Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Pediatr., 14 March 2016
Sec. Pediatric Critical Care
Volume 4 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fped.2016.00020

Stone Throwing in the Glass House

  • Division of Pediatric Critical Care Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, Mattel Children’s Hospital UCLA, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA

A small girl with a unique genetic syndrome lies in bed since birth. Her brain does not work. She is in a persistent vegetative state. Recurrent seizures punctuate her intractable epilepsy. She shows no meaningful interaction. Her breathing is insufficient. Upper airway obstruction, repeated aspiration events, chronic lung disease, and scoliosis make her struggle to breathe. Her parents never wanted her to suffer. Two years ago, they agreed to a Physician Orders for Life-Sustaining Treatment (POLST). Their daughter would not undergo resuscitation. Death did not come, however. With meticulous medical care, her body grew. Her breathing worsened. Offered either palliation or a tracheostomy tube, her parents chose “trach.” With easier breathing, the girl expended fewer calories. She continued gastrostomy tube feeds and gained 10 pounds in 3 months. Now she is rehospitalized with respiratory distress. Her weight rises another 2 pounds. We put off calorie reduction for the outpatient setting. For the first time, we discharge the family home with a mechanical ventilator. Our collective health-care efforts have set up a protracted course whose likely end will come in overwhelming sepsis or ARDS, rather than allowing natural death with palliative care. How many ethical issues were raised by this case? I count more than 40 (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Ethical issues raised by the case.

Physicians make ethical decisions constantly in health care without formal training in moral practice. Consequently, we leave most moral issues unaddressed. Instead, we concretize and categorize patient complaints into physical and mental issues to be addressed functionally (1). For instance, I may not be able to help a boy dying of cancer with his existential loneliness, but I can prescribe morphine for his respiratory distress. As a doctor trained in physiological intervention, this essentialization of clinical questions is justified by the ethical principle of beneficence. We focus on how we can help patients. However, this practice dehumanizes patients and leaves important overarching questions unaddressed. As Agledahl et al. wrote, “Even if your clinically sound decision is morally motivated, it may not necessarily be the morally good thing to do” (1).

What is the effect of practicing medicine without addressing moral issues? Does clinical practice diminish our own humanity? Studies of our medical trainees may shed some light. Remarkably consistent studies have observed that empathy declines in medical students and resident trainees during their clinical years, but not during preclinical education (2). Students are subjected to mistreatment by mentors, loss of idealism when faced with clinical realities, isolation from social supports, high workload, lack of sleep and personal time, and a fragmented patient–physician relationship (2). Furthermore, the evolving electronic medical environment may be reducing trainees’ time spent in patients’ rooms. Pediatric interns and residents now spend only 12% of their time with patients, but 21% on computers and 35% communicating with colleagues (3).

One could argue that limited empathy can improve problem-solving ability and competent health care (4). But what about the moral aspect of medicine? Unfortunately, clinical training impairs moral development. Repeated studies over the past 20 years observed that medical students fail to attain normal moral development and may even decline in moral reasoning capacity during their 4 years of medical school (59). Ethics lectures and clinical experience fail to correct this deficiency. One reasonable hypothesis supposes that overemphasis on memorization of facts, obedience to authority, and application of rules based on practice standards, evidence, and cost limits might impede the development of higher order “postconventional,” or ethically principled, thought (7, 8). An evidence-based solution for teaching moral clinical practice is lacking. Despite that, the next time a pediatrics intern struggles with an uncooperative family and wants to spend more time considering the merits of the parents’ arguments, I will quell my inner urge to cut them off in order to move on to the next patient.

Years ago as a sleepless young attending, I paced behind a nursing station. Alarms rang and were silenced, while a faint scent of bleach permeated the air. A previously vivacious toddler, now glassy eyed, was breathless with acute viral bronchiolitis. For the second time, I approached the mother and said, “Your daughter needs help to breathe. We need to place her on a mechanical ventilator.”

Cradling her daughter in her arms, the mother looked directly in my eyes. She said, “No.”

