Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 30 October 2019
Sec. Aging Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic Prevention of Alzheimer’s Disease: from Cognitive Reserve to Precision Medicine View all 11 articles

A Systematic Review of Evidence for a Role of Rest-Activity Rhythms in Dementia

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 2Department of Epidemiology, Graduate School of Public Health, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 3Veterans Administration, Veterans Integrated Service Network 4, Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center of Excellence, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 4Department of Biostatistics, Graduate School of Public Health, University of Pittsburgh, PA, United States

Background: Rest-activity rhythm (RAR) disruption may be a risk factor for dementia that can be objectively measured with wearable accelerometers. It is possible that risk monitoring and preventive interventions could be developed targeting RARs. To evaluate whether current evidence supports these applications, we systematically reviewed published studies linking RARs with dementia, its course, and mechanisms.

Methods: Entering pre-defined search terms in PsycINFO, MEDLINE, and PubMed databases returned 192 unique titles. We identified 32 articles that met our primary inclusion criteria, namely, that they examined objective RAR measures in the context of dementia, cognition, or brain biomarkers.

Results: Cross-sectional studies consistently found that people with dementia had less stable (5/6 studies), more fragmented (4/6 studies), lower amplitude rhythms (5/5 studies), that had a worse fit to 24-h models (3/3 studies). Findings from studies relating RARs to cognitive test performance (rather than diagnostic status) were more nuanced. RAR fragmentation was associated with neurodegeneration biomarkers in 2/2 studies; and 1/1 study found 24-h model fit related to hippocampal hyperactivation. Although 2/2 studies found RARs related to markers of cerebrovascular disease, the specific RARs and cerebrovascular disease measures were not consistent. Longitudinal studies (3/3 articles) reported that lower amplitude and worse 24-h rhythm fit predicted future cognitive impairment and executive function. However, interventions aimed at modifying RARs had mixed effects (e.g., 0/4 studies demonstrated effects of morning light on 24-h model fit; evening light was associated with improved 24-h fit in 2/2 studies reporting); these effects may be more evident in subgroups.

Conclusions: Consistent evidence shows that dementia is associated with disrupted RARs. Importantly, recent studies have shown that RAR disruption is associated with dementia biomarkers and, prospectively, with the risk of cognitive impairment. Interventions mostly tried using bright light to modify RARs in people who already have dementia; these studies produced modest effects on RARs and did not show modification of dementia’s course. Altogether, these findings suggest studies are needed to understand how RARs relate to changes in brain health earlier in the disease process. Better understanding of the biopsychosocial mechanisms linking RARs with future dementia risk can help further target intervention development.

Introduction

The first case report documenting rest-activity rhythm (RAR) disruption in people with dementia was published in 1986 (1). This confirmed observations of clinicians and caregivers but had the advantage of objectively specifying the activity patterns that characterized people with dementia. RAR characteristics including regularity, fragmentation, amplitude, and fit to 24-h models can be objectively quantified by applying a variety of methods to several days of continuous accelerometer recordings (see brief description of RAR methods in Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Commonly used rest-activity rhythm (RAR) measures. The top row represents high values on the given metric and the bottom row represents low values. From left to right: note stability, measured as nonparametric inter-daily stability, indicates how much the typical 24-h profile varies from day to day; fragmentation, measured as nonparametric intra-daily variability, indicates the frequency and extent of transitions in activity levels; 24-h model fit, from an extended cosine model (see line) pseudo-F statistic, indicates how well the data fits to a 24-h model; amplitude, derived from an extended cosine model, indicates rhythm height. More details on these common metrics are provided in the reviewed articles and elsewhere, e.g., see the study by Smagula (2).

Almost three decades later, literature on relationships of RARs with dementia and cognition continues to grow. Published research currently includes observational, biomarker, and intervention studies (Figure 2). Now that consumer-friendly accelerometer technology is increasingly available, clear evidence for a role of RARs in dementia’s course could lead to useful new clinical applications. Specifically, risk stratification approaches could be designed to detect RAR characteristics that mark or hasten dementia risk. Preventive interventions could also be developed targeting RARs to protect brain health and cognition.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Number of articles identified by year and study design.

