Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 17 September 2020
Sec. Addictive Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Neurobiological Biomarkers for Developing Novel Treatments of Substance and Non-Substance Addiction View all 45 articles

The Effects of Benzodiazepine Use and Abuse on Cognition in the Elders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Comparative Studies

Linzi LiuLinzi Liu1Linna JiaLinna Jia1Peiying JianPeiying Jian2Yifang ZhouYifang Zhou3Jian ZhouJian Zhou1Feng WuFeng Wu1Yanqing Tang,*Yanqing Tang1,3*
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, The First Affiliated Hospital, China Medical University, Shenyang, China
  • 2Department of Psychology, Queen’s University, Kingston, ON, Canada
  • 3Department of Geriatrics, The First Affiliated Hospital, China Medical University, Shenyang, China

Objective: Benzodiazepines (BZD) are one of the most frequently prescribed drugs worldwide. However, the cognitive effects of benzodiazepines in the elderly are highly debated. This systematic review and meta-analysis aims to explore the following two questions in the elderly population: (i) Do BZD lead to any impairments in cognitive functions in elderly users? and (ii) Which specific cognitive domains are most affected by BZD use and abuse?

Methods: First, we performed a literature search following the PRISMA guidelines. Electronic databases, including PubMed, PsycINFO, EMBASE, Cochrane Library, and Web of Science were searched until May 14th, 2020. After selecting the relevant articles, we integrated the results of the selected studies with a standardized cognitive classification method. Next, we performed meta-analyses with the random-effects model on the cognitive results. Finally, we specifically examined the cognitive impairments of BZD in the abuse subgroup.

Results: Of the included studies, eight of the thirteen had meta-analyzable data. Compared to the controls, elderly BZD users had significantly lower digital symbol test scores (n=253; SMD: -0.61, 95% CI: -0.91 to 0.31, I² = 0%, p < 0.0001). There was no significant difference in Mini-Mental State Examination, Auditory Verbal Learning Test, and Stroop Color and Word Test scores between BZD users and controls. According to the subgroup analyses, BZD abusers performed significantly worse than controls in Mini-Mental State Examination (n=7726; SMD: -0.23, 95% CI: -0.44 to -0.03, I² = 86%, p = 0.02), while there was no significant difference between the regular BZD users and the controls (n=1536; SMD: -0.05, 95% CI: -0.59 to 0.48, I² = 92%, p =0.85).

Conclusion: In the elderly population, the processing speed (digital symbol test scores) was significantly impaired in BZD users; global cognition (Mini-Mental State Examination scores) was significantly impaired in BZD abusers but not in BZD regular users. This study provides insight into the factors that interact with BZD cognitive effects, such as aging, testing tools, and abuse. Clinicians should be cautious when prescribing BZD for the elderly.

Systematic Review Registration: PROSPERO, identifier CRD42019124711.

Introduction

Benzodiazepines (BZD) are two-ring heterocyclic compounds consisting of a benzene ring fused with a diazepine ring. Since its discovery in the 1950s, BZD’s sedative, hypnotic, anti-anxiety, and anti-convulsive effects have been increasingly accepted, making BZD use highly prevalent among adults (1) and especially in the elders (2). The prevalence of BZD use in elders varies between 10% and 42% worldwide (3). For example, BZD and related drugs are the third most abused prescription drug in America, with roughly 1-3% of the world population being subject to abuse (4). However, inappropriate BZD prescriptions can promote BZD misuse, facilitate the development of BZD addiction, and significantly affect the users’ overall quality of life (5, 6). Therefore, it is critical for pharmacists, clinicians, and patients to be informed on the latest research regarding the adverse effects of BZD use and abuse.

Since the 1970s, research has found negative effects of BZD on recipients’ cognitive functions (7). A meta-analysis in 2017 investigated the long-term cognitive impacts of BZD among adults. This analysis reported impairments in working memory, language, and processing speed, but not in executive function (reasoning and planning) (8). The participants in the meta-analysis, however, included adults of all ages. Compared to young adults, elderly populations require more cautiousness when undertaking BZD therapy. The elderly are more susceptible to cognitive impairment than young adults (9). During the progress of aging, cognitive function continues to decline with structural and functional neurological changes (10, 11). The pharmacokinetics of BZD in the elderly are different than in young adults, so the effects of BZD in older adults may have unique characteristics than in any other age group (12).

In recent years, several systematic reviews have found that BZD use was significantly associated with a higher risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairments (MCI). Dementia and MCI introduced a significant growth in mortality and financial burdens worldwide (13, 14). The most prominent symptom of these neurological disorders is a decline in cognitive function, measured by tests such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Sufficient evidence has shown that the risk of dementia correlates with cumulative dose, treatment duration, and long-acting effects of BZD molecules (1518). However, previous literature failed to provide a causal account for the link between BZD use and the risk of dementia (18). To understand the mechanism behind the adverse effects of BZD, it is crucial to investigate the specific areas of cognitive functions that are impacted by BZD use and abuse.

Literature in the past 50 years presents contradicting evidence on whether BZD impairs cognitive functioning in the elderly (1924). Among the studies that suggest an association between BZD use and cognitive impairment in elders, the type and degree of cognitive impairment reported are inconsistent across studies (19, 20, 22). Moreover, the neurological alterations due to the long-term effects of BZD use remains unclear (9). In 2018, Picton and colleagues summarized the cognitive effects of BZD in the elderly from published evidence. They found mixed findings of the association between BZD therapy and cognitive decline in elderly users (25). Given the lack of consensus in the current literature, a meta-analysis study may help reveal the critical effects of BZD use in the elderly and identify areas that require further research. To our knowledge, there are no systematic reviews with a meta-analysis that summarizes the current status of the cognitive effect of BZD use and abuse in the elderly population.

