Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 09 June 2021
Sec. Decision Neuroscience

Special Patterns of Dynamic Brain Networks Discriminate Between Face and Non-face Processing: A Single-Trial EEG Study

\nZhongliang Yin,Zhongliang Yin1,2Yue WangYue Wang1Minghao DongMinghao Dong2Shenghan RenShenghan Ren2Haihong HuHaihong Hu2Kuiying YinKuiying Yin3Jimin Liang
Jimin Liang1*
  • 1School of Electronic Engineering, Xidian University, Xi'an, China
  • 2Engineering Research Center of Molecular and Neuro Imaging of Ministry of Education, School of Life Science and Technology, Xidian University, Xi'an, China
  • 3Nanjing Research Institute of Electronics Technology, Nanjing, China

Face processing is a spatiotemporal dynamic process involving widely distributed and closely connected brain regions. Although previous studies have examined the topological differences in brain networks between face and non-face processing, the time-varying patterns at different processing stages have not been fully characterized. In this study, dynamic brain networks were used to explore the mechanism of face processing in human brain. We constructed a set of brain networks based on consecutive short EEG segments recorded during face and non-face (ketch) processing respectively, and analyzed the topological characteristic of these brain networks by graph theory. We found that the topological differences of the backbone of original brain networks (the minimum spanning tree, MST) between face and ketch processing changed dynamically. Specifically, during face processing, the MST was more line-like over alpha band in 0–100 ms time window after stimuli onset, and more star-like over theta and alpha bands in 100–200 and 200–300 ms time windows. The results indicated that the brain network was more efficient for information transfer and exchange during face processing compared with non-face processing. In the MST, the nodes with significant differences of betweenness centrality and degree were mainly located in the left frontal area and ventral visual pathway, which were involved in the face-related regions. In addition, the special MST patterns can discriminate between face and ketch processing by an accuracy of 93.39%. Our results suggested that special MST structures of dynamic brain networks reflected the potential mechanism of face processing in human brain.

1. Introduction

Face processing is a well-developed human capability, and thanks to it, we can recognize faces more quickly and accurately than non-face objects in complex environments. In the past decades, the distinct processing mechanism of faces in the brain has been extensively explored by neuroscientists and psychologists (Bentin et al., 1996; Ishai et al., 2005; Yang et al., 2015; Uono et al., 2017; Fan et al., 2020; Muukkonen et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2020; Yin et al., 2020). It is well-accepted that face processing is a spatiotemporally dynamic process involving widely distributed and closely connected brain regions (Ishai et al., 2005; Uono et al., 2017; Muukkonen et al., 2020). From the perspective of temporal dimension, the event related potential (ERP) studies show that face processing is distinctive from non-face processing in human brain in different time windows (Bentin et al., 1996; Yang et al., 2015). Specifically, the amplitude of P1 occurring around 80–100 ms after stimuli onset is larger for faces than for non-face objects; the amplitude of N1 increases and the latency of N1 is about 30 ms longer for faces (arising around 160–190 ms) than for non-face objects (arising around 130–160 ms). From the spatial dimension, researches with high spatial resolution (such as fMRI) propose that the neural activities in and between the face-related brain regions are distinctive compared with non-face processing (Ishai et al., 2005; Axelrod and Yovel, 2013; Renzi et al., 2013; Duchaine and Yovel, 2015; Muukkonen et al., 2020), demonstrating that these regions and their connections construct a unique topology for face processing. However, few studies have investigated the time-varying patterns of the topology at different processing stages to fully characterize the mechanism of face processing.

The human brain is a complex network with highly connected and widely distributed regions (Meunier et al., 2010; van den Heuvel and Sporns, 2011; Bullmore and Sporns, 2012). Analyses based on graph theoretical approaches suggest that human brain networks are organized according to an efficient topology that integrates brain regions with similar function into groups (called communities) and maintains short path lengths between regions, i.e., the small-world organization (Sporns and Honey, 2006; Bullmore and Sporns, 2012). The efficient topology of brain networks has been considered to play a key role in cognitive processing, and different topologies are associated with specific stages of the cognitive process (Zhang et al., 2019; Allegra et al., 2020; Delgado Reyes et al., 2020; Finc et al., 2020; Si et al., 2020).

Topological organization analysis of brain networks characterizes the integration and segregation of information between distributed brain regions, whereas dynamic brain networks investigate changes in the topological organization of a set of brain networks which are typically constructed from brain activities recorded in sequential time windows. When employed to study human cognitive processes, dynamic brain networks enable the integration of multiple dimensions of the information about neural activities in the brain (Bola and Sabel, 2015; Hassan et al., 2015; Rizkallah et al., 2018). In this study, we investigated the differences in brain networks corresponding to face processing and non-face processing in different time windows. The goal is to understand how the differences change in different processing stages, thus enabling us to reveal the underlying mechanism of face processing from a spatiotemporal perspective.

When comparing the topologies of graph networks, the conventional operations suffer from some methodological problems (van Wijk et al., 2010; Fornito et al., 2013; Tewarie et al., 2015). Specifically, for binary network analysis, the networks being compared are obtained by binarizing the original weighted networks using a fixed threshold, which may change the average degree of the networks differently. As the graph measures are influenced by the average degree of network, the comparison may lead to biased results. Although weighted network analysis can alleviate the bias caused by the thresholding operation, the difference in the averaged weights will influence the graph measures of weighted networks. Recently, the minimum spanning tree (MST) has been adopted to tackle the aforementioned issues (Stam et al., 2014; Tewarie et al., 2015; van Dellen et al., 2018). The MST is a sub-network without loops and is the backbone of the original network. It has been used to explore the mechanism of mental illnesses and cognitive functions (Fraschini et al., 2016; Tóth et al., 2017; Utianski et al., 2017; Cao et al., 2020; Das and Puthankattil, 2020). Moreover, various measures of MST were used as features to distinguish different cognitive states or different groups via machine learning methods (Cui et al., 2018; Guo et al., 2018; Mehraram et al., 2019; Saba et al., 2019).

In this study, we made an exploratory research on the mechanism of face processing by dynamic brain networks based on electroencephalography (EEG) recordings. Firstly, the EEG data recorded during face and non-face (ketch) processing were filtered into 5 classical frequency bands, including delta (0.5–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), and gamma (31–46 Hz) bands. Secondly, trials were extracted and each trial was divided into 5 time windows with the length of 100 ms. Thirdly, MSTs of the brain networks corresponding to face and ketch processing in each time window over each frequency band were constructed. Fourthly, the MST topologies of face and ketch processing were compared and the MST measures with significant differences were selected as features. Finally, the discriminability of the selected MST measures was validated by a classification model using the support vector machine (SVM).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Twenty-eight volunteers (15 male, 13 female; age = 27.41 ± 5.47, mean ± standard deviation) were recruited from the Xidian Community. They were all right-handed, reported normal or corrected to normal visual acuity, and did not have any history of psychiatric or neurological disorders. All the volunteers provided written informed consents and received monetary payment for the participation. The experimental procedures complied with Helsinki Declaration of 1975 which was revised in 2000.

