Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 06 September 2019
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic New Perspectives in Psychopathology View all 29 articles

ERP Evidence for Inhibitory Control Deficits in Test-Anxious Individuals

  • 1Department of Psychology, Nanjing University, Nanjing, China
  • 2Department of Business Administration, School of Business, Anhui University of Technology, Maanshan, China
  • 3Institute for Management Research, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
  • 4Department of Personnel Management, Work and Organizational Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
  • 5Key Laboratory for NeuroInformation of Ministry of Education, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China

Introduction: Individuals with test anxiety [i.e., high test anxiety (HTA)] always treat tests/examinations as a potential threat. This cognitive mode impairs these individuals’ ability of inhibitory control and leads to a high level of anxiety. However, characterizing aspects of HTA’s impaired inhibitory control ability are unclear and need to be studied.

Methods: Forty-six participants were recruited and divided into a HTA (N = 26) and low test anxiety (LTA; i.e., healthy control; N = 20) group. Self-reports (Test Anxiety Scale, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for negative emotions) were obtained. An emotional Stroop (ES) task and a numerical Stroop (NS) task, causing different types of interferences, were used for assessing the emotional and cognitive aspects of attentional control ability (behavioral data). Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were registered to further assess processing stages related to different aspects of attentional control ability.

Results: Compared with the LTA group, the HTA group has inhibitory control deficits of both emotional (see ERP components P1-P2-N2 and P3) and cognitive (see ERP component P3) interference. Compared with the LTA group, the HTA doesn't have lower accuracy in neither ES nor NS but displays longer reaction times only in ES. Additionally, the HTA group’s ES results also show that (1) the degree of emotional interference indicates the level of an individual’s anxiety, and (2) the ERP component P2 may serve as an index of the level of test anxiety.

Conclusion: HTA individuals have extensive inhibitory deficits for both emotional and cognitive aspects; however, impairment impacts more on emotional aspects than on cognitive aspects. Additionally, as compared to NS, the negative impact of more impaired processing stages on task performance is more substantial in ES.

Introduction

Test anxiety is a situation-specific form of anxiety disorder; individuals who suffer from this anxiety disorder tend to appraise performance evaluative situations (e.g., taking an exam) as threatening, and continue to be in high anxiety (1). Test anxiety manifests itself at the individual level in cognitive, affective-physiological, and behavioral characteristics (2). If the individual expects exam results to have great impact (e.g., on the course of his/her life), the exam is perceived as “threatening,” and the individual will display symptoms characterizing high anxiety (3). In most etiology models of anxiety disorders, cognitive evaluations of a (threatening) stimulus (i.e., a “cognitive pattern”) affects the etiology and the maintenance of anxiety in the individual (46).

The cognitive pattern may cause and maintain anxiety through affecting an individual’s ability of attentional inhibitory control. An individual who exerts inhibitory control is able to inhibit task-unrelated information or incorrect automatic responses when reacting to task-related information (7, 8). Worry caused by negative cognition stemming from an exam’s appraisal leads to anxiety and elevates the individual’s vulnerability to anxious cues. This vulnerability increases the degree of interference from exam/test-related information, which is unrelated to completing the task (i.e., task unrelated information), and decreases the ability of inhibitory control (9, 10). So, in comparison to low test anxiety (LTA) individuals, high test anxiety (HTA) individuals tend to show higher interference by test-related information than by neutral (i.e., including test-unrelated) information (1113), an interference that may affect the allocation of attentional resources during the processing of task stimuli (13). More specifically, this interference may reduce the attentional resources used for the task at hand, impairing the individual’s task performance and increasing the level of anxiety in the individual (14, 15).

Attentional control theory assumes that anxiety impacts two dimensions of task performance: (performance) “effectiveness” and (processing) “efficiency” (5). Effectiveness refers to an individual’s “performance quality” usually measured as accuracy in task performance. Efficiency is conceptualized as the level of allocated resources used by an individual to process task stimuli, a conceptualization which is usually measured by the reaction time (RT) (5, 16). Most studies have shown that an inhibitory control deficit in HTA individuals led to detrimental efficiency, and sometimes even to detrimental effectiveness (1315). Whether anxiety impairs the effectiveness may depend on cognitive load (17); when cognitive load is relatively low, anxious individuals apply some compensatory strategies, such as sacrificing on efficiency (causing the RTs to increase), to achieve equal effectiveness as non-anxious individuals [see, for instance, Refs. (1821)].

However, previous evidence on these impairments all stem from emotion-activating experimental conditions in which participants needed to inhibit (test-related) threatening interference (1113). In our view, it is necessary to make a conceptual distinction between (and also examine) different types of interference, namely, emotional and cognitive interference. Two reasons (explained below) make such distinctive conceptualization necessary.

Reason no. 1: Emotional and cognitive interference may reveal different cognitive aspects (i.e., the cognitive pattern) in HTA individuals. Two cognitive aspects are worthwhile mentioning. The first cognitive aspect concerns the impairment of emotional interference (such as test-related threatening interference), which is indicative of a negative cognitive mode related to test-related threatening information (13, 22, 23), including vigilance and further processing of threatening information (24); this first cognitive aspect manifests itself in both an early and a late stage of processing [e.g., Refs. (25, 26)]. The second cognitive aspect concerns the impairment of cognitive interference, which is indicative of an impaired cognitive function, attesting to a difficulty in inhibiting general interference (24); this second cognitive aspect manifests itself in a late stage of processing only (27, 28).

Reason no. 2: These two types of interference may lead to differential task performance. As impaired processing stages go hand in hand with a relatively high cognitive load (5), an impact on task performance is to be expected. The nature of the impact (on cognitive pattern, impaired processing stage, and task performance) is unclear at present. The differentially impaired processing stages involved in emotional and cognitive inhibition (as caused by different types of interference) may subsequently result in a differential impact on task performance.

In the present study, we relied on an emotional Stroop (ES) (29, 30) and a numerical Stroop (NS) (31, 32) paradigm to study the characteristics of inhibitory control ability when HTA individuals are exposed to test-related threatening interference and cognitive interference, respectively. In the Stroop paradigm, the stimulus, which is one word/number, reflects two dimensions at the time: (a) a task-related dimension, and (b) an interfering dimension. Consequently, one may systematically compare the time-course of an ongoing “competition” (for available attentional resources) between the two dimensions, and also assess how exactly different stages of processing are influenced by different types of interference (33). The ES paradigm is instrumental in studying vulnerability to threatening information, which is indicative of a negative mode of threats (3436). Individuals are expected to display longer RTs and/or lower accuracy for threatening words, especially if they experience difficulties in inhibiting affective interference and, due to this inhibition, do not manage to recruit attentional resources (13, 3739). The NS paradigm is instrumental in studying impairment of the cognitive inhibitory function. Individuals are expected to display longer RTs and/or lower accuracy for task-irrelevant aspects (for instance: the one-digit number that is larger in value is depicted in a smaller physical size), especially if they experience difficulties in inhibiting general interference (4044).