But, the likelihood that these clinical applications will succeed, and what specifically they should target, depends on knowing 1) what aspects of RARs affect the course of cognitive aging and its underlying mechanisms, 2) whether these aspects of RARs are modifiable, and 3) if doing so improves cognitive aging. To identify gaps between current knowledge and potential clinical applications of wearable technology targeting RARs, we systematically reviewed evidence relating RARs, brain health, and cognition.

Methods

Target article inclusion criteria: We included articles that reported results from data-based human subject research studies with more than 20 participants (i.e., we excluded review articles, animal studies, small pilot studies, and case reports). In addition, studies were included only if they examined cognition, a biomarker related to cognition in aging, Alzheimer disease, or a related dementia (except for Parkinson’s due to differences in the biological mechanisms and low number of Parkinson’s articles). To be included, articles were required to have quantified 24-h RAR variable(s) from objectively measured activity time series data. We only included articles that were written in English.

Article search and selection process (Figure 3): We searched PsycINFO, MEDLINE, and PubMed databases using pre-specified terms [(“sleep-wake rhythm” or “rest-activity rhythm” or “activity rhythm”)) AND (“Alzheimer’s” or “dementia” or “cognition” or “cognitive” or “neurodegenerative” or “cerebrovascular”)]. To minimize bias and errors in article selection, two raters (SFS and CC) independently reviewed the title, abstract, and text (as needed) of the 192 returned articles to determine if they met article inclusion criteria. Rating discrepancies were minimal and resolved via consensus discussion referencing the article’s text. Returned articles were excluded because 97 were not the correct article type, 47 did not have a cognitive or relevant biomarker outcome, 13 did not examine objectively quantified RARs, 6 were not in English, and 1 reported the same results in two studies. Next, to minimize bias and errors, two raters (SFS and SG) reviewed each article and recorded selected summary information (see Supplemental Tables). Finally, the frequency and consistency of statistical results were then summarized across design, exposures, and outcome factors.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Search process and article selection.

Results

The search process outlined above returned 28 articles, meeting our inclusion criteria. Four additional articles were identified from their reference lists. Most studies used cross-sectional observational designs (n = 18) and five of these included biomarkers. Intervention studies were also common (n = 11). Few studies used longitudinal observational designs (n = 3).

Cross-sectional studies(Table 1): Consistent evidence demonstrated that people with dementia had less stable rhythms (5/6 studies 38). For example, the recent work by Saito et al. (5) demonstrated that, among nursing home residents, those with dementia had lower IS values (reflecting less stability across days). The one study (8) that failed to detect an overall statistically significant overall association between dementia and RAR stability did find stability was lower among people with dementia who were living in institutional settings (compared with community dwelling people with dementia and healthy controls); the sample of people with Alzheimer’s disease living independently in this study was small (n = 8).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Summary of evidence relating RAR and cognitive measures.

In addition, 4/6 studies (positive studies (3, 4, 7, 9 and nonsignificant studies 5, 8) found that cognitive impairment was related to higher RAR fragmentation. For example, the early work by Wittin et al. (3) demonstrated that Alzheimer disease patients had higher intra-daily variability, reflecting more fragmented (less consistent) rhythms. All of the studies examining goodness-of-fit and amplitude measures found that cognitive impairment was associated with less robust rhythm fits and lower amplitude rhythms (3/3 4, 6, 12 and 5/5 4, 6, 7, 14, 15, respectively).

Findings regarding associations of RARs and cognitive performance have been more nuanced. Both Luik et al. (9) and Oosterman et al. (10) showed that RAR fragmentation related to processing speed and executive function measures; furthermore, Luik et al. (9) found that the association of fragmentation and executive function was stronger among older compared with younger participants. Luik et al. (9) also found stability related to these cognitive measures, but Oosterman et al. (10) did not. Gehrman et al. (13) did not find an association between rhythm model fit and global dementia severity.

