudes toward vegan dishes or toward veganism in general. This was unexpected given that Gal and Wilkie (2010) found that describing dishes with masculine or feminine attributes affected men's preferences for the dishes. An explanation for this finding might be that the effects of masculine framing on gender associations were too small to affect the attitudes toward vegan dishes and veganism, which men typically strongly associate with femininity and perceive as appropriate for women (Thomas, 2016). Indeed, even in the masculine framing condition, none of the dishes was rated as “more suitable for men” and the perceptions shifted only slightly toward a neutral rating. For the two dishes with a significant change in gender association (carbonara and burger), we replicated the reported analyses separately, but the results were similar to the analysis, in which all dishes were included. It must be noted that the validation of adjectives used for the experimental manipulation indicated that some of the attributes used in the masculine framing condition were only moderately associated with masculinity. Therefore, it is quite possible that attributes, which are more strongly associated with masculinity, will have stronger effects on gender associations and on attitudes toward vegan dishes and veganism. Future studies could explore whether some words might be more suitable for a masculine framing than others, and whether words perceived as more masculine might lead to findings hypothesized in the present study, as was the case in other interventions (e.g., electric car in Brough et al., 2016). Additionally, other framing interventions could prove successful. Szczebyło et al. (2022) found a strong attachment to meat due to hedonistic reasons among Polish millennials, and taste-focused labeling could be used to encourage healthier eating (e.g., Turnwald et al., 2020). Furthermore, masculine framings could be tried on other diets for a better understanding of potential effects (e.g., the Mediterranean diet which is characterized by a focus on local food and the sustainability of ecosystems and landscapes; Medina, 2019). Another complementary explanation for the non-significant findings in this study might be that veganism is too strongly associated with femininity for a short-term intervention to have any effects on attitudes toward vegan dishes and veganism in general. Indeed, for many men masculinity is associated with meat consumption (Adams, 1990; Rothgerber, 2013), so eating meat can even stand as a metaphor for masculinity (Rozin et al., 2012). In contrast, eating only plant-based nutrition (i.e., being vegan) is associated with femininity (Adams, 1990), with men perceiving other vegan men as less masculine (Thomas, 2016). Therefore, long-term interventions might be needed to successfully change attitudes toward vegan dishes and particularly veganism in general, and we encourage future studies to explore such long-term interventions (for instance in field settings such as a cafeteria). Furthermore, we examined whether identification with new masculinity (Kaplan et al., 2017) moderates the effects of a masculine framing of vegan dishes. We expected that men who identify more strongly with new forms of masculinity are influenced less by a masculine framing. As expected, higher identification with new masculinity correlated with more favorable attitudes toward veganism. There was a small to medium relationship between men's new masculinity identification and lower meat consumption. This agrees with De Backer et al.'s (2020) finding that men who identified with new forms of masculinity ate less meat, had less attachment to meat, were more likely to reduce their meat consumption, and were more positive about vegetarians. However, with one exception, new masculinity did not moderate the effect of the experimental manipulation. This exception was the rating of the dishes, in which men identifying more strongly with new masculinity were even more affected by masculine framing. This disagrees with our hypothesis and implies that the relationship between masculine identity and masculine framing might be less straightforward than assumed. Importantly, it has to be noted that most men in our study seemed to strongly identify with new masculinity, which restricts any conclusions on men who have a stronger traditional male identification.

4.1. Limitations

As in any study, some limitations of the present study have to be noted. A limitation is that the stimuli used for the experimental conditions were not piloted but taken from previous studies. Although we obtained ratings for a selection of the attributes, which confirmed that the masculine and the conventional conditions did differ in the intended direction, this led to the use of attributes in the masculine framing condition, which showed only moderate masculinity ratings. Consistently using attributes that are more strongly associated with masculinity could potentially have increased the effects of the experimental manipulation. Another limitation is that some scales used in the present study were adapted and partly self-developed due to a lack of suitable measures. Although the internal consistencies were good and we used two dependent variables per hypothesis, the scales might not have been particularly suitable to capture the effects of a short intervention. For example, the feeling of hunger as a dependent variable contains only one statement that might not vary as a consequence of the short exposure to different dishes.