2.2. Experimental Design

The stimuli were three categories of pictures, including 48 faces (24 males and 24 females), 48 ketches and 48 watches. Face and ketch pictures were presented in two conditions: upright and inverted. In addition, 48 scrambled pictures were included as the baseline stimuli. The scrambled pictures were obtained from face pictures by two steps. First, each picture was divided into small blocks, and then, the small blocks were rearranged until the identity of the scrambled picture could not be discerned. The face pictures were collected from public-accessed websites, the ketch and watch pictures were chosen from the Caltech256 image set (Griffin et al., 2006). To eliminate the influence of physical properties of stimuli, grayscale pictures were used, and all of them were cropped into 300 × 300 pixels. Pixel values of pictures were normalized to make them have similar luminance levels. Moreover, the object in each picture occupied as least 75% space of the picture. Figure 1 shows examples of the stimuli.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Examples of each kind of stimuli. (B) Procedure of experiment.

The experiment was conducted in an electromagnetically shielded and semi-dark room. The introduction and stimuli were presented on an LCD screen (resolution: 1980 × 1080 pixels, refresh rate: 60 Hz). The participants were positioned at a viewing distance of approximately 80 cm and seated in a comfortable armchair with their eyes fixating on the center of the screen. The stimuli were presented using E-Prime 2.0 presentation software running under Windows 7 with an Nvidia GeForce GTX 750 graphics card.

During the experiment, before a stimulus was presented, a red cross was displayed in the center of the screen for 500–800 ms. Each stimulus was presented for 500 ms and followed by an empty screen for 500–700 ms. The red-cross, stimulus and empty screen constituted an experimental trial the duration of which was approximately 1,750 ms. All the stimuli were divided into 3 blocks. Each block included 96 different stimuli (16 upright faces, 16 inverted faces, 16 upright ketches, 16 inverted ketches, 16 watches and 16 scrambled pictures), and lasted approximately 2.8 min. Each block was repeated six times, and the pictures were shuffled in each block. There were 18 blocks in this experiment, with 288 trials for each type of pictures, and the total time was about 70 min. The participants were requested to observe the stimuli carefully and make keyboard responses during the empty screen after they saw the watches. During the experiment, after one block was finished, the experiment was paused. Then, the subject could pressed the “space key” to go on or had a short break when he felt tired. The block design avoided the subjects' fatigue. The experimental procedure was similar to the ones used in previous studies (Bentin et al., 1996; Uono et al., 2017), in order to keep them consistent, the inverted faces and ketches were included. The goal of our study was to investigate the potential mechanism of face processing by compared with non-face processing, so the EEG data of inverted faces and ketches was not analyzed in this study. Figure 1 shows the experimental procedure.

2.3. EEG Recording and Preprocessing

When the participants undertook the task, EEG data were recorded using a 64-channel amplifier provided by Brain Products Company (ActiCHamp system). All the channels, including a reference channel (channel Iz) and 63 EEG channels, were deployed over the head according to the international 10-10 system (Robert and Peter, 2001; Jurcak et al., 2007). The sampling rate was set to 1,000 Hz, the contact impedance of each channel was kept below 10kΩ, the pass-band filter with 0.5–100 Hz and notch filter with 50 Hz were employed when EEG data were recorded.

The EEG data were preprocessed off-line in the following steps. Firstly, EEG data were re-referenced to the common average reference and inspected by visual observation using EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) to remove epochs containing artifacts such as slow drift. Secondly, the electrooculogram (EOG), electromyography (EMG) and other non-cognitive related artifacts were removed by independent component analysis (ICA). Thirdly, five narrowband filters were used to obtain 5 classical frequency-band EEG data [delta (0.5–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), gamma (31–46 Hz)]. In this study, the delta, theta, alpha, and beta frequency bands were similar with other EEG studies on face processing (Sakihara et al., 2012; Yu et al., 2016; Tóth et al., 2017). The gamma bands were defined according to face studies (Gao et al., 2013; Uono et al., 2017; Frauscher et al., 2018). Finally, trials were extracted according to the type of stimuli, ranging from −200 ms before to 800 ms after the stimulus onset. Each trial was baseline-corrected by subtracting the mean amplitude over the epoch between −200 and 0 ms.

In this study, the total number of the trials of all the subjects per category was 8064 (28 × 48 × 6). After preprocessed, noisy trials were removed. Then, we chose nearly equal number of trials corresponding to faces, ketches, and scrambled pictures for each subject for further analysis. The number of the trials of each subject per category was about 274. Finally, the total number of trials was 7,665 per category. The dataset was randomly divided into two subsets, where the first subset contained 70% of trials (5,366 trials per category) and the second contained the remaining 30% of trials (2,299 trials per category). The first subset was used to investigate the distinctive processing of face compared with non-face (ketch) through dynamic brain network analysis. The second one was used to test the discriminatory ability of brain network features in face and non-face processing classification.

2.4. Data Analysis

Figure 2 shows the methodological workflow for data analysis in this study. The data processing and analysis were performed in MATLAB 2015b including EEGLAB (version 13_6_5b, http://sccn.ucsd.edu/eeglab/).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Methodological workflow. (A) EEG recording and pre-processing. Five frequency bands of EEG data were calculated and divided equally into 5 time windows. (B) Original brain network construction for each time window and each frequency band. (C) MST construction. (D) MST representation and analysis. Measures with significant differences between face and ketch processing were concatenated as features for classification. (E,F) Classification of face and ketch processing using MST features and EEG time segments, respectively. (G) Evaluation of classification performance.

2.4.1. Time Window Division

For each trial, the data segment from 0 to 500 ms after stimulus onset was selected for analysis. Each data segment was divided equally into 5 time windows (0–100, 101–200, 201–300, 301–400, 401–500 ms), denoted as T1 to T5, respectively. In each time window, a brain network was constructed over each frequency band separately.

In this study, the length of 100 ms was used for two reasons. First, we found that longer data segment may lead to severe averaging effects that reduced the temporal resolution, while too short data segments caused excessive computational load. Second, the P1-N170-P2 effect of face processing indicates that the unique processing of face may occur in 0–100, 100–200, and 200–300 ms sequentially (Gu et al., 2010; Yang et al., 2015).

2.4.2. Functional Connectivity

The phase lag index (PLI) (Stam et al., 2007) was used to estimate the strength of correlations between EEG channels that, in neuroscience research, is referred to as functional connectivity. The PLI evaluates the phase synchronization between EEG signals of two channels. Before calculating PLI, the instantaneous phases of an EEG channel data x(t) should be obtained from the corresponding analytical signal z(t) which is a complex signal with a real part of x(t) and an imaginary part of x~(t):

z(t)=x(t)+ix~(t)=A(t)eiϕ(t),    (1)

where x~(t) is the Hilbert transform of x(t). The instantaneous amplitude A(t) and phase ϕ(t) of x(t) can be calculated as

A(t)=[x~(t)]2+[x(t)]2,    (2)
ϕ(t)=arctanx~(t)x(t).    (3)

The PLI between EEG signals of two channels is computed as

PLI=|1Nk=1Nsign {sin[Δϕ(tk)]}|,    (4)

where ΔΦ(tk) is the phase difference between two time series at time instant k, N is the total number of samples, “sign” refers to the Heaviside function, and “sin” is the sinusoidal function. As the “sign” function is introduced in Equation (4), the zero-lag synchronization is removed, which makes the PLI less affected by the volume conduction effect compared with other methods (Stam et al., 2007). The value of PLI ranges from 0 to 1. A PLI value of 0 indicates either no coupling or coupling with phase difference locking at a value different from 0, such that the functional connectivity will be 0. The larger the PLI, the stronger the functional connectivity. More details of the PLI computation can be found in Stam and his colleagues' work (Stam et al., 2007).