Biomarker studies (Table 2): Only five studies investigated RARs and dementia biomarkers. Three studies assessed markers of neurodegenerative processes (2123: 1) Musiek et al. (22) found fragmentation related to markers of amyloid deposition; 2) Van Someren (21) reported that greater fragmentation correlated with lower medial temporal lobe volume better than any other risk factor studied; and 3) Sherman et al. (23) noted a relationship between 24-h rhythm fit and memory performance that was statistically mediated by anterior hippocampal hyperactivation during an associative memory task.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Summary of evidence relating RAR and neurobiological measures.

Two studies linked RAR measures with cerebrovascular disease markers. Oosterman et al. (20) found that white matter lesions were related to RAR stability, but Zuurbier et al. (19) did not; however, note that, while Oosterman et al. found associations in occipital periventricular and frontal regions, but not elsewhere, Zuurbier et al. examined only whole brain white matter lesion volume and not regional levels. Zuurbier et al. (19) found an association of RAR fragmentation with overall white matter lesion burden, and while Oosterman et al. did not (20), correlations of RAR fragmentation with white matter lesion volumes were similar in both studies (r = 0.10–0.12 in the study by Oosterman et al. (20) and 0.10 in the study by Zuurbier et al. (19); sample sizes differed substantially: 135 (20) and 970 (19).

Longitudinal studies (Table 1): In both studies reporting (16, 17), lower rhythm robustness (24-h model goodness-of-fit measure) and amplitude were associated with future cognitive decline. In terms of changes in cognitive test performance, Walsh et al. (18) found that lower robustness and amplitude were associated with executive function test performance 5 years later; in this study, the association of amplitude, but not robustness, was independent of other health characteristics (e.g., BMI, medical morbidities, medication use, and physical activity levels). Note that these prospective studies all included over 1,000 participants.

Intervention studies (Table 3): An early report noted that unattended daytime bright light has beneficial effects on RAR stability and fragmentation among patients with intact vision (31). However, additional studies of bright light interventions have shown modest or no effects on rhythms in people with dementia. Regarding the strength of intervention evidence, note (Supplemental Table 4) that published studies almost always focused on pre-post effects (rather than whether pre-post effects differed by randomized group) and always had follow-up periods of less than 3 months; therefore, these interventions were all considered studies with “level II” evidence strength according to previously published criteria (30). Two studies (27, 28) did report controlled results. That said, the intervention studies have shown:

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Summary of light interventions targeting RARs.

● Morning bright light did not have statistically significant effects on RAR stability (25), fragmentation (25), 24-h model fit (2427)| and amplitude (2426). Among people classified as having aberrant rhythms at baseline, however, Dowling et al. (25) observed that morning light was associated with increases in stability and a trend towards increased amplitude. One controlled study (28) found that morning bright light plus melatonin has positive effects on 24-h model fit and amplitude.

● Afternoon light had no effects on 24-h model fit and amplitude in one study reporting controlled results (27).

● Evening light did have significant pre-to-post intervention effects on 24-h model fit in the two studies reporting (24, 26), no effects on amplitude were observed.

A few reports examined effects of other intervention approaches. Two studies (29, 32) using peripheral electrical stimulation did not have effects on stability or fragmentation. Moving to a small-scale care facility (33) and a hand movement intervention (34) also did not have statistically significant effects on RARs. Finally, one study found that 2 weeks of institutional care lead to a dampening (relative amplitude reduction) of RARs that recovered after returning home (35).

Discussion

In summary, this systematic review of available evidence found consistent evidence that RAR disruption is more common in people with dementia. New evidence from large studies shows that RAR disruption predicts future cognitive decline. Evidence for these associations is strong, given the consistency of results across studies and the wide range of sample sizes and populations examined. Recently, biomarker studies have shown RAR disruption relates to the neurobiological hallmarks of dementia; however, as discussed below, these studies have been in relatively small cross-sectional samples and therefore cannot infer temporality in these relationships. Finally, note that several studies investigated whether light interventions modify RAR disruption or the disease course in people who already have dementia. These studies did not consistently show light interventions modify RARs; the most promising findings were from two studies using evening light (24, 26), one study using a combination of morning light and melatonin (28), and when analyses restricted to patient subgroups (those with intact vision (23 and RAR disruption at baseline 25).

