Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 31 May 2018
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychological and Neural Mechanisms of Pain and Empathy View all 19 articles

Magnitude and Temporal Variability of Inter-stimulus EEG Modulate the Linear Relationship Between Laser-Evoked Potentials and Fast-Pain Perception

\r\nLinling Li,Linling Li1,2Gan Huang,Gan Huang1,2Qianqian Lin,Qianqian Lin1,2Jia Liu,Jia Liu1,2Shengli ZhangShengli Zhang3Zhiguo Zhang,,*Zhiguo Zhang1,2,4*
  • 1School of Biomedical Engineering, Health Science Center, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
  • 2Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Biomedical Measurements and Ultrasound Imaging, Shenzhen, China
  • 3Department of Communication Engineering, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
  • 4Experimental Center of Fundamental Teaching, Sun Yat-Sen University, Zhuhai, China

The level of pain perception is correlated with the magnitude of pain-evoked brain responses, such as laser-evoked potentials (LEP), across trials. The positive LEP-pain relationship lays the foundation for pain prediction based on single-trial LEP, but cross-individual pain prediction does not have a good performance because the LEP-pain relationship exhibits substantial cross-individual difference. In this study, we aim to explain the cross-individual difference in the LEP-pain relationship using inter-stimulus EEG (isEEG) features. The isEEG features (root mean square as magnitude and mean square successive difference as temporal variability) were estimated from isEEG data (at full band and five frequency bands) recorded between painful stimuli. A linear model was fitted to investigate the relationship between pain ratings and LEP response for fast-pain trials on a trial-by-trial basis. Then the correlation between isEEG features and the parameters of LEP-pain model (slope and intercept) was evaluated. We found that the magnitude and temporal variability of isEEG could modulate the parameters of an individual's linear LEP-pain model for fast-pain trials. Based on this, we further developed a new individualized fast-pain prediction scheme, which only used training individuals with similar isEEG features as the test individual to train the fast-pain prediction model, and obtained improved accuracy in cross-individual fast-pain prediction. The findings could help elucidate the neural mechanism of cross-individual difference in pain experience and the proposed fast-pain prediction scheme could be potentially used as a practical and feasible pain prediction method in clinical practice.

Introduction

Pain is a subjective perception and is primarily assessed by means of self-report. Because the capacity to effectively report pain is limited in vulnerable population groups (e.g., babies and people with cognitive or communicative impairments), assessment of pain levels based on physiological signals has attracted a growing interest (Wager et al., 2013). For example, several pain prediction models based on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have been developed to assess pain in healthy persons and patients with chronic pain (Wager et al., 2013; López-Solà et al., 2017). As compared to fMRI, electroencephalography (EEG) could capture a wealth of pain-related brain activities in a cheap and easy-to-use manner, so EEG-based pain assessment is a promising technique in clinical settings. The most commonly used pain-related EEG activities in research are laser-evoked potentials (LEP), and the strong relationship between the LEP amplitudes and the subjective pain intensity on a trial-by-trial basis has been well characterized (Garcíalarrea et al., 1997; Iannetti et al., 2005; Hu et al., 2014). Based on the singe-trial LEP-pain relationship, our previous study has been able to predict subjective pain ratings from single-trial LEP features and achieved an accuracy greater than 80% (Huang et al., 2013a). The relationship between EEG activities and pain rating in other pain paradigms has also been identified and been used to develop EEG-based pain prediction models. For example, based on the spectral characteristics of EEG before drug treatment, one study classified responders and non-responders and presented the application of EEG-based pain models in the prediction of analgesic effect (Gram et al., 2015).

An EEG-based pain prediction model can be achieved at within-individual level (the model is trained on and applied to the same individual) or at cross-individual level (the model is trained on a group of individuals but applied to different individuals) (Huang et al., 2013b). Cross-individual pain prediction is more desired in clinical practice but it normally has a lower accuracy than within-individual prediction (Huang et al., 2013b), because the relationship between brain activity and pain ratings could vary largely among individuals. Actually, each individual has a unique prediction model that links his/her neural activities and pain perception. The substantial cross-individual variability in pain experience has been well studied, and it could be attributed to genetic constitution, socio-cultural variables, and cognitive states (Coghill et al., 2003; Tracey and Mantyh, 2007).