This systematic review and meta-analysis aims to explore the following two questions in the elderly: (i) Is BZD use associated with impairment in cognitive functions in the elderly? and (ii) Which cognitive domains have declined functionality associated with BZD use and abuse? The answers could help characterize the specific cognition impairments associated with BZD use, and identify individuals vulnerable to the negative effects of BZD. This meta-analysis may also help identify and monitor the cognitive effects associated with BZD use and abuse to prevent BZD addiction. With the high prevalence of BZD being prescribed to older populations worldwide, it is essential to inform clinicians and patients about the possible cognitive impairments associated with BZD use and abuse. Given the refractory rate and adverse effects of dementia and MCI (26), the results may also help reduce inappropriate BZD prescriptions to attenuate dementia risk in the elderly population.

Methods

Systematic Review Protocol

The process of this systematic review follows the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines, 2009 (27).

Databases and Search Strategy

We searched relevant articles in electronic databases, PubMed, PsycINFO, EMBASE, Cochrane Library, and Web of Science, from their inception to May 14, 2020. Specific search keywords included “benzodiazepines”, “cognition”, and “aged”. The query used for PubMed is ((Benzodiazepines[mh]) OR (Benzodiazepines Compounds[Title/Abstract]) OR (Benzodiazep*[Title/Abstract])) AND (cognit *’ OR memory OR attention OR visual-spatial OR visuospatial OR recall OR recognition OR problem solving OR reaction time OR vigilance OR executive function*’ OR reasoning OR psychomotor OR motor OR processing OR planning OR verbal fluency OR inhibit *’) AND ((Aged[mh]) OR (Elder*[Title/Abstract]) OR (older adults[Title/Abstract])).

Additionally, we searched in Google Scholar and searched the reference list for relevant articles to ensure that no studies were missed.

Inclusion Criteria and the Process to Identify Studies

We developed the inclusion criteria according to the PICOS guideline. Appropriate papers met the following criteria: 1) Participants were human adults, older than 60 years, mean sample age over 65 years; 2) the treatment groups were BZD users; 3) the studies had placebo or non-users of BZD as controls; 4) the outcomes included performances of cognitive functions measured using any standardized neuropsychological instruments; and 5) any type of published clinical studies except for case reports or conference abstracts were used. We only included articles reported in English.

Endnote was used to delete the duplicated articles. Two reviewers (LL and JL) independently scanned the references by title/abstract to exclude irrelevant articles, then read the full text to identify the appropriate studies based on the above inclusion criteria. Finally, the debated studies were determined through discussions with a third reviewer (YT).

Risk of Bias and Quality Assessment

Two reviewers assessed the quality of each included study independently with the modified Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (mNOS) (28). The mNOS examines the quality of non-randomized studies by four bias reduction items: (1) selection bias, (2) performance bias, (3) detection bias, and (4) reporting bias. Each item is graded from 0 (low quality) to 3 (high quality) according to the example given for each risk level. Disagreements were resolved by discussions with a third reviewer (Y.T.). Publication bias was tested by using the funnel plot and Begg’s tests on the outcome which synthesizes more than five studies.

Data Extraction

After generating a list of the included articles, two reviewers made the data extraction form collectively. The two reviewers then extracted the data independently. The data extraction form contained the following information: (1) author and publication year, (2) study design, (3) setting (country), (4) study design, (5) mean age, (6) gender distribution, (7) education, (8) participants sources, (9) details of BZD use (BZD types, using time, dosage, and the definition of BZD use), and (10) outcomes (cognitive task, cognitive domain, and main findings). The authors were contacted in order to obtain any missing data.

Data Synthesis and Statistical Analyses

The included studies utilized a variety of psychometric measurements, rendering it challenging to produce generalizable and informative conclusions. In this paper, we organize cognitive domains based on a commonly used framework (29, 30). According to this framework, we summarized the outcomes of the cognitive task from each included study to include a balanced and comprehensive view. Due to the discrepancy in psychometric screening tools between studies, the meta-analysis would only synthesis the data from the same neuropsychological instruments, such as the MMSE and the Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT).

The RevMan software (31) was used to perform the meta-analysis. We adopted the random-effects model in our meta-analysis because it is more conservative than the fixed-effects model. Standardized mean difference (SMD) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) was used for continuous outcomes. The study’s heterogeneity was measured using I2. I2>50% indicated significant heterogeneity (32). The meta-analytic outcomes were two-tailed, with a significance level of 0.05. Based on BZD use information reported in each study, we divided all the BZD users into two categories: regular BZD users and BZD abusers. BZD regular use is an appropriate pattern of BZD use that follows the prescription instruction. BZD abuse behaviors include meeting sedative, hypnotic, and anxiolytic use disorder criteria according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, and using a higher frequency or dose, or longer use time than prescribed, or without a prescription (1, 4, 33). We then conducted a subgroup analysis to explore the cognitive changes in BZD abusers and regular BZD users.