The largest and most consistent evidence indicates that RAR disruption is more common in people who have dementia. Every study identified found that people with dementia have lower activity levels (reflected in RAR amplitude measures) (4, 6, 7, 14, 15), and most studies found that people with dementia have more fragmented (3, 4, 7, 11) less stable (38) rhythms, that fit a 24-h pattern relatively poorly (4, 6, 12). While these past studies were limited by their cross-sectional design, evidence from recent large prospective cohort studies have shown that RARs disruption also predicts future cognitive function (in people free of dementia at study baseline) (16, 17). However, it is not yet clear whether RAR disruption relates to dementia and dementia risk as a pathogenic contributor or as a downstream marker of existing pathology.

When reaching the severity that typically mandates institutional care (the setting where most RAR research began), people with dementia have already experienced significant neurodegeneration and likely have cerebrovascular disease (36). This macrostructural damage can affect the neural circuits that control circadian rhythms including the suprachiasmatic nucleus (37). Thus, it is not surprising that people with dementia frequently exhibit RAR disruption. Advanced-stage pathology could also explain why, in cases of overt dementia, increasing exogenous signals to the circadian system via light produces mixed or no effects (e.g., light may be inputting on already-damaged circadian circuits that have little remaining control effects).

Furthermore, recent studies have shown that RAR characteristics are associated with the neurobiological biological hallmarks of dementia early in the disease process (i.e., in community and pre-clinical samples) including amyloid deposition (22), medial temporal lobe atrophy (21), and cerebrovascular disease (although associations may be region specific) (19, 20). In addition, the study by Sherman et al. showed that RAR disruption related to hippocampal hyperactivation (23), which is considered a component of pre-clinical dementia pathophysiology related to early memory decline (e.g., see the study by Smagula et al. (38). In this context, findings from longitudinal epidemiological studies (showing RAR disruption predicts future cognitive decline) might be due to RARs already reflecting early-stage neuropathology (which was not controlled for in existing longitudinal studies). Thus, available evidence does not resolve whether RAR disruption hastens the progression of, or simply reflects, early-stage pathology to dementia.

However, there are other sources of evidence that suggest rhythm disruption can contribute to dementia etiology; for example, an animal model of circadian misalignment affects neuronal structure (39), and chronic jet-lag (disrupting natural rhythms) is associated with reduced temporal lobe volume (40). Future prospective observational studies can be designed to determine whether RARs reflect or affect the pathogenesis of dementia. If RAR fragmentation in everyday human life uniquely contributes to the progression of amyloid pathology and neurodegeneration, then interventions aimed at modifying RAR fragmentation would be warranted. Alternatively, if RARs do not contribute to the progression of dementia above and beyond pre-existing neurobiological pathology, then interventions modifying RARs are less likely to succeed (although measuring RARs might still be useful for risk stratification).

Thus, longitudinal and intervention studies are needed to place associations of RARs and brain health in greater context. Apparent discrepancies in available literature may be because the role of RAR differs by age, life stage, or pre-existing pathology (which vary across study samples). It is not clear why associations between RAR fragmentation and cognition are stronger in older subsets of the sample in the study of Luik et al. (9) The meaning or role of RAR stability might differ depending on life stage (e.g., pre- or post- retirement) or other factors like the presence of cardiovascular risk factors, sedentary behavior, or poor sleep. Differences in these factors across samples could explain why Luik et al. (community sample; sample average age = 59) (9) found stability related to cognitive test performance, but Oosterman et al. (clinic-based sample; average age = 69) (10) did not. Future longitudinal studies are needed to identify the modifiable factors that lead to RAR disruption.

Psychosocial and physical activity have been associated with stronger RARs in people with dementia (41). This suggests that boosting or maintaining activity participation would be a logical approach to modify RARs. But, there remains a need to clarify which, of several inter-related risk factors (RARs, physical activity, social activities, and sleep), have the most potent and unique affects on brain health, and at what point in life. Our review of existing evidence suggests that interventions to curtail effects of RARs on brain health may require targeting specific subgroups (25, 31) and/or using multiple approaches (28).

