Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Ecol. Evol., 18 December 2017
Sec. Behavioral and Evolutionary Ecology
Volume 5 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2017.00163

Urban Great Tits (Parus major) Show Higher Distress Calling and Pecking Rates than Rural Birds across Europe

Juan Carlos Senar1* Laszlo Z. Garamszegi2 Vallo Tilgar3 Clotilde Biard4 Gregorio Moreno-Rueda5 Pablo Salmón6 J. M. Rivas5 Philipp Sprau7 Niels J. Dingemanse7 Anne Charmantier8,9 Virginie Demeyrier4,8 Helena Navalpotro1 Caroline Isaksson6
  • 1Unit of Evolutionary and Behavioural Ecology, Natural History Museum of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 2Department of Evolutionary Ecology, Estación Biológica de Doñana, Sevilla, Spain
  • 3Department of Zoology, Institute of Ecology and Earth Sciences, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia
  • 4Institut d'Ecologie et des Sciences de l'Environnement, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, UPEC, Paris 7, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, INRA, IRD, Paris, France
  • 5Departamento de Zoología, Universidad de Granada, Granada, Spain
  • 6Evolutionary Ecology Unit, Department of Biology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden
  • 7Department of Biology, Ludwig Maximilians University Munich, Munich, Germany
  • 8Centre d'Ecologie Fonctionnelle et Evolutive, Montpellier, France
  • 9Département des Sciences Biologiques, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada

Environmental change associated with urbanization is considered one of the major threats to biodiversity. Some species nevertheless seem to thrive in the urban areas, probably associated with selection for phenotypes that match urban habitats. Previous research defined different “copying styles” in distress behavior during the handling of birds. These behaviors vary along a continuum from “proactive” to “reactive” copers. By studying avian distress behaviors we aimed to broaden our understanding of the relationship between coping styles and urbanization. Using a large-scale comparative study of seven paired rural and urban sites across Europe, we assayed distress behaviors during handling of urban and rural-dwelling populations of the great tit Parus major. We detected no consistent pairwise differences in breath rate between urban and rural habitats. However, urban great tits displayed more distress calling (fear screams) and higher pecking rate (handling aggression) than rural birds. These findings suggest that urban great tits have a more proactive coping strategy when dealing with stressful conditions. This finding is in line with previous studies implying that urban great tits are more explorative, less neophobic, and display shorter flight distances than their rural counterparts, representing further aspects of the same “proactive,” coping strategy. Future research should investigate whether reported differences in distress behavior are due to local adaption caused by natural selection or due to phenotypic plasticity.

Introduction

Urban environments are expanding worldwide with an unprecedented speed (United Nations, 2014); environmental change associated with urbanization is being considered as one of the main current threats to biodiversity (Turner et al., 2004). However, urbanization also represents a potential source of selection and a new avenue to study evolutionary processes (Donihue and Lambert, 2014; Isaksson, 2015; Alberti et al., 2017; Hendry et al., 2017).

For wildlife, urban habitats differ in many respects from rural habitats, including microclimate, food abundance, pollution, abundance of exotic species, and predation risk (Luniak and Pisarski, 1982; Marzluff et al., 2001; Gaston, 2010; Gil and Brumm, 2014). Many of these factors induce stress, defined in a broad sense as changes away from an organisms' physiological homeostasis that emerge in response to a change in the environment (sensu Badyaev, 2005; Tuomainen and Candolin, 2012). This is especially the case for factors that represent, or are associated with increased perceived predation risk, such as elevated disturbance due to the presence of humans, pet mammals, and cars (Buchanan and Partecke, 2012). The obvious hypothesis based on this known disturbance or perceived threat is that urban animals would have an enhanced stress response than rural-dwelling animals. However, the results are mixed. For example, some studies reveal higher corticosterone levels (avian stress hormones) in urban compared to rural bird populations (Schoech et al., 2007; Fokidis et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2011), whereas other studies found the opposite pattern or no significant differences (Partecke et al., 2006; French et al., 2008; Abolins-Abols et al., 2016).

Nevertheless, whatever the direction of the effects, they are likely to influence aspects of behavior, physiology, and life history of urban-dwellers in their coping response to the urban environment (Tuomainen and Candolin, 2012; Sprau and Dingemanse, 2017). Consequently, stress-responses, i.e., the behavioral and physiological means by which animals cope with environmental stress, play an important role in local adaptation (Badyaev, 2005). Hence, comparing stress-responses between urban- and rural-dwelling populations can shed light on the mechanisms underpinning adaptation to urban habitats. For example, predatory attack is most likely one of the highest stresses that a prey animal can experience, and this is irrespective of whether the animal is habituated to the urban environment. Behavioral responses to such threats are commonly referred to as distress behaviors (Møller and Ibáñez-Álamo, 2012). Previous research has shown that distress behaviors displayed during handling are parts of the same phenotypic syndrome that encapsulates an entire suite of behavioral and physiological traits that individual display in order to adaptively respond to stressful situations (Koolhaas et al., 1999, 2010; Carere and van Oers, 2004; Coppens et al., 2010; Carvalho et al., 2013; Class et al., 2014; Kluen et al., 2014). Responses can be categorized into different “coping styles,” varying along a continuum from “proactive” to “reactive” copers, and they relate to the way organisms deal with stressors. Summarizing, proactive individuals are more aggressive, explorative, neophilic, and risk-prone than reactive individuals (Carere et al., 2010).