The functional connectivities of channel pairs constitute the adjacency matrix of a brain network. For each trial, each frequency band and each time window, a brain network was constructed accordingly.

2.4.3. MST Construction and Representation

To investigate the topological organization of the brain networks, the MST of each brain network was constructed and the graph theoretical representations of the MSTs were analyzed. Only a brief description of the MST is given here, the detailed information can be found in previous studies (Stam et al., 2014; Tewarie et al., 2015; van Dellen et al., 2018).

MST is a sub-network of the original network in which all nodes are connected without forming loops and has the minimum total weight of all possible spanning trees (Tewarie et al., 2015). Since the PLI values, which can be considered as an inverse distance, were used in this study as the link weights for the original brain network, we retained the maximum link weights to construct the MSTs (formally a maximum spanning tree) using the Kruskal's algorithm (Kruskal, 1956). Specifically, given an original network, the weights of all its links were sorted and the link with the maximum weight was added to the MST. Then, the link with the next largest weight was added, and this process continued until all nodes were connected. If adding a link resulted in the formation of a loop, the link was skipped. At the end of the link addition process, a weighted MST was generated. Finally, the weighed MST was binarized.

Several measures are usually used to characterize the MST, including degree (K), leaf fraction (Lf), eccentricity (E), diameter (D), betweenness centrality (BC), and tree hierarchy (Th). The detailed information of these measures can be found in Boersma et al. (2013), Stam et al. (2014), and Tewarie et al. (2015). In addition to the graph measures, the sum of PLI (denoted as MST PLI) is also an important measure of MST, which estimates the degree of regional coupling in the MST.

There are two extreme structures of MST—line and star structures, corresponding to regular and random networks, respectively. For an MST with N nodes and N-1 links, the two extreme structures have deterministic measures. For a line structure, the maximum of degree K is 2, the number of leaf nodes is 2 and the diameter D is N-1. For a star structure, the maximum of K is N-1, the number of leaf nodes is N-1, and D is 2. An MST is considered as line-like if its measures are closer to that of a line structure than to a star structure, otherwise it is star-like. In general, the structure of a brain network is between a line and a star structure.

When comparing the topologies of two MSTs, the dissimilarity between them should first be estimated. In this study, the dissimilarity is quantified using a measure based on the information theory. For two different MSTs (MSTn and MSTm) with the same number of nodes, the dissimilarity measure computes how much information is needed, on average, to explain MSTn given MSTm (Lee et al., 2006). The dissimilarity is defined as

Sn/m=1Ni=1Nlog10 |Sn(i)Sm(i)|,    (5)

where Sn(i) and Sm(i) are the sum of distances from a reference node i to all its neighbors in MSTn and MSTm, respectively. Distances refer to the path length based on the PLI. More detailed explanations about the dissimilarity can be found in Lee et al. (2006) and Tewarie et al. (2014).

In this study, the dissimilarity of MSTs between face and ketch processing was evaluated through a reference MSTref. Specifically, a reference MSTref was firstly constructed from the network obtained by averaging the adjacency matrixes corresponding to all the scrambled pictures. Then, the dissmilarities of MSTs during face (MSTx) and ketch processing (MSTy) compared with the reference MSTref (Sx/ref and Sy/ref) were calculated, respectively. Finally, Sx/ref and Sy/ref was compared to evaluate the dissimilarity of MSTs betweeen face and ketch processing.

2.4.4. Statistical Analysis

Wilcoxon rank sum test was conducted for each measure of the MSTs between face and ketch stimuli in each time window and each frequency band. The dissimilarity between the corresponding MSTs was analyzed firstly. Further analyses were carried out only for the pairs of MSTs demonstrating significant dissimilarity. The measures including D, Lf, maximum BC (MaxBC), maximum degree (MaxK), Th, MST PLI, and each node's BC and K were calculated. Wilcoxon rank sum test was performed for each measure and the p-values were corrected by false discovery rate (FDR). The correction took two factors into account - the time window (T1, T2, T3, T4, T5) and the frequency band (delta, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma). Moreover, for the K and BC of each node, the number of nodes was also considered. In this study, the significant level was set to 0.05.

In this study, Wilcoxon rank sum test with FDR correction was chosen for the statistical analysis for two reasons. Frist, Wilcoxon rank sum test does not require the data to follow a normal distribution. Second, since there were several factors (including time windows and frequency bands) that influenced the analysis results, the multiple comparison correction should be used to correct the statistical results. There are two routine methods of multiple comparison correction, FDR and Bonferroni correction. Bonferroni correction is stricter than FDR correct, but it often rejects not only false positives but also many positive results (Benjamini and Yekutieli, 2001), so we used FDR.

In summary, in this study, the EEG data were firstly filtered into five frequency bands and then each original EEG trial was divided into five time segments. For each time segment, a brain network was constructed based on the adjacency matrix defined by phase lagged index. After the brain networks were constructed, we extracted MSTs of the brain networks and calculated their measures. Then, we compared and analyzed the topology of MSTs corresponding to faces and ketches in each time window over each frequency band across subjects.

2.5. Classification Based on SVM

A machine learning approach was used to validate the single-trail discriminatory ability of the MST measures for face and non-face processing classification. After analysis of the first dataset, all the MST measures with significant differences between the two types of stimuli across all time windows and frequency bands were concatenated as features. For the second dataset, we calculated the network features, and a SVM classifier was adopted for classification. The total number of the second dataset was 2299 for face and ketch stimuli respectively. We performed five-fold cross-validation to train SVM classifiers using the LIBSVM library (Chang and Lin, 2011) and reported the results averaged over 5 repetitions. For comparison with the MST measures, we also used the EEG time segments (TS) over occipito-temporal areas as features for SVM classification. Details about the construction of TS samples was presented in the Supplementary Material. The classification performance was quantified using the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve, mean accuracy, sensitivity, specificity, and area under the ROC curve (AUC).