Conventional Stroop studies are limited in that they only collect behavioral (i.e., mainly RT) data (45), while the use of event-related potentials (ERPs) in combination with behavioral data can provide many details on the stage in which the processing difficulties occur (46, 47). ERPs are known to have high temporal resolution, and therefore allow adequately “tracking” fast and temporal processing changes (48). Through the analysis of the “magnitude” of very specific ERP components (49), typically for different groups of stimuli (e.g., threatening and neutral stimuli), a researcher may collect clear indications of the nature of processing and processing difficulties encountered. For example, the magnitude of the ERP components P1, P2, and N2 are always examined when analyzing the automatic processing of emotional stimuli. The magnitudes of ERP components P1 and P2 indicate the allocation of attentional resources during the early (especially perceptual) stage of stimulus processing, an allocation that is indicative of the degree of hypervigilance of the stimulus (5053). Additionally, the magnitude of ERP component N2 (especially when followed by perceptual components such as P1 and P2) indicates the automatic and facilitated processing following hypervigilance of the stimulus (50, 51). The magnitude of ERP component P3 is indicative of the cognitive processing of both emotional and cognitive stimuli; P3 is a component that reflects the allocation of attentional resources during a relatively late and cognitive stage of stimulus processing (44, 5052). ERPs are known to change as a response to processing affective (i.e., emotional) stimulus content (rather than non-affective, that is, neutral stimulus content) (5456). In addition, the magnitude and latency of ERP components adequately describe (processing) efficiency, and the activation of a (compensatory) strategy to reduce the effect of impaired efficiency on task performance (17, 28, 55). Therefore, ERPs offer very suitable descriptors of attention paid to stimuli/information in different processing stages, especially for HTA individuals as contrasted to LTA individuals (10, 48).

In sum, the purpose of the present study is to further examine how test-related threatening and cognitive interference impact inhibitory control deficits, processing efficiency, and task performance in HTA (as opposed to LTA) individuals. This examination is based on ES and NS paradigms, but relies, in addition to registering conventional behavioral outcomes (RTs), also on the registration and analysis of ERPs. As such, this study’s design may offer useful insights in the nature of stimulus processing under different conditions of interference. Based on our reasoning presented above, we predict that (a) HTA individuals have both emotional and cognitive inhibitory control deficits; the emotional inhibitory control deficit manifests itself in both the early and late processing stages, while the cognitive inhibitory control deficit manifests itself in the late processing stage only, and (b) for HTA individuals, the different processing stages involved in emotional and cognitive control deficit attest to a more severe impairment of task performance in ES than in NS.

Methods and Materials

Participants and Assignment to the LTA and the HTA Group

Individuals (from now on referred to as “participants”) were recruited through posters or online advertisements. Eighty-two students from Nanjing University signed up for participating in the experiments, and 46 students got eventually selected for participation in the study. According to conventional criteria set for high statistical power (57), we must conclude that our actual sample size combined with the effect sizes as reported in this study leads to high levels of statistical power, that is, the statistical power for each significant difference reported in our (repeated-measures) ANOVA is consistently higher than 98% [1-β > .98, α = .05, using the G-power software (58)]. Participants (aged from 18–26 years; all self-declared right handed) were chosen and assigned to a high test-anxious (HTA) group (n = 26; 12 males; 21.27 ± 1.89 years) and to a low test-anxious (LTA) group (n = 20; 9 males; 21.35 ± 2.96 years; not significantly different across groups: t(1,44) = .113, p = .910) based on their score obtained with the (self-reported) Test Anxiety Scale survey instrument (TAS) (6). Thirty-six students were excluded because (a) they are neither HTA nor LTA, and/or (b) they appeared to have suffered or still suffer from currently known psychiatric disorders (e.g., depression, bipolar disorder, and substance abuse) as diagnosed (just before the start of this study) by a self-completed Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (59). For descriptive purposes also the (self-reported) State-Trait Anxiety Inventory survey instrument (STAI) (60) was administered. STAI was used to unravel the effects of state and trait aspects of test anxiety on interference. Our study was carried out in accordance with a study protocol designed by ourselves, a study protocol that was approved by the Ethics Committee of Nanjing University, School of Social and Behavioral Sciences; the principles on the basis of which the Ethics Committee approves study protocols are described in a document entitled “Ethical Evaluation of Research Projects at the Department of Psychology, School for Social and Behavioral Sciences, Nanjing University.” All study participants gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. In the next paragraphs, details on both survey instruments are given.

Questionnaires

The Chinese version of TAS (6) was administered. The TAS contains 37 survey items reflecting symptoms of test anxiety, all of which are scored using the two true/false answer categories. A participant’s total score ranges from 0 to 37. The higher the participant’s total score, the more test anxiety symptoms are experienced. As documented in other studies (61), the Chinese version of TAS shows a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of .64 and a test-retest reliability coefficient of.61. Participants are assigned to (a) the HTA group if their TAS total score exceeds or equals 20, and (b) assigned to the LTA group if their TAS total score falls below or equals 12 (61, 62).

The Chinese version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) was administered (60). The STAI contains 40 survey items, all of which are scored on a four-point, Likert type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much so). Twenty survey items measure state anxiety (S-STAI) and the other 20 items measure trait anxiety (T-STAI). Obviously, the higher the S-STAI/T-STAI total score, the more symptoms of state-/trait- anxiety are observed, respectively. As documented by Li and Qian (60), the Chinese STAI scale shows a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of .91 for S-STAI and .88 for T-STAI (60). In contrast to TAS, no assessments of test-retest reliability have been published for STAI.

Stimuli, Behavioral Paradigm, and Test Procedure

Participants showed up in the laboratory to participate in the experiment within 1 week after completing TAS and STAI. Participants were connected to a 64-electrode head cap (technical details are given further on) and instructed to relax for 2 min. Then, they either started with the ES or the NS experiment (determined at random) and, after completion, went on with “the other” experiment. Both the ES and NS experiment consisted of a “practice block” (not providing data for analysis) and, subsequently, an “experimental block” (providing data for analysis). When participating in the ES and NS, participants were always asked to finish the tasks “as quickly and accurately as possible.” After completion of both the ES and NS experiment, the electrode head cap was removed and participants got paid the equivalent of about 6 US dollars (i.e., 40 Renminbi).

Emotional Stroop (ES)

The ES task design was similar to that of Thomas et al. (52). Participants were asked to name the color of the word displayed (on screen) regardless of the word’s meaning. The stimuli (words) chosen constitute two experimental conditions (or experimental groups): test-related threatening (e.g., “test paper” and “score”) words and neutral (e.g., “garden” and “shoes”) words. Each experimental condition includes 15 words that were selected based on scores (see Table S1) from a pool of 30 potential neutral/test-related threatening words that were scored by 40 individuals who did not participate in the present study. Words ranged in length from two to four Chinese characters. The words subtended a maximum of 3.81 degrees of visual angle in width. The words subtended 1.27 degrees in height.

Test-related threatening words and neutral words were picked out by following a procedure similar to that of Thomas et al. (52); in particular, they were picked out according to their threatening degree (by asking “How threatening is this word to you [including related unpleasant thoughts and feelings, such as worry or/and anxiety]?”) and test-related degree (by asking “How relevant is this word to “test”)?, and matched for frequency of usage (by asking “How often do you use or see this word?”). A seven point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (severely threatening/strongly relevant/always, respectively) was used. A significant difference exists in the threatening degree (t(28) = 30.190, p < .001) and the test-related degree (t(28) = 38.166, p < .001) between the two groups of words, of course in the expected direction, that is “threatening” words are relatively more threatening and test-related. As far as frequency of usage is concerned, no significant difference exists between the group of threatening words and the group of neutral words (t(28) = 1.436, p = .162).

Two experimental blocks were offered: (a) a practice block, and, afterwards, (b) an experimental block. The practice block contained six trials; the set-up of the practice block is analogous to the experimental block, except for (a) the number of words offered (6 rather than 120 words), and (b) the fact that all words were neutral. All six words offered in the practice block were not used in the experimental block. To enable participants’ learning in the practice block, the message “correct” or “false” was given after each trial (obviously, such message was not given in the experimental block). The experimental block contained 120 trials (every word was offered four times, picked randomly, and the word was displayed 50% of the times in red, and 50% of the times in blue). Each trial started with a fixation point “+” on a computer screen; that fixation point stayed on screen for 200 ms. Next, the screen turned blank and lasted for a time in-between 800 and 1200 ms (duration was randomly picked). Subsequently, the word (randomly picked) was presented in the center of the screen against a white background. This trial ended when (a) the participant reacted (i.e., pushed a button to indicate the color of the word) or (b) failed to react within 2000 ms. In-between consecutive trials, a screen with a white background was shown for a time in-between 1000 and 2000 ms (duration was randomly picked).