Accordingly, birds displaying a high frequency of distress behaviors represent proactive individuals. We focus in this paper on three distress behaviors easily recorded during handling: distress calling, handling aggression, and breath rate. Distress calling rate has previously been related to proactivity: in black-capped chickadees Poecile atricapillus, distress calling rate was positively related to exploration rate, which is one of the main characteristics of the proactive coping style (Guillette and Sturdy, 2011). In siskins Carduelis spinus, individuals uttering more often distress calls, displayed also bolder behaviors in front of a novel object (Mateos-González and Senar, 2012; Pascual and Senar, 2014). In the Japanese quail Coturnix japonica, distress calling was positively related to the excretion of corticosterone metabolites, implying that high levels of distress calling are related to proactivity (Niall Daisley et al., 2005). Distress calling has therefore been used as a proxy of proactivity in several studies (Andersen, 2012; Pascual and Senar, 2014; Thorsteinsen, 2015; Richardson et al., 2016). Aggression has also been regarded as a typical response of proactive copers to stressful situations (Koolhaas et al., 1999; Carere et al., 2010), and pecking rate during handling is classically used to compare level of proactivity among individuals (Brommer and Kluen, 2012; van den Brink et al., 2012a,b; Class et al., 2014; Kluen et al., 2014; Dubuc-Messier et al., 2017). Breath rate has also been suggested as an indicator of coping style, with breath rates being higher in proactive individuals than in reactive ones (Carere and van Oers, 2004; van Oers and Carere, 2007; Torné-Noguera et al., 2014; Charmantier et al., 2017). Additionally, breath rate has been found to correlate positively to exploration rate, which as previously stated, is one of the main characteristics of the proactive coping style (Charmantier et al., 2017). The three behaviors have also been found to be heritable (Koenig et al., 1991; Brommer and Kluen, 2012). Therefore, these three distress behaviors can be used as an accurate proxy of coping style and their study may allow a broader understanding of the role of coping style mediating processes of urbanization in birds.

Recent work has found that distress behaviors during handling differ between urban and rural population of birds. For example, an interspecific comparison revealed that urban birds showed higher frequencies of distress calling when handled than their rural counterparts (Møller and Ibáñez-Álamo, 2012). Aggression, another typical response to stressful situations (Koolhaas et al., 1999), has also been found to differ in inter-specific comparisons between urban and rural individuals during handling (Møller and Ibáñez-Álamo, 2012). At the intra-specific level, urban birds have also been shown to display different breath rates compared to their rural counterparts during handling (Torné-Noguera et al., 2014; Abolins-Abols et al., 2016; Charmantier et al., 2017). Yet, this relationship may differ between species. To date, studies comparing distress behaviors between urban and rural bird populations (Møller and Ibáñez-Álamo, 2012; Torné-Noguera et al., 2014) have lacked sufficient replication to draw firm conclusions regarding the relationship between urbanization and avian distress behavior (Evans et al., 2009).

The main aim of the present paper was to investigate whether urban and rural populations of the great tits (Parus major) differ consistently in their behavioral response to an acute perceived threat, assayed during capturing and handling (Møller and Nielsen, 2010; Markó et al., 2013; Class et al., 2014; Kluen et al., 2014; Dubuc-Messier et al., 2017). To acquire a robust insight into the relationship between urbanization and distress behavior, we assayed three different distress behaviors (distress calling rate, pecking rate, and breath rate) in seven replicate pairs of urban and rural/forest populations across Europe. Previous work on this species has revealed many differences between urban and rural great tits in terms of morphology (Horak et al., 1995; Senar et al., 2014; Biard et al., 2017), physiology (Andersson et al., 2015; Salmón et al., 2016; Toledo et al., 2016), genetics (Björklund et al., 2010; Riyahi et al., 2015), life-history (Berressem et al., 1983; Schmidt and Einloft-Achenbach, 1984; Isaksson and Andersson, 2007; Hedblom and Soderstrom, 2012; Bailly et al., 2015; Vaugoyeau et al., 2016; Charmantier et al., 2017), population dynamics (Horak and Lebreton, 1998), and behavior (Slabbekoorn and Peet, 2003; Salaberria and Gil, 2010; Riyahi et al., 2017). Urban great tits have been found to be more explorative and less neophobic than rural tits (Tryjanowski et al., 2016; Charmantier et al., 2017; Riyahi et al., 2017), to display shorter flight initiation distances (Møller et al., 2015), and to show a higher problem solving performance than rural birds (Preiszner et al., 2017). Based on this growing body of literature, which suggests that a proactive coping strategy fits urban conditions, we predict that urban great tit populations show a higher distress calling rate, pecking rate and breath rate compared to their rural conspecifics.

Materials and Methods

Study Areas and Sampling

Great tits were caught in seven distinct cities paired with a close rural site: Granada (Spain), Barcelona (Spain), Montpellier (France), Munich (Germany), Paris (France), Malmö (Sweden), and Tartu (Estonia; Table 1). Paired sites within localities were separated with distances ranging from 5 to 106 km (Table 1). Natural habitats were either forest or rural areas, but we refer to them as rural habitats for simplicity. To quantify the degree of urbanization in each locality we used aerial images from Google Maps following the methods previously described to assess the effect of the degree of urbanization on wild bird populations (Seress et al., 2014; Vincze et al., 2017). Briefly, each locality was represented by a 1 × 1 km2 rectangular area around the birds' capture site. The content in each rectangle was evaluated by dividing the image in 100 × 100 m2 cells and considering three land-cover characteristics in each: proportion of buildings, vegetation (including cultivated fields), and paved surfaces. The different land-cover measures obtained per site fitted into a principal component analysis (PCA) to estimate an urbanization score (PC1) per site (Table 1). The PC1 values were multiplied with −1 to obtain more positive values in the more urbanized localities.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sampling localities of urban and rural great tits (Parus major) across Europe.

Great tits were captured using spring traps, mist nets or funnel traps (Senar et al., 1997). Though trapping methods differed among localities, they were identical in six out of seven urban/rural pair sites (only Granada differed). Sampling was conducted during the main breeding season at each site (April–June), except for the Barcelona population, which was sampled from January to April. Overall, we quantified distress behaviors of 1,539 individual great tits. All individuals represent independent data points as re-captures were not included in our analyses comparing distress behavior between habitats (see below). However, behavioral information of individuals recorded in multiple years was used to estimate repeatability (68 birds for breath rate and 22 for pecking and distress calling rates, see below). All birds were sexed and aged (yearling vs. adult) according to plumage characteristics (Svensson, 1992).

Behavioral Tests

We performed measurements of distress behavior within 5 min after capture. We recorded three behaviors known to reflect coping style and stress responsiveness and/or fighting propensity during handling (Carere and van Oers, 2004; Fucikova et al., 2009; Laiolo et al., 2009; Markó et al., 2013; Torné-Noguera et al., 2014). Breath rate, distress call rate, and pecking rate were recorded in the same consecutive order. The target traits were not recorded in all localities: in Montpellier and Munich only breath rate was recorded.