3. Results

3.1. MST Dissimilarity Test

The results of dissimilarity test demonstrated that there were significant differences in the processing of faces and ketches in certain time windows and frequency bands, as shown in Table 1. Specifically, over theta band, the dissimilarity presented in T2 and T3; over alpha band, the dissimilarity presented in T1, T2, and T3. Figure 3 shows the sketch maps of MSTs of face and ketch processing in different windows over theta and alpha bands. The MSTs were constructed from the adjacent matrices averaged from all trails of each situation, respectively. The balls represent the nodes (channels) and their size is the degree of the node. The red balls are leaf nodes, other colored balls are hub nodes with degree greater than 1. The larger the size of the ball, the greater the node's degree, and the more important the hub node is in the MST. The sketch maps were made by BrainNet Viewer (Xia et al., 2013).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results of MST dissimilarity test.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Sketch maps of MSTs of face and ketch processing in axial view (dorsal side). The top part corresponds to anterior part of brain, and left part corresponds to left part of brain.

3.2. MST Measure Analysis

Table 2 and Figure 4 present the statistical results of the MST measures of face processing compared with ketch processing. From these, we can draw the following observations.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of MST measure test.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Diameter, leaf fraction, MaxBC, MaxK, Th, and MST PLI of MSTs of face and ketch processing in different time windows over theta and alpha frequency bands. The asterisks indicate results with significant differences after FDR correction, and bars represent standard error.

Over theta band, in T2, the face processing had a significantly shorter MST diameter compared with ketch processing, whereas the other measures (including Lf, MaxBC, MaxK, Th, and MST PLI) were significantly larger. In T3, the MST diameter of face processing was also significantly shorter than that of ketch processing, but unlike in T2, only the leaf fraction and MST PLI were significantly larger than that of ketch processing. These results indicated that the structures of MSTs were more star-like during face processing compared with during ketch processing in T2 and T3 over theta band.

Over alpha band, in T1, the MST diameter was significantly longer during face processing compared with ketch processing, while the other measures (except for Th) were significantly smaller. These results indicated that the structure of MST was more line-like during face processing. In contrast, in T2 and T3, the MST diameter was significantly shorter and the other measures were significantly larger (except for Th in T3). These results indicated that the MST structures were more star-like during face processing in T2 and T3 over alpha band.

The statistical analysis of node BC presented that some nodes had significantly larger BC values during face processing than that during ketch processing. Specifically, over theta band and in T2, these nodes were mainly located in the left frontal area, and bilateral ventral visual pathway of the brain (including Fp1, AF3, TP9, P7, PO7, PO3, O1, POz, Oz, O2, P4, P8, TP10, PO4, and PO8). Over alpha band, in T2, these nodes appeared in the left frontal region (Fp1 and AF3), and right occipito-temporal region (POz, PO4, PO8, P8, and TP10); in T3, these nodes mainly appeared in the left frontal region (Fp1 and AF3), and right parietal-temporal region (Pz, P8, and TP10). The results of node degree were similar to the results of BC. Several nodes presented significant larger degree during face processing than that during ketch processing. Over theta band, in T2, the nodes were Fp1, PO3, PO7, P7, O1, Pz, Oz, O2, PO4, PO8, P6, P8, and TP10. Over alpha band, in T2, the nodes were Fp1, AF3, POz, O2, PO4, PO8, P8, and TP10; in T3, the nodes were Fp1, AF3, P8, and TP10. Figure 5 shows the topographic mapping of the p-values of node BC and degree, respectively.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Topographic mapping of the p-values of BC and degree of each node in T2 over theta band, in T2 and T3 over alpha band. After FDR correction, the critical p-value of BC value and node degree was 0.00037 in T2 over theta band, 0.00018 in T2 and 0.00014 in T3 over alpha band.

3.3. Classification

In this study, the SVM classifier was used to perform the classification. The related measures, including the classification accuracy, sensitivity, specificity and AUC of the ROC curve, were calculated to quantify the results. The MST measures which were significantly different between face and ketch processing were chosen as features to train and test the SVM classifier. Based on the results of above sections, the number of MST features was 82. Figure 6 shows the ROC curves for the SVM classifiers using MST and TS features, respectively. Table 3 presents the classification performance of the MST and TS features. It is obvious that the MST features have better classification performance.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. ROC curves of SVM classifiers using MST and TS features.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Classification performance of MST and TS features.

4. Discussion

In this study, the MST topologies of dynamic brain networks of face and ketch processing were compared to explore the mechanism of face processing. The MSTs were constructed based on single-trial EEG data in sequential time windows after stimulus onset over different frequency bands. We found that the MSTs of face processing were more star-like compared with that of ketch processing over theta band in T2 and T3; over alpha band, the MSTs of face processing were more line-like in T1 while more star-like in T2 and T3. The nodes with significant differences in BC value and degree were located mainly in the left frontal area, and bilateral ventral visual pathway of the brain in T2 over theta band. Over alpha band, these nodes were located mainly in the left frontal, and right occipito-temporal regions in T2 and T3. In addition, the classification results showed that the special MST topologies in specific time windows over specific frequency bands might reflect the potential mechanism of face processing in human brain. The results were discussed in detail in the following sections.

4.1. Topological Organization of Brain Networks

4.1.1. Global Topological Differences of MSTs

In this study, we found that the MSTs were more star-like during face processing than that during non-face processing in T2 and T3 over theta and alpha bands. The MST includes the most important connections of the original network and represents the “high-way” in the network (Wu et al., 2006; Braunstein et al., 2007; Stam et al., 2014; van Dellen et al., 2018). In star-like MST, the topological organization follows a hierarchical pattern (large Th) composed of a few layers and some densely connected nodes with most of the nodes serving as periphery within the network (large Lf) (Stam et al., 2014; Tewarie et al., 2015). In addition, the star-like MST has smaller diameter that corresponds to shorter path length in original network, which facilitates the communication between regions with long spatial distance and promotes the processing speed. This topological structure of MST contributes to fast and efficient information transfer in the network (Boersma et al., 2013; Cao et al., 2020). Our findings indicated that information transfer and processing were faster and more efficient among brain regions during face processing compared with non-face processing in T2 and T3 over theta and alpha bands. The efficient MST structure might be correspond to the results of previous studies. It is known that the top-down control mechanism plays an important role in face processing (Duchaine and Yovel, 2015; Fan et al., 2020). The top-down processing is associated with neuronal coupling between the frontal and posterior brain regions over theta/alpha bands (Anderson, 2011; Maurer et al., 2015; Bossi et al., 2020; Yin et al., 2020). The top-down processing during face processing is also associated with the alpha band neuronal coupling between parietal and temporal regions (Klimesch et al., 2011; Heyselaar et al., 2018). The top-down mechanism shortens the functional distance between regions with long spatial distance and makes large-scale integration in the brain network. We put forward that the top-down control mechanism might be one of the reasons for the formation of the more star-like topology of brain networks during face processing. Moreover, the duration in which the top-down processing worked during face processing was not clear in previous studies. Based on the above analysis, the findings that the more star-like topology occurred in T2 and T3 suggested that the top-down processing might occur during 100–300 ms after the face onset.