Some studies have also explored neural correlates of cross-individual variability in pain experience. The modulating effect of EEG on perceived pain intensity and pain-evoked neural responses (such as LEP) has been well documented in literature (Babiloni et al., 2008; Zhang and Ding, 2010; Anderson and Ding, 2011; Lange et al., 2012) and studied in our previous work (Bai et al., 2016; Tu et al., 2016). For example, an individual's pain-evoked LEP responses are significantly correlated with his/her spontaneous EEG in terms of magnitude (Bai et al., 2016), and the magnitudes of pre-stimulus EEG alpha and gamma oscillations modulate the forthcoming pain perception and LEP amplitude (Tu et al., 2016). Besides the magnitude of EEG, the temporal variability of EEG is also found to play a key role in effective functioning of various sensory systems, including visual (Treisman, 1964), auditory (Galin, 1964) and somatosensory (Zotterman, 1953). But, it remains unknown whether temporal variability of EEG is related to pain perception. As for fMRI studies, it has been shown that temporal variability of fMRI Blood Oxygenation Level Dependent (BOLD) signals is correlated with cross-individual variability in pain perception (Rogachov et al., 2016). Actually, investigating the functions and mechanisms of temporal signal variability, or moment-to-moment variability, of neural activities has gradually gained popularity. Temporal variability of neural signals can be observed at every level of the nervous system (Faisal et al., 2008), and it is not merely noise but functionally meaningful. An increasing number of work has confirmed the role of temporal brain signal variability as an individual differences measure across cohorts and across tasks (Garrett et al., 2013b). Based on above-mentioned studies, we can conclude that the mean and temporal variability of EEG and fMRI in the resting-state could be correlated with subjective pain ratings as well as pain-related neural responses.

In this study, we hypothesize that the parameters of a single-trial LEP-based pain prediction model are correlated with features of inter-stimulus EEG (isEEG). We are interested in isEEG correlates because isEEG is readily available during LEP recording and it is not necessary to do extra experiments to collect resting-state EEG or potential correlates of other modalities, such as genomic data and socio-culture variables. Also, previous studies have shown that the features extracted from inter-stimulus brain signals and spontaneous brain signals shared some similarities (Fair et al., 2007; Ganger et al., 2015). We aim to explore isEEG features (in terms of magnitude and temporal variability of isEEG rhythms) that are correlated with an individual's LEP-pain model parameters. More precisely, for each individual we train a simple linear prediction model to link the intensities of perceived pain and corresponding LEP magnitudes and then correlate the model parameters (slope and intercept) with isEEG features. Toward this goal, we collected isEEG/LEP data from 34 healthy subjects in a laser-evoked pain experiment. Then, we extracted magnitude and temporal variability of isEEG and correlated them with parameters of LEP-pain models across individuals.

Further, we aim to utilize the isEEG correlates of the cross-individual variability in the LEP-pain model to guide the design of individualized LEP-pain prediction models. To this end, we proposed a scheme to individualize the parameters of the LEP-pain prediction model based on isEEG correlates to improve the accuracy of cross-individual pain prediction. More precisely, an individual's pain prediction model will be trained only from those individuals with similar isEEG correlates, and our results show that such a scheme can achieve higher accuracy in pain prediction.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Thirty-four right-handed healthy undergraduates (21.6 ± 1.7 years; 17M/17F) were enrolled and the inclusion criteria include: (1) non-smokers; (2) no history of chronic pain; (3) no acute pain symptom up to 4 weeks before the experiment; (4) no currently use of any medication. Before the experiment, all participants were familiarized with the details of procedure and gave written informed consent. The study was approved by the local ethics committee. Note that this dataset has been used in previously published articles (Hu et al., 2014; Bai et al., 2016).

Experimental Design

Nociceptive stimulation was produced by an infrared neodymium yttrium aluminum perovskite (Nd:YAP) laser with a wavelength of 1.34 μm (Electronical Engineering, Italy). With this wavelength, laser pulse could activate nociceptive terminals in superficial skin (Iannetti et al., 2006). The stimulation site was located at the medial side of the dorsum of left hand, between the first and third metacarpus. A helium-neon laser pointed to the region to be stimulated and the laser beam was transmitted via an optic fiber with a preset diameter of 38 mm2. The duration of laser stimulation was fixed at 4 ms and the target region was shifted by more than 1 cm in a random direction to avoid sensitization or nociceptor fatigue.

For each trial, the intensity of perceived pain was evaluated using a numerical rating scale (NRS) from 0 (no pain) to 10 (pain as bad as possible). In this experiment, participants were asked to report the intensity of pain perception with 4 as the pinprick pain (typical sensation for Aδ fiber-related fast-pain) threshold (Magerl et al., 1999; Basbaum and Bushnell, 2009). Only fast-pain trials will be included in following data analysis since the N2-P2 amplitude increased significantly only when the subjective pain intensity was larger than 4 (Bai et al., 2016). Prior to EEG experiment, the tolerable highest energy of the laser stimulation was determined for each participant by increasing the energy in steps of 0.25 J, until an NRS rating of 8 was reported. For each participant, 12–15 different levels of laser energies (from 1 to the tolerable highest energy in the range of 3.75–4.5 J, in step of 0.25 J) were adopted and 10 laser pulses at each energy level were delivered, resulting in a total of 120–150 pulses. The number of laser energy levels was different between subjects, because of the fixed lower limit (1 J) and the variable upper limit (3.75–4.5 J). The order of energy levels was pseudorandomized and the inter-stimulus interval varied randomly between 10 and 15 s (rectangular distribution).