Conclusion

Decades of research has produced solid evidence that people with dementia are more likely to exhibit RAR disruption. Recent studies have demonstrated that, even in pre-clinical samples, RARs relate to early markers of dementia neurobiology and may increase dementia risk. Several questions remain to be answered. Different measures of RAR robustness appear related to dementia neurobiology and risk, but future research is needed to identify the specific aspects of RARs that best signal these processes. Furthermore, it remains unclear how relationships between RARs and brain health play out time, what leads to RAR disruption, and whether modifying RARs at different stages in disease pathogenesis could prevent dementia. Available evidence supports the idea of promoting healthy RARs to prevent dementia, but doing so may require intervening early in disease pathogenesis and using multiple approaches (e.g., scheduling activity, bright light, and sleep interventions).

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/Supplementary Material.

Author Contributions

SS and RK conceptualized the project and literature search. SS, CC, and SG performed the search and tabulated the results. SS drafted the manuscript, and all authors critically revised the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by K01MH112683 (to SS) and R01GM113242 (to RK). SG is supported by a VA VISN 4 MIRECC Competitive Pilot Grant and a Veterans Research Foundation Gerald Goldstein Early Career Mental Health Research Award. The authors also acknowledge Annemarie Luik, PhD, for providing helpful feedback on the content of this manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00778/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Teicher MH, Lawrence JM, Barber NI, Finklestein SP, Lieberman H, Baldessarini RJ. Altered locomotor activity in neuropsychiatric patients. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (1986) 10(6):755–61. doi: 10.1016/0278-5846(86)90061-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Smagula SF. Opportunities for clinical applications of rest-activity rhythms in detecting and preventing mood disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2016) 29(6):389–96. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Wittin W, Kwa I, Eikelenboom M, Mirmiran M, Swaab D. Alterations in the circadian rest-activity rhythm in aging and Alzheimer's disease. Biol. Psychiatry (1990) 27:563–72. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(90)90523-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Harper DG, Stopa EG, McKee AC, Satlin A, Harlan PC, Goldstein R, et al. Differential circadian rhythm disturbances in men with Alzheimer disease and frontotemporal degeneration. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2001) 58(4):353–60. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.4.353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Saito Y, Kume Y, Kodama A, Sato K, Yasuba M. The association between circadian rest-activity patterns and the behavioral and psychological symptoms depending on the cognitive status in Japanese nursing-home residents. Chronobiol Int (2018) 35(12):1670–9. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2018.1505752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Satlin A, Volicer L, Stopa EG, Harper D. Circadian locomotor activity and core-body temperature rhythms in Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. (1995) 16(5):765–71. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(95)00059-N

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Hatfield CF, Herbert J, van Someren EJ, Hodges JR, Hastings MH. Disrupted daily activity/rest cycles in relation to daily cortisol rhythms of home-dwelling patients with early Alzheimer's dementia. Brain. (2004) 127(Pt 5):1061–74. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. van Someren EJ, Hagebeuk EE, Lijzenga C, Scheltens P, de Rooij SE, Jonker C, et al. Circadian rest-activity rhythm disturbances in Alzheimer's disease. Biol. Psychiatry (1996) 40(4):259–70. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00370-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Luik AI, Zuurbier LA, Hofman A, Van Someren EJ, Ikram MA, Tiemeier H. Associations of the 24-h activity rhythm and sleep with cognition: a population-based study of middle-aged and elderly persons. Sleep Med (2015) 16(7):850–5. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Oosterman JM, van Someren EJ, Vogels RL, Van Harten B, Scherder EJ. Fragmentation of the rest-activity rhythm correlates with age-related cognitive deficits. J Sleep Res (2009) 18(1):129–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00704.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Hooghiemstra AM, Eggermont LH, Scheltens P, van der Flier WM, Scherder EJ. The rest-activity rhythm and physical activity in early-onset dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord (2015) 29(1):45–9. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Aharon-Peretz J, Masiah A, Pillar T, Epstein R, Tzischinsky O, Lavie P. Sleep-wake cycles in multi-infarct dementia and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neurology. (1991) 41(10):1616–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.41.10.1616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Gehrman P, Marler M, Martin JL, Shochat T, Corey-Bloom J, Ancoli-Israel S. The relationship between dementia severity and rest/activity circadian rhythms. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat (2005) 1(2):155–63. doi: 10.2147/nedt.1.2.155.61043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Paavilainen P, Korhonen I, Lötjönen J, Cluitmans L, Jylhä M, Särelä A, et al. Circadian activity rhythm in demented and non-demented nursing-home residents measured by telemetric actigraphy. J Sleep Res. (2005) 14(1):61–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00433.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Ancoli-Israel S, Klauber MR, Jones DW, Kripke DF, Martin J, Mason W, et al. Variations in circadian rhythms of activity, sleep, and light exposure related to dementia in nursing-home patients. Sleep (1997) 20(1):18–23.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