5. Conclusion

In this study, we have examined the effects of manipulating vegan dishes with masculine attributes on gender perception and attitudes toward veganism. The results highlight the intriguing possibility of successful alteration of gender associations with vegan dishes through a brief masculine framing intervention. However, while this shift in perception was observed, it did not lead to significant changes in men's broader attitudes toward vegan dishes or veganism in general. Our findings suggest that the potency of a short-term intervention might not be sufficient to counterbalance the prevailing feminine connotations associated with veganism. Despite the modest impact on gender appropriateness ratings for men, it is imperative to acknowledge the gender-neutral response exhibited by women, indicating that the intervention did not negatively affect their perception. Thus, we encourage further exploration of masculine framing to improve men's perception of vegan food and the vegan concept, but with stronger stimuli and/or longer intervention duration. Furthermore, it is prudent to delve deeper into the factors that may modulate the effectiveness of such interventions. Our initial exploration into new forms of masculinity in this context underscores the need for a comprehensive understanding of the intricate interplay between gender identity and dietary preferences. Finally, research on veganism is relatively new compared to vegetarianism. Given the relevance of this diet to health and environmental issues, further empirical research is worthwhile to develop targeted interventions that could foster a reduction in meat consumption and engender a more appealing perception of vegan cuisine, particularly among men.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://osf.io/3mdbq/?view_only=dba6ef50f0f941dfb4940695e68eaba6.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans ethics approval was not required for this study because according to the ethics guidelines of the German Society for Psychology (DGPs) and the regulations of the Ethics Committee of the Institute for Psychology of the Faculty for Human Sciences of the Julius-Maximilians-University of Würzburg, prior review is not mandatory for research that provides signed informed consent from study participants, collects data anonymously, and has no foreseeable negative impact on participants. However, the study was reviewed post-hoc by the Ethics Committee of the University of Bamberg, Germany, and approval was granted. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

AES contributed to the conception of the study, performed the statistical analysis, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. AES and JL contributed to the design of the study and organized the database. All authors contributed to the manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fcomm.2023.1244471/full#supplementary-material

References

Adams, C. J. (1990). The Sexual Politics of Meat: A Feminist-Vegetarian Critical Theory. New York, NY: Continuum.

Google Scholar

Appleby, P. N., and Key, T. J. (2016). The long-term health of vegetarians and vegans. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 75, 287–293. doi: 10.1017/S0029665115004334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Athenstaedt, U., and Alfermann, D. (2011). Geschlechterrollen und ihre Folgen: Eine sozialpsychologische Betrachtung [Gender Roles and Their Consequences: A Social Psychology Perspective]. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer Verlag.

Google Scholar

Bock, B. C., and Kanarek, R. B. (1995). Women and men are what they eat: the effects of gender and reported meal size on perceived characteristics. Sex Roles 33, 109–119. doi: 10.1007/BF01547938

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogueva, D., Marinova, D., and Bryant, C. (2022). Meat me halfway: Sydney meat-loving men's restaurant experience with alternative plant-based proteins. Sustainability, 14, 1290. doi: 10.3390/su14031290

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brough, A. R., Wilkie, J. E. B., Ma, J., Isaac, M. S., and Gal, D. (2016). Is eco-friendly unmanly? The green-feminine stereotype and its effect on sustainable consumption. J. Consum. Res. 43, 567–582. doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucw044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, C. J. (2019). We can't keep meating like this: attitudes towards vegetarian and vegan diets in the United Kingdom. Sustainability, 11, 6844. doi: 10.3390/su11236844

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brysbaert, M. (2019). How many words do we read per minute? A review and meta-analysis of reading rate. J. Mem. Lang. 109, 104047. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2019.104047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavazza, N., Guidetti, M., and Butera, F. (2015). Ingredients of gender-based stereotypes about food. Indirect influence of food type, portion size and presentation on gendered intentions to eat. Appetite 91, 266–272. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Contois, E. (2020). “Blogging food, performing gender,” in The Cambridge Companion to Literature and Food, eds Coghlan, J. M (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 243–261.

