Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 09 October 2020
Sec. Schizophrenia
This article is part of the Research Topic Autobiographical Memory, Narrative Identity, and Mental Health View all 22 articles

Involuntary Autobiographical Memories in Schizophrenia: Characteristics and Conditions of Elicitation

  • 1Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Center on Autobiographical Memory Research, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
  • 2Inserm U1114, Strasbourg University, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France

Involuntary autobiographical memories are mental representations of personally experienced past events that come to mind spontaneously, with no preceding attempt to recall them. They have been showed to be more frequent and more emotional in the psychosis continuum. Although schizophrenia is strongly associated with thought disorders, including cognitive intrusions of thought, images, semantic knowledge, research on patients' involuntary autobiographical memories is limited. We undertook two studies to compare involuntary and voluntary remembering in schizophrenia and the conditions in which involuntary memories occurs in those patients, both in daily life (n = 40), using a diary method, and in an experimental context (n = 50). Overall, results showed that the conditions of elicitation of involuntary memories differ in patients, as patients were more sensitive to memory triggers, especially internal triggers, in comparison to controls. Relatedly, patients' involuntary memories—mostly related to mundane events with low emotional load—were experienced more frequently. Although patients' involuntary and voluntary memories were less clear, more poorly contextualized and associated with a lower belief in occurrence than those of controls, patients considered them as more central to the self, in comparison to controls. The results are discussed in relation to patients' self-reflective impairments.

Introduction

Involuntary autobiographical memories are mental representations of personally experienced past events that come to mind unexpectedly, that is, with no preceding attempt to recall them (1, 2). Such memories are frequent in everyday life, happening mostly during periods of inactivity and boredom (3, 4) and their occurrence is generally preceded by identifiable cues in the environment or in thought (1, 2, 5). Indeed, involuntary retrieval is based on associative processes that make a connection between the current (inner and outer) situation and features of personal memories (1). These automatic processes are beyond cognitive control (6). The conceptual contrast to involuntary autobiographical memories is voluntary autobiographical memories—that is, memories of events that are the result of a consciously and deliberately initiated retrieval process.

Everyday involuntary autobiographical memories have been described as functional in different ways [see (7)]. For example, such memories allow us to effortlessly mentally navigate between past and future, which is considered to be a major evolutionary advantage (8, 9). In addition, involuntary episodic remembering is central to the regulation of behavior through the knowledge transfer from a past to a present situation (i.e., from the remembered event to the situation in which the memory arises). Indeed, at times, an involuntary memory may instigate a direct change in an ongoing activity or suggest a solution to a problem and thus hold a directive function (10, 11).

Although normally adaptive, or at least benign, involuntary autobiographical remembering can be a source of distress. This is most clearly observed in relation to post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). In PTSD, the traumatic event is “persistently re-experienced” in terms of, among other things, “recurrent and intrusive distressing recollections of the event” (12).

In schizophrenia, thought intrusion (i.e., repetitive and unwanted thoughts, images, or impulses interrupting ongoing activity) (13, 14), is very common and has been related to psychotic symptoms and hallucination (15). More precisely, people with schizophrenia experiencing hallucinations also experience more anxiety-related and depression-related intrusive thoughts than both individuals with or without psychiatric disorders (16). People with schizophrenia found these intrusions more distressing, less controllable and tended to feel sadder as a result of their intrusions in comparison to both control groups (16).

In addition, patients with schizophrenia experience mind-pops—that is, fragments of semantic knowledge (words, songs, phrases, or images) coming to mind unexpectedly—more frequently than depressed or non-clinical controls (17, 18). Particularly, patients in the study by Elua et al. (18) reported significantly more verbal and visual mind-pops than controls, but the groups did not differ in the number of musical mind-pops. Patients' mind-pops were also more associated with negative content than those of controls. In this study, almost half of the mind-pops were reported with no apparent triggers, which can make them seem “uncontrollable,” a key characteristic of hallucination as well (19, 20).

With regard to involuntary autobiographical memory per se, very few studies have investigated them throughout the psychosis continuum (which includes psychotic symptoms from subclinical manifestations to the clinically significant ones typically observed in individuals diagnosed with a psychiatric illness) (21).

Jones and Steel (22) investigated the propensity of involuntary retrieval in individuals with schizotypy (i.e., a personality trait related to psychosis) (23) and showed that individuals scoring high on schizotypy reported more involuntary autobiographical memory in a free-association word task than those who scored low. Most of the memories reported had a neutral or positive content, for all participants.