EEG Data Acquisition and Preprocessing

EEG experiment was carried out in a silent and temperature-controlled room. Participants were seated in a comfortable chair and wore protective goggles. The EEG data collection was performed with a 64-channel EEG cap (Brain Products GmbH, Munich, Germany). The sampling rate was 1,000 Hz and the passband was 0.01–100 Hz. The electrode impedances were kept lower than 10 kΩ. Electrooculographic (EOG) signals were simultaneously recorded using surface electrodes to monitor ocular movements and eye blinks.

EEG data were analyzed using EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) and in-house MATLAB scripts (MathWorks). Continuous EEG data were band-pass filtered from 1 to 30 Hz using FIR filters. EEG data were further corrected using independent component analysis (ICA) algorithm (Makeig et al., 1997; Delorme and Makeig, 2004; Onton et al., 2006). ICA components that were considered as purely or predominantly driven by artifacts (such as ocular artifacts, myogenic artifacts) were discarded based on visual inspection of power spectrum, time course and topography. The remaining components were re-referenced to nose.

Feature Extraction

The N2-P2 complex is the largest deflection in LEP, which is a negative-positive vertex potential with maximal scalp distribution over the central region (Iannetti et al., 2008). LEP was quantified by the N2-P2 amplitude (peak-to-peak), which was calculated as the absolute difference between the N2 and P2 peak amplitudes measured from the most negative and positive deflections between 150 and 500 ms after stimulus onset.

The isEEG features (magnitude and temporal variability) were estimated from spontaneous EEG data recorded between adjacent laser stimuli. As shown in Figure 1, in the whole isEEG/LEP recording, we extracted a number of isEEG epochs from 2 s after each stimulus to the onset time of the next stimulus. Because the inter-stimulus interval varied randomly between 10 and 15 s, these isEEG epochs have a length ranging from 8 to 13 s. To avoid the influence of different data lengths on the estimation of isEEG features, these isEEG epochs were further segmented into isEEG epochs of 2 s so that each isEEG epoch could have 4–6 epochs. Next, we used random sampling with 100 repeated times to approximate the distribution of isEEG features. At each random sampling, we randomly selected one 2 s-epoch from each isEEG trial, and calculated its isEEG feature (magnitude or temporal variability), and then averaged the isEEG feature across epochs. After 100 times of random resampling, a distribution of isEEG features was obtained and its 50% percentile was used in subsequent data analysis procedures.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of extraction of isEEG epochs in the pain experiments. These 2s isEEG epochs were extracted from inter-stimulus EEG activity between laser stimuli.

The magnitude of each isEEG epoch is quantified by root mean square (RMS):

RMS=1Kk=1KSk2    (1)

where sk is the k-th sample point of one isEEG epoch, and K = 2000 is the total number of time points of one isEEG epoch.

Temporal variability can be calculated in many forms, such as variance (He, 2011), standard deviation (SD) (Garrett et al., 2013a), mean square successive difference (MSSD) (Samanez-Larkin et al., 2010; Li et al., 2017). Here in this study, the temporal variability of isEEG was quantified by the normalized mean squared successive difference (nMSSD) (Neumann et al., 1941). Mean squared successive difference (MSSD) is a popularly used metric to characterize the temporal variability of physiological signals such as heart rate and fMRI BOLD signals (Berntson et al., 2005; Samanez-Larkin et al., 2010). Compared with other measures of temporal variability (such as SD), MSSD is not affected by low frequency drift and thus is more robust and reliable (Neumann et al., 1941; Li et al., 2017). Furthermore, because of the inherent relationship between signal strength and signal variability (i.e., the temporal variability calculated as MSSD or SD is positively correlated with the magnitude), MSSD should be normalized by RMS to disassociate the influence of signal strength (i.e., the magnitude) from signal variability. Hence, the normalized MSSD (nMSSD) was calculated as MSSD divided by the square of RMS:

nMSSD=[1K-1k=1K-1(Sk+1-Sk)2]/RMS2    (2)

where sk and sk+1 are the k-th and (k + 1)-th sample points one isEEG epoch, and K = 2000 is the total number of time points of one isEEG epoch.

The magnitude and temporal variability of isEEG (as quantified by RMS and nMSSD) were calculated for full-band isEEG waveforms as well as for band-limited isEEG waveforms at five frequency bands: delta (1–3 Hz), theta (4–7 Hz), alpha-1 (8–10 Hz), alpha-2 (11–13 Hz) and beta (14–30 Hz). The isEEG epochs were band-pass filtered with minimum-phase causal FIR filters to obtain waveforms at these five frequency bands.