Results

Search Results and Studies Included

The study selection process of this systematic review is summarized in Figure 1. A total of 5072 references were returned by the initial search and scan, with 44 from relevant reviews and Google Scholar. After removing duplicated and irrelevant articles by title and abstract, the full texts of the remaining 79 were screened and 15 articles finally met the inclusion criteria. Two articles (34, 35) were from the same cohort study named The Canadian Study of Health and Aging (36). Another two articles had repeated participants (22, 37). Overall, 13 studies were included in the literature review. In the 13 studies, eight had meta-analyzable data.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Flow diagram of the search and study identification process.

Risk of Bias and Quality Assessment

The risk bias of the included studies was assessed by the mNOS and detailed in Table S1 in the Supplementary Materials. The scores ranged from 13-19 out of 21. Nine studies (34, 3845) had a sample size of more than one thousand participants. Nine studies (34, 38, 4042, 4447) used instruments that measured multi cognition domains. Eleven studies matched or adjusted education level as covariates and 12 studies matched or adjusted age as covariates. Therefore, the overall risk bias of the included studies is reasonably low. We tested the publication bias of the meta-analysis results of MMSE scores; the funnel plot (Figure S1 in Supplementary Materials) and Begg’s tests (p=0.673) did not find publication bias.

Studies Characteristics

There were 26033 participants (6374 BZD users) included in this systematic review and 10666 (2318 BZD users) included in the meta-analysis. As shown in Table 1, all studies were carried out in America or Europe. Six out of thirteen were published in the last five years. Eight were cohort studies; the remaining five were cross-sectional studies. The mean age of the participants is 72.5 in the cohort studies at baseline, and 83.8 in the cross-sectional studies. Therefore, both users and controls included in this review were elderly participants. In the 13 studies, a total of 36 tasks were used to measure cognitive functions; each study utilized one to nine cognitive tasks.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Summary of included articles.

Synthesized Findings

Benzodiazepine Use and Cognitive Decline in the Elders

Table 2 provides an overview of the effects on cognitive function from BZD use consolidated from all the included studies. The classification cognitive function division was adapted from a commonly used method of understanding and measuring cognitive domains (29). Eight of the thirteen studies with cognitive performance data had meta-analyzable data. They used the same cognitive measurements and reported the quantitative results required for meta-analysis (Table 1). Our meta-analysis synthesized data obtained with the same cognitive tasks across the included studies. Overall, the meta-analysis included the following cognitive functions: global cognition measured with the MMSE, processing speed measured with the digital symbol test, memory (recognition) measured with the AVLT, and executive functions (inhibitory control) measured with the Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT) (Figures 2A–D).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Overview of tasks used to assess cognitive functioning in benzodiazepine users across different cognitive domains.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 (A) Effect of BZD use on Mini Mental State Examination in the elderly: forest plot. (B) Effect of BZD use on Digital Symbol test in the elderly: forest plot. (C) Effect of BZD use on Auditory Verbal Learning Test in the elderly: forest plot. (D) Effect of BZD use on Stroop Color and Word Test in the elderly: forest plot.

Eleven out of 14 experiments on global cognition in the 13 studies showed no relationship between BZD use and decreased performance in global cognition. However, two experimental results (42, 47) showed that BZD users with higher socioeconomic status and BZD abusers performed worse in the MMSE than non-using controls. The remaining study by Bierman et al. (38) provided a third alternative result. The population-based 9-year cohort study concluded that, although there is a significant negative correlation between the MMSE scores and the accumulated BZD, the decrease in the MMSE scores with BZD use was relatively small. Moreover, the authors reported no correlation between dosage of BZD and MMSE scores. Taken together, the conclusions drawn from the literature are consistent with our meta-analysis results (Figure 2A); BZD users did not show significantly worse performance in the MMSE compared to the controls (n=9262; SMD: -0.18, 95% CI: -0.36 to 0.00, I² = 87%, p=0.05).

Seven studies tested the processing speed of BZD users. Four studies concluded that BZD use in the elderly population may result in significant impairment (41, 42, 47). They tested this domain mainly with the Trail Making Test (TMT) or the digit symbol test. The results showed that BZD users performed significantly worse across all the abovementioned tests than the controls. A meta-analysis (Figure 2B) was performed using the two studies that utilized the TMT. The results showed that in the elderly, the BZD users performed significantly worse than controls in digit symbol tests (n=253; SMD: -0.61, 95% CI: -0.91 to 0.31, I² = 0%, p < 0.0001). However, three other studies (34, 38, 45) suggested that the BZD users’ processing speed was not significantly impaired during a coding task and block design task.

An overview of the eight studies that tested memory and learning ability is presented in Table 2. Five out of nine tasks showed impairment in verbal/visual immediate recall among BZD users. More specifically, three out of five verbal immediate recall tasks and two out of four visual immediate recall tasks showed worse performances in BZD users than controls. When measuring with verbal/visual delayed recall tasks, four out of nine studies showed less accurate responses among BZD users. In these tasks, three out of seven verbal delayed recall tasks and one out of two visual delayed recall tasks showed BZD users performed worse than controls. The meta-analysis of the AVLT results (Figure 2C) is high in heterogeneity and non-significant (n=2580; SMD: -0.04, 95% CI: -0.15 to 0.06, I² = 84%, p=0.41).