Exhortations by our chief of emergency medicine yielded the same negative response. Against my better judgment, I agreed to trial high flow oxygen in my pediatric intensive care unit. The poor girl struggled all night, lost her intravenous (IV) catheter, and decompensated during attempts to regain IV access. After I finally intubated the child against the wishes of the parents, shortly thereafter the girl suffered cardiac arrest. Two hours of coding with a multidisciplinary team of 20 people was of no use. She was dead. First, I lost faith in parental “autonomy” to make health-care decisions for their children. Worse yet, I then lost faith in my colleagues. While making allowance for my emotionally laden and possibly inaccurate perceptions, I recall uniformly negative feedback from all corners. There was neither support nor empathy.

Why did this happen? Was I treated as less than a person in both the clinical encounter and sequelae? While we discussed above the failure of doctors to treat the whole person, we now focus on how doctors are themselves treated. Dehumanization in medicine goes both ways. People who feel a greater need for medical services recall fewer personal facts about their doctors, want physicians who focus only on their patients, and are less likely to perceive their physicians as having personal attributes or personal lives (10). This leads to the counterproductive result that patients with the greatest medical need are least able to communicate effectively with their most “instrumental” physicians.

A cultural loss of trust in the doctor–patient relationship also plays a role. As famously pointed out by the Institute of Medicine (IOM) (11), deadly medical errors are common. Despite tremendous improvements in US health-care quality since the IOM report (12), popular news continues to trumpet, “Medical errors in Indiana hit another high” (13). Loss-frame messaging (14) undermines the public trust in health-care providers, prejudicing perceptions of incompetence and aloofness.

Physicians ourselves add to the negative chorus. This was formally documented in a standardized covert patient study of consenting physicians (15). In a referral group of oncologists and family physicians, conversations about the other doctor were recorded and analyzed. The large majority of comments – 67% – were Critical, while only 29% were Supportive and 4% Neutral. For most health-care providers, these results should be no surprise. Doctors criticize doctors as lustily as do patients. Nevertheless, I seem to perceive the situation improving recently as doctors place greater emphasis on professionalism.

Some criticism may be understandable, however. Our own “standard of care” is riddled with mistakes. In the decade from 2001 to 2010, a study of 363 original articles testing standard of care found that 146 (40%) reversed accepted medical practices (16). Among these reversals were such entrenched pediatric interventions as imaging studies after first febrile urinary tract infection in young children, high-dose epinephrine for children with in-hospital cardiac arrest, and early insertion of tympanostomy tubes for persistent otitis media >3 months or persistent middle ear effusions. The critical care literature has noted reversals in low-dose dopamine to improve renal blood flow (17), early goal-directed therapy bundles and activated protein C for septic shock (18), gastric residual volumes to guide enteral feeds (19), standard single syringe size for pediatric inotrope infusions (20), and bowel rest for acute pancreatitis (19). A particularly troubling study demonstrated longer waiting times, slower and less use of thrombolysis, and worse mortality for in-hospital than community onset stroke (21). In the last few months of 2015 alone, additional decades-old practices were called into question. Advanced life support (ALS) may be inferior to basic life support (BLS) for out-of-hospital medical emergencies (22). Calcium supplementation may offer no net benefit for bone mineral density or fracture prevention (23, 24). Hypotonic fluids should no longer be used for maintenance fluids (25). One could understand why the public, and physicians ourselves, are confused about whether doctors actually know anything. Thus, the glass house of medicine requires constant rebuilding, not stones cast from within.

The evidence is consistent and clear on deficiencies in physician empathy, moral judgment, and collegial humility. Is this a work–life issue? Do the compromises we make to our core values at work affect our overall personhood? Indeed, yes. This goes beyond feeling sad about a patient death or being inconvenienced by our hectic work schedules. Much is written about what “you” the individual can do to achieve work–life balance, but how we treat each other at work affects all of our lives. At the end of the day, we cannot dismiss our work ills as easily as doffing our white coat. Some dare go so far as to declare our work problems a matter of life and death. In one of the most thorough studies of its kind, a 20-year prospective observational study of 820 healthy employees examined multiple job stresses and found only one overall independent predictor of mortality: peer social support (26). In other words, the way we treat each other at work affects our ultimate outcome measure for clinical studies, mortality. Fortunately, the problems we have created in our health-care system are something we can correct together. Together, we need to remember who we hoped to become when we wrote our medical school application essays. Together, we can strive to show the compassion we feel inside, allow each other to say the word love, and provide the care we yearn to, for our patients and our coworkers. As we heal our patients, we can also heal each other.