Relationship Between isEEG Features and LEP-Pain Model Parameters

A linear regression model was fitted to investigate the single-trial relationship between pain ratings and LEP response for fast-pain trials. Note that, here we only consider fast-pain trials (NRS>4) because our previous study (Bai et al., 2016) has shown that significant correlation has only been observed between pain ratings and LEP responses of fast-pain trials. Actually, the magnitude of low-pain LEPs has no difference with that of isEEG.

For the i-th trial, the relationship between perceived pain rating and corresponding LEP magnitude could be described with a simple linear regression model as:

yi=axi+b    (3)

where yi is the pain rating, xi is the LEP magnitude, a and b are respectively the slope and intercept of the linear regression model. Therefore, for each participant, its individual LEP-pain model is characterized by two parameters: the slope a and the intercept b.

For a better interpretation of the model parameters, the linear model of (3) can be re-written as another equivalent linear model:

xi=(1a)yi-ba=cyi+d    (4)

where c = 1/a and d = –b/a are respectively the slope and intercept of the linear model (4). It can be seen clearly that, the slopes of two linear models (3) and (4) are reciprocals, and d in (4) is the “x-intercept” (where the linear line crosses the x-axis or the value of x when y = 0) in (3).

Subsequently, the correlation between isEEG features (RMS and nMSSD of full-band isEEG waveforms and isEEG at five frequency bands) and the parameters of LEP-pain model (4) was evaluated in order to identify isEEG correlates of the LEP-pain model parameters. Bonferroni correction is used to address the multiple comparison problem.

Individualized LEP-Pain Prediction

Based on the identified isEEG correlates of the linear LEP-pain prediction model, we further develop a scheme for individualized fast-pain prediction. The basic idea is: an individual's pain rating will be predicted only using models from those individuals with similar distributions of model-related isEEG correlates. These isEEG correlates include magnitude and temporal variability of full-band isEEG and band-limited isEEG at each frequency band, and they were identified by the method in section of feature extraction. The scheme is detailed as follows.

(1) A linear LEP-pain prediction model is trained within each individual, resulting in N prediction models.

(2) Taking m-th individual as the test individual, his/her pain ratings can be predicted using each of the prediction models of other N – 1 training individuals. That is, the i-th LEP trial of the m-th individual has N – 1 predicted NRS values, denoted as Rm,i(n), n = 1, 2, …, N and nm.

(3) The difference (measured as the Euclid distance) of isEEG correlates, diffn, is calculated between the test individual and the n-th training individual. A weight wn is then calculated from diffn and assigned to the predicted NRS values of the m-th individual from the n-th training individual. The weight is calculated as follows: (i) if diffn is larger than the mean value across all N – 1 individuals, wn is set to 0; (ii) otherwise, the weight is wn = max(diff)–diffn, where max(diff) is the maximum value of all N – 1 diffn values.

(4) Finally, the pain rating of the i-th LEP trial of the m-th individual is calculated as the weighted average of Rm,i(n):

Rm,i=1wn=1nmNwnRm,i(n),withw=n=1nmNwn    (5)

It can be seen from the proposed scheme that only individuals with similar values of isEEG correlates (i.e., individuals with similar LEP-pain relationship) are used to predict the pain ratings of the test individual, and the weight is inversely proportional to the distance of isEEG correlates between the test individual and the training individual.

Leave-one-individual-out cross validation was performed for the proposed individualized pain prediction scheme as well as for the conventional cross-individual pain prediction. To evaluate the prediction performance, Mean Absolute Error (MAE) was calculated as follows,

MAE=1Ti=1T|Ri-R^l|    (6)

where T is the number of trials, Ri and R^l are the true and predicted pain rating for the i-th trial. The MAE values obtained from the proposed individualized pain prediction scheme and from the conventional cross-individual pain prediction model were compared using two-sample paired t-test.