Numerical Stroop (NS)

The NS task design was similar to that of previous studies (63, 64). Participants were asked to compare the numerical value (i.e., indicate the larger value) of a pair of two white one-digit numbers positioned left and right against a gray background. In order to reduce the impact of distance in value, one digit was always three units larger in value than the other (1-4 or 4-1; 2-5 or 5-2; 3-6 or 6-3, etcetera) in each pair (65). Which position (left or right) was larger in value was determined at random. The pairs of one-digit numbers constituted three experimental conditions: (a) congruent, (b) incongruent, and (c) neutral. In a congruent pair, the one-digit number showing the larger numerical value was also the larger one in physical size (200 points in physical size and the other one-digit number 140 points in physical size). In an incongruent pair, the one-digit number showing the larger numerical value was the smaller one in physical size, that is, 140 points in physical size, whereas the other one-digit number was 200 points in physical size. In a neutral pair, two digits were of the same physical size (half of the times 140 points and half of the times 200 points). The words subtended almost 3.81 degrees of visual angle in width. The words subtended 1.59 degrees for larger physical size number in height and 0.5 degrees for smaller physical size number in height.

Two experimental blocks were offered: (a) a practice block afterwards, and (b) an experimental block. The practice block contained six trials; the set-up of the practice block is analogous to the experimental block, except for the fact that in the practice block only neutral pairs are offered. Just as in ES, participants were given feedback (correct/false) after each trial in the practice block (but not in the experimental block). The experimental block contained 108 trials (each experimental condition consisted of 36 trials). Each trial started with a fixation point “+” on a computer screen; that fixation point stayed on screen for 200 ms. Next, the screen turned blank and lasted for a time in-between 800 and 1200 ms (duration was randomly picked). Subsequently, the number pair (randomly picked) was presented in the center of the screen against a gray background. This trial ended when the participant gave a reaction (pushing a button to indicate which one-digit number was the larger one in value) or failed to react within 5000 ms. In-between consecutive trials, a screen with a gray background was shown for a time in-between 1000 and 2000 ms (duration was randomly picked).

Electrophysiological Recording and Analysis

A NeuroScan recording system and a 64-electrode head cap designed according to the International 10/20 system formed the technical equipment to collect electrophysiological (EEG) data. The electrode placed on the left mastoid served as a reference during recording. A horizontal electrooculogram (HEOG) was derived from EEG data collected by electrodes placed on the outer canthi of the eyes, and a vertical electrooculogram (VEOG) was derived from EEG data collected by an electrode placed above and below the left eye. The impedances were kept below 5 kOhm. EEG was recorded by a DC model with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz.

Offline analysis of EEG data was enabled through the software “Curry 7.0.8.” EEG data was re-referenced to the average of the left and right mastoids, filtered using a 30 Hz bandwidth (24 dB/octave slope), and corrected for ocular artifacts. To retrieve ERPs from participants’ responses to stimuli, data were epoched from 200 ms pre-stimulus to 1,000 ms post-stimulus, baseline-corrected by 200 ms pre-stimulus, and averaged for all experimental conditions. Interest areas were defined based on two steps. First, the interest areas of related ERP components from previous studies were identified to select specific ERP components for use in the present study; in ES: ERP components P1, P2, N2, and P3 components were selected (5053); in NS: ERP component P3 was selected (44). Second, in the present study, the exact interest areas of each ERP component were identified based on the grand average latency (i.e., the average time intervals between stimulus onset and peak of each condition for each experimental group) determining the middle point of the interest area. Eventually, time windows of each ERP component were, in ES, P1 (120–170 ms), P2 (210–260 ms), N2 (240–290 ms), and P3 (320–370 ms) and, in NS, P3 (neutral: 300–400 ms; congruent: 300–350 ms; incongruent: 420–470 ms). ERP components’ data were analyzed at five electrodes (Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz, and Pz). Mean amplitudes (i.e., mean amplitude of the peaks) and latency of each ERP component were calculated for each condition and for each participant. Per electrode (measuring a component) data on 60 trials in ES and 36 trials in NS were collected for each condition.

Statistical Analysis

TAS and STAI data (i.e., self-reported data) were analyzed using (standard) independent samples t-tests. In agreement with other ERP studies on attention [e.g., Refs. (8, 66)], ES and NS data from (a) erroneous trials (i.e., false answers) and (b) trials with RTs exceeding three standard deviations from the participant’s mean RT (as calculated for the experimental condition) were removed prior to analysis. Also in agreement with previous studies (11, 6769), statistical analyses mainly aimed at examining the statistical significance of between-group (i.e., HTA versus LTA) differences in attentional control ability as observed in given (experimental) conditions, rather than between-condition differences in attentional control ability for HTA individuals or, alternatively, LTA individuals. For behavioral and ERP data, in ES, RT (in milliseconds), accuracy (percent correct, %), and ERP amplitudes data were analyzed using a 2 group (HTA, LTA) × 2 condition (test-related threatening, neutral) repeated-measures ANOVA. In NS, RT, accuracy, and ERP amplitudes data were analyzed using a 2 group (HTA, LTA) × 3 condition (neutral, congruent, incongruent) repeated-measures ANOVA. Both behavioral and ERP data were corrected according to the Greenhouse–Geisser correction after running the ANOVAs. In our correlation analysis, we relied on the Pearson correlation. In all significance testing, we relied on the conventional criterion of p < .05. Significance is reported by reporting the exact significance (format used: “p = …”), unless the p-value is less than.001 (format used: “p < .001”).

Results

TAS and STAI Results

In the present study, TAS shows a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.87 and a test-retest reliability coefficient of 0.74, and STAI shows a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.95 for S-STAI and 0.90 for T-STAI. Descriptive statistics (on TAS and STAI) for both HTA and LTA individuals are shown in Table 1. In line with the expectations, the HTA group displays a higher degree of test anxiety, state anxiety, and trait anxiety than the LTA group.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 The TAS and STAI subscales scores in the HTA and LTA group (M ± SD).

Behavioral Results

Behavioral Results in ES

Mean RTs, accuracy, and stimulus types are shown in Table 2. RT data show a significant condition main effect (F(1,44) = 6.005, p = .018, η2 = .120) and a significant group × condition interaction effect (F(1,44) = 4.447, p = .041, η2 = .092). No significant group main effect was found (F(1,44) = 2.323, p = .135, η2 = .050). Additional simple effect analysis shows that, in contrast to the LTA group (F(1,44) = .052, p = .821, η2 = .001), the HTA group has a longer RT for test-related threatening words than for neutral words (F(1,44) = 11.953, p < .001, η2 = .214).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Emotional Stroop results on the HTA and LTA group: (1) reaction times (RTs, in ms) of test-related threatening words, neutral words, and test-related threatening interference, and (2) accuracy (in %) of test-related threatening and neutral words (M ± SD).

Accuracy data show a significant condition main effect (F(1,44) = .038, p = .846, η2 = .001) and a significant group × condition interaction effect (F(1,44) = 13.833, p < .001, η2 = .239). No significant group main effect was found (F(1,44) = .982, p = .327, η2 = .022). Additional simple effect analysis shows that the LTA group has higher accuracy for neutral words than for test-related threatening words (F(1,44) = 5.492, p = .024, η2 = .111); on the contrary, the HTA group has higher accuracy for test-related threatening words than for neutral words (F(1,44) = 8.814, p = .005, η2 = .167).

A substantial correlation between behavioral data and TAS or STAI scores was found; in particular, a higher accuracy for test-related threatening words is associated with a higher T-STAI total score (r = .344, p = .019).