Larger MaxBC and MaxK were found during face processing compared with that during ketch processing in T2 over theta band, and in T2 and T3 over alpha band. If a network has larger MaxBC and MaxK, then most of the nodes are connected by several hub nodes, making the network more star-like (Stam et al., 2014; Tóth et al., 2017; Cao et al., 2020). The hub nodes carry the most amount of information transfer and make the processing of information in the network more efficient. Our results indicated that there might exist some hub nodes in the brain network which played an important role during face processing, which was consistent with the results of pervious face studies. In previous studies, brain areas that were very special to face processing were found (Haxby et al., 2000; Ishai et al., 2005; Duchaine and Yovel, 2015; Uono et al., 2017; Fan et al., 2020). In these facial areas, neural activities were distinctive between face and non-face processing. Unlike previous studies, our results based on MaxBC and MaxK did not provide detailed information about the nodes, nor do they provide information about which nodes were more important during face processing. In this study, the result that larger MaxBC and MaxK appeared in T2 not T3 over theta band reminded us of N170. N170 that has been considered to be a biomarker of face processing occurs in the duration of 150–200 ms after stimuli onset over the occipito-temporal regions (part of facial areas) (Bentin and Deouell, 2000; Yang et al., 2015). The similar occurrence interval indicated that our result might be related to N170 occurrence during face processing. Moreover, the results that larger MaxBC and MaxK for face processing in T2 and T3 over alpha band might be consistent with the inter-trail phase coherence (ITPC) study of face processing (Gu et al., 2010). The ITPC study showed that higher alpha band phase synchronization appeared in the duration of 100–270 ms over occipito-temporal regions after stimuli onset during face stimuli compared with non-face stimuli.

The hub nodes facilitate the information integrating in the network, but if they are overloaded, the network will be broken and its efficiency will be seriously reduced (Stam et al., 2014; Yu et al., 2017). Tree hierarchy (Th) characterizes the balance between integration and node load in the network. The larger the Th value, the more efficient the information integrating of the network, however, the higher the risk of the hub nodes being in an overloaded state. We found that Th was larger only in T2 over theta and alpha bands, which indicated that the T2 was important for face processing. In T2, the brain network of face processing organized into a structure with several hub nodes that bore more information processing. According to previous studies (Bentin and Deouell, 2000; Schiltz and Rossion, 2006; Uono et al., 2017), we inferred that this structure of brain network might facilitate the unique processing of face including holistic encoding and configural processing which occurs in 160–210 ms after faces onset. Moreover, it is supposed that overloaded nodes in the brain network may process the most urgent information, but they must not work in an overloaded state for long time to avoid making themselves damaged (Olde Dubbelink et al., 2014; Stam et al., 2014; Fraga González et al., 2016). The result that Th was not larger in T3 during face processing over theta and alpha bands indicated that the high loaded nodes restored to normal state after processing of main facial features, although the topology of MST was still more efficient (more star-like MST) for face processing.

We found that MST PLI was larger during face processing compared with that during ketch processing in T2 and T3 over theta and alpha bands. MST PLI measures the strength of functional connection of the brain on average. Our findings indicated that face processing resulted in enhanced functional connection in the brain network, which were consistent with pervious face processing studies. Specifically, previous EEG studies presented that the functional connection increased between the occipito-temporal regions and other brain regions over theta band, and between frontal/parietal regions and posterior region over alpha band during face processing compared with non-face processing (Yang et al., 2015); moreover, the average functional connection of the brain network increased over theta band during face processing (Yin et al., 2020). Different from these studies in which the temporal information was lost, our results provided the duration in which the functional connection became stronger during face processing.

Besides showing more star-like topology in T2 and T3, we found that the MST of brain network was more line-like during face processing in T1 over alpha band. More line-like MST indicated that the structure of the brain networks tended to be regular (Boersma et al., 2013; Stam et al., 2014; Tóth et al., 2017), which had more clustered regions that contributed to local processing. In this type of network, most of the information processing took place in the clustered regions, with little information transfer between them. Our finding suggested that more clustered regions were formed in the initial stage of face processing and contributed to the processing of basic face features. In previous study, the role of functional brain network's topology in cognition has been postulated by the global workspace theory (GWT) (Baars et al., 2013). GWT proposes that in human cognition, local processing within specific modules occurs in the beginning, after which the local information needs to be integrated within a global workspace that can be identified by a network comprising hub-nodes and inter-modular connections. In our study, dynamical changes of the MST topology from more line-like to more star-like supported the GWT, and indicated that the information separation and integration might be more significant during face processing.

In addition, although the topological organization of MST was more star-like during face processing in T2 and T3 over theta and alpha bands, it could be observed that the difference between face and ketch processing became not significant in terms of several measures in T3. Specifically, the MaxBC, MaxK, and Th over theta band, and Th over alpha band became not significantly different in T3. The results indicated that the brain networks of face and ketch processing were organized dynamically, and their structures trended to be similar after the completion of face feature processing. In previous studies (Bentin and Deouell, 2000; Yang et al., 2015; Uono et al., 2017), they presented that the basic visual features were extracted in the early stage of visual object processing and the unique processing of face occurred during 80–280 ms after stimuli onset, which demonstrated the dynamic nature of face processing. Our results validated this unique property of face processing from the perspective of dynamic brain networks.

4.1.2. Node Feature Differences of MSTs

The node BC and degree provide information about the single nodes, reflecting the local processing in the brain network (Sporns et al., 2007; van den Heuvel and Sporns, 2011). The nodes with larger BC and degree regulate the information transfer and processing efficiently in the network. In our study, we found several nodes had significantly larger BC, and most of them also had significantly larger degree during face processing. Over theta band, these nodes were mainly located in the left frontal area, and bilateral ventral visual pathway of the brain in T2. This result suggested that neural activity in these nodes might regulate the transfer of face-related information more efficiently and play more important role in face processing. The locations of these nodes were consistent with the previous studies on the “core” and “extent” system of face processing (Ishai et al., 2005; Duchaine and Yovel, 2015), which involved the inferior occipital gyrus, fusiform gyrus, superior temporal sulcus, hippocampus, amygdala, inferior frontal gyrus, and orbitofrontal cortex.

Over alpha band, the nodes with larger BC and degree during face processing were mainly located in the left frontal and right occipito-temporal regions in T2, while in the left frontal, and right posterior temporal regions in T3. The results indicated that the mechanism of face processing might be related to the alpha band neuronal synchronization among the left frontal, and right occipito-temporal regions. It has been reported that the alpha band neural activities in left frontal and posterior regions are associated with the top-down processing in face processing (Klimesch et al., 2011; Heyselaar et al., 2018; Yin et al., 2020). However, the time window in which these special neural activities occurs is not clear. Our findings suggested that the T2 and T3 time windows might be the duration in which these neural activities occurred during face processing. Moreover, the special alpha neural activities were found in the right but not in the left occipito-temporal regions. This result might be related to the right hemisphere effect of face processing (Bentin and Deouell, 2000; Duchaine and Yovel, 2015).

In summary, in the previous research on face processing, the functional connectivity and roles of brain regions were investigated. They presented “core” and “extent” systems of face processing. The functional connectivity between the regions in these systems were enhanced, and the regions played specific roles. However, none of them investigated the topology of the brain network during face processing by using graphic theory. Our findings about nodes and MST PLI were consistent with the previous studies. Furthermore, as the above analysis, other MST measures provided new perspective on understanding the mechanism of face processing in the brain.