Breath rate was recorded by counting the number of breast respiratory movements within 30 s, while holding the wings fixed and also ensuring reduced disturbance by visual and sound cues (Markó et al., 2013; Torné-Noguera et al., 2014). Pecking rate and distress calling rate were subsequently quantified by counting the number of pecks and distress calls that were emitted during the next 15 s of handling (Fucikova et al., 2009; Laiolo et al., 2009). Pecking rate refers to the number of pecks against a straight finger positioned at 1–2 cm from the beak of the focal bird while holding its legs (Markó et al., 2013). Distress calling rate was defined as the number of vocalized distress calls recorded during the same pecking rate trial (Markó et al., 2013). Breath rate was measured by multiple observers in Montpellier (n = 4) and Munich (n = 14). In the other localities, a single observer recorded all data in both the rural and urban site.

Statistical Analyses

To test for general differences in distress behavior between the urban and rural habitats across the seven localities, data were analyzed using univariate linear mixed-effects models (LMM), in which breath rate, pecking rate, or distress calling rate were sequentially included as the response variable. As fixed effects, we included habitat (rural vs. urban), sex and age (yearling vs. adult). Locality (n = 7 levels) and population (n = 2 populations* 7 localities = 14 populations) were included as random effects. LRT refers to a likelihood ratio test. As breath rate was measured by multiple observers in two of the localities (see above), we also included observer as random effect (23 levels, nested within locality) in models explaining variation in breath rate. Since the seven localities represented a wide range of different habitats, a habitat-specific residual variance was fitted to account for unequal residual variances between the two types (rural vs. urban). Breath rate was analyzed assuming a Gaussian error distribution, while pecking rate and distress call were analyzed assuming a Poisson error distribution.

Bivariate LMM were used to estimate among-population and among-individual-within-population covariances (and correlations) between each unique combination of two behaviors, where the two focal behaviors were entered as response variables (following procedures detailed by Dingemanse and Dochtermann, 2013).

Only a priori considered combinations of predictor variables were entered into the statistical models, which was based on biological thinking. This approach to the explicit test of a priori formulated hypotheses has been suggested to allow more general inferences than more traditional exploratory analyses in which various possible interactions are tested sequentially or based on statistical algorithm (Dochtermann, 2010). Accordingly, we were interested in the interaction between habitat and locality, because the difference between urban and rural habitats might differ among localities (see also below). We also included the interaction between habitat and sex, because previous analyses revealed sex differences in the expression of some of the focal behaviors (Markó et al., 2013). We also included an interaction term between habitat and age to test for age-specific differences between urban and rural populations (Markó et al., 2013). In cases where the interaction between sex and habitat was significant, we additionally modeled the sexes separately, in order to understand for which sex the differences between habitats were statistically supported.

We assumed that previous “capture history” did not affect distress behavior, an assumption that was confirmed for the Barcelona site, where the number of preceding captures had no effect on distress behaviors measured during the present study [distress calling rate: F(1, 119) = 1.4, p = 0.32; pecking rate: F(1, 119) = 0.6, p = 0.52; breath rate: F(1, 119) = 0.2, p = 0.73]. Furthermore, we can infer that “capture history” should not be an important confounder, as the data used in our analyses were largely based on first captures.

Adjusted repeatability was calculated following Nakagawa and Schielzeth (2010) using series of univariate LMMs where individual identity and population identity were fitted as a random effects, and where sex, age, and habitat as fixed effects. We repeated these analyses for rural and urban birds separately to calculate repeatability within each habitat type (urban vs. rural).

All statistical analyses were carried out using the package lme4, version 1.1-12 (Bates et al., 2011) and MCMCglmm version 2.23 (Hadfield, 2010) within the R (version 3.3.1) computing environment (R Development Core Team, 2017). For the modeling based on MCMCglmm, we ran Markov chains up to 230,000 iterations with 30,000 iterations of burn in and with 500 iterations of thinning interval (longer runs, such as 1,300,000 iterations, did not improve the results). For prior definition, we followed a technique called parameter expansion as was suggested by Hadfield (2010), and set V = 1, nu = 0.02, and alpha.V = 1,000. We repeated each run for each model 3–4 times to check the stability of results. After each run, the trace and distribution of all estimated parameters were checked visually, as well as autocorrelation between iterations. Furthermore, mixing and convergence were checked with Gelman-Rubin statistics (Gelman and Rubin, 1992).

Results

The selected urban areas had significantly higher urbanization scores than the selected rural areas [F(1, 13) = 96.08, p < 0.0001] based on the satellite data, confirming that our assignments were appropriate.

Distress calling rate showed a low and non-significant between-year repeatability within individuals (Table 2). However, repeatabilities of pecking and breath rates were of higher magnitude, and were similar between rural and urban populations (Table 2). Distress calling rate and pecking rate were positively correlated within populations (Table 3) but distress calling and pecking rates did not correlate significantly with breath rate (Table 3), suggesting that breath rate was independent from the two other traits. None of the between-population correlations were statistically significant (Table 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Within-individual and between-year repeatability of distress calling, pecking, and breath rates.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlations coefficients at different levels among distress calling, pecking, and breath rates.

Distress calling rate was higher in birds from urban compared to rural habitats (Figure 1, Table 4). There was a non-significant trend toward an interaction between sex and habitat on distress calling rate (Table 4). Though the difference in distress calling rate between urban and rural areas tended to be stronger in females compared to males, urban great tits of both sexes had higher distress calling rates compared to their rural conspecifics. The interaction between locality and habitat could not be tested because models incorporating this fixed effect did not converge.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Between-individual variation (mean with standard error) in distress calling rate along habitats (urban vs. rural) and localities (see Table 4; both sexes combined). Distress calling rate is number of calls during 15 s.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Statistical summary of the analysis of distress calling rate between rural and urban habitats.