Behavioral Results in NS

Mean RTs, accuracy, and stimulus types are shown in Table 3. RT data show a significant condition main effect (F(1,43) = 90.210, p < .001, η2 = .672) and a significant group × condition interaction effect (F(1,43) = 5.435, p = .011, η2 = .110). No significant group main effect was found (F(1,43) = 1.165, p = .286, η2 = .026). Additional simple effect analysis shows that (a) for each group, significant RT differences exist between the three conditions (HTA: F(1,43) = 24.225, p < .001, η2 = .530; LTA: F(1,43) = 39.288, p < .001, η2 = .646) and (b) for each condition, no significant RT difference is observed between the HTA and the LTA group (Neutral: F(1,44) = 1.776, p = .090, η2 = .039, Congruent: F(1,44) = 2.294, p = .137, η2 = .050, Incongruent: F(1,44) = .066, p = .799, η2 = .001).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Numerical Stroop results on the HTA and LTA group: (1) reaction times (RTs, in ms) and (2) accuracy (in %) in three experimental conditions (M ± SD).

Accuracy results show a significant condition main effect (F(1,43) = 31.054, p < .001, η2 = .414). No significant group × condition interaction effect (F(1,43) = .141, p = .729, η2 = .003) and group main effect (F(1,43) < .001, p = .996, η2 < .001) is reported.

No substantial correlations between behavioral data and TAS or STAI scores were found.

ERP Results

ERP Results in ES

Grand average ERP waveforms and scalp topographic maps for ES are shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Emotional Stroop ERP waveforms and scalp topographic maps for both the high test-anxious (HTA) and the low test-anxious (LTA) group: (A) grand average ERPs elicited by test-related threatening and neutral words for the HTA and the LTA group at Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz, and Pz sites; P1, P2, and P3 are all registered at site Cz, CPz, and Pz; N2 was registered at site Fz, FCz, and Cz; (B) topographic distribution of the amplitude at the peak latency of the P1, P2, N2, and P3 difference waveforms (i.e., the amplitude emerging from exposure to test-related threatening words minus the amplitude emerging from exposure to neutral words) for both the HTA and the LTA group.

The amplitude of ERP component P1 shows a significant condition main effect (F(1,44) = 4.989, p = .031, η2 = .102) and a significant group × condition interaction effect (F(1,44) = 4.928, p = .032, η2 = .101). No significant group main effect was found (F(1,44) = 2.74, p = .105, η2 = .059; at site Pz). Additional simple effect analysis shows that, in contrast to the LTA group (F(1,44) < .001, p = .993, η2 < .001), the HTA group has a larger P1 amplitude for test-related threatening words than for neutral words (F(1,44) = 11.405, p = .002, η2 = .206).

The amplitude of ERP component P2 shows a significant condition main effect (F(1,44) = 12.696, p = .001, η2 = .224) and a significant group × condition interaction effect (F(1,44) = 5.384, p = .025, η2 = .109). No significant group main effect was found (F(1,44) = .013, p = .909, η2 < .001; at site Cz, CPz, and Pz, and take site Pz as an example for presenting significant results). Additional simple effect analysis shows that, in contrast to the LTA group (F(1,44) = .683, p = .413, η2 = .015), the HTA group has a larger P2 amplitude for test-related threatening words than for neutral words (F(1,44) = 19.903, p < .001, η2 = .311).

The amplitude of ERP component N2 shows a significant condition main effect (F(1,44) = 24.378, p < .001, η2 = .357) and a significant group × condition interaction effect (F(1,44) = 15.989, p < .001, η2 = .267). No significant group main effect was found (F(1,44) = .817, p = .370, η2 = .018; at site Fz, FCz, and Cz, and take site Cz as an example for presenting significant results). Additional simple effect analysis shows that, in contrast to the LTA group (F(1,44) = .390, p = .536, η2 = .009), the HTA group has a smaller N2 amplitude for test-related threatening words than for neutral words (F(1,44) = 45.916, p < .001, η2 = .511).

The amplitude of ERP component P3 shows a significant condition main effect (F(1,44) = 64.656, p < .001, η2 = .595) and a significant group × condition interaction effect (F(1,44) = 27.817, p < .001, η2 = .387). No significant group main effect was found (F(1,44) = .320, p = .574, η2 = .007; at site Cz and Pz, and take site Pz as an example for presenting significant results). Additional simple effect analysis shows that, in contrast to the LTA group (F(1,44) = 3.386, p = .073, η2 = .071), the HTA group has a larger P3 amplitude for test-related threatening words than for neutral words (F(1,44) = 101.943, p < .001, η2 = .699).

No significant results are reported for the latency of the ERP components P1, P2, N2, and P3 (all Fs < 1.126, all ps > .335, all η2s < .06).

Significant correlations between ERP emotional interference (ERP amplitude as registered for the test-related threatening condition minus ERP amplitude as registered for the neutral condition) and TAS or STAI subscales scores are reported (see scatter plots in Figure 2): (a) larger P2 emotional interference is associated with a higher TAS total score (r = .377, p = .010); (b) larger N2 emotional interference is associated with a higher TAS and a higher STAI subscale score (for TAS: r = .366, p = .012; for STAI: rS-STAI = .343, pS-STAI = .020; rT-STAI = .384, pT-STAI = .008); and (c) larger P3 emotional interference is associated with a higher TAS total score and a higher STAI subscale score (for TAS: r = .550, p < .001; for STAI: rS-STAI = .554, pS-STAI < .001; rT-STAI = .546, pT-STAI = < .001).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Scatter plots (including the best fitting linear regression line) describing correlations between P3 emotional interference (ERP amplitude as registered for the test-related threatening condition minus ERP amplitude as registered for the neutral condition) and (A) scores on the Test Anxiety Scale survey instrument (TAS), (B) scores on the State subscale of State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (S-STAI), and (C) scores on the Trait subscale of State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (T-STAI), respectively.

ERP Results in NS

Grand average ERP waveforms and scalp topographic maps for NS are shown in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Numerical Stroop ERP waveforms and scalp topographic maps for both the high test-anxious (HTA) and the low test-anxious (LTA) group: (A) grand average ERPs elicited by the three experimental conditions (neutral, congruent, and incongruent); the P3 component was registered at Cz, CPz, and Pz sites. (B) topographic distribution of the amplitude at the peak latency of the P3 difference waveforms (i.e., the amplitude emerging from exposure to the congruent condition minus the amplitude emerging from exposure to the neutral condition, and the amplitude emerging from exposure to the incongruent condition minus the amplitude emerging from exposure to the neutral condition) for both the HTA and the LTA group.

The amplitude of ERP component P3 shows a significant condition main effect (F(1,44) = 57.740, p < .001, η2 = .568) and a significant group × condition interaction effect (F(1,43) = 3.702, p = .033, η2 = .147). No significant group main effect was found (F(1,43) = .673, p = .417, η2 = .015; site CPz). Additional simple effect analysis shows that (a) in contrast to the LTA group, the HTA group has a different P3 amplitude for three conditions (F(1,43) = 18.929, p < .001, η2 = .468), and (b) in contrast to the neutral condition, the HTA group has a larger P3 amplitude for the congruent condition (p < .001) and the incongruent condition (p = .004).

The latency of ERP component P3 shows a significant condition main effect (F(1,43) = 86.342, p < .001, η2 = .662) and a significant group × condition interaction effect (F(1,43) = 3.784, p = .038, η2 = .079). No significant group main effect was found (F(1,43) = .142, p = .708, η2 = .003). Additional simple effect analysis shows that for each experimental condition, no significant ERP difference exists between the LTA and the HTA group (Neutral: F(1,44) = .433, p = .514, η2 = .010; Congruent: F(1,44) = .510, p = .479, η2 = .011; Incongruent: F(1,44) = .106, p = .746, η2 = .002).