Results

Relationship Between LEP Responses and Pain Ratings

Two participants were discarded from the further analysis because of he/she only had fast-pain trials with NRS < 7. Figure 2 shows the group-averaged waveforms of the LEP responses elicited at Cz with three different NRS levels (4<NRS≤6, 6<NRS≤8, 8<NRS≤10), and the scalp topographies at the peak latencies of the N2 and P2 waves. When only fast-pain trials were included, linear correlation was observed between pain perception and corresponding brain responses (as shown in Figure 3).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Group averages and scalp topographies of LEP responses at different NRS levels. LEP waveforms were recorded from the vertex (Cz-nose). The group mean value for N2 peak is −7.58 ± 8.88 μV, −19.06 ± 12.72 μV and −30.13 ± 18.84 μV for three levels respectively. The group mean value for P2 peak is 7.14 ± 8.20 μV, 19.38 ± 11.98 μV and 31.34 ± 12.70 μV for three levels respectively. The scalp topographies of N2 and P2 waves are displayed at their peak latencies.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Relationship between pain intensity and N2-P2 amplitude at Cz. Colored dots represent the N2-P2 amplitudes of fast-pain trials averaged across participants, and error bars denote the SD values across participants. The blue line represents the fitted linear model for low-pain trials and fast-pain trials separately. Note that, when NRS = 10, the N2-P2 amplitudes are slightly lower than those of NRS = 9. The possible reason is that only 18 participants had fast-pain trials of NRS = 10, which might make the estimation have large variance and not reliable.

Relationship Between isEEG Features and LEP-Pain Model Parameters

The isEEG of alpha-2 band (11–13 Hz) across almost the whole brain showed significant negative correlation between nMSSD and the model slope of (4), as shown in Figure 4. Also, we observed that the model intercept of (4) showed significant positive correlation with RMS of isEEG across almost the whole brain in all frequency bands, as shown in Figure 5. Non-significant results are only reported in the Supplementary Material, as shown in Figure S1 and Figure S2.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Significant correlation between temporal variability (nMSSD) of isEEG in alpha-2 band (11–13 Hz) and the fitting slope of the linear LEP-pain model (4).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Significant correlation between isEEG magnitude (RMS) and the fitting intercept of the linear LEP-pain model (4).

Individualized LEP-Pain Prediction Model

The individualized LEP-based fast-pain prediction scheme can achieve a prediction error (MAE) of 1.19 ± 0.35 (mean ± SD), which is significantly smaller than the MAE, 1.36 ± 0.51, of the conventional cross-individual LEP-pain prediction (p = 0.002; two-sample paired t-test).

Discussion

In this study, by analyzing isEEG data recorded during pain experiments from healthy participants, we obtained two main findings. First, both the magnitude (measured by RMS) and temporal variability (measured by nMSSD) of isEEG are related to cross-individual difference in the parameters of LEP-pain model for fast-pain prediction. Second, by selecting and weighting training individuals according to isEEG correlates of LEP-pain model, cross-individual fast-pain prediction accuracy could be significantly improved.

Linear LEP-Pain Prediction Model

The strong correlation between perceived pain intensity and the N2-P2 amplitude of LEP response at the single-trial level has been validated by numerous studies (Iannetti et al., 2008; Huang et al., 2013b; Hu et al., 2014). However, previous LEP studies usually adopted painful stimuli which can produce a clear pinprick pain sensation (Iannetti et al., 2008; Huang et al., 2013b; de Tommaso et al., 2017). Different from that, this study collected LEP data during stimulation with a wide range of energy intensities, including lower energy intensities which could not induce Aδ-related N2-P2 response. Based on this dataset, it has been shown that the linear relationship between pain ratings and LEP amplitude only exists for fast-pain trials (with NRS > 4) (Bai et al., 2016). Therefore, if the N2-P2 amplitude at Cz is used as the LEP feature to predict pain ratings of fast-pain trials, then the LEP-pain prediction model is defined by two parameters (slope and intercept). Different individuals have different LEP-pain relationship, so there is substantial cross-individual variability in the slope and intercept of the linear pain prediction models. A complex and nonlinear pain prediction model (other than the linear model) could be used for pain prediction, but it contains more model parameters whose meanings are difficult to be interpreted. So, we used a simple linear model to describe the relationship between single-trial LEP and pain rating.

Positive Correlation Between isEEG Magnitude and Model Intercept

Significant positive correlation was observed between RMS of isEEG and the intercept of the LEP-pain model for fast-pain prediction (4) at almost all frequency bands. Because the intercept is an inherent part of the LEP responses in (4) and the magnitude of LEP responses is positively correlated with isEEG magnitude for each individual (Bai et al., 2016), the intercept also has a positive relationship with isEEG magnitude. Actually, the positive relationship between the magnitude of isEEG and LEP has been observed in our earlier research from the same data set (Bai et al., 2016), which is due to anatomical factors (such as skull thickness and the orientation of the gray matter) and scale-electrode impedances.