In the full model for pecking rate, there was no significant effect of habitat, age or their interaction (habitat: LRT = 2.75, p = 0.10; age LRT = 0.00, p = 0.98; habitat × age: LRT = 0.29, p = 0.59). Both locality and population were significantly related with great tit pecking rate (locality: LRT = 7.24, p = 0.01; population: LRT = 2.41, p < 0.01). In addition, males displayed higher pecking rates than females (sex: LRT = 4.63, p = 0.03). However, we also found a non-significant trend for an interaction between sex and habitat on pecking rate (sex × habitat: LRT = 3.34, p = 0.07). A post-hoc analysis separating the sexes, revealed that there was an effect of habitat on pecking rate in males (LRT = 4.79, p = 0.03, Figure 2, Table 5) but not in females (LRT = 0.01, p = 0.95). The interaction between locality and habitat was non-significant (LRT: 0.34, p = 0.95) in the model for males indicating that the higher pecking rate of urban than rural males was consistent across localities (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Between-individual variation (mean with standard error) in pecking rate along habitats (urban vs. rural) and localities (see Table 5). Pecking rate is number of peckings during 15 s.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Statistical summary of the analysis of pecking rate for male great tits between rural and urban habitats.

Breath rate did not differ between urban and rural habitats when controlling for sex, age, locality, population, and observer effects (Table 6). The interaction between locality and habitat was also non-significant (LRT < 0.001, p = 0.99), indicating that this lack of difference was consistent across localities.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Statistical summary of the analysis of breath rate between rural and urban habitats.

Discussion

The present study reveals consistent differences in distress calling rate between urban and rural populations of great tits, across multiple European localities, with urban birds having, in general, a higher distress calling rate than their rural conspecifics. In line with this finding, pecking rate showed similar tendencies, but only for males. By contrast, we did not find a general difference in breath rate between urban and rural birds.

Distress behaviors displayed during handling have been hypothesized to reflect the coping style of the different individuals in the presence of an acute stressful situation (Brommer and Kluen, 2012; Class et al., 2014; Kluen et al., 2014; Class and Brommer, 2016). Among the three measured distress behaviors, only distress calling rate showed a clear habitat effect. The higher distress calling rate of urban great tits compared to their rural counterparts is consistent with findings of previous studies that compared bird species (Møller and Ibáñez-Álamo, 2012). Given that high distress calling rates have been related to a proactive coping style (Guillette and Sturdy, 2011; Pascual and Senar, 2014), our results support the view that urban birds are more proactive than rural birds.

Because distress calling and pecking rates were correlated, we expected similar habitat differences for the two traits. Indeed, pecking rate was related to urbanization, but only for males. This sexual difference has previously been observed (Markó et al., 2013), and is congruent with the general observation that males are more aggressive than females. Under this general view, the higher aggression of urban male great tits could be boosted by intense intra-specific competition for a limited number of territories and a potentially higher breeding density in urban areas (Chamberlain et al., 2009). In line with this finding, elevated levels of male territorial aggression have been found in urban song sparrows Melospiza melodia, compared to rural male conspecifics. However, this pattern is not necessarily supported in other species (Newman et al., 2006; Bókony et al., 2010; Atwell et al., 2014; Hasegawa et al., 2014). Furthermore, bird species living in urban landscapes have been shown to peck in lower frequencies in response to handling than those inhabiting rural habitats (Møller and Ibáñez-Álamo, 2012). Yet, Møller and Ibáñez-Álamo (2012) did not discriminate between males and females, which hampers a straightforward comparison with our findings. Taken together, these inconsistent results do not allow assessing whether pecking rate is more related to aggression and the latter trait that differ between habitat types, or if it is per se a distress behavior that is subjected to selection. In any case, both aggression and behavior upon handling have been regarded as typical indicators of the same proactive coping style (Koolhaas et al., 1999; Carere et al., 2010; Class et al., 2014; Kluen et al., 2014; Dubuc-Messier et al., 2017). Accordingly, our data on great tit pecking rate suggests that urban males are more proactive than rural males. The interaction between habitat and locality was not significant, indicating that the pecking rate differences among habitats was consistent across localities.

In songbirds, breath rate has been proposed as an indicator of acute stress resulting from a predatory attack, and more generally, as an indicator of stress sensitivity and coping style (Carere and van Oers, 2004; van Oers and Carere, 2007; Torné-Noguera et al., 2014). Indeed, some studies found higher breath rates in urban great tits compared to their rural counterparts (Torné-Noguera et al., 2014; Charmantier et al., 2017). Our study suggests that this finding does not represent a general pattern. In other species, such as dark-eyed juncos Junco hyemalis, urban birds even had a lower breath rate than their rural conspecifics (Abolins-Abols et al., 2016). Our study further reveals that breath rate did not correlate with any of the other behaviors in great tits. This is surprising, since a positive association between pecking rate and breath rate has previously been documented in the closely related blue tit Cyanistes caeruleus (Brommer and Kluen, 2012). However, breath rate has been shown to be affected by seasonal, annual and other ecological factors, hence it may be especially prone to micro-climatic and habitat variation which may mask links to both general urbanization and other behaviors (Torné-Noguera et al., 2014; Charmantier et al., 2017).

The relatively low repeatability of distress calling, potentially due to the low number of repeats per individual (Dingemanse and Dochtermann, 2013; Markó et al., 2013), points to a conservative measure of differences among habitats. Despite the low repeatability, there existed strong differences in distress calling among rural and urban individuals, what points to important differences among habitats, suggesting that distress calling may have evolved in response to urban-induced selection pressures. The between-year repeatability estimates for pecking and breath rate that we recorded, were generally higher than repeatabilities for other behaviors reported in literature (Bell et al., 2009). Given that male pecking rate was different across the habitats and that the repeatability was high, this particular trait seems part of the “urban behavioral phenotype.” This finding is also supported by a recent study indicating a non-random distribution of behavioral types along urban gradients, but little behavioral plasticity in response to within-individual variation to urbanization (Sprau and Dingemanse, 2017).

The combined datasets from different European projects across the distribution of great tits enabled us to conduct a cross-population comparison over a large geographic scale. It entailed study-specific differences in capture time, trapping methods, and observer effects. Nevertheless, we were able to detect consistent differences between habitats, implying that effects reported should be conservative. Altogether, our data suggest that urban great tits, especially males, have a more proactive coping strategy when dealing with stressful conditions than rural birds. This finding is in line with studies showing that urban great tits are more explorative and less neophobic than their rural counterparts (Tryjanowski et al., 2016; Charmantier et al., 2017; Riyahi et al., 2017), and display shorter flight initiation distances (Møller, 2008, 2012), which represent other aspects of the proactive coping strategy. Findings on the great tit are also in line with data from other bird species, for which urban populations have also been found to display proactive behaviors (Evans et al., 2010; Carrete and Tella, 2017), but these studies compared few populations (Miranda et al., 2013). Our study included 14 populations from a large geographic area and revealed consistent results across localities, allowing to generalize the impact of urbanization on great tit coping styles.