Results of the tasks for motor, language ability, and executive functions are controversial (Table 2). In the domains of language ability and reasoning/planning function, three (34, 38, 47) out of four studies (44) that tested the respective domain showed a significantly worse performance in BZD users. Only one paper tested working memory (47) and showed that BZD users had a significantly worse performance than controls. In the domain of inhibitory control, the SCWT data sourced from two studies can be used for meta-analysis (Figure 2D). The results were non-significant and had high heterogeneity (n=1802; SMD: -0.07, 95% CI: -0.43 to 0.30, I² = 84%, p =0.72).

Benzodiazepine Abuse and Cognitive Decline in the Elders

We conducted subgroup analyses of BZD abusers (Figure 3). Seven studies included participants with BZD abuse (38, 40, 41, 43, 45, 47, 48) (Table 1). In the BZD abuse subgroup, the abusers received significantly lower MMSE scores compared to the controls (n=7726; SMD: -0.23, 95% CI: -0.44 to -0.03, I² = 86%, p =0.02). Meanwhile, in the BZD regular use subgroup, the MMSE scores of BZD users versus controls were not significantly different (n=1536; SMD: -0.05, 95% CI: -0.59 to 0.48, I² = 92%, p =0.85). Considering I² >50%, the heterogeneity between the studies cannot be ignored.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Subgroup analysis of the effect of BZD abuse and regular use on mini mental state examination in the elderly.

Due to the discrepancy in the type of cognitive tasks used in the included studies, we could not obtain meta-analyzable data to examine the effects of BZD abuse on specific cognitive functions. Nevertheless, the results of the experiments in these studies were summarized in Table 2. Notably, for language ability, Mura et al. used the Isaacs Set Test to explore the effect of BZD abuse (41), and Ros-Cucurull used the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (47). Both results showed that BZD abusers performed worse than the control groups. In the domain of recognition, BZD abusers performed worse than controls in three different tasks in two studies (38, 47). Experiments testing performance on other cognitive divisions showed mixed results.

Discussion

Main Findings of BZD Use

This meta-analysis and systematic review included 6374 BZD users and 19,659 controls to comprehensively investigate the affected cognition domains by BZD use and abuse in elders. A total of 13 papers met the inclusion criteria and were included in the literature review. Eight out of the 13 papers had appropriate tests and sufficient information for the meta-analysis.

Consistent with previous systematic reviews, our meta-analysis suggests no impairment in global cognition among elderly BZD users (49, 50). Interestingly, studies with young adults showed opposing results (51, 52), suggesting BZD use significantly impairs participants’ global cognitive functioning. One reason for these results may be the fact that the negative effect of various risk factors on global cognitive decline decreases with age (53).

The results consistently showed impairment in elderly BZD users’ processing speed, but not inhibitory control. Processing speed, defined by the time it takes for a person to complete a mental task, has been found to be associated with caudate activity in neuroimaging studies (54). Meanwhile, a higher BZD dose is associated with volume reductions in the caudate nucleus (55). This imaging evidence supports our results of decreased processing speed among BZD users. Our result in memory functions, however, is inconsistent with previous studies. While the meta-analysis (8) with adults of all ages found that BZD users had a significantly impaired working memory and immediate memory ability, our systematic review and meta-analysis did not find sufficient support for the effect. However, the interaction between age and BZD’s effect on memory requires more robust validation.

Main Findings of BZD Abuse

The BZD abuse subgroup in this review included 1673 BZD abusers and 6053 controls. The subgroup analysis showed that BZD abusers received significantly lower MMSE scores than the controls, while the BZD regular users’ scores were not significantly different from controls. These results demonstrated that impairment in global cognition occurs after the BZD user develops into abuse. However, the high heterogeneity in the results cannot be ignored. According to the results of the subgroup analysis, the confidence intervals of the two subgroups overlapped, and the difference between the two groups was not statistically significant (56). However, the subgroup analyses results, especially when not pre-specified at the beginning of the trial, cannot reflect the differences between the effect of interventions in subgroups (57). Future studies that directly contrast the cognition of BZD users and abusers are necessary. Only two studies tested language ability in BZD abusers; both found significantly worse language performance in BZD abusers compared to controls. Meanwhile, only two studies tested recognition ability; neither found a significant difference in the performance between BZD abusers and controls. These findings, however, are not conclusive because of the heterogeneity in the statistical results, and the small number of studies in the analyses. Therefore, more experiments are needed to reach more reliable conclusions.

After searching the databases, we did not find a meta-analysis investigating the cognitive effects of BZD abuse. The most relevant studies are two systematic reviews and meta-analyses papers on dementia risk in the elderly with long-duration and high dosage BZD users (58, 59). In 2015, Zhong and colleagues summarized six nested case-control or prospective cohort studies and concluded that higher dosage BZD users had an increased risk of dementia (59). In 2019, He et al. found that the risk of dementia was higher in patients taking BZD for a longer duration (>3 years) among six case-control and four cohort studies (58). Although long-term or high dosage use is not equivalent to dependence, BZD addiction is more likely to occur in long-term and high-dose users (60). These studies support the results of our subgroup analysis to some extent, but the mechanism and effects behind BZD addiction and abuse needs further investigation.

Strengths and Limitations of This Review

This study is, to our knowledge, the first systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of BZD use and abuse in elders. We attempted to consolidate results from studies testing different cognitive functions and data from a variety of cognitive tasks by utilizing a mature cognitive domain classification catalogue. The subgroup analysis of BZD abusers allowed us to preliminarily compare the effects of BZD use and abuse. This analysis encourages further studies to examine the qualitative difference of BZD use and abuse. These results can help identify and monitor the cognitive effects of BZD use and abuse, shedding light on awareness and prevention of BZD addiction at early stages, providing evidence for clinical decision-making, and improving the life quality of the elderly.