16. Tranah GJ, Blackwell T, Stone KL, Ancoli-Israel S, Paudel ML, Ensrud KE, et al. Circadian activity rhythms and risk of incident dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older women. Ann Neurol. (2011) 70(5):722–32. doi: 10.1002/ana.22468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Rogers-Soeder TS, Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, Redline S, Cauley JA, et al. Rest-activity rhythms and cognitive decline in older men: the osteoporotic fractures in men sleep study. J Am Geriatr Soc (2018) 66(11):2136–43. doi: 10.1111/jgs.15555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Walsh CM, Blackwell T, Tranah GJ, Stone KL, Ancoli-Israel S, Redline S, et al. Weaker circadian activity rhythms are associated with poorer executive function in older women. Sleep (2014) 37(12):2009–16. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Zuurbier LA, Ikram MA, Luik AI, Hofman A, Van Someren EJ, Vernooij MW, et al. Cerebral small vessel disease is related to disturbed 24-h activity rhythms: a population-based study. Eur J Neurol (2015) 22(11):1482–7. doi: 10.1111/ene.12775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Oosterman J, van Harten B, Vogels R, Gouw A, Weinstein H, Scheltens P, et al. Distortions in rest-activity rhythm in aging relate to white matter hyperintensities. Neurobiol Aging. (2008) 29(8):1265–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Van Someren EJW, Oosterman JM, Van Harten B, Vogels RL, Gouw AA, Weinstein HC, et al. Medial temporal lobe atrophy relates more strongly to sleep-wake rhythm fragmentation than to age or any other known risk. Neurobiol Learn Mem (2018) doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.05.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Musiek ES, Bhimasani M, Zangrilli MA, Morris JC, Holtzman DM, Ju YS. Circadian rest-activity pattern changes in aging and preclinical alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol (2018) 75(5):582–90. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.4719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Sherman SM, Mumford JA, Schnyer DM. Hippocampal activity mediates the relationship between circadian activity rhythms and memory in older adults. Neuropsychologia (2015) 75:617–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Ancoli-Israel S, Gehrman P, Martin JL, Shochat T, Marler M, Corey-Bloom J, et al. Increased light exposure consolidates sleep and strengthens circadian rhythms in severe Alzheimer’s disease patients. Behav Sleep Med. (2003) 1(1):22–36. doi: 10.1207/S15402010BSM0101_4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Dowling GA, Hubbard EM, Mastick J, Luxenberg JS, Burr RL, Van Someren EJ. Effect of morning bright light treatment for rest-activity disruption in institutionalized patients with severe Alzheimer’s disease. Int Psychogeriatr (2005) 17(2):221–36. doi: 10.1017/S1041610205001584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ancoli-Israel S, Martin JL, Kripke DF, Marler M, Klauber MR. Effect of light treatment on sleep and circadian rhythms in demented nursing home patients. J Am Geriatr Soc (2002) 50(2):282–9. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50060.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Dowling GA, Mastick J, Hubbard EM, Luxenberg JS, Burr RL. Effect of timed bright light treatment for rest-activity disruption in institutionalized patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry (2005) 20(8):738–43. doi: 10.1002/gps.1352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Dowling GA, Burr RL, Van Someren EJ, Hubbard EM, Luxenberg JS, Mastick J, et al. Melatonin and bright-light treatment for rest-activity disruption in institutionalized patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc (2008) 56(2):239–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01543.