This proactive strategy of urban great tits could be advantageous in the urban environment. Proactive individuals, for instance, more rapidly discover and use new food resources (van Overveld and Matthysen, 2010), and are less afraid of novel objects or environments than reactive birds (Tryjanowski et al., 2016; Charmantier et al., 2017; Riyahi et al., 2017), which could help urban dwellers to take profit of the new feeding opportunities that a city can provide (e.g., Fisher and Hinde, 1949). Our study therefore implies that urban birds may adapt to human disturbance by a process of local adaptation, as suggested previously (Partecke et al., 2006; Sol et al., 2013). Future research should investigate whether the differences in distress behaviors reported here have an underlying genetic base, hence are due to local adaption caused by divergent natural selection, or originate from phenotypic plasticity.

Ethics Statement

Behavioral data was recorded during ringing operations. Capture and ringing was performed under individual ringing permits delivered by the ringing office within each country.

Author Contributions

The study was conceived by JCS, LG, VT, CB, GM-R, and CI. The data were collected by VT, CB, GM-R, PaS, JR, PhS, ND, AC, VD, and HN. The statistical analyses were conducted by JCS and LG. The manuscript was written by JCS and LG with input from all other co-authors. Contributed funding and materials: JCS, VT, CB, GM-R, PhS, ND, AC, CI.

Funding

This work was supported by funds from the Ministry of Economy and Competitivity, Spanish Research Council (CGL-2016-79568-C3-3-P; to JCS. CGL2015-70639-P; to LG. CGL2014-55969-P; to GM-R), from the National Research, Development and Innovation Office (Hungary) (K-115970; to LG), from the Estonian Ministry of Education and Research (institutional research funding IUT no. 34-8; to VT), from the European Research Council (ERC-2013-StG-337365-SHE; to AC), from the OSU-OREME (to AC), from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) (SP 1450/3-1; to PhS), from the European Union a Marie Curie Re-Integration grant (CIG 322217; to CI) and from the Swedish Research council (C0361301; to CI).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the Institute of Parks and Gardens of Barcelona (Parcs i Jardins), the mairie de Montpellier and the cities of Paris and Rueil-Malmaison, for allowing us to sample birds in their city parks. We also thank the National Forest Office (ONF) for allowing us to survey tit populations breeding in state-owned forest, the late Leopoldo Gil, who always supported our work, for allowing us to sample birds in Can Catà forest area, and Hermanitas de la Asunción for allowing us to sample birds in the Sarria area. We also thank Lluïsa Arroyo, Arnaud Grégoire, Marcel Lambrechts, Mar Comas, Nuria Mora, Abelardo Requena, Alexia Mouchet, Maria Moiron, Robin Abbey-Lee, Natalia Perez Ruiz, Mattia Bessone, Rui Machado, Lucila Fernandez, Alexander Hutfluss, Diogo Barros, Gülperi Stenhouse, Josefa Bleu, Simon Agostini, Beatriz Decencière, Erika Beaugeard, Jeanne Dupuy, Eva Du Tien Hat, Laura Grosvalet, Lucie Mathieu, Juliette Rabdeau, Marine Ramirez, Baptiste Vancostenoble, Antonin Waterschoot, Audrey Fournier, Estelle Garrouste, Marilou Keck, Alice Le Priol, Cécilia Mondet, Amélie Pinel, and Quentin Pionneaud for help during field work. Anders P. Møller also provided useful advice during the early stages of the work.

References

Abolins-Abols, M., Hope, S. F., and Ketterson, E. D. (2016). Effect of acute stressor on reproductive behavior differs between urban and rural birds. Ecol. Evol. 6, 6546–6555. doi: 10.1002/ece3.2347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alberti, M., Marzluff, J., and Hunt, V. M. (2017). Urban driven phenotypic changes: empirical observations and theoretical implications for eco-evolutionary feedback. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 372:20160029. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersen, M. (2012). What is Maintaining Variation in Personalities in Wild Populations of Great Tits and Blue Tits? Master thesis, University of Oslo, Oslo.

Andersson, M. N., Wang, H. L., Nord, A., Salmon, P., and Isaksson, C. (2015). Composition of physiologically important fatty acids in great tits differs between urban and rural populations on a seasonal basis. Front. Ecol. Evol. 3:93. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2015.00093

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atwell, J. W., Cardoso, G. C., Whittaker, D. J., Price, T. D., and Ketterson, E. D. (2014). Hormonal, behavioral, and life-history traits exhibit correlated shifts in relation to population establishment in a novel environment. Am. Nat. 184, E147–E160. doi: 10.1086/678398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badyaev, A. V. (2005). Stress-induced variation in evolution: from behavioural plasticity to genetic assimilation. Proc. Biol. Sci. 272, 877–886. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.3045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailly, J., Scheifler, R., Berthe, S., Clément-Demange, V. A., Leblond, M., Pasteur, B., et al. (2015). From eggs to fledging: negative impact of urban habitat on reproduction in two tit species. J. Ornithol. 157, 377–392. doi: 10.1007/s10336-015-1293-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Maechler, M., and Bolker, B. (2011). lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using S4 Classes. Available online at: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4

Bell, A. M., Hankison, S. J., and Laskowski, K. L. (2009). The repeatability of behaviour. A meta-analysis. Anim. Behav. 77, 771–783. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berressem, K. G., Berressem, H., and Schmidt, K. H. (1983). Vergleich der Brutbiologie von Höhlenbrütern in innerstädtischen und stadtfernen Biotopen. J. Ornithol. 124, 431–445. doi: 10.1007/BF01640362

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biard, C., Brischoux, F., Meillère, A., Michaud, B., Nivière, M., Ruault, S., et al. (2017). Growing in cities: an urban penalty for wild birds? A study of phenotypic differences between urban and rural great tit chicks (Parus major). Front. Ecol. Evol. 5:79. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2017.00079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Björklund, M., Ruiz, I., and Senar, J. C. (2010). Genetic differentiation in the urban habitat: the great tits (Parus major) of the parks of Barcelona city. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 99, 9–19. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2009.01335.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bókony, V., Kulcsár, A., and Liker, A. (2010). Does urbanization select for weak competitors in house sparrows? Oikos 119, 437–444. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.17848.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brommer, J. E., and Kluen, E. (2012). Exploring the genetics of nestling personality traits in a wild passerine bird: testing the phenotypic gambit. Ecol. Evol. 2, 3032–3044. doi: 10.1002/ece3.412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchanan, K. L., and Partecke, J. (2012). “The endocrine system: can homeostasis be maintained in a changing world?,” in Behavioural Responses to a Changing World: Mechanisms and Consequences, eds U. Candolin and B. B. M. Wong (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 32–45.