There are some limitations to this meta-analysis and systematic review. First, the number of studies and cognitive domains examined in this meta-analysis was limited. Five of the thirteen studies did not report cognitive tests data, and the domains of motor and language did not have meta-analysis results. Second, although the n is quite large, the meta-analysis results of the digital symbol test, AVLT, and SCWT were drawn from two small sample size studies, which is difficult to justify or interpret, considering the biased nature of the original studies. In the subgroup analysis of MMSE, the sample size of studies in the BZD regular users’ subgroup varied from hundreds to 2500, which are much smaller than the sample size of the abusers’ subgroup (>7000). Although the random effects model was used to reduce the impact caused by the difference between the sample size of the two subgroups, there was no analyzable data for further analysis. Third, due to the limitation of meta-analysis methodology, we could not directly compare the cognitive differences between BZD users and abusers. Fourth, other variabilities existed in the 13 studies in terms of the participants’ source (hospital or community), sample size, sex ratio, and cognitive measurements. These factors could also bias the findings to an uncertain extent. Fifth, users and controls were not well matched in the analysis. Participants in the experimental groups of the studies had different preexisting conditions such as anxiety, depression, insomnia, and APOE e4 status. One study did not age match the intervention group with the control group or as a covariable, which means the study could not completely distinguish the cognition impaired by BZD and by natural aging. Other factors related to cognition, such as the use of other psychotropic medications, certain physical diseases, and educational level, were not examined in most studies. Finally, due to the limited number of studies, sources of heterogeneity were not examined.

Implications and Future Directions

Our meta-analysis confirmed the negative effects of BZD on elderly users’ processing speed. Therefore, doctors should be cautious when prescribing BZD drugs to elderly patients, especially those with family histories of dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, and other aging-related cognitive deficits. Additionally, although global cognition was not impaired in BZD regular users, BZD abusers had significantly worse performance in global cognition. This research can inform more individualized prescription decisions. For example, elderly patients whose daily activities require higher cognitive processing should be informed of BZD’s potential side effects on their cognitive processing speed. Patients with a history of addiction should prioritize alternative treatments to BZD therapy to prevent BZD dependence and abuse.

Another important finding in this study is that the results of cognitive performance are highly dependent on the type of cognitive measurements in the study. For example, as previously reported, BZD users had significantly lower processing speeds when tested with the TMT. However, studies measuring processing speed with the coding task or block design task did not reveal any significant findings. Therefore, clinical practitioners should be mindful when selecting cognitive tests. It might be reasonable to use tests with higher sensitivity to reduce missed diagnoses.

In addition, through our exploration in the literature on the cognitive effects of BZD use, few studies paid attention to BZD abuse and addiction in participants (47, 61, 62). BZD use and abuse can be qualitatively different from BZD use. A survey conducted in 2015-2016 showed that BZD abusers accounted for about 17% of BZD users (6). Moreover, approximately half of the patients who used BZD for longer than 1 month are subject to BZD abuse or addiction (60). The neglect of the BZD abuse subgroup may be accountable for the mixed conclusions from studies on the cognitive effects of BZD use. Therefore, we encourage future researchers to separate BZD regular users and BZD abusers to achieve more precise and rigid conclusions.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this meta-analysis indicated no significant global cognition deficit (MMSE scores) in BZD users, but did reveal deficits in elders with BZD abuse behaviors. BZD users performed significantly worse in the cognition domain of processing speed (digit symbol test scores) than the controls, but not in memory and learning (AVLT scores) or inhibitory control (SCWT scores). Studies that tested the other cognitive domains, however, showed conflicting results. Unfortunately, these cognitive domains’ measurements varied across studies, rendering it unavailable to be merged into meta-analysis. Clinicians should be cautious when prescribing BZD for the elderly, especially to patients with a family history of age-related cognitive deficits. Moreover, the majority of the included studies did not clearly distinguish between the use and abuse of BZD, making it challenging to evaluate the effects of BZD abuse. Future well-designed studies are needed in order to verify the cognitive effects of BZD use and abuse.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Author Contributions

LL and YT: conceptualization. LL and LJ: data curation and analysis. FW and YT: project administration. LL, YZ, JZ, and PJ: supervision, writing—review, and editing. LL and PJ: writing—original draft.