Google Scholar

De Backer, D., Erreygers, C., De Cort, S., Vandermoere, C., Dhoest, F., Vrinten, A., et al. (2020). Meat and masculinities. Can differences in masculinity predict meat consumption, intentions to reduce meat and attitudes towards vegetarians? Appetite 147, 104559. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Garine, I. (2001). Views about food prejudice and stereotypes. Soc. Sci. Inf. 40, 487–507. doi: 10.1177/053901801040003006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinu, M., Abbate, R., Gensini, G. F., Casini, A., and Sofi, F. (2017). Vegetarian, vegan diets and multiple health outcomes: a systematic review with meta-analysis of observational studies. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 57, 3640–3649. doi: 10.1080/10408398.2016.1138447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckes, T. (2008). “Geschlechterstereotype: Von Rollen, Identitäten und Vorurteilen in Becker,” in Handbuch Frauen- Und Geschlechterforschung, ed Kortendiek, B. (Wiesbaden: VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften), 171–182.

Google Scholar

Eldridge, J. J., and Gluck, J. P. (1996). Gender differences in attitudes toward animal research. Ethics Behav. 6, 239–256. doi: 10.1207/s15327019eb0603_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

FAO. (2018). World Livestock: Transforming the livestock sector through the Sustainable Development Goals. Available online at: www.fao.org/3/CA1177EN/ca1177en.pdf (accessed January 27, 2021).

Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

FORSA. (2020). Ernährungsreport 2019/2020: Ergebnisse einer Repräsentativen Bevölkerungsbefragung [Nutrition Report 2019/2020: Results of a Representative Survey of the Population]. Bundesministerium für Ernährung und Landwirtschaft. Available online at: https://www.bmel.de/DE/themen/ernaehrung/ernaehrungsreport2020.html (accessed January 27, 2021).

Google Scholar

Gal, D., and Wilkie, J. (2010). Real men don't eat quiche: regulation of gender-expressive choices by men. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 1, 291–301. doi: 10.1177/1948550610365003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenebaum, J., and Dexter, B. (2018). Vegan men and hybrid masculinity. J. Gender Stu. 27, 637–648. doi: 10.1080/09589236.2017.1287064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grubb, E. L., and Grathwohl, H. L. (1967). Consumer self-concept, symbolism and market behavior: a theoretical approach. J. Market. 31, 22–27. doi: 10.1177/002224296703100405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, D. (2018). Faux-meat and masculinity: the gendering of food on three vegan blogs. Can. Food Stu. 5, 133–155. doi: 10.15353/cfs-rcea.v5i1.233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jennings, L., Danforth, C. M., Dodds, P. S., Pinel, E., and Pope, L. (2019). Exploring Perceptions of Veganism. Available online at: https://arxiv.org/abs/1907.12567 (accessed January 27, 2021).

Google Scholar

Kaplan, D., Rosenmann, A., and Shuhendler, S. (2017). What about nontraditional masculinities? Toward a quantitative model of therapeutic new masculinity ideology. Men Masc. 20, 393–426. doi: 10.1177/1097184X16634797

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knight, S., Vrij, A., Cherryman, J., and Nunkoosing, K. (2004). Attitudes towards animal use and belief in animal mind. Anthrozoös, 17, 43–62. doi: 10.2752/089279304786991945

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leiner, D. J. (2019). SoSci Survey (Version 3, 1.06). [Computer software]. Available online at: https://www.soscisurvey.de (accessed January 31, 2022).