Google Scholar

Carere, C., Caramaschi, D., and Fawcett, T. W. (2010). Covariation between personalities and individual differences in coping with stress. Converging evidence and hypotheses. Curr. Zool. 56, 728–740.

Google Scholar

Carere, C., and van Oers, K. (2004). Shy and bold great tits (Parus major): body temperature and breath rate in response to handling stress. Physiol. Behav. 82, 905–912. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(04)00312-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrete, M., and Tella, J. L. (2017). Behavioral correlations associated with fear of humans differ between rural and urban burrowing owls. Front. Ecol. Evol. 5:54. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2017.00054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carvalho, C. F., Leitao, A. V., Funghi, C., Batalha, H. R., Reis, S., Mota, P. G., et al. (2013). Personality traits are related to ecology across a biological invasion. Behav. Ecol. 24, 1081–1091. doi: 10.1093/beheco/art034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamberlain, D. E., Cannon, A. R., Toms, M. P., Leech, D. I., Hatchwell, B. J., and Gaston, K. J. (2009). Avian productivity in urban landscapes: a review and meta-analysis. Ibis 151, 1–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.2008.00899.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charmantier, A., Demeyrier, V., Lambrechts, M., Perret, S., and Grégoire, A. (2017). Urbanization is associated with divergence in pace-of-life in great tits. Front. Ecol. Evol. 5:53. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2017.00053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Class, B., and Brommer, J. E. (2016). Senescence of personality in a wild bird. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70, 733–744. doi: 10.1007/s00265-016-2096-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Class, B., Kluen, E., and Brommer, J. E. (2014). Evolutionary quantitative genetics of behavioral responses to handling in a wild passerine. Ecol. Evol. 4, 427–440. doi: 10.1002/ece3.945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coppens, C. M., Boer, S. F., and de Koolhaas, J. M. (2010). Coping styles and behavioural flexibility. Towards underlying mechanisms. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 365, 4021–4028. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Dingemanse, N. J., and Dochtermann, N. A. (2013). Quantifying individual variation in behaviour: mixed-effect modelling approaches. J. Anim. Ecol. 82, 39–54. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dochtermann, N. A. (2010). Behavioral syndromes: carryover effects, false discovery rates, and a priori hypotheses. Behav. Ecol. 21, 437–439. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arq021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donihue, C. M., and Lambert, M. R. (2014). Adaptive evolution in urban ecosystems. Ambio 44, 194–203. doi: 10.1007/s13280-014-0547-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubuc-Messier, G., Réale, D., Perret, P., and Charmantier, A. (2017). Environmental heterogeneity and population differences in blue tits personality traits. Behav. Ecol. 28, 448–445. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arw148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, J., Boudreau, K., and Hyman, J. (2010). Behavioural syndromes in urban and rural populations of song sparrows. Ethology 116, 588–595. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01771.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, K. L., Gaston, K. J., Sharp, S. P., McGowan, A., and Hatchwell, B. J. (2009). The effect of urbanisation on avian morphology and latitudinal gradients in body size. Oikos 118, 251–259. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2008.17092.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, J., and Hinde, R. A. (1949). The opening of milk bottles by birds. Br. Birds 42, 347–357.

Fokidis, H. B., Orchinik, M., and Deviche, P. (2009). Corticosterone and corticosteroid binding globulin in birds: relation to urbanization in a desert city. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 160, 259–270. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2008.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

French, S. S., Fokidis, H. B., and Moore, M. C. (2008). Variation in stress and innate immunity in the tree lizard (Urosaurus ornatus) across an urban-rural gradient. J. Comp. Physiol. B Biochem. Syst. Environ. Physiol. 178, 997–1005. doi: 10.1007/s00360-008-0290-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fucikova, E., Drent, P. J., Smits, N., and van Oers, K. (2009). Handling stress as a measurement of personality in great tit nestlings (Parus major). Ethology 115, 366–374. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01618.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaston, K. J. (2010). Ecological Reviews: Urban Ecology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Gelman, A., and Rubin, D. B. (1992). Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Stat. Sci. 7, 457–472. doi: 10.1214/ss/1177011136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gil, D., and Brumm, H. (eds.). (2014). Avian Urban Ecology. Behavioural and Physiological Adaptations, 1st Edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Guillette, L. M., and Sturdy, C. B. (2011). Individual differences and repeatability in vocal production: stress-induced calling exposes a songbird's personality. Naturwissenschaften 98, 977–981. doi: 10.1007/s00114-011-0842-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadfield, J. D. (2010). MCMC methods for multi-response generalized linear mixed models: the MCMCglmm R package. J. Stat. Softw. 33, 1–20. doi: 10.18637/jss.v033.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasegawa, M., Ligon, R. A., Giraudeau, M., Watanabe, M., and McGraw, K. J. (2014). Urban and colorful male house finches are less aggressive. Behav. Ecol. 25, 641–649. doi: 10.1093/beheco/aru034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedblom, M., and Soderstrom, B. (2012). Effects of urban matrix on reproductive performance of Great Tit (Parus major) in urban woodlands. Urban Ecosyst. 15, 167–180. doi: 10.1007/s11252-011-0204-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hendry, A. P., Gotanda, K. M., and Svensson, E. I. (2017). Human influences on evolution, and the ecological and societal consequences. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 372, 1712. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horak, P., and Lebreton, J. D. (1998). Survival of adult Great tits Parus major in relation to sex and habitat; a comparision of urban and rural populations. Ibis 140, 205–209. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1998.tb04380.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horak, P., Mand, R., Ots, I., and Leivits, A. (1995). Egg size in the great tit Parus major: individual, habitat and geographic differences. Ornis Fennica 72, 97–114.