Discussion

Considering both the behavioral and ERP results we suggest that, compared to LTA individuals, HTA individuals show deficits in inhibitory control and show different behavioral and ERP characteristics for different aspects of attentional inhibitory control. For test-related threatening interference, inhibitory deficit is observed in HTA individuals in both the perceptual and the cognitive processing stage, while for cognitive interference, inhibitory deficit is observed in HTA individuals only in the cognitive processing stage. This stage-related difference between two types of interference attest to the existence of different task performance in HTA individuals; more specifically, HTA individuals show more aspects of impaired task performance with lower efficiency (i.e., longer RTs) for test-related threatening interference than that for cognitive interference. Also, having observed no impaired effectiveness of task performance (i.e., accuracy), we suggest that HTA individuals apply a cautious strategy to complete the task in situations in which task performance could be impaired by inhibitory control deficits. Additionally, because of meaningful correlations between questionnaire scales’ scores and ES results, we present the results on questionnaire scales’ scores after the presentation of ES results.

ES Results

In ES, longer RTs for test-related threatening words indicate that (a) HTA individuals show an inhibitory control deficit if they are exposed to a test-related threatening interference [see similar evidence, (1113)] and (b) HTA individuals allocate extra attentional resources to process the threatening information instead of focusing on the task at hand. Additionally, ERP results further indicate that attentional resources are extra allocated in the early and late processing stages.

The Early/Perceptual Processing Stage

Through the study of characteristics of early/perceptual processing, one may appreciate the cognitive mode of emotional disorders. When individuals treat tests/examinations as threatening (i.e., are HTA individuals), emotionally congruent information is (i.e. test-related threatening words are) preferentially processed (70); preferential processing of information implies that the information is easily detected (in the perceptual stage) and processed prior to other information that is presented at the very same time. As the meaning of the word (i.e., the stimulus in ES Stroop) is irrelevant to the experimental task at hand, preferential processing of the emotional significance of the word should be conceived as an extra consumption of attentional resources of the central executive system and makes the nature of processing stimulus-driven rather than goal-driven (11, 18).

In our ERP results related to ES, the larger P1, P2, and smaller N2 amplitudes for test-related threatening words (in comparison to neutral words) in HTA individuals are indicative of a stimulus-driven processing in those individuals. Specifically, the ERP component P1 is known to be sensitive to (i.e., captures) emotional cues (71, 72), and the amplitudes of P1 may reveal hypervigilance (73) and one’s orientation to the information (74). If the information has negative meaning to the individual, a situation may occur in which the individual could easily detect this negative information (i.e., hypervigilance) and allocate extra attentional resources to rapidly orient to this negative information in the perceptual stage; typical in such situation is that P1 amplitudes are increased. The ERP component P2 captures both attention towards stimuli with negative meaning as well as inhibition of the interference (53, 75). If the negative information activates hypervigilance and occupies additional resources of the individual, the individual may attempt to inhibit the interference caused by the processing of this negative information and focus on the task at hand. If making such attempt is too difficult or simply unsuccessful (e.g., because of an inhibitory control deficit), the individual will continue to pay extra attention on processing the negative information, as indicated by increasing P2 amplitudes (52, 76). The ERP component N2 captures changes in attention automatically paid to certain information content (77, 78). Previous studies claimed that early ERP components jointly (e.g., P1, P2, and N2) capture perceptual processing and low-level attentional allocation (79, 80). Relatively small N2 amplitudes followed by relatively large P1 and P2 amplitudes would indicate that both automatic and facilitated processing of negative information are manifest (81, 82). The automatic nature of stimulus-driven processing reflects the cognitive schemata of HTA individuals in which tests/examinations are automatically treated as threatening (Beck’s theory) (83).

The Late/Cognitive Processing Stage

The larger P3 amplitudes for test-related threatening words (in comparison to neutral words) indicate that, in the late/cognitive processing stage, HTA individuals engage in top-down and elaborate processing of threatening stimulus content (78, 84, 85). The parallel distributed processing network model of ES by Mathews and Mackintosh (86) can provide a theoretical framework for understanding inhibitory deficits involved in early and late processing stages in HTA individuals. Information processed in the perceptual stage is monitored by the “threat assessment system.” The threat assessment system works as follows: when relevant threatening information (i.e., test-related threatening word) is presented to an HTA individual, the threatening information is encoded and marked by the threat assessment system; subsequently, semantic content and/or feelings of threat are activated. The way of processing in the early, perceptual stage may affect the way of processing in late, cognitive stage (43, 44). Once stimuli are intense enough, the threat assessment system will recruit attentional resources to process/assess stimulus content and respond to the task at hand. Intense stimuli cause individuals to consume more top-down attentional resources to process stimulus content, and exert greater efforts to inhibit interference.

Additionally, the level of emotional inhibitory control deficit relates to the level of anxiety. Specifically, (a) test-anxiety correlates substantially and positively with the degree of difficulty in inhibiting test-related threatening interference (difference of P2, N2, and P3 amplitudes between the threatening condition and the neutral condition). This positive correlation is in line with our ERP results. Compared to individuals with less symptoms of test anxiety (i.e., LTA individuals), HTA individuals are more strongly affected by test-related threatening stimulus content; in addition, the higher the threat caused by the stimulus, the easier it becomes for HTA individuals to process and detect stimulus content in the early, perceptual stage, followed by the more elaborate processing in the late processing stage; in sum, there are more attentional resources allocated to process the threatening stimulus; (b) the level of trait anxiety correlates substantially and positively with the degree of difficulty in inhibition (see similar results) (21, 87). Not completely unexpected, our study results also demonstrate that HTA individuals have a higher level of state and trait anxiety than LTA individuals. However, the difficulty of inhibition to threat is higher with higher levels of trait anxiety (but not higher with higher levels of state anxiety). Reasoning in a causal way, one could (cautiously) state that especially the trait aspects of test anxiety may impact the impairment of inhibition, whereas the state aspects of test anxiety have far less impact (88); (c) test-related threatening interference of P2 amplitudes is only found to be substantially correlated with TAS, and the correlation is found to be positive. As expressed before, difficulty in inhibition of processing threatening information in the early stage of processing is evident from the P2 component, and is known to be a characteristic element of the cognitive pattern associated with test anxiety (89, 90). This characteristic element signifies that test-related threatening information can be detected and cannot be inhibited. So, the P2 component may serve as a diagnostic for identifying test anxiety.

NS Results

In NS, the behavioral results do not attest to an impairment of cognitive inhibitory control in HTA individuals. However, ERP results do provide evidence that HTA individuals display inhibitory control deficits for cognitive interference. Specifically, the larger P3 amplitudes for the congruent/incongruent condition (in comparison to the neutral condition) indicate that HTA individuals may have cognitive inhibitory control deficits, but such deficits were only encountered in the cognitive processing stage, a clear difference between the NS and ES results. This difference in impaired processing stages between ES and NS results may be an important reason for the difference in behavioral aspects of task performance (especially efficiency, that is, RT) between ES and NS results in HTA individuals. Specifically, the more impaired processing stages in ES (i.e., early and late processing stages), in comparison to NS (i.e., late processing stages only), the more cognitive load is involved in HTA individuals completing the tasks at hand (91). As indicated in the Introduction section, relative high cognitive load may lead to detrimental efficiency (13, 15), so the impaired behavioral aspect of task performance (i.e., longer RT) was observed in ES, but not in NS.