4.2. Classification Performance

As discussed above, the dynamical changes of the brain network structures reflected the mechanism of face processing. Our results indicated that the structure of brain network might contain common patterns that could distinguish between face and non-face (ketch) processing based on single-trial EEG data. The results of the machine learning method demonstrated that SVM classifier based on MST measures had better performance compared with that based on conventional temporal features. In this study, we chose MST measures that presented significant difference between face and ketch processing as the features. These features included not only the temporal but also the spatial information of neuronal activities, which characterized the face processing more comprehensively.

Compared with previous related studies, the data analysis method of this study. First, the brain networks were constructed based on single-trial EEG data. In most of previous studies, the EEG segments were firstly averaged over single subject, and then the brain networks were constructed using the averaged EEG data for each subject (Yu et al., 2016; Mehraram et al., 2019; Cao et al., 2020). However, not all the EEG segments were time-locked to human cognitive stages, so the averaging operation might make the intrinsic information broken. The brain networks based on single-trial data could supply many more samples, thus enabling the statistical analysis to find the intrinsic patterns in the data. Second, the SVM classifier was trained using samples from all the subjects. Whereas in previous studies, individual classifiers were constructed for each subject (Wang and Jung, 2011; Barngrover et al., 2016), thus each subject had his own classification performance. But the subject-specific classifiers were hard to apply to other subjects because of the individual differences. In our study, we obtained the common patterns of all the subjects. It can be easily applied to distinguish the brain states regardless of the individual differences.

Finally, we should clarify the data reference issue. When preprocessing the EEG data, data reference should be considered. There are several reference types in EEG study, including the nose, electrode Cz, REST (Dong et al., 2017), and common average reference. In this study, we referenced our EEG data by using common average reference for two reasons. First, in the most of previous studies on face processing, the common average reference was used. In order to compare our result with the results in previous studies (Bentin et al., 1996; Kang et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2015; Foley et al., 2018; Ambrus et al., 2019; Mehraram et al., 2019), we chose the common average reference. Second, Yao's study (Yao, 2001) mentioned that the use of scalp potentials to determine the neural electrical activities or their equivalent sources does not depend on the reference, so we used the common average reference in this study.

4.3. Limitations and Future Work

There were several limitations should be paid attention to when interpreting the findings in this study. First, the MST discards the weak connections and only includes the strong connections of the original brain network. It should be noted that, no matter strong or weak it is, each connection may play a certain role in the network. So discarding weak connections may loss some information of the brain network. However, in this study, the advantages of using MST outweighed the disadvantages. For one thing, previous studies have presented that larger neural activities and functional connections occurred during face processing (Yang et al., 2015; Yin et al., 2020). These findings suggested that the selection of strong connections might be rational to study the mechanism of face processing. For another, in contrast to the original brain network-based analysis, the MST-based analysis is not biased by the network size, average degree, or density effects and allows for unbiased comparison between networks of equal size (Stam et al., 2014; Tewarie et al., 2015; van Dellen et al., 2018).

Second, the MST does not supply directional information in the brain network. However, in the brain, the information flow is directional during visual objects processing. Specifically, the basic features of visual objects are extracted firstly on the primary visual cortex, and then the combination and other processings are done along the ventral and dorsal visual pathway or modulated by other functional cortices. In the future study, the methods which can characterize the direction of information flow in the brain network should be explored.

Third, unlike previous studies in EEG segment division, in our study, five EEG segments were selected after the onset of stimulus presentation, each with a duration of 100 ms without overlap. Our data segment was longer than that in previous studies (Bola and Sabel, 2015; Yang et al., 2015; Rizkallah et al., 2018). Longer data segment may lead to severely average effect that reduces the temporal resolution. However, data segments that are too short will lead to excessive computational load. The P1-N170-P2 effect of face processing indicates that the unique processing of face may occur in 0–100, 100–200, and 200–300 ms sequentially (Gu et al., 2010; Yang et al., 2015). Therefore, in order to balance the temporal resolution and computational load, we chose 100 ms as the unit of analysis. Whether 100 ms is the optimal duration should be explored in the future work.

Fourth, recently, the spatial pattern of the brain networks has been investigated to explore the neural mechanism of decision-making and schizophrenia (Li et al., 2019; Si et al., 2020), which is a new perspective to characterize the brain. In our study, we focused on the topological difference of the brain networks during face and non-face processing in different time window, so the spatial pattern of the brain network was not considered. In our future work, we will investigate the spatial pattern of the brain networks during face and non-face processing.

5. Conclusions

In this study, we investigated the mechanism of face processing from the spatiotemporal perceptive by using the dynamic brain network method. The MSTs were extracted from the original brain networks and their structures were compared between face and non-face (ketch) processing. The results demonstrated that the MST topology was more line-like in T1 over alpha band, while was more star-like in T2 and T3 over theta and alpha bands. From the graph theory perspective, the special dynamic organization of the brain network facilitated the information transfer and processing during face processing. The locations of the nodes with larger BC values and degrees were consistent with the previous studies. The classification performance based on MST measures was superior to that using the EEG time segment. Our study indicated that the special dynamic organization of the brain network might reflect the potential mechanism of face processing in human brain.

Data Availability Statement

The data and code that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants as been approved by Ethics Committee of Xidian University and complied with Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

Data collection, analysis, and writing of manuscript was mainly performed by ZY. YW and SR assisted analysis and writing of manuscript. Study design and supervision was conducted by MD, KY, and JL. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This paper was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (U19B2030, 61976167), Basic Scientific Research program (No. JCKY2017204B102) and the Science Technology Projects of Xi'an, China (201809170CX11JC12) and Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (No. JB191206).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2021.652920/full#supplementary-material

References

Allegra, M., Seyed-Allaei, S., Schuck, N. W., Amati, D., Laio, A., and Reverberi, C. (2020). Brain network dynamics during spontaneous strategy shifts and incremental task optimization. Neuroimage 217:116854. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ambrus, G. G., Kaiser, D., Cichy, R. M., and Kovacs, G. (2019). The neural dynamics of familiar face recognition. Cereb. Cortex 29, 4775–4784. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, K. L. (2011). Functional roles of theta- and alpha-band neural oscillations in memory and attention (Ph.D. thesis), University of Florida, Gainesville, FL.