Isaksson, C. (2015). Urbanization, oxidative stress and inflammation: a question of evolving, acclimatizing or coping with urban environmental stress. Funct. Ecol. 29, 913–923. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12477

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaksson, C., and Andersson, S. (2007). Carotenoid diet and nestling provisioning in urban and rural great tits Parus major. J. Avian Biol. 38, 564–572. doi: 10.1111/j.0908-8857.2007.04030.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kluen, E., Siitari, H., and Brommer, J. E. (2014). Testing for between individual correlations of personality and physiological traits in a wild bird. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 68, 205–213. doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1635-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, W. D., Stanback, M. T., Hooge, P. N., and Mumme, R. L. (1991). Distress calls in the acorn woodpecker. Condor 93, 637–643. doi: 10.2307/1368195

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koolhaas, J. M., Boer, S. F., de Coppens, C. M., and Buwalda, B. (2010). Neuroendocrinology of coping styles. Towards understanding the biology of individual variation. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 31, 307–321. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2010.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koolhaas, J. M., Korte, S. M., De Boer, S. F., Van Der Vegt, B. J., Van Reenen, C. G., Hopster, H., et al. (1999). Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 925–935. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00026-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laiolo, P., Banda, E., Lemus, J. A., Aguirre, J. I., and Blanco, G. (2009). Behaviour and stress response during capture and handling of the red-billed chough Pyrrhocorax pyrrhocorax (Aves: Corvidae). Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 96, 846–855. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2008.01174.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luniak, M., and Pisarski, B. (1982). Animals in Urban Environment. Wroclaw: Polish Academy Sciences, Inst. Zoology.

Google Scholar

Markó, G., Azcárate, M., Hegyi, G., Herceg, G., Laczi, M., Nagy, G., et al. (2013). Behavioural responses to handling stress in the Great Tit: within-individual consistency and the effect of age, sex and body condition. Ornis Hungarica 21, 12–25. doi: 10.2478/orhu-2013-0012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marzluff, J. M., Donnelly, R. E., and Bowman, R. (eds.). (2001). Avian Ecology and Conservation in An Urbanizing World. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Mateos-González, F., and Senar, J. C. (2012). Melanin-based trait predicts individual exploratory behaviour in siskins, Carduelis spinus. Anim. Behav. 83, 229–232. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda, A. C., Schielzeth, H., Sonntag, T., and Partecke, J. (2013). Urbanization and its effects on personality traits: a result of microevolution or phenotypic plasticity? Glob. Change Biol. 19, 2634–2644. doi: 10.1111/gcb.12258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Møller, A. P. (2008). Flight distance of urban birds, predation, and selection for urban life. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 63, 63–75. doi: 10.1007/s00265-008-0636-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Møller, A. P. (2012). “Reproductive behaviour,” in Behavioural Responses to a Changing World: Mechanisms and Consequences, eds U. Candolin and B. B. M. Wong (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 106–118.

Møller, A. P., and Ibáñez-Álamo, J. D. (2012). Escape behaviour of birds provides evidence of predation being involved in urbanization. Anim. Behav. 84, 341–348. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Møller, A. P., and Nielsen, J. T. (2010). Fear screams and adaptation to avoid imminent death: effects of genetic variation and predation. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 22, 183–202. doi: 10.1080/03949371003707968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Møller, A. P., Tryjanowski, P., Díaz, M., Kwiecinski, Z., Indykiewicz, P., Mitrus, C., et al. (2015). Urban habitats and feeders both contribute to flight initiation distance reduction in birds. Behav. Ecol. 26, 861–865. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arv024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakagawa, S., and Schielzeth, H. (2010). Repeatability for Gaussian and non-Gaussian data: a practical guide for biologists. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 85, 935–956. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00141.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, M. M., Yeh, P. J., and Price, T. D. (2006). Reduced territorial responses in dark-eyed juncos following population establishment in a climatically mild environment. Anim. Behav. 71, 893–899. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niall Daisley, J., Bromundt, V., Möstl, E., and Kotrschal, K. (2005). Enhanced yolk testosterone influences behavioral phenotype independent of sex in Japanese quail chicks Coturnix japonica. Horm. Behav. 47, 185–194. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Partecke, J., Schwabl, I., and Gwinner, E. (2006). Stress and the city: urbanization and its effects on the stress physiology in European Blackbirds. Ecology 87, 1945–1952. doi: 10.1890/0012-9658(2006)87[1945:SATCUA]2.0.CO;2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual, J., and Senar, J. C. (2014). Antipredator behavioural compensation of proactive personality trait in male Eurasian siskins. Anim. Behav. 90, 297–303. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preiszner, B., Papp, S., Pipoly, I., Seress, G., Vincze, E., Liker, A., et al. (2017). Problem-solving performance and reproductive success of great tits in urban and forest habitats. Anim. Cogn. 20, 53–63. doi: 10.1007/s10071-016-1008-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Development Core Team (2017). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Richardson, K. M., Ewen, J. G., Brekke, P., Doerr, L. R., Parker, K. A., and Armstrong, D. P. (2016). Behaviour during handling predicts male natal dispersal distances in an establishing reintroduced hihi (Notiomystis cincta) population. Anim. Conserv. 20, 135–143. doi: 10.1111/acv.12296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riyahi, S., Björklund, M., Mateos-Gonzalez, F., and Senar, J. C. (2017). Personality and urbanization: behavioural traits and DRD4 SNP830 polymorphisms in great tits in Barcelona city. J. Ethol. 35, 101–108. doi: 10.1007/s10164-016-0496-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riyahi, S., Sánchez-Delgado, M., Calafell, F., Monk, D., and Senar, J. C. (2015). Combined epigenetic and intraspecific variation of the DRD4 and SERT genes influence novelty seeking behavior in great tit Parus major. Epigenetics 10, 516–525. doi: 10.1080/15592294.2015.1046027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salaberria, C., and Gil, D. (2010). Increase in song frequency in response to urban noise in the great tit Parus major as shown by data from the Madrid (Spain) city noise map. Ardeola 57, 3–11.