Additionally, in NS, the fact that no inhibitory control deficit was observed in the perceptual processing stage in HTA individuals may be because NS does not include emotional stimuli; as such, hypervigilance and related automatic processing were simply not activated. However, despite the fact that no inhibitory control deficit was observed in the perceptual processing stage, in NS, deficits in cognitive processing were still observed, indicating that HTA individuals may suffer from extensive inhibitory deficits (81). Particularly, HTA individuals were not only found to have difficulty in inhibiting the incongruent interference, an interference that negatively impacts task completion (see similar results) (92, 93), but were also found to have difficulty in inhibiting the congruent interference, an interference that facilitates a quick accomplishment of the task at hand. This difficulty in inhibiting the cognitive interference indicates that the extensive cognitive inhibitory deficits of HTA individuals are merely related to the appearance of interference rather than the exact nature (incongruent vs. congruent) of interference. Besides, we also observed a substantial difference in the latency of P3 among different conditions, a difference that is in line with RT differences observed across these different conditions. This difference in P3 latency may be due to varying difficulty in task completion across these different conditions, and this difference observed is also supported by previous studies (44, 94).

Strategy for Dealing With Deficits in HTA

In contrast to an impaired efficiency observed in ES and NS observed in HTA individuals, no impaired effectiveness (i.e., accuracy) in ES and NS was observed in these individuals. As explained in the Introduction, HTA individuals may apply a “cautious strategy” to achieve satisfactory effectiveness (equal or better accuracy than LTA individuals) by decreasing efficiency (more attentional resources consumed or/and longer RT) (95). Additionally, accuracy of inhibition is known to be associated with the trait referred to as “emotion-driven impulsivity” (96). Individuals who are impulsive, increase the frequency of inhibitory errors (i.e., low accuracy) when they are in an intense emotional state (97, 98). Our study results demonstrate that, during the experiment, HTA individuals do not show behavior that typically characterizes the emotionally driven impulsive trait. When inhibiting interference in an ES and a NS task, HTA individuals make a comparable number of mistakes as LTA individuals, and, when processing threatening words rather than neutral words in ES, HTA individuals make fewer mistakes (i.e., have a higher level of effectiveness, that is, accuracy) than LTA individuals; moreover, for the ES task, a higher level of trait anxiety was found to correspond with fewer mistakes (i.e., higher accuracy), indicating that emotionally driven impulsivity is not a characteristic of HTA individuals when it comes to explaining task performance (measured as accuracy). Instead, HTA individuals who are in an anxious state tend to be “cautious” in that they avoid mistakes in completing the task. Being cautious in such state may, at least partially, explain why test anxiety does not always lead to impaired task performance (especially when assessed in terms of effectiveness; in other words, accuracy).

The present study has some limitations. We got some indications of (possible study restrictions due to) the occurrence of “ceiling effects” in ES and NS. More specifically, accuracy was very high for each condition (above 95%). The occurrence of ceiling effects may be the consequence of two limitations of our present study: (a) low task pressure (or the absence of high task pressure); alternatively, high task pressure (e.g., great importance of task performance) would allow for a better differentiation of participants’ task performance (e.g., accuracy) without imposing study restrictions due to the occurrence of ceiling effects (3). The participants in the present study were not subjected to high task pressure: given low task difficulty, participants could achieve high accuracy (99); and (b) highly educated participants: all HTA and LTA participants are highly educated, so these two groups may both have high accuracy, and this not being different in terms of accuracy (100). Future studies are required to further confirm (or partially disconfirm) our present study findings, for instance, by imposing higher task pressure and recruiting participants from a more heterogeneous (less highly educated) population.

In conclusion, HTA individuals show deficits of inhibitory control that can consume (additional) attentional resources without impairing accuracy. The inhibitory deficits reflect the etiological cognitive pattern of HTA individuals; during task completion and when confronted with interference (emotional or cognitive interference), HTA individuals recruit attentional resources to inhibit the interference. The deficits of inhibitory control can also appear in conditions (e.g., congruent interference) that are beneficial to accomplish the experimental task quickly. A crucial difference in the cognitive pattern of HTA individuals when confronted with emotional versus cognitive interference is that, in comparison to cognitive interference, emotional interference additionally affects the early processing stage and increases the required cognitive load. Thus, in comparison to LTA individuals, HTA individuals have lower behavioral efficiency of emotional interference as opposed to cognitive interference. Furthermore, considering the strong relationship between impaired attentional control ability and test anxiety, future studies aiming at diagnosing test anxiety could consider measuring attentional control ability by relying on the registration of important ERP components (such as P2 in this study) as an alternative method complementing or substituting the diagnosis of test anxiety through a (traditional) self-report questionnaire.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Ethical Evaluation of Research Projects at the Department of Psychology, School for Social and Behavioral Sciences, Nanjing University, Ethics Committee. The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

RZ and WZ contributed the study structure. WZ and LC collected the experimental data. WZ wrote the first draft of the manuscript. RZ and AB made critical revisions. All authors approved the submitted version of this manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the Key Project of Philosophy and Social Science Research in Colleges and Universities in Jiangsu Province (2015JDXM001), the Research and Innovation Foundation of Academic Degree Graduate of Jiangsu Province (KYZZ16_0010), and the Humanity and Social Science Foundation of Education Committee of Anhui province (Key Program) (SK2017A0084).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00645/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Spielberger C, Vagg P. Test anxiety: a transactional process model. In: Series in clinical and community psychology. Test anxiety: theory, assessment, and treatment. US: Taylor & Francis (1995). p. 3–14.

Google Scholar

2. Rydell J. Behavioural variation in echolocation pulses of the northern bat, Eptesicus nilssoni. Ethology (1990) 85:103–13. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1990.tb00390.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Lotz C, Sparfeldt JR. Does test anxiety increase as the exam draws near? – Students’ state test anxiety recorded over the course of one semester. Pers Individ Dif (2017) 104:397–400. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull (2007) 133:1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, Calvo MG. Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion (2007) 7:336–53. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Sarason IG. Anxiety, self-preoccupation and attention. Anxiety Res (1988) 1:3–7. doi: 10.1080/10615808808248215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Brydges CR, Clunies-Ross K, Clohessy M, Lo ZL, Nguyen A, Rousset C, et al. Dissociable components of cognitive control: an event-related potential (ERP) study of response inhibition and interference suppression. PLoS One (2012) 7:3–7. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034482

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Friedman NP, Miyake A. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen (2004) 133:101–35. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Eysenck MW, Derakshan N. New perspectives in attentional control theory. Pers Individ Dif (2011) 50:955–60. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Pergamin-Hight L, Naim R, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, Bar-Haim Y. Content specificity of attention bias to threat in anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev (2015) 35:10–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2014.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kalanthroff E, Henik A, Derakshan N, Usher M. Anxiety, emotional distraction, and attentional control in the Stroop task. Emotion (2016) 16:293–300. doi: 10.1037/emo0000129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Lawson DJ. Test anxiety: a test of attentional bias [Doctoral dissertation] University of Maine (2006).