Google Scholar

Axelrod, V., and Yovel, G. (2013). The challenge of localizing the anterior temporal face area: a possible solution. Neuroimage 81, 371–380. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baars, B. J., Franklin, S., and Ramsoy, T. Z. (2013). Global workspace dynamics: cortical “binding and propagation” enables conscious contents. Front. Psychol. 4:200. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barngrover, C., Althoff, A., Deguzman, P., and Kastner, R. (2016). A brain-computer interface (BCI) for the detection of mine-like objects in sidescan sonar imagery. IEEE J. Ocean. Eng. 41, 123–138. doi: 10.1109/JOE.2015.2408471

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Yekutieli, D. (2001). The control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing under dependency. Ann. Stat. 29, 1165–1188. doi: 10.1214/aos/1013699998

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., and McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 551–565. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentin, S., and Deouell, L. Y. (2000). Structural encoding and identification in face processing: ERP evidence for separate mechanisms. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 17, 35–55. doi: 10.1080/026432900380472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boersma, M., Smit, D. J., Boomsma, D. I., de Geus, E. J., delemarre-Van de Waal, H. A., and Stam, C. J. (2013). Growing trees in child brains: graph theoretical analysis of electroencephalography-derived minimum spanning tree in 5- and 7-year-old children reflects brain maturation. Brain Connect. 3, 50–60. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bola, M., and Sabel, B. A. (2015). Dynamic reorganization of brain functional networks during cognition. Neuroimage 114, 398–413. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bossi, F., Premoli, I., Pizzamiglio, S., Balaban, S., Ricciardelli, P., and Rivolta, D. (2020). Theta- and gamma-band activity discriminates face, body and object perception. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:74. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braunstein, L. A., Wu, Z., Chen, Y., Buldyrev, S. V., Kalisky, T., Sreenivasan, S., et al. (2007). Optimal path and minimal spanning trees in random weighted networks. Int. J. Bifurc. Chaos 17, 2215–2255. doi: 10.1142/S0218127407018361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2012). The economy of brain network organization. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 336–349. doi: 10.1038/nrn3214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, R., Hao, Y., Wang, X., Gao, Y., Shi, H., Huo, S., et al. (2020). EEG functional connectivity underlying emotional valance and arousal using minimum spanning trees. Front. Neurosci. 14:355. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C.-C., and Lin, C.-J. (2011). LIBSVM: a library for support vector machines. ACM Trans. Intell. Syst. Technol. 2, 1–27. doi: 10.1145/1961189.1961199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, X., Xiang, J., Wang, B., Xiao, J., Niu, Y., and Chen, J. (2018). Integrating the local property and topological structure in the minimum spanning tree brain functional network for classification of early mild cognitive impairment. Front. Neurosci. 12:701. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Das, S., and Puthankattil, S. D. (2020). Complex network analysis of MCI-AD EEG signals under cognitive and resting state. Brain Res. 1735:146743. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2020.146743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delgado Reyes, L., Wijeakumar, S., Magnotta, V. A., Forbes, S. H., and Spencer, J. P. (2020). The functional brain networks that underlie visual working memory in the first two years of life. Neuroimage 219:116971. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, L., Li, F., Liu, Q., Wen, X., Lai, Y., Xu, P., et al. (2017). MATLAB toolboxes for reference electrode standardization technique (REST) of scalp EEG. Front. Neurosci. 11:601. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duchaine, B., and Yovel, G. (2015). A revised neural framework for face processing. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 1, 393–416. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-082114-035518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, X., Wang, F., Shao, H., Zhang, P., and He, S. (2020). The bottom-up and top-down processing of faces in the human occipitotemporal cortex. eLife 9:e48764. doi: 10.7554/eLife.48764.sa2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finc, K., Bonna, K., He, X., Lydon-Staley, D. M., Kühn, S., Duch, W., et al. (2020). Dynamic reconfiguration of functional brain networks during working memory training. Nat. Commun. 11, 1–15. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-17747-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foley, E., Rippon, G., and Senior, C. (2018). Modulation of neural oscillatory activity during dynamic face processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 1–15. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fornito, A., Zalesky, A., and Breakspear, M. (2013). Graph analysis of the human connectome: promise, progress, and pitfalls. Neuroimage 80, 426–444. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraga González, G., Van der Molen, M. J., Žarić, G., Bonte, M., Tijms, J., Blomert, L., et al. (2016). Graph analysis of EEG resting state functional networks in dyslexic readers. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 3165–3175. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.06.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraschini, M., Demuru, M., Hillebrand, A., Cuccu, L., Porcu, S., Di Stefano, F., et al. (2016). EEG functional network topology is associated with disability in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Sci. Rep. 6, 1–7. doi: 10.1038/srep38653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frauscher, B., von Ellenrieder, N., Zelmann, R., Rogers, C., Nguyen, D. K., Kahane, P., et al. (2018). High-frequency oscillations in the normal human brain. Ann. Neurol. 84, 374–385. doi: 10.1002/ana.25304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Z., Goldstein, A., Harpaz, Y., Hansel, M., and Bentin, S. (2013). A magnetoencephalographic study of face processing: M170, gamma-band oscillations and source localization. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 783–1795. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffin, G., Holub, A. D., and Perona, P. (2006). The Caltech 256. Caltech Technical Report. Available online at: http://www.vision.caltech.edu/Image_Datasets/Caltech256/

Gu, G., Yang, Y., Zhu, Y., and Qiu, Y. (2010). “The phase analysis of ongoing EEG oscillations under face/object perception,” in Proceedings - 2010 3rd International Conference on Biomedical Engineering and Informatics, BMEI 2010 (Yantai), 1063–1066. doi: 10.1109/BMEI.2010.5640542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, H., Yan, P., Cheng, C., Li, Y., Chen, J., Xu, Y., et al. (2018). fMRI classification method with multiple feature fusion based on minimum spanning tree analysis. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 277, 14–27. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hassan, M., Benquet, P., Biraben, A., Berrou, C., Dufor, O., and Wendling, F. (2015). Dynamic reorganization of functional brain networks during picture naming. Cortex 73, 276–288. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., and Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 223–233. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01482-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyselaar, E., Mazaheri, A., Hagoort, P., and Segaert, K. (2018). Changes in alpha activity reveal that social opinion modulates attention allocation during face processing. Neuroimage 174, 432–440. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishai, A., Schmidt, C. F., and Boesiger, P. (2005). Face perception is mediated by a distributed cortical network. Brain Res. Bull. 67, 87–93. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jurcak, V., Tsuzuki, D., and Dan, I. (2007). 10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: Their validity as relative head-surface-based positioning systems. Neuroimage 34, 1600–1611. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, J. H., Kim, S. J., Cho, Y. S., and Kim, S. P. (2015). Modulation of alpha oscillations in the human EEG with facial preference. PLoS ONE 10:e0138153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Fellinger, R., and Freunberger, R. (2011). Functional roles of alpha-band phase synchronization in local and large-scale cortical networks. Front. Psychol. 2:204. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruskal, J. B. (1956). On the shortest spanning subtree of a graph and the traveling salesman problem. Proc. Am. Math. Soc. 7:48. doi: 10.1090/S0002-9939-1956-0078686-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, U., Kim, S., and Jung, K. Y. (2006). Classification of epilepsy types through global network analysis of scalp electroencephalograms. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlinear Soft Matter Phys. 73, 1–9. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.73.041920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, F., Wang, J., Liao, Y., Yi, C., Jiang, Y., Si, Y., et al. (2019). Differentiation of schizophrenia by combining the spatial eeg brain network patterns of rest and task P300. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 27, 594–602. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2019.2900725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maurer, U., Brem, S., Liechti, M., Maurizio, S., Michels, L., and Brandeis, D. (2015). Frontal midline theta reflects individual task performance in a working memory task. Brain Topogr. 28, 127–134. doi: 10.1007/s10548-014-0361-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehraram, R., Kaiser, M., Cromarty, R., Graziadio, S., O'Brien, J. T., Killen, A., et al. (2019). Weighted network measures reveal differences between dementia types: an EEG study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 1573–1590. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meunier, D., Lambiotte, R., and Bullmore, E. T. (2010). Modular and hierarchically modular organization of brain networks. Front. Neurosci. 4:200. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2010.00200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muukkonen, I., Ölander, K., Numminen, J., and Salmela, V. R. (2020). Spatio-temporal dynamics of face perception. Neuroimage 209, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116531