Google Scholar

Leip, A., Billen, G., Garnier, J., Grizzetti, B., Lassaletta, L., Reis, S., et al. (2015). Impacts of European livestock production: nitrogen, sulphur, phosphorus and greenhouse gas emissions, land-use, water eutrophication and biodiversity. Environ. Res. Lett. 10, 1–13. doi: 10.1088/1748-9326/10/11/115004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leitzmann, C. (2014). Vegetarian nutrition: past, present, future. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 100, 496S−502S. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.071365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, S. J. (1959). Symbols for sale. Harvard Bus. Rev. 37, 117–124.

Google Scholar

Love, H. J., and Sulikowski, D. (2018). Of meat and men: sex differences in implicit and explicit attitudes toward meat. Front. Psychol. 9, 559. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malitska, J. (2021). The peripheries of omnivorousness: vegetarian canteens and social activism in the early twentieth-century Russian empire. Global Food History 7, 140–175. doi: 10.1080/20549547.2021.1923997

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, C. L. (1990). Attitudes and expectations about children with nontraditional and traditional gender roles. Sex Roles 22, 151–166. doi: 10.1007/BF00288188

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, C. L., Wood, C. H., and Little, J. K. (1990). The development of gender stereotype components. Child Dev. 61, 1891–1904. doi: 10.2307/1130845

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McAfee, A. J., McSorley, E. M., Cuskelly, G. J., Moss, B. W., Wallace, J. M. W., Bonham, M. P., et al. (2010). Red meat consumption: an overview of the risks and benefts. Meat Sci. 84, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2009.08.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McPhail, D., Beagan, B., and Chapman, G. E. (2012). “I don't want to be sexist but …” Denying and re-inscribing gender through food. Food Cult. Soc. 15, 473–489. doi: 10.2752/175174412X13276629246046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medina, F. X. (2019). “Sustainable food systems in culturally coherent social contexts: discussions around culture, sustainability, climate change and the mediterranean diet,” in Climate Change-Resilient Agriculture and Agroforestry: Ecosystem Services and Sustainability, eds Castro, P., Azul, A. M., Leal Filho, W., and Azeiteiro, U. M (Basel: Springer, Cham), 189–196.

Google Scholar

Mensink, G. B. M., Lage Barbosa, C., and Brettschneider, A. K. (2016). Verbreitung der vegetarischen Ernährungsweise in Deutschland. J. Health Monit. 1, 2–15. doi: 10.17886/RKI-GBE-2016-033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Micha, R., Wallace, S. K., and Mozaffarian, D. (2010). Red and processed meat consumption and risk of incident coronary heart disease, stroke, and diabetes mellitus: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Circulation 56, 2271–2283. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.109.924977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niederle, P., and Schubert, M. N. (2020). How does veganism contribute to shape sustainable food systems? Practices, meanings and identities of vegan restaurants in Porto Alegre, Brazil. J. Rur. Stu. 78, 304–313. doi: 10.1016/j.jrurstud.2020.06.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piazza, J., Ruby, M. B., Loughnan, S., Luong, M., Kulik, J., Watkins, H. M., et al. (2015). Rationalizing meat consumption the 4Ns. Appetite 91, 114–128. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poore, J., and Nemecek, T. (2018). Reducing food's environmental impacts through producers and consumers. Science 360, 987–992. doi: 10.1126/science.aaq0216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team. (2021). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. (Version 4, 0.4). [Computer software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online at: https://www.R-project.org/

Google Scholar

Ritchie, H., and Roser, M. (2017). Meat and Dairy Production. Our World In Data. Available online at: https://ourworldindata.org/meat-production (accessed March 10 2021).