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Scherder E, Knol D, van Tol MJ, van Someren E, Deijen JB, Swaab D, et al. Effects of high-frequency cranial electrostimulation on the rest-activity rhythm and salivary cortisol in Alzheimer's disease: a pilot study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord (2006) 22(4):267–72. doi: 10.1159/000095108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Martinez-Martin P, Deuschl G. Effect of medical and surgical interventions on health-related quality of life in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord (2007) 22(6):757–65. doi: 10.1002/mds.21407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Van Someren EJ, Kessler A, Mirmiran M, Swaab DF. Indirect bright light improves circadian rest-activity rhythm disturbances in demented patients. Biol Psychiatry (1997) 41(9):955–63. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(97)89928-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Van Dijk KR, Luijpen MW, Van Someren EJ, Sergeant JA, Scheltens P, Scherder EJ. Peripheral electrical nerve stimulation and rest-activity rhythm in Alzheimer's disease. J Sleep Res. (2006) 15(4):415–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00548.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Kok JS, Berg IJ, Blankevoort GCG, Scherder EJA. Rest-activity rhythms in small scale homelike care and traditional care for residents with dementia. BMC Geriatr (2017) 17(1):137. doi: 10.1186/s12877-017-0525-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Eggermont LH, Knol DL, Hol EM, Swaab DF, Scherder EJ. Hand motor activity, cognition, mood, and the rest-activity rhythm in dementia: a clustered RCT. Behav Brain Res. (2009) 196(2):271–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Lee D, Morgan K, Lindesay J. Effect of institutional respite care on the sleep of people with dementia and their primary caregivers. J Am Geriatr Soc (2007) 55(2):252–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01036.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Strozyk D, Dickson DW, Lipton RB, Katz M, Derby CA, Lee S, et al. Contribution of vascular pathology to the clinical expression of dementia. Neurobiol Aging. (2010) 31(10):1710–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Scheer FA, Pirovano C, Van Someren EJ, Buijs RM. Environmental light and suprachiasmatic nucleus interact in the regulation of body temperature. Neuroscience. (2005) 132(2):465–77. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Smagula SF, Karim HT, Rangarajan A, Santos FP, Wood SC, Santini T. Association of Hippocampal Substructure Resting-State Functional Connectivity with Memory Performance in Older Adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry (2018) 26(6):690–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2018.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Karatsoreos IN, Bhagat S, Bloss EB, Morrison JH, McEwen BS. Disruption of circadian clocks has ramifications for metabolism, brain, and behavior. Proc Nat Acad Sci U S A (2011) 108(4):1657–62. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018375108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Cho K. Chronic ‘jet lag’ produces temporal lobe atrophy and spatial cognitive deficits. Nat Neurosci (2001) 4(6):567–8. doi: 10.1038/88384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Sullivan SC, Richards KC. Predictors of circadian sleep-wake rhythm maintenance in elders with dementia. Aging Ment Health. (2004) 8(2):143–52. doi: 10.1080/13607860410001649608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: rest-activity rhythms, sleep-wake rhythms, actigraphy, dementia, cerebrovascular disease, neurodegeneration

Citation: Smagula SF, Gujral S, Capps CS and Krafty RT (2019) A Systematic Review of Evidence for a Role of Rest-Activity Rhythms in Dementia. Front. Psychiatry 10:778. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00778

Received: 02 May 2019; Accepted: 30 September 2019;
Published: 30 October 2019.

Edited by:

Hyun Kook Lim, The Catholic University of Korea, South Korea

Reviewed by:

Meritxell Valenti-Soler, Alzheimer Center Reina Sofía Foundation, Spain
Domenico Passafiume, University of L'Aquila, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Smagula, Gujral, Capps and Krafty. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stephen F. Smagula, smagulasf@upmc.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.