Negative Correlation Between Alpha-2 isEEG Temporal Variability and Model Slope

Significant negative correlation between isEEG temporal variability (nMSSD) and the model slope was only observed in alpha-2 band (11–13 Hz). Alpha band activities are the dominant oscillations in the human brain, with a mean frequency of approximately 10 Hz. Growing evidence has shown that two independent alpha rhythmical components exist (alpha-1 and alpha-2 sub-bands) and they have different cortical sources and different physiological meanings and functional roles (Bazanova and Vernon, 2014). For example, their activities are different for dissimilar cognitive demands (Michels et al., 2008). Therefore, in pain research, alpha activities mediating the cognitive modulations of pain experience may display different activities in two alpha sub-bands. In this study, the significant correlation we observed in alpha-2 was maximal over ipsilateral and central electrodes. During the inter-stimulus period, alpha activities over central regions may mainly reflect the cognitive modulations such as anticipation and attention, since previous studies have revealed that the expectation period before the noxious stimulus is characterized by increased activations within pain-related cortical regions, including the bilateral anterior cingulate cortex, and medial prefrontal cortex (Keltner et al., 2006). Besides, as revealed in our previous study, pre-stimulus alpha activities, located bilaterally over central regions, significantly modulated the perceived pain intensity of subsequent stimulus and may partly reflect the neural activity of the sensory-motor network (Tu et al., 2016). Thus, alpha oscillation with greater dynamic range may represent better capacity for subject to engage antinociceptive and sensory-motor system and to manage their pain.

Individualized Cross-Individual Pain Prediction Using isEEG Correlates

Compared with within-individual pain prediction, cross-individual prediction is more useful for clinical uses because it does not need subjective pain ratings for new individuals. However, the performance of cross-individual pain prediction is still not satisfactory because of the inherent cross-individual variability in pain perception. Cross-individual variability in pain perception could be observed in subjective pain ratings, pain-evoked brain responses or the relationship between them. In order to solve this problem for higher accuracy in cross-individual pain prediction, our previous study employed EEG-based normalization to incorporate individual traits that are related to cross-individual variability into the pain prediction model (Bai et al., 2016). The proposed normalization method was based on the stronger cross-individual correlation between the magnitude of isEEG and LEP (Bai et al., 2016). Here in this study, we observed that isEEG magnitude and temporal variability are related to cross-individual variability that in the LEP-pain model, and used isEEG correlates to tailor the parameters of the LEP-pain model to improve the accuracy of fast-pain prediction. The idea is to only use individuals with similar isEEG correlates to construct a LEP-pain prediction model for a new individual. The proposed method has achieved significantly lower estimation error in cross-individual pain prediction and could be potentially used as a practical and feasible pain prediction method in clinical practice.

Limitations and Future Work

Proper assessment of pain is imperative for the development of an effective pain management plan, however, self-report pain is not available in some vulnerable populations (e.g., non-communicative patients with disorders of consciousness) (Schnakers et al., 2010). Therefore, the objective assessment of perceived pain from brain activity would be of enormous clinical implications (Huang et al., 2013b; Hu and Iannetti, 2016). In order to develop a more accurate and practical EEG-based pain prediction method, it is an important issue to reduce the error brought by cross-individual difference in pain (Fillingim, 2016). Characterizing the relationship between isEEG activities and LEP-pain prediction model parameters enables to capture isEEG correlates of cross-individual difference in the LEP-pain relationship, which is an important step to achieve individualized pain prediction. Here are some limitations of the current work and also outlines directions for future research. In this study, we observed isEEG features which were related to cross-individual differences in pain perception. However, much more factors, including genetic, environmental, psychological, and cognitive factors, may contribute to cross-individual differences in pain perception (Ochsner et al., 2006; Coghill, 2010). Future study with larger sample size and integrated dataset might find more kinds of features with significant correlation, then can make much more improvement in cross-individual pain prediction. Besides, we used isEEG data extracted from the inter-stimulus periods from nociceptive-stimulation experiment, which were inevitably affected by the periods with stimulation. Thus, future study could record resting-state EEG signal to validate the results we have achieved. Last but not least, advanced machine learning methods, such as transfer learning, could be applied to reduce cross-individual variability.

Ethics Statements

The protocol was approved by the ethics committee of Southwest University. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

LL, GH, and ZZ contributed to the construction of the study hypothesis. QL and JL collected the data. LL performed the data analysis and drafted the manuscript. Critical revisions were contributed by SZ and ZZ. All authors approved the manuscript for submission.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer RG and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Science, Technology and Innovation Commission of Shenzhen Municipality Technology Fund (No. JCYJ20170818093322718), Zhuhai Specially Appointed Scholar Program (No. 67000-42070001), Shenzhen Peacock Plan (No. KQTD2016053112051497), National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 61701316), and Research Project of State Key Laboratory of Mechanical System and Vibration (No. MSV201710).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2018.00340/full#supplementary-material

References

Anderson, K. L., and Ding, M. (2011). Attentional modulation of the somatosensory mu rhythm. Neuroscience 180, 165–180. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Del, P. C., Brancucci, A., Capotosto, P., Le, P. D., Marzano, N., et al. (2008). Pre-stimulus alpha power affects vertex N2-P2 potentials evoked by noxious stimuli. Brain Res. Bull. 75, 581–590. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bai, Y., Gan, H., Tu, Y., Ao, T., Sam, H. Y., and Zhang, Z. (2016). Normalization of pain-evoked neural responses using spontaneous EEG improves the performance of EEG-based cross-individual pain prediction. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 10:31. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2016.00031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basbaum, A., and Bushnell, M. (2009). Science of Pain. Oxford: Academic.