Google Scholar

Rogers, R. A. (2008). Beasts, burgers, and hummers: meat and the crisis of masculinity in contemporary television advertisements. Environ. Commun. 2, 281–301. doi: 10.1080/17524030802390250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothgerber, H. (2013). Real men don't eat (vegetable) quiche: masculinity and the justification of meat consumption. Psychol. Men Masc. 14, 363–375. doi: 10.1037/a0030379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozin, P., Hormes, J. M., Faith, M. S., and Wansink, B. (2012). Is meat male? A quantitative multimethod framework to establish metaphoric relationships. J. Consum. Res. 39, 629–643. doi: 10.1086/664970

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruby, M. B., and Heine, S. J. (2011). Meat, morals, and masculinity. Appetite, 56, 447–450. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sexton, A. E., Garnett, T., and Lorimer, J. (2019). Framing the future of food: the contested promises of alternative proteins. Environ. Plan. Nat. Space 2, 47–72. doi: 10.1177/2514848619827009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva Souza, L. G., Atkinson, A., and Montague, B. (2020). Perceptions About Veganism. Available online at: https://www.vegansociety.com/get-involved/research/research-projects/perceptions-about-veganism (accessed January 27, 2021).

Google Scholar

SKOPOS. (2020). 1, 3. Millionen Deutsche leben vegan. SKOPOS Group. Available online at: https://www.skopos-group.de/news/13-millionen-deutsche-leben-vegan.html (accessed December 20, 2020).

Google Scholar

Steins, G. (2010). “Einführung in Psychologie und Geschlechterforschung”, in Handbuch Psychologie und Geschlechterforschung (Wiesbaden: VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften), 11–23.

Google Scholar

Szczebyło, A., Halicka, E., and Rejman, K. (2022). Is eating less meat possible? Exploring the willingness to reduce meat consumption among millennials working in Polish cities. Foods 11, 358. doi: 10.3390/foods11030358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, M. A. (2016). Are vegans the same as vegetarians? The effect of diet on perceptions of masculinity. Appetite 97, 79–86. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turnwald, B., Boles, D., Perry, M., Conner, A., Crum, A., Tumolillo, M. A., et al (2020). Edgy Veggies Toolkit: How to Incorporate Taste-Focused Labeling to Encourage Healthier Eating. Stanford SPARQtools. Available online at: http://sparqtools.org/edgyveggies/ (accessed December 20, 2020).

Google Scholar

Twigg, J. (1983). “Vegetarianism and the meanings in meat”, in The Sociology of Food and Eating: Essays on the Sociological Significance of Food, ed A. Murcott (Berlin: Grover), 18–30.

Google Scholar

WCRF. (2007). Food, nutrition, physical activity, and the prevention of cancer: a global perspective. Ernährung 1, 464–469. doi: 10.1007/s12082-007-0105-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, K., and Dahl, D. W. (2006). To be or not be? The influence of dissociative reference groups on consumer preferences. J. Consum. Psychol. 16, 404–414. doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp1604_11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. E., Satterwhite, R. C., and Best, D. L. (1999). Pancultural gender stereotypes revisited: the five factor model. Sex Roles 40, 513–525. doi: 10.1023/A:1018831928829

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worth, L. T., Smith, J., and Mackie, D. M. (1992). Gender schematicity and preference for gender-typed products. Psychol. Market. 9, 17–30. doi: 10.1002/mar.4220090104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: veganism, masculinity, gender association, gender stereotypes, framing

Citation: Scholz AE and Lenhart J (2023) Masculinity and veganism: the effect of linking vegan dishes with masculinity on men's attitudes toward vegan food. Front. Commun. 8:1244471. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2023.1244471

Received: 22 June 2023; Accepted: 17 August 2023;
Published: 05 October 2023.

Edited by:

Victoria Team, Monash University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Araceli Galiano-Coronil, University of Cádiz, Spain
F. Xavier Medina, Fundació per a la Universitat Oberta de Catalunya, Spain

Copyright © 2023 Scholz and Lenhart. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alma Elisabeth Scholz, almascholzgm@gmail.com

Download