A common theme underlay my medical school, pediatric residency, and pediatric critical care fellowship training: “You never learn so much as when you’re screwed.” Putting medical students and trainees on the spot with pimping and publicly pronouncing negative feedback were embraced. As medical educators, mentors, and colleagues in the modern era we now must teach and behave differently from how we learned. What was once malignant needs to be replaced with understanding and care. Collegiality must replace competition. Expressions of gratitude benefit both the recipient and the person who expresses it (27). Incidentally, the same principles apply to how we treat our families outside of work. The happiest families remember to thank each other (28), and the happiest lives are lived in love (29). Even though physicians’ work–life balance may be dominated by our work, one of our most important clinical responsibilities is to help each other be and live well.

Author Contributions

JL conceived of the idea for this manuscript, conducted the literature review, and drafted the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

ALS, advanced life support; ARDS, acute respiratory distress syndrome; BLS, basic life support; IOM, Institute of Medicine; IV, intravenous.

References

1. Agledahl KM, Førde R, Wifstad A. Clinical essentialising: a qualitative study of doctors’ medical and moral practice. Med Health Care Philos (2010) 13(2):107–13. doi: 10.1007/s11019-009-9193-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Neumann M, Edelhäuser F, Tauschel D, Fischer MR, Wirtz M, Woopen C, et al. Empathy decline and its reasons: a systematic review of studies with medical students and residents. Acad Med (2011) 86(8):996–1009. doi:10.1097/ACM.0b013e318221e615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Starmer AJ, Destino L, Yoon CS, Landrigan CP. Intern and resident workflow patterns on pediatric inpatient units: a multicenter time-motion study. JAMA Pediatr (2015) 169(12):1175–7. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.2471.Author

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Haque OS, Waytz A. Why doctors should be more empathetic – but not too much more. Sci Am (2011). Available from: http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/doctors-and-dehumanization-effect/

Google Scholar

5. Self DJ, Schrader DE, Baldwin DC, Wolinsky FD. The moral development of medical students: a pilot study of the possible influence of medical education. Med Educ (1993) 27(1):26–34. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2923.1993.tb00225.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Self DJ, Baldwin DC. Does medical education inhibit the development of moral reasoning in medical students? A cross-sectional study. Acad Med (1998) 73(10 Suppl):S91–3. doi:10.1097/00001888-199810000-00056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Patenaude J, Niyonsenga T, Fafard D. Changes in students’ moral development during medical school: a cohort study. CMAJ (2003) 168(7):840–4.

Google Scholar

8. Neves Feitosa H, Rego S, Unger Raphael Bataglia P, Castelo Branco Sancho KF, Rego G, Nunes R. Moral judgment competence of medical students: a transcultural study. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract (2013) 18(5):1067–85. doi:10.1007/s10459-013-9449-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Murrell VS. The failure of medical education to develop moral reasoning in medical students. Int J Med Educ (2014) 5:219–25. doi:10.5116/ijme.547c.e2d1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Schroeder J, Fishbach A. The “empty vessel” physician: physicians’ instrumentality makes them seem personally empty. Soc Psychol Personal Sci (2015) 6(8):940–9. doi:10.1177/1948550615597976

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Institute of Medicine. To Err is Human: Building a Safer Health System. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences (1999). 8 p.