Google Scholar

Bazanova, O. M., and Vernon, D. (2014). Interpreting EEG alpha activity. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 44, 94–110. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntson, G. G., Lozano, D. L., and Chen, Y. J. (2005). Filter properties of root mean square successive difference (RMSSD) for heart rate. Psychophysiology 42, 246–252. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00277.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coghill, R. C. (2010). Individual differences in the subjective experience of pain: new insights into mechanisms and models. Headache 50, 1531–1535. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2010.01763.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coghill, R. C., McHaffie, J. G., and Yen, Y. F. (2003). Neural correlates of interindividual differences in the subjective experience of pain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 8538–8542. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1430684100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Tommaso, M., Ricci, K., Montemurno, A., and Vecchio, E. (2017). Age-related changes in laser-evoked potentials following trigeminal and hand stimulation in healthy subjects. Eur. J. Pain 21, 1087–1097. doi: 10.1002/ejp.1010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fair, D. A., Schlaggar, B. L., Cohen, A. L., Miezin, F. M., Dosenbach, N. U., Wenger, K. K., et al. (2007). A method for using blocked and event-related fMRI data to study “resting state” functional connectivity. Neuroimage 35, 396–405. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faisal, A. A., Selen, L. P., and Wolpert, D. M. (2008). Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 292–303. doi: 10.1038/nrn2258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fillingim, R. B. (2016). Individual differences in pain: understanding the mosaic that makes pain personal. Pain 158 (Suppl 1), S11–S18. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galin (1964). Auditory nuclei: distinctive response patterns to white noise and tones in unanesthetized cats. Science 146, 270–272. doi: 10.1126/science.146.3641.270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganger, S., Hahn, A., Küblböck, M., Kranz, G. S., Spies, M., Vanicek, T., et al. (2015). Comparison of continuously acquired resting state and extracted analogues from active tasks. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 4053–4063. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcíalarrea, L., Peyron, R., Laurent, B., and Mauguière, F. (1997). Association and dissociation between laser-evoked potentials and pain perception. Neuroreport 8, 3785–3789. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199712010-00026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, D. D., Kovacevic, N., Mcintosh, A. R., and Grady, C. L. (2013a). The modulation of BOLD variability between cognitive states varies by age and processing speed. Cereb. Cortex 23, 684–693. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrett, D. D., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Macdonald, S. W., Lindenberger, U., Mcintosh, A. R., and Grady, C. L. (2013b). Moment-to-moment brain signal variability: a next frontier in human brain mapping? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 610–624. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gram, M., Graversen, C., Olesen, A. E., and Drewes, A. M. (2015). Machine learning on encephalographic activity may predict opioid analgesia. Eur. J. Pain 19, 1552–1561. doi: 10.1002/ejp.734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, B. J. (2011). Scale-free properties of the functional magnetic resonance imaging signal during rest and task. J. Neurosci. 31, 13786–13795. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2111-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., Cai, M. M., Xiao, P., Luo, F., and Iannetti, G. D. (2014). Human brain responses to concomitant stimulation of Aδ and C nociceptors. J. Neurosci. 34, 11439–11451. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1355-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Iannetti, G. D. (2016). Painful issues in pain prediction. Trends Neurosci. 39, 212–220. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, G., Xiao, P., Hu, L., and Hung, Y. S. (2013a). Single-trial laser-evoked potentials feature extraction for prediction of pain perception. Conf. Proc. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2013, 4207–4210. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2013.6610473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, G., Xiao, P., Hung, Y. S., Iannetti, G. D., Zhang, Z. G., and Hu, L. (2013b). A novel approach to predict subjective pain perception from single-trial laser-evoked potentials. Neuroimage 81, 283–293. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iannetti, G. D., Hughes, N. P., Lee, M. C., and Mouraux, A. (2008). Determinants of laser-evoked EEG responses: pain perception or stimulus saliency? J. Neurophysiol. 100, 815–828. doi: 10.1152/jn.00097.2008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iannetti, G. D., Zambreanu, L., Cruccu, G., and Tracey, I. (2005). Operculoinsular cortex encodes pain intensity at the earliest stages of cortical processing as indicated by amplitude of laser-evoked potentials in humans. Neuroscience 131, 199–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.10.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iannetti, G. D., Zambreanu, L., and Tracey, I. (2006). Similar nociceptive afferents mediate psychophysical and electrophysiological responses to heat stimulation of glabrous and hairy skin in humans. J. Physiol. 577, 235–248. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.115675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keltner, J. R., Furst, A., Fan, C., Redfern, R., Inglis, B., and Fields, H. L. (2006). Isolating the modulatory effect of expectation on pain transmission: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 26, 4437–4443. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4463-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, J., Halacz, J., van Dijk, H., Kahlbrock, N., and Schnitzler, A. (2012). Fluctuations of prestimulus oscillatory power predict subjective perception of tactile simultaneity. Cereb. Cortex 22, 2564–2574. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Z., Zang, Y.-F., Ding, J., and Wang, Z. (2017). Assessing the mean strength and variations of the time-to-time fluctuations of resting-state brain activity. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 55, 631–640. doi: 10.1007/s11517-016-1544-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Solà, M., Woo, C. W., Pujol, J., Deus, J., Harrison, B. J., Monfort, J., et al. (2017). Towards a neurophysiological signature for fibromyalgia. Pain 158, 34–47. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magerl, W., Ali, Z., Ellrich, J., Meyer, R. A., and Treede, R.-D. (1999). C- and Aδ-fiber components of heat-evoked cerebral potentials in healthy human subjects. Pain 82, 127–137. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00061-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makeig, S., Jung, T. P., Bell, A. J., Ghahremani, D., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1997). Blind Separation of Auditory Event-Related Brain Responses into Independent Components. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 10979–10984. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.20.10979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michels, L., Moazami-Goudarzi, M., Jeanmonod, D., and Sarnthein, J. (2008). EEG alpha distinguishes between cuneal and precuneal activation in working memory. Neuroimage 40, 1296–1310. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neumann, J. V., Kent, R. H., Bellinson, H. R., and Hart, B. I. (1941). The Mean Square Successive Difference. Ann. Math. Stat. 12, 153–162. doi: 10.1214/aoms/1177731746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ochsner, K. N., Ludlow, D. H., Knierim, K., Hanelin, J., Ramachandran, T., Glover, G. C., et al. (2006). Neural correlates of individual differences in pain-related fear and anxiety. Pain 120, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onton, J., Westerfield, M., Townsend, J., and Makeig, S. (2006). Imaging human EEG dynamics using independent component analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 808–822. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogachov, A., Cheng, J. C., Erpelding, N., Hemington, K. S., Crawley, A. P., and Davis, K. D. (2016). Regional brain signal variability: a novel indicator of pain sensitivity and coping. Pain 157, 2483–2492. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Kuhnen, C. M., Yoo, D. J., and Knutson, B. (2010). Variability in nucleus accumbens activity mediates age-related suboptimal financial risk taking. J. Neurosci. 30, 1426–1434. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4902-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnakers, C., Chatelle, C., Majerus, S., Gosseries, O., De, V. M., and Laureys, S. (2010). Assessment and detection of pain in noncommunicative severely brain-injured patients. Expert Rev. Neurother. 10, 1725–1731. doi: 10.1586/ern.10.148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tracey, I., and Mantyh, P. W. (2007). The cerebral signature for pain perception and its modulation. Neuron 55, 377–391. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treisman, M. (1964). Noise and Weber's law: the discrimination of brightness and other dimensions. Psychol. Rev. 71, 314–330. doi: 10.1037/h0042445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tu, Y., Zhang, Z., Tan, A., Peng, W., Hung, Y. S., Moayedi, M., et al. (2016). Alpha and gamma oscillation amplitudes synergistically predict the perception of forthcoming nociceptive stimuli. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 501–514. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wager, T. D., Atlas, L. Y., Lindquist, M. A., Roy, M., Woo, C. W., and Kross, E. (2013). An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. N. Engl. J. Med. 368, 1388–1397. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y., and Ding, M. (2010). Detection of a weak somatosensory stimulus: role of the prestimulus mu rhythm and its top-down modulation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 307–322. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zotterman, Y. (1953). Special senses: thermal receptors. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 15, 357–372. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ph.15.030153.002041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: pain prediction, cross-individual prediction, inter-stimulus EEG, single-trial analysis, machine learning

Citation: Li L, Huang G, Lin Q, Liu J, Zhang S and Zhang Z (2018) Magnitude and Temporal Variability of Inter-stimulus EEG Modulate the Linear Relationship Between Laser-Evoked Potentials and Fast-Pain Perception. Front. Neurosci. 12:340. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00340

Received: 02 February 2018; Accepted: 01 May 2018;
Published: 31 May 2018.

Edited by:

Li Hu, Institute of Psychology (CAS), China

Reviewed by:

Ruolei Gu, Institute of Psychology (CAS), China
Yifei Guo, University College London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2018 Li, Huang, Lin, Liu, Zhang and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Zhiguo Zhang, zgzhang@szu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.