In the same population, Holmes and Steel (24) showed that individuals with high schizotypy scores reported more trauma-related intrusions after watching a trauma film, in comparison to individuals with low schizotypy scores. The authors emphasized the parallel between intrusions in PTSD and some psychotic disorders. Relatedly, people diagnosed with schizophrenia and comorbid PTSD have more involuntary autobiographical memories in comparison to people with schizophrenia without PTSD (25). This increase of involuntary autobiographical memory frequency was not associated with psychotic symptoms severity (25).

Following up upon some of these earlier studies, we showed previously (26) that both neutral and trauma-related involuntary autobiographical memories occur more frequently in the daily life of individuals with attenuated psychotic symptoms [that is, individuals who are part of the so-called “psychosis continuum” reflecting a continuum encompassing a full range of psychotic symptom expressions from subclinical to clinical disorders, see (21)]. Importantly, we also observed that involuntary autobiographical memories and future projections were robustly related to hallucination proneness in the general population (27). This relationship was consistent even when controlling for a wide range of other factors known to be associated with hallucination, such as thought intrusion, rumination, executive functioning, depressive, and dissociative symptoms, personality traits, and mental imagery (27).

Moreover, individuals with attenuated psychotic symptoms experienced their involuntary memories as more vivid and intrusive, and associated with a stronger feeling of reliving, in comparison to control individuals (26). In addition, involuntary autobiographical memories were more emotionally intense, had greater negative mood impact and were associated with negative content, such as past traumatic experiences, in individuals with attenuated psychotic symptoms compared to control participants. These results suggested enhanced emotional responses associated with involuntary autobiographical memory in individuals with attenuated psychotic symptoms.

Importantly, these studies on involuntary autobiographical memories in psychosis were conducted with individuals in the subclinical end of the psychosis continuum and did not involve patients diagnosed with schizophrenia (the extremity of the psychosis continuum). Thus, in spite of the overall importance of involuntary autobiographical memories in daily life cognition (2) and their potential role on hallucination symptoms (27), little is known about involuntary autobiographical memory in schizophrenia.

In contrast, substantial work on voluntary retrieval of autobiographical memories has been conducted in this population. Patients with schizophrenia exhibit consistent and severe autobiographical memory impairments when those memories are strategically and voluntarily retrieved, in response to specific instructions or sensory cues (28, 29). Recently, we compared voluntary vs. involuntary retrievals of autobiographical memories in schizophrenia, in order to disentangle the role of retrieval deficits in patients' autobiographical memory impairments (30). This work consistently showed that involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories were similarly impaired in schizophrenia, ruling out the hypothesis that autobiographical memory impairment is caused by patients' problems with self-initiated voluntary retrieval.

Here we report two studies that pursued a thorough investigation of the context in which involuntary autobiographical memories arise in schizophrenia, their mechanisms of activation, but also the content of patients' involuntary autobiographical memories and some of their qualitative characteristics. Both studies were designed to compare involuntary and voluntary remembering in schizophrenia and to provide convergent evidence using two different research methodologies.

Study 1 employed an Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA); which is an ecological method that involves the repeated collection of real-time data on subjects' behavior and experience in their natural environments (i.e., involuntary autobiographical memories in this context). This methodology allowed us to collect critical information, using a diary, about the contexts in which involuntary memories arise in people with schizophrenia in the course of their everyday lives, which no other method can supply. Importantly, only very few studies have used this methodology in individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia (18, 31, 32) and none have used it in relation to involuntary autobiographical memory, as it is challenging to run such protocols in patients presenting with cognitive disorders and motivation impairments.

Study 2 was designed to examine involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories in individuals with schizophrenia, using a new experimental set up developed especially for this purpose. Study 1 had high ecological validity, but was time-consuming and cognitively-demanding for patients, whereas Study 2 enabled memory assessment in a controlled experimental setting, and in ways that were easier for the patients to accomplish.

Both studies examined a theoretically motivated selection of variables exploring the mechanisms of activation of involuntary autobiographical memories (including the attentional state preceding the memory, type of triggers and the participants' sensitivity to triggers), as well as memory content and memory qualities (including emotion, mood impact, intrusiveness, feeling of control, belief in occurrence, and me-ness). These variables were selected on the basis of previous research on involuntary autobiographical memories in healthy populations [e.g., (2, 33)] as well as previous research on voluntary autobiographical memory in schizophrenia [e.g., (28, 34, 35)].

Despite the exploratory nature of the present studies, some hypotheses were made based on the literature on spontaneous thoughts and involuntary memories in the psychosis continuum. First, based on thought intrusion research (16), we hypothesized that involuntary autobiographical memories of patients would be associated with emotional impairments, in comparison to their voluntary memories as well as compared with both involuntary and voluntary memories of controls. Second, we hypothesized that patients' involuntary memories would also be associated with a higher feeling of intrusiveness and a lower feeling of control (16), compared to their voluntary memories as well as compared with both involuntary and voluntary memories of controls. Third, we hypothesized that patients with schizophrenia would experience involuntary memories more frequently than control participants (18, 22, 25, 26).