Funding

National Key Research and Development Program of China (2018YFC1311604 and 2016YFC1306900 to YT), National Science Fund for Distinguished Young Scholars (81725005 to Fei Wang), Liaoning Education Foundation (Pandeng Scholar to Fei Wang), Innovation Team Support Plan of Higher Education of Liaoning Province (LT2017007 to Fei Wang), Major Special Construction Plan of China Medical University (3110117059 and 3110118055 to Fei Wang), Joint fund of National Natural Science Foundation of China (U1808204 to FW), Natural Science Foundation of Liaoning Province (2019-MS-05 to FW).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Xiang YT and Guo WB for providing theoretic guidance.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00755/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Blanco C, Han B, Jones CM, Johnson K, Compton WM. Prevalence and Correlates of Benzodiazepine Use, Misuse, and Use Disorders Among Adults in the United States. J Clin Psychiatry (2018) 79(6). doi: 10.4088/JCP.18m12174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Marra EM, Mazer-Amirshahi M, Brooks G, van den Anker J, May L, Pines JM. Benzodiazepine Prescribing in Older Adults in U.S. Ambulatory Clinics and Emergency Departments (2001-10). J Am Geriatr Soc (2015) 63(10):2074–81. doi: 10.1111/jgs.13666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Voshaar RC, Couvée JE, van Balkom AJ, Mulder PG, Zitman FG. Strategies for discontinuing long-term benzodiazepine use: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci (2006) 189:213–20. doi: 10.1192/bjp.189.3.213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Votaw VR, Geyer R, Rieselbach MM, McHugh RK. The epidemiology of benzodiazepine misuse: A systematic review. Drug Alcohol Depend (2019) 200:95–114. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2019.02.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Airagnes G, Pelissolo A, Lavallee M, Flament M, Limosin F. Benzodiazepine Misuse in the Elderly: Risk Factors, Consequences, and Management. Curr Psychiatry Rep (2016) 18(10):89. doi: 10.1007/s11920-016-0727-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Maust DT, Lin LA, Blow FC. Benzodiazepine Use and Misuse Among Adults in the United States. Psychiatr Serv (2019) 70(2):97–106. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201800321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Salzman C, Shader RI, Harmatz J, Robertson L. Psychopharmacologic investigations in elderly volunteers: Effect of diazepam in males. J Am Geriatr Soc (1975) 23(10):451–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1975.tb00929.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Crowe SF, Stranks EK. The Residual Medium and Long-term Cognitive Effects of Benzodiazepine Use: An Updated Meta-analysis. Arch Clin Neuropsychol Off J Natl Acad Neuropsychol (2018) 33(7):901–11. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acx120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Lipnicki DM, Crawford JD, Dutta R, Thalamuthu A, Kochan NA, Andrews G, et al. Age-related cognitive decline and associations with sex, education and apolipoprotein E genotype across ethnocultural groups and geographic regions: a collaborative cohort study. PloS Med (2017) 14(3):e1002261. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Goyal MS, Vlassenko AG, Blazey TM, Su Y, Couture LE, Durbin TJ, et al. Loss of Brain Aerobic Glycolysis in Normal Human Aging. Cell Metab (2017) 26(2):353–60.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2017.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kovacs GG, Milenkovic I, Wöhrer A, Höftberger R, Gelpi E, Haberler C, et al. Non-Alzheimer neurodegenerative pathologies and their combinations are more frequent than commonly believed in the elderly brain: a community-based autopsy series. Acta Neuropathol (2013) 126(3):365–84. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1157-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Dingemanse J, Häussler J, Hering W, Ihmsen H, Albrecht S, Zell M, et al. Pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic modelling of the EEG effects of Ro 48-6791, a new short-acting benzodiazepine, in young and elderly subjects. Br J anaesth (1997) 79(5):567–74. doi: 10.1093/bja/79.5.567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Wimo A, Guerchet M, Ali GC, Wu YT, Prina AM, Winblad B, et al. The worldwide costs of dementia 2015 and comparisons with 2010. Alzheimer’s Dementia J Alzheimer’s Assoc (2017) 13(1):1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.07.150

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Alzheimer's Association. 2016 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s Dementia J Alzheimer’s Assoc (2016) 12(4):459–509. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Penninkilampi R, Eslick GD. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Risk of Dementia Associated with Benzodiazepine Use, After Controlling for Protopathic Bias. CNS Drugs (2018) 32(6):485–97. doi: 10.1007/s40263-018-0535-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Islam MM, Iqbal U, Walther B, Atique S, Dubey NK, Nguyen PA, et al. Benzodiazepine Use and Risk of Dementia in the Elderly Population: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Neuroepidemiology (2016) 47(3-4):181–91. doi: 10.1159/000454881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Gomm W, von Holt K, Thome F, Broich K, Maier W, Weckbecker K, et al. Regular Benzodiazepine and Z-Substance Use and Risk of Dementia: An Analysis of German Claims Data. J Alzheimer’s Dis JAD (2016) 54(2):801–8. doi: 10.3233/jad-151006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. de Gage SB, Pariente A, Begaud B. Is there really a link between benzodiazepine use and the risk of dementia? Expert Opin Drug Saf (2015) 14(5):733–47. doi: 10.1517/14740338.2015.1014796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Panel BtAGSBCUE. American Geriatrics Society 2019 Updated AGS Beers Criteria® for Potentially Inappropriate Medication Use in Older Adults. J Am Geriatr Soc (2019) 67(4):674–94. doi: 10.1111/jgs.15767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Coyle-Gilchrist IT, Peck LF, Rowe JB. Research paper does not show causal link between benzodiazepine use and diagnosis of dementia. BMJ (Clin Res ed) (2012) 345:e7984; author reply e93. doi: 10.1136/bmj.e7984