Google Scholar

12. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. 2014 National Healthcare Quality and Disparities Report. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (2015). AHRQ Pub No. 15-0007. Available from: http://www.ahrq.gov/research/findings/nhqrdr/nhqdr14/index.html

Google Scholar

13. Rudavsky S. Medical Errors in Indiana Hit Another High: Would You Be Better Off as a Hospital Patient in Indiana or Washington? Indianapolis Star (2015). Available from: http://www.indystar.com/story/news/2015/10/21/medical-errors-report-2015-indiana-state-department-of-health/74342862/

Google Scholar

14. Verma AA, Razak F, Detsky AS. Understanding choice: why physicians should learn prospect theory. JAMA (2014) 311(6):571–2. doi:10.2307/1914185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. McDaniel SH, Morse DS, Reis S, Edwardsen EA, Gurnsey MG, Taupin A, et al. Physicians criticizing physicians to patients. J Gen Intern Med (2013) 28(11):1405–9. doi:10.1007/s11606-013-2499-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Prasad V, Vandross A, Toomey C, Cheung M, Rho J, Quinn S, et al. A decade of reversal: an analysis of 146 contradicted medical practices. Mayo Clin Proc (2013) 88(8):790–8. doi:10.1016/j.mayocp.2013.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Fink MP. Integrated critical care organizations. Crit Care Med (2015) 43(10):2047–8. doi:10.1097/CCM.0000000000001286

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Perner A, Myburgh J. Ten “short-lived” beliefs in intensive care medicine. Intensive Care Med (2015) 41:1703–6. doi:10.1007/s00134-015-3733-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Marik PE. Enteral nutrition in the critically ill. Crit Care Med (2014) 42(4):962–9. doi:10.1097/CCM.0000000000000051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Neal D, Lin JA. The effect of syringe size on reliability and safety of low-flow infusions. Pediatr Crit Care Med (2009) 10(5):592–6. doi:10.1097/PCC.0b013e3181a0e2e9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Saltman AP, Silver FL, Fang J, Stamplecoski M, Kapral MK. Care and outcomes of patients with in-hospital stroke. JAMA Neurol (2015) 72(7):749–55. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.0284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Sanghavi P, Jena AB, Newhouse JP, Zaslavsky AM. Outcomes of basic versus advanced life support for out-of-hospital medical emergencies. Ann Intern Med (2015) 163(9):681–90. doi:10.7326/M15-0557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Bolland MJ, Leung W, Tai V, Bastin S, Gamble GD, Grey A, et al. Calcium intake and risk of fracture: systematic review. BMJ (2015) 351:h4580. doi:10.1136/bmj.h4580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Tai V, Leung W, Grey A, Reid IR, Bolland MJ. Calcium intake and bone mineral density: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ (2015) 351:h4183. doi:10.1136/bmj.h4183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Ingelfinger JR, Moritz ML, Ayus JC. Maintenance intravenous fluids in acutely ill patients. N Engl J Med (2015) 373(14):1350–60. doi:10.1056/NEJMra1412877

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Shirom A, Toker S, Alkaly Y, Jacobson O, Balicer R. Work-based predictors of mortality: a 20-year follow-up of healthy employees. Health Psychol (2011) 30(3):268–75. doi:10.1037/a0023138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Jackowska M, Brown J, Ronaldson A, Steptoe A. The impact of a brief gratitude intervention on subjective well-being, biology and sleep. J Health Psychol (2015). doi:10.1177/1359105315572455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lin G. Where the Mountain Meets the Moon. 2nd ed. New York; Boston: Little, Brown Books for Young Readers (2009). 278 p.

Google Scholar

29. Vaillant GE. Triumphs of Experience: The Men of the Harvard Grant Study. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press (2012). 480 p.

Google Scholar

Keywords: empathy, moral judgment competence, doctor–patient relationship, dehumanization, standard of care, work–life balance

Citation: Lin JA (2016) Stone Throwing in the Glass House. Front. Pediatr. 4:20. doi: 10.3389/fped.2016.00020

Received: 02 November 2015; Accepted: 29 February 2016;
Published: 14 March 2016

Edited by:

Stephania Cormier, University of Tennessee Health Science Center, USA

Reviewed by:

Michele Kong, The University of Alabama at Birmingham, USA

Copyright: © 2016 Lin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: James Anthony Lin, jameslin@mednet.ucla.edu

Download