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olde Dubbelink, K. T., Hillebrand, A., Stoffers, D., Deijen, J. B., Twisk, J. W., Stam, C. J., et al. (2014). Disrupted brain network topology in Parkinson's disease: a longitudinal magnetoencephalography study. Brain 137, 197–207. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renzi, C., Schiavi, S., Carbon, C. C., Vecchi, T., Silvanto, J., and Cattaneo, Z. (2013). Processing of featural and configural aspects of faces is lateralized in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a TMS study. Neuroimage 74, 45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizkallah, J., Benquet, P., Kabbara, A., Dufor, O., Wendling, F., and Hassan, M. (2018). Dynamic reshaping of functional brain networks during visual object recognition. J. Neural Eng. 15, 1–10. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aad7b1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robert, O., and Peter, P. (2001). The five percent electrode system for high-resolution EEG and ERP measurements. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 713–719. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00527-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saba, V., Premi, E., Cristillo, V., Gazzina, S., Palluzzi, F., Zanetti, O., et al. (2019). Brain connectivity and information-flow breakdown revealed by a minimum spanning tree-based analysis of mri data in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Front. Neurosci. 13:211. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakihara, K., Gunji, A., Furushima, W., and Inagaki, M. (2012). Event-related oscillations in structural and semantic encoding of faces. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 270–277. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.06.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiltz, C., and Rossion, B. (2006). Faces are represented holistically in the human occipito-temporal cortex. Neuroimage 32, 1385–1394. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Si, Y., Li, F., Duan, K., Tao, Q., Li, C., Cao, Z., et al. (2020). Predicting individual decision-making responses based on single-trial EEG. Neuroimage 206, 1–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sporns, O., and Honey, C. J. (2006). Small worlds inside big brains. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 19219–19220. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609523103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sporns, O., Honey, C. J., and Kötter, R. (2007). Identification and classification of hubs in brain networks. PLoS ONE 2:e1049. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C., Tewarie, P., Dellen, E. V., van Straaten, E., Hillebrand, A., and Mieghem, P. V. (2014). The trees and the forest: characterization of complex brain networks with minimum spanning trees. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 92, 129–138. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C. J., Nolte, G., and Daffertshofer, A. (2007). Phase lag index: assessment of functional connectivity from multi channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 1178–1193. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tewarie, P., Hillebrand, A., Schoonheim, M. M., van Dijk, B. W., Geurts, J. J., Barkhof, F., et al. (2014). Functional brain network analysis using minimum spanning trees in multiple sclerosis: an MEG source-space study. Neuroimage 88, 308–318. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tewarie, P., van Dellen, E., Hillebrand, A., and Stam, C. J. (2015). The minimum spanning tree: an unbiased method for brain network analysis. Neuroimage 104, 177–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tóth, B., Urbán, G., Háden, G. P., Márk, M., Török, M., Stam, C. J., et al. (2017). Large-scale network organization of EEG functional connectivity in newborn infants. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 4019–4033. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uono, S., Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Kubota, Y., Sawada, R., Yoshimura, S., et al. (2017). Time course of gamma-band oscillation associated with face processing in the inferior occipital gyrus and fusiform gyrus: a combined fMRI and MEG study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 2067–2079. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Utianski, R. L., Caviness, J. N., van Straaten, E. C., Beach, T. G., Dugger, B. N., Shill, H. A., et al. (2017). Graph theory network function in Parkinson's disease assessed with electroencephalography. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 2228–2236. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dellen, E., Sommer, I. E., Bohlken, M. M., Tewarie, P., Draaisma, L., Zalesky, A., et al. (2018). Minimum spanning tree analysis of the human connectome. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 2455–2471. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Heuvel, M. P., and Sporns, O. (2011). Rich-club organization of the human connectome. J. Neurosci. 31, 15775–15786. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3539-11.2011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Wijk, B. C., Stam, C. J., and Daffertshofer, A. (2010). Comparing brain networks of different size and connectivity density using graph theory. PLoS ONE 5:e13701. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., and Jung, T.-P. (2011). A collaborative brain-computer interface for improving human performance. PLoS ONE 6:e20422. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Metoki, A., Smith, D. V., Medaglia, J. D., Zang, Y., Benear, S., et al. (2020). Multimodal mapping of the face connectome. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 397–411. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0811-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Z., Braunstein, L. A., Havlin, S., and Stanley, H. E. (2006). Transport in weighted networks: partition into superhighways and roads. Phys. Rev. Lett. 96, 1–4. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.96.148702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xia, M., Wang, J., and He, Y. (2013). BrainNet viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS ONE 8:e68910. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Qiu, Y., and Schouten, A. C. (2015). Dynamic functional brain connectivity for face perception. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:662. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, D. (2001). A method to standardize a reference of scalp EEG recordings to a point at infinity. Physiol. Meas. 22, 693–711. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/22/4/305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, Z., Wang, Y., Dong, M., Wang, Y., Ren, S., and Liang, J. (2020). Short-range and long-range neuronal oscillatory coupling in multiple frequency bands during face perception. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 152, 26–35. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, M., Engels, M. M., Hillebrand, A., Van Straaten, E. C., Gouw, A. A., Teunissen, C., et al. (2017). Selective impairment of hippocampus and posterior hub areas in Alzheimer's disease: an MEG-based multiplex network study. Brain 140, 1466–1485. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, M., Gouw, A. A., Hillebrand, A., Tijms, B. M., Stam, C. J., van Straaten, E. C., et al. (2016). Different functional connectivity and network topology in behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease: an EEG study. Neurobiol. Aging 42, 150–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, G., Li, Y., and Zhang, J. (2019). Tracking the dynamic functional network interactions during goal-directed auditory tasks by brain state clustering. Front. Neurosci. 13:1220. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: face processing, dynamic brain network, minimum spanning tree, electroencephalography, classification

Citation: Yin Z, Wang Y, Dong M, Ren S, Hu H, Yin K and Liang J (2021) Special Patterns of Dynamic Brain Networks Discriminate Between Face and Non-face Processing: A Single-Trial EEG Study. Front. Neurosci. 15:652920. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.652920

Received: 13 January 2021; Accepted: 17 May 2021;
Published: 09 June 2021.

Edited by:

Xujun Duan, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, China

Reviewed by:

Fali Li, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, China
Yingjie Li, Shanghai University, China

Copyright © 2021 Yin, Wang, Dong, Ren, Hu, Yin and Liang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jimin Liang, jimleung@mail.xidian.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.