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Basu R, Dodge H, Stoehr GP, Ganguli M. Sedative-hypnotic use of diphenhydramine in a rural, older adult, community-based cohort: effects on cognition. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry Off J Am Assoc Geriatr Psychiatry (2003) 11(2):205–13. doi: 10.1097/00019442-200303000-00011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Vaapio S, Puustinen J, Salminen MJ, Vahlberg T, Salonoja M, Lyles A, et al. Symptoms associated with long-term benzodiazepine use in elderly individuals aged 65 years and older: a longitudinal descriptive study. Int J Gerontol (2015) 9(1):34–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ijge.2014.03.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Murphy P, Hindmarch I, Hyland CM. Aspects of short-term use of two benzodiazepine hypnotics in the elderly. Age Ageing (1982) 11(4):222–8. doi: 10.1093/ageing/11.4.222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Nikaido AM, Ellinwood EH Jr., Heatherly DG, Gupta SK. Age-related increase in CNS sensitivity to benzodiazepines as assessed by task difficulty. Psychopharmacology (1990) 100(1):90–7. doi: 10.1007/bf02245796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Picton JD, Marino AB, Nealy KL. Benzodiazepine use and cognitive decline in the elderly. Am J Health-Syst Pharm (2018) 75(1):E6–E12. doi: 10.2146/ajhp160381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Grasset L, Matthews FE, Pérès K, Foubert-Samier A, Helmer C, Dartigues JF, et al. Evolution of dementia diagnosis over time (1988-2013): Evidence from French and English cohorts. Implication for secular trends analyses. Alzheimer’s Dementia (Amsterdam Netherlands) (2018) 10:490–7. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2018.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PloS Med (2009) 6(7):e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Bawor M, Dennis BB, Anglin R, Steiner M, Thabane L, Samaan Z. Sex differences in outcomes of methadone maintenance treatment for opioid addiction: a systematic review protocol. System Rev (2014) 3:45. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-3-45

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Galioto R, Spitznagel MB. The Effects of Breakfast and Breakfast Composition on Cognition in Adults. Adv Nutr (Bethesda Md) (2016) 7(3):576s–89s. doi: 10.3945/an.115.010231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Lezak MD HD, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment. 4th ed ed. Oxford University Press: New York (2012).

Google Scholar

31. Review Manager (RevMan). The Cochrane Collaboration. 2019. Copenhagen: The Nordic Cochrane Centre TCC (2014). Available at: revman.cochrane.org

Google Scholar

32. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ (Clin Res ed) (2003) 327(7414):557–60. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Yen CF, Ko CH, Chang YP, Yu CY, Huang MF, Yeh YC, et al. Dependence, misuse, and beliefs regarding use of hypnotics by elderly psychiatric patients taking zolpidem, estazolam, or flunitrazepam. Asia-Pacific Psychiatry Off J Pacific Rim Coll Psychiatrists (2015) 7(3):298–305. doi: 10.1111/appy.12147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Helmes E, Ostbye T. Associations between Benzodiazepine Use and Neuropsychological Test Scores in Older Adults. Can J Aging = La Rev Can du vieillissement (2015) 34(2):207–14. doi: 10.1017/s0714980815000082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ebly EM, Hogan DB, Fung TS. Potential adverse outcomes of psychotropic and narcotic drug use in Canadian seniors. J Clin Epidemiol (1997) 50(7):857–63. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(97)00118-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Canadian Study of Health and Aging working Group. Canadian study of health and aging: study methods and prevalence of dementia. CMAJ Can Med Assoc J = J l’Association Med Can (1994) 150(6):899–913. doi: 10.1001/jama.271.11.872

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Puustinen J, Nurminen J, Kukola M, Vahlberg T, Laine K, Kivela SL. Associations between use of benzodiazepines or related drugs and health, physical abilities and cognitive function - A non-randomised clinical study in the elderly. Drugs Aging (2007) 24(12):1045–59. doi: 10.2165/00002512-200724120-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Bierman EJM, Comijs HC, Gundy CM, Sonnenberg C, Jonker C, Beekman ATF. The effect of chronic benzodiazepine use on cognitive functioning in older persons: Good, bad or indifferent? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry (2007) 22(12):1194–200. doi: 10.1002/gps.1811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Gray SL, Dublin S, Yu O, Walker R, Anderson M, Hubbard RA, et al. Benzodiazepine use and risk of incident dementia or cognitive decline: Prospective population based study. BMJ: Br Med J (2016) 352:i90. doi: 10.1136/bmj.i90