Google Scholar

Salmón, P., Nilsson, J. F., Nord, A., Bensch, S., and Isaksson, C. (2016). Urban environment shortens telomere length in nestling great tits, Parus major. Biol. Lett. 12:20160155. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2016.0155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, K. H., and Einloft-Achenbach, H. (1984). Könnenisolierte meisenpopulationen in städtenihrenbestanderhalten? Vogelwelt 105, 97–105.

Schoech, S. J., Bowman, R., Bridge, E. S., and Boughton, R. K. (2007). Baseline and acute levels of corticosterone in Florida Scrub-Jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens): effects of food supplementation, suburban habitat, and year. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 154, 150–160. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2007.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senar, J. C., Conroy, M. J., Quesada, J., and Mateos-Gonzalez, F. (2014). Selection based on the size of the black tie of the great tit may be reversed in urban habitats. Ecol. Evol. 4, 2625–2632. doi: 10.1002/ece3.999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senar, J. C., Domènech, J., Carrascal, L. M., and Moreno, E. (1997). A funnel trap for the capture of tits. Butll. GCA 14, 17–24.

Google Scholar

Seress, G., Lipovits, Á., Bókony, V., and Czúni, L. (2014). Quantifying the urban gradient: a practical method for broad measurements. Landsc. Urban Plan. 131, 42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2014.07.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slabbekoorn, H., and Peet, M. (2003). Birds sing at a higher pitch in urban noise. Nature 424, 267–268. doi: 10.1038/424267a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sol, D., Lapiedra, O., and González-Lagos, C. (2013). Behavioural adjustments for a life in the city. Anim. Behav. 85, 1101–1112. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.01.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sprau, P., and Dingemanse, N. J. (2017). An approach to distinguish between plasticity and non-random distributions of behavioral types along urban gradients in a wild passerine bird. Front. Ecol. Evol. 5:92. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2017.00092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svensson, L. (1992). Identification Guide to European Passerines. Stockholm: L. Svensson.

Google Scholar

Thorsteinsen, C. (2015). Reproductive Success and Survival during the Breeding Season in Relation to Individual Behaviour in the Great Tit, Parus Major. Oslo: University of Oslo.

Google Scholar

Toledo, A., Andersson, M. N., Wang, H. L., Salmón, P., Watson, H., Burdge, G. C., et al. (2016). Fatty acid profiles of great tit (Parus major) eggs differ between urban and rural habitats, but not between coniferous and deciduous forests. Sci. Nat. 103, 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s00114-016-1381-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torné-Noguera, A., Pagani-Nú-ez, E., and Senar, J. C. (2014). Great Tit (Parus major) breath rate in response to handling stress: urban and forest birds differ. J. Orn. 155, 315–318. doi: 10.1007/s10336-013-1025-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tryjanowski, P., Møller, A. P., Morelli, F., Biadun, W., Brauze, T., Ciach, M., et al. (2016). Urbanization affects neophilia and risk-taking at bird-feeders. Sci. Rep. 6:28575. doi: 10.1038/srep28575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuomainen, U., and Candolin, U. (2012). Behavioural responses to human-induced environmental change. Biol. Rev. 86, 640–657. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00164.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, W. R., Nakamura, T., and Dinetti, M. (2004). Global urbanization and the separation of humans from nature. Bioscience 54, 585–590. doi: 10.1641/0006-3568(2004)054[0585:GUATSO]2.0.CO;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

United Nations (2014). World Urbanization Prospects: The 2014 Revision, Highlights (ST/ESA/SER.A/352). New York, NY: Department of Economic and Social Affairs/Population Division 3.

van den Brink, V., Dolivo, V., Falourd, X., Dreiss, A. N., and Roulin, A. (2012a). Melanic color-dependent antipredator behavior strategies in barn owl nestlings. Behav. Ecol. 23, 473–480. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arr213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Brink, V., Henry, I., Wakamatsu, K., and Roulin, A. (2012b). Melanin-based coloration in juvenile kestrels (Falco tinnunculus) covaries with anti-predatory personality traits. Ethology 118, 673–682. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2012.02057.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Oers, K., and Carere, C. (2007). Long-term effects of repeated handling and bleeding in wild caught Great Tits Parus major. J. Ornithol. 148, 185–190. doi: 10.1007/s10336-007-0200-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Overveld, T., and Matthysen, E. (2010). Personality predicts spatial responses to food manipulations in free-ranging great tits (Parus major). Biol. Lett. 6, 187–190. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaugoyeau, M., Adriaensen, F., Artemyev, A., Banbura, J., Barba, E., Biard, C., et al. (2016). Interspecific variation in the relationship between clutch size, laying date and intensity of urbanization in four species of hole-nesting birds. Ecol. Evol. 6, 5907–5920. doi: 10.1002/ece3.2335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vincze, E., Seress, G., Lagisz, M., Nakagawa, S., Dingemanse, N. J., and Sprau, P. (2017). Does urbanization affect predation of bird nests? A meta-analysis. Front. Ecol. Evol. 5:29. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2017.00029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, S., Lei, F., Liu, S., Li, D., Chen, C., and Wang, P. (2011). Variation in baseline corticosterone levels of Tree Sparrow (Passer montanus) populations along an urban gradient in Beijing, China. J. Orn. 152, 801–806. doi: 10.1007/s10336-011-0663-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: urbanization, distress behavior, handling, coping strategy, distress calling, pecking rate, local adaptation, great tit

Citation: Senar JC, Garamszegi LZ, Tilgar V, Biard C, Moreno-Rueda G, Salmón P, Rivas JM, Sprau P, Dingemanse NJ, Charmantier A, Demeyrier V, Navalpotro H and Isaksson C (2017) Urban Great Tits (Parus major) Show Higher Distress Calling and Pecking Rates than Rural Birds across Europe. Front. Ecol. Evol. 5:163. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2017.00163

Received: 05 June 2017; Accepted: 29 November 2017;
Published: 18 December 2017.

Edited by:

Patrick S. Fitze, Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales (CSIC), Spain

Reviewed by:

Deseada Parejo, Universidad de Extremadura, Spain
Gonçalo C. Cardoso, Universidade do Porto, Portugal

Copyright © 2017 Senar, Garamszegi, Tilgar, Biard, Moreno-Rueda, Salmón, Rivas, Sprau, Dingemanse, Charmantier, Demeyrier, Navalpotro and Isaksson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Juan Carlos Senar, jcsenar@bcn.cat

Download