Google Scholar

13. Putwain DW, Langdale HC, Woods KA, Nicholson LJ. Developing and piloting a dot-probe measure of attentional bias for test anxiety. Learn Individ Differ (2011) 21:478–82. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Chapell MS, Blanding ZB, Silverstein ME, Takahashi M, Newman B, Gubi A, et al. Test anxiety and academic performance in undergraduate and graduate students. J Educ Psychol (2005) 97:268–74. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.268

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Szafranski DD, Barrera TL, Norton PJ. Test anxiety inventory: 30 years later. Anxiety Stress Coping (2012) 25:667–77. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2012.663490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Ansari TL, Derakshan N. The neural correlates of cognitive effort in anxiety: effects on processing efficiency. Biol Psychol (2011) 86:337–48. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Ansari TL, Derakshan N. The neural correlates of impaired inhibitory control in anxiety. Neuropsychologia (2011) 49:1146–53. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Berggren N, Derakshan N. Attentional control deficits in trait anxiety: why you see them and why you don’t. Biol Psychol (2013) 92:440–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Chamberlain SR, Blackwell AD, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2005) 29:399–419. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Derakshan N, Smyth S, Eysenck MW. Effects of state anxiety on performance using a task-switching paradigm: an investigation of attentional control theory. Psychon Bull Rev (2009) 16:1112–7. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.6.1112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Wieser MJ, Pauli P, Mühlberger A. Probing the attentional control theory in social anxiety: an emotional saccade task. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2009) 9:314–22. doi: 10.3758/CABN.9.3.314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Gao X, Zhou RL. On the inhibition of selective attention by people with test anxiety. Chin J Spec Educ (2013) 151:85–91. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1007-3728.2013.01.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Liu Y, Zhang W, Zhou R. Cognitive and neural basis of attentional bias in test anxiety students. Psychol Explor (2015) 35:233–8. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1003-5184.2015.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Mincic AM. Neural substrate of the cognitive and emotional interference processing in healthy adolescents. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) (2010) 70:406–22. doi: 7046 [pii]

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

25. Amir N, Elias J, Klumpp H, Przeworski A. Attentional bias to threat in social phobia: facilitated processing of threat or difficulty disengaging attention from threat? Behav Res Ther (2003) 41:1325–35. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00039-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Buckner JD, Maner JK, Schmidt NB. Difficulty disengaging attention from social threat in social anxiety. Cognit Ther Res (2010) 34:99–105. doi: 10.1007/s10608-008-9205-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Basten U, Stelzel C, Fiebach CJ. Trait anxiety modulates the neural efficiency of inhibitory control. J Cogn Neurosci (2011) 23:3132–45. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Righi S, Mecacci L, Viggiano MP. Anxiety, cognitive self-evaluation and performance: ERP correlates. J Anxiety Disord (2009) 23:1132–8. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Macleod CM. Haifa century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull (1991) 109:163–203. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. MacLeod CM, MacDonald PA. Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends Cogn Sci (2000) 4:383–91. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01530-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Besner D, Coltheart M. Ideographic and alphabetic processing in skilled reading of English. Neuropsychologia (1979) 17:467–72. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(79)90053-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Tzelgov J, Meyer J, Henik A. Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn (1992) 18:166–79. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.1.166

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. TrendsCogn Sci (2000) 4:215–22. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Pury CLS. Information-processing predictors of emotional response to stress. Cogn Emot (2002) 16:667–83. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000400

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Van Bockstaele B, Verschuere B, Tibboel H, De Houwer J, Crombez G. Koster EHW. A review of current evidence for the causal impact of attentional bias on fear and anxiety. Psychol Bull (2014) 140:682–721. doi: 10.1037/a0034834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Verhaak CM, Smeenk JMJ, van Minnen A, Kraaimaat FW. Neuroticism, preattentive and attentional biases towards threat, and anxiety before and after a severe stressor: a prospective study. Pers Individ Dif (2004) 36:767–778. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00131-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Cisler JM, Koster EHW. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review. Clin Psychol Rev (2010) 30:203–16. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Hennessey NW, Dourado E, Beilby JM. Anxiety and speaking in people who stutter: an investigation using the emotional Stroop task. J Fluency Disord (2014) 40:44–57. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2013.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Rutherford EM, MacLeod C, Campbell LW. Negative selectivity effects and emotional selectivity effects in anxiety: differential attentional correlates of state and trait variables. Cogn Emot (2004) 18:711–20. doi: 10.1080/02699930341000121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Girelli L, Lucangeli D, Butterworth B. The development of automaticity in accessing number magnitude. J Exp Child Psychol (2000) 76:104–22. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Kaufmann L, Koppelstaetter F, Delazer M, Siedentopf C, Rhomberg P, Golaszewski S, et al. Neural correlates of distance and congruity effects in a numerical Stroop task: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage (2005) 25:888–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Pinel P, Piazza M, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S. Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgments. Neuron (2004) 41:983–93. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00107-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Szűcs D, Soltész F. Functional definition of the N450 event-related brain potential marker of conflict processing: a numerical stroop study. BMC Neurosci (2012) 13:35. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Szucs D, Soltész F. The interaction of task-relevant and task-irrelevant stimulus features in the number/size congruency paradigm: an ERP study. Brain Res (2008) 1190:143–58. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Mama Y, Ben-Haim MS, Algom D. When emotion does and does not impair performance: a Garner theory of the emotional Stroop effect. Cogn Emot (2013) 27:589–602. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.726212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Edwards MS, Burt JS, Lipp OV. Selective attention for masked and unmasked threatening words in anxiety: effects of trait anxiety, state anxiety and awareness. Behav Res Ther (2010) 48:210–8. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Mangun GR, Hillyard SA. Mechanisms and models of selective attention. In: electrophysiology of mind (1995) 40–86. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524168.003.0003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Albert J, López-Martín S, Carretié L. Emotional context modulates response inhibition: neural and behavioral data. Neuroimage (2010) 49:914–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Luck SJ, Woodman GF, Vogel EK. Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends Cogn Sci (2000) 4:432–40. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01545-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Fisher JE, Sass SM, Heller W, Silton RL, Edgar JC, Stewart JL, et al. Time course of processing emotional stimuli as a function of perceived emotional intelligence, anxiety, and depression. Emotion (2010) 10:486–97. doi: 10.1037/a0018691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Gootjes L, Coppens LC, Zwaan RA, Franken IHA, Van Strien JW. Effects of recent word exposure on emotion-word Stroop interference: an ERP study. Int J Psychophysiol (2011) 79:356–63. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Thomas SJ, Johnstone SJ, Gonsalvez CJ. Event-related potentials during an emotional Stroop task. Int J Psychophysiol (2007) 63:221–31. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. van Peer JM, Spinhoven P, Roelofs K. Psychophysiological evidence for cortisol-induced reduction in early bias for implicit social threat in social phobia. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2010) 35:21–32. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Carretié L, Martín-Loeches M, Hinojosa JA, Mercado F. Emotion and attention interaction studied through event-related potentials. J Cogn Neurosci (2001) 13:1109–28. doi: 10.1162/089892901753294400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Dennis TA, Chen CC. Trait anxiety and conflict monitoring following threat: an ERP study. Psychophysiology (2009) 46:122–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00758.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Gu R, Lei Z, Broster L, Wu T, Jiang Y, Luo Y. Beyond valence and magnitude: a flexible evaluative coding system in the brain. Neuropsychologia (2011) 49:3891–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Routledge (1988). doi: doi.org/10.4324/9780203771587

Google Scholar

58. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. GPOWER: a general power analysis program. Behav Res Methods (2007) 39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. First MB, Gibbon M. The structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders (SCID-I) and the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis II disorders (SCID-II). In: Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment, Vol. 2. Personality assessment. John Wiley & Sons Inc. (2004). p. 134–43.