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Hanlon JT, Horner RD, Schmader KE, Fillenbaum GG, Lewis IK, Wall WE Jr., et al. Benzodiazepine use and cognitive function among community-dwelling elderly. Clin Pharmacol Ther (1998) 64(6):684–92. doi: 10.1016/S0009-9236(98)90059-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Mura T, Proust-Lima C, Akbaraly T, Amieva H, Tzourio C, Chevassus H, et al. Chronic use of benzodiazepines and latent cognitive decline in the elderly: Results from the Three-city study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol (2013) 23(3):212–23. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Paterniti S, Dufouil C, Alperovitch A. Long-term benzodiazepine use and cognitive decline in the elderly: the Epidemiology of Vascular Aging Study. J Clin Psychopharmacol (2002) 22(3):285–93. doi: 10.1097/00004714-200206000-00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Zhang Y, Zhou XH, Meranus DH, Wang L, Kukull WA. Benzodiazepine Use and Cognitive Decline in Elderly with Normal Cognition. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord (2016) 30(2):113–7. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Del Ser T, Zea MA, Valenti M, Olazaran J, Lopez-Alvarez J, Rebollo-Vazquez A, et al. Effects of commonly prescribed drugs on cognition and mild cognitive impairment in healthy elderly people. J Psychopharmacol (2019) 33(8):965–74. doi: 10.1177/0269881119857206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. van Vliet P, van der Mast RC, van den Broek M, Westendorp RGJ, de Craen AJM. Use of benzodiazepines, depressive symptoms and cognitive function in old age. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry (2009) 24(5):500–8. doi: 10.1002/gps.2143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hoiseth G, Tanum L, Tveito M, Kristiansen KM, Kvande K, Lorentzen B, et al. A Clinical Study of the Cognitive Effects of Benzodiazepines in Psychogeriatric Patients. Pharmacopsychiatry (2013) 46(6):209–13. doi: 10.1055/s-0033-1349131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Ros-Cucurull E, Palma-Álvarez RF, García-Raboso E, Cardona-Rubira C, Jacas C, Grau-López L, et al. Benzodiazepine Use Disorder and Cognitive Impairment in Older Patients: A Six-Month-Follow-Up Study in an Outpatient Unit in Barcelona. J Stud Alcohol Drugs (2018) 79(6):844–52. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2018.79.844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Hessmann P, Dodel R, Baum E, Muller MJ, Paschke G, Kis B, et al. Prescription of Benzodiazepines and Related Drugs in Patients with Mild Cognitive Deficits and Alzheimer’s Disease. Pharmacopsychiatry (2019) 52(2):84–91. doi: 10.1055/s-0044-100523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Nader D, Gowing L. Is Long-Term Benzodiazepine Use a Risk Factor for Cognitive Decline? Results of a Systematic Review. J Addict (2020) 2020:1569456. doi: 10.1155/2020/1569456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Lapeyre-Mestre M. [Benzodiazepines, cognitive decline and dementia: A review of causality criteria from published observational studies]. Therapie (2019) 74(3):407–19. doi: 10.1016/j.therap.2018.09.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Barker MJ, Greenwood KM, Jackson M, Crowe SF. An evaluation of persisting cognitive effects after withdrawal from long-term benzodiazepine use. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2005) 11(3):281–9. doi: 10.1017/s1355617705050332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Federico A, Tamburin S, Maier A, Faccini M, Casari R, Morbioli L, et al. Multifocal cognitive dysfunction in high-dose benzodiazepine users: a cross-sectional study. Neurol Sci Off J Ital Neurol Soc Ital Soc Clin Neurophysiol (2017) 38(1):137–42. doi: 10.1007/s10072-016-2732-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Legdeur N, Heymans MW, Comijs HC, Huisman M, Maier AB, Visser PJ. Age dependency of risk factors for cognitive decline. BMC Geriatr (2018) 18(1):187. doi: 10.1186/s12877-018-0876-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. De Simoni S, Jenkins PO, Bourke NJ, Fleminger JJ, Hellyer PJ, Jolly AE, et al. Altered caudate connectivity is associated with executive dysfunction after traumatic brain injury. Brain J Neurol (2018) 141(1):148–64. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Huhtaniska S, Jaaskelainen E, Heikka T, Moilanen JS, Lehtiniemi H, Tohka J, et al. Long-term antipsychotic and benzodiazepine use. and brain volume changes in schizophrenia: The Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1966 study. Psychiatry Resh-Neuroimaging (2017) 266:73–82. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.05.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Higgins JPT TJ, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, Welch VA. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. 2nd Edition ed. Chichester (UK): John Wiley & Sons (2019).

Google Scholar

57. Wang SS, Ou YC, Cheng CL, Dahm P. Evidence-based urology in practice: when to believe a subgroup analysis? BJU Int (2010) 105(2):162–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-410X.2009.09053.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. He Q, Chen X, Wu T, Li L, Fei X. Risk of Dementia in Long-Term Benzodiazepine Users: Evidence from a Meta-Analysis of Observational Studies. J Clin Neurol (Seoul Korea) (2019) 15(1):9–19. doi: 10.3988/jcn.2019.15.1.9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Zhong G, Wang Y, Zhang Y, Zhao Y. Association between Benzodiazepine Use and Dementia: A Meta-Analysis. PloS One (2015) 10(5):e0127836. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0127836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Soyka M. Treatment of Benzodiazepine Dependence. New Engl J Med (2017) 376(12):1147–57. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1611832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Bergman H, Borg S, Engelbrektson K, Vikander B. Dependence on sedative-hypnotics: neuropsychological impairment, field dependence and clinical course in a 5-year follow-up study. Br J Addict (1989) 84(5):547–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1989.tb00612.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Chu C, Sivakumar K, Melinosky C. A Case of Reversible Bilateral Hippocampal Injury Secondary to Benzodiazepine Abuse (P3.1-017). Neurology (2019) 92(15 Supplement):P3.1–017.

Google Scholar

Keywords: benzodiazepines, cognitive function, substance addiction, aged, meta-analysis, cognitive dysfuction

Citation: Liu L, Jia L, Jian P, Zhou Y, Zhou J, Wu F and Tang Y (2020) The Effects of Benzodiazepine Use and Abuse on Cognition in the Elders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Comparative Studies. Front. Psychiatry 11:00755. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00755

Received: 21 May 2020; Accepted: 16 July 2020;
Published: 17 September 2020.

Edited by:

Yanhui Liao, Sir Run Run Shaw Hospital, China

Reviewed by:

Yu-Tao Xiang, University of Macau, China
Xiao Jun Xiang, Central South University, China
Yanbo Zhang, University of Alberta, Canada

Copyright © 2020 Liu, Jia, Jian, Zhou, Zhou, Wu and Tang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yanqing Tang, tangyanqing@cmu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.