Google Scholar

60. Li W, Qian M. Revision of the state-trait anxiety inventoty with sample of Chinese college students. Acta Scicentiarum Nat Univ Pekinesis (1995) 31:108–14. doi: 10.13209/j.0479-8023.1995.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Wang C. Reliability and validity of test anxiety scale-Chinese version. Chin Ment Heal J (2001) 15:96–7. doi: 10.3321/j.issn:1000-6729.2001.02.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Newman E. No more test anxiety: Effective steps for taking tests and achieving better grades. Los Angeles: Learning Skills Publications (1996)

Google Scholar

63. Liu C, Yao R, Wang Z, Zhou R. N450 as a candidate neural marker for interference control deficits in children with learning disabilities. Int J Psychophysiol (2014) 93:70–7. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Soltész F, White S, Szucs D. Event-related brain potentials dissociate the developmental time-course of automatic numerical magnitude analysis and cognitive control functions during the first three years of primary school. Dev Neuropsychol (2011) 36:682–701. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Soltész F, Goswami U, White S, Szucs D. Executive function effects and numerical development in children: behavioural and ERP evidence from a numerical Stroop paradigm. Learn Individ Differ (2011) 21:662–71. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Choi JM, Padmala S, Pessoa L. Impact of state anxiety on the interaction between threat monitoring and cognition. Neuroimage (2012) 59:1912–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Dresler T, Attar CH, Spitzer C, Löwe B, Deckert J, Büchel C, et al. Neural correlates of the emotional Stroop task in panic disorder patients: an event-related fMRI study. J Psychiatr Res (2012) 46:1627–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Dieter J, Hoffmann S, Mier D, Reinhard I, Beutel M, Vollstädt-Klein S, et al. The role of emotional inhibitory control in specific internet addiction – an fMRI study. Behav Brain Res (2017) 324:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.01.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Ashley V, Honzel N, Larsen J, Justus T, Swick D. Attentional bias for trauma-related words: exaggerated emotional Stroop effect in Afghanistan and Iraq war veterans with PTSD. BMC Psychiatry (2013) 13:86. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-13-86

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Moriya J, Tanno Y. Relationships between negative emotionality and attentional control in effortful control. Pers Individ Dif (2008) 44:1348–55. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Eldar S, Bar-Haim Y. Neural plasticity in response to attention training in anxiety. Psychol Med (2010) 40:667. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Santesso DL, Meuret AE, Hofmann SG, Mueller EM, Ratner KG, Roesch EB, et al. Electrophysiological correlates of spatial orienting towards angry faces: a source localization study. Neuropsychologia (2008) 46:1338–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Mueller EM, Hofmann SG, Santesso DL, Meuret AE, Bitran S, Pizzagalli DA. Electrophysiological evidence of attentional biases in social anxiety disorder. Psychol Med (2009) 39:1141. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Morel S, George N, Foucher A, Chammat M, Dubal S. ERP evidence for an early emotional bias towards happy faces in trait anxiety. Biol Psychol (2014) 99:183–92. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Rossignol M, Campanella S, Bissot C, Philippot P. Fear of negative evaluation and attentional bias for facial expressions: an event-related study. Brain Cogn (2013) 82:344–52. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Glickman S. Attentional bias in anxiety: a behavioral and ERP study. Brain Cogn (2005) 59:11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Yao Y, Du F, Wang C, Liu Y, Weng J, Chen F. Numerical processing efficiency improved in children using mental abacus: ERP evidence utilizing a numerical Stroop task. Front Hum Neurosci (2015) 9:245. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Atchley R, Klee D, Memmott T, Goodrich E, Wahbeh H, Oken B. Event-related potential correlates of mindfulness meditation competence. Neuroscience (2016) 320:83–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.01.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Telles S, Deepeshwar S, Naveen KV, Pailoor S. Long latency auditory evoked potentials during meditation. Clin EEG Neurosci (2015) 46:299–309. doi: 10.1177/1550059414544737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Pérez-Edgar K, Fox NA. Individual differences in children’s performance during an emotional Stroop task: a behavioral and electrophysiological study. Brain Cogn (2003) 52:33–51. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00007-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Kóbor A, Takács Á, Bryce D, Szucs D, Honbolygó F, Nagy P, et al. Children with ADHD show impairments in multiple stages of information processing in a Stroop task: an ERP study. Dev Neuropsychol (2015) 40:329–47. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2015.1086770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. O’Toole L, Dennis TA. Attention training and the threat bias: an ERP study. Brain Cogn (2012) 78:63–73. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Beck AT, Clark DM. An information processing model of anxiey: automatic and strategic processes. Behav Res Ther (1997) 35:49–58. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00069-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Jo HG, Schmidt S, Inacker E, Markowiak M, Hinterberger T. Meditation and attention: a controlled study on long-term meditators in behavioral performance and event-related potentials of attentional control. Int J Psychophysiol (2016) 99:33–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Fan Y, Tang Y-Y, Tang R, Posner MI. Time course of conflict processing modulated by brief meditation training. Front Psychol (2015) 6:911. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00911

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Mathews A, Mackintosh B. A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Therapy (1998) 22:539–60. doi: 10.1023/A:1018738019346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Savostyanov AN, Tsai AC, Liou M, Levin EA, Der Lee J, Yurganov AV, et al. EEG-correlates of trait anxiety in the stop-signal paradigm. Neurosci Lett (2009) 449:112–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.10.084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Osinsky R, Gebhardt H, Alexander N, Hennig J. Trait anxiety and the dynamics of attentional control. Biol Psychol (2012) 89:252–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Calvo MG, Eysenck MW, Castillo MD. Interpretation bias in test anxiety: the time course of predictive inferences. Cogn Emot (1997) 11:43–64. doi: 10.1080/026999397380023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. van den Heuvel OA, Veltman DJ, Groenewegen HJ, Witter MP, Merkelbach J, Cath DC, et al. Disorder-specific neuroanatomical correlates of attentional bias in obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic disorder, and hypochondriasis. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2005) 62:922. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.8.922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Junhong H, Renlai Z, Senqi H. Effects on automatic attention due to exposure to pictures of emotional faces while performing Chinese word judgment tasks. PLoS One (2013) 8:e75386. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Grandjean J, D’Ostilio K, Fias W, Phillips C, Balteau E, Degueldre C, et al. Exploration of the mechanisms underlying the ISPC effect: evidence from behavioral and neuroimaging data. Neuropsychologia (2013) 51:1040–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Tillman CM, Wiens S. Behavioral and ERP indices of response conflict in Stroop and flanker tasks. Psychophysiology (2011) 48:1405–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01203.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Osinsky R, Alexander N, Gebhardt H, Hennig J. Trait anxiety and dynamic adjustments in conflict processing. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2010) 10:372–81. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.3.372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Bettina O, Kingstone A, Olk B, Kingstone A. Why are antisaccades slower than prosaccades ? A novel ¢ nding using a new paradigm. October (2003) 14:1–5. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000053062.88427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Fernández-Serrano MJ, Perales JC, Moreno-López L, Pérez-García M, Verdejo-García A. Neuropsychological profiling of impulsivity and compulsivity in cocaine dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2012) 219:673–83. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2485-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Lawrence AJ, Luty J, Bogdan NA, Sahakian BJ, Clark L. Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2009) 207:163–72. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1645-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Marczinski CA, Abroms BD, Van Selst M, Fillmore MT. Alcohol-induced impairment of behavioral control: differential effects on engaging vs. disengaging responses. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2005) 182:452–9. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0116-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Keng SL, Smoski MJ, Robins CJ. Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: a review of empirical studies. Clin Psychol Rev (2011) 31:1041–56. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Kozasa EH, Sato JR, Lacerda SS, Barreiros MAM, Radvany J, Russell TA, et al. Meditation training increases brain efficiency in an attention task. Neuroimage (2012) 59:745–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: test anxiety, inhibitory control, emotional Stroop (ES), numerical Stroop (NS), ERPs

Citation: Zhang W, De Beuckelaer A, Chen L and Zhou R (2019) ERP Evidence for Inhibitory Control Deficits in Test-Anxious Individuals. Front. Psychiatry 10:645. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00645

Received: 19 June 2018; Accepted: 08 August 2019;
Published: 06 September 2019.

Edited by:

Diogo Telles-Correia, University of Lisbon, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Alexander Prehn-Kristensen, Zentrum für Integrative Psychiatrie (ZIP), Germany
Wei Xu, Nanjing Normal University, China
Scott Meier, University at Buffalo, United States

Copyright © 2019 Zhang, De Beuckelaer, Chen and Zhou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Renlai Zhou, rlzhou@nju.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.