Study 1

Method

Participants

Thirty outpatients with schizophrenia (8 women and 22 men) from the Psychiatry Department of Strasbourg University Hospitals (France) took part in the study. They met the DSM-5 (12) criteria for schizophrenia spectrum disorder (patients with schizophrenia, n = 25; patients with schizoaffective disorder, n = 5) and, in the preceding 3 months, they had not experienced any change in their symptoms or medication, or been hospitalized. Symptoms of schizophrenia were assessed by a psychiatrist using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (36). All patients were receiving long-term, second-generation antipsychotic treatment. A group of 23 healthy control participants was matched to patients with respect to age, gender, and level of schooling (see Table 1). None of the control participants had a psychiatric illness or was taking medication. No participants from either group had a history of neurological disorders or substance abuse. Control participants who scored higher than 8 on the Beck Depression Inventory (37) and patients scoring higher than 4 on the Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia (38) were excluded from the study. The Strasbourg Ethics Committee approved the study (IC-RCB: 2017-A00316-47), and all participants gave their written, informed consent after the procedures had been fully explained to them. All participants who completed the study were compensated for their participation.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Patients with schizophrenia and control participants' demographic information and clinical measures for study 1 (n = 40) and study 2 (n = 50).

Design

The design was a 2 (Group: individuals with schizophrenia vs. control participants) ×2 (Retrieval: involuntary vs. voluntary) mixed design with Retrieval as within-subject variable.

Materials

Memory Diary

A well-established ecological momentary assessment method of collecting involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories was employed in the current study (39). Participants were instructed to record 15 involuntary and 15 voluntary memories in a diary over the course of several days and to self-rate the characteristics of their memories. As mentioned in the introduction, some measures of memory, related to executive functions (i.e., specificity, reliving, vividness, context, coherence), are reported in Allé et al. (30), which addresses the role of executive deficit in autobiographical memory impairment in schizophrenia.

Involuntary autobiographical memory collection

The first step consisted in recording involuntary memories in a small notebook that was to be carried at all times. Participants were instructed to record information about the first two memories that occurred spontaneously (without preceding attempt to recall the memory) on a given day. Immediately after a memory had occurred, participants were asked to write down brief details of the memory and to answer questions assessing the characteristics of the current situation in which the autobiographical memory popped-up (e.g., location, ongoing action, ongoing thinking and memory triggers), using five-point Likert scales (see Table 2 for details). There was no requirement to record involuntary memories every day.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Questions included in the memory questionnaires.

The second step was to complete a more detailed questionnaire about their involuntary memories, later on the same day, supported by the records in the notebook (see Table 2 for details). Participants used a structured memory diary booklet to record responses to a series of questions meant to explore the subjective characteristics of the memory.

Voluntary autobiographical memory collection

Immediately after completing the involuntary memory questionnaire, the last step of the procedure consisted of uncovering a cue-word in the diary and then generating a voluntary memory in response to the cue word. The word cues employed were working, snow, sport, school, happy, book, friends, biking, shirt, family, rain, cinema, traveling, phone, and car (39). Participants then answered the same series of questions as before, but about the voluntary memory they had just retrieved (see Table 2 for details).

Participants received a reminder phone call about the study every 2 weeks until they had completed the diary. This was done to motivate them to continue the study and to verify that participants, those with schizophrenia in particular, were doing well and/or to resolve any difficulties they might experience in relation to the study.

Coding

Attention

Participants reported what they were doing when an involuntary memory came to mind and whether they were thinking of something else in parallel with their current task. On the basis of these descriptions, attention was scored as focused or diffuse following a previously used coding scheme (1, 33). Attention was scored as diffuse if (1) the person had explicitly written that he or she was bored, tired and, unengaged with his or her current task; (2) if the person had been thinking of something else in parallel with the current task and the content of this thought appeared to be irrelevant to the current task (e.g., daydreaming about vacations while attending a meeting); (3) if the subject had responded “Nothing” to the question “What were you doing?” or mentioned an activity that probably did not demand much attention, for example “I was waiting for the doctor in the waiting room.” Otherwise, attention was scored as focused. Scores were assigned by the experimenter and by an independent judge; inter-rater agreement was good (κ = 0.72) and disagreements were resolved by discussion.

Thematic content

Participants' descriptions were used to code the content of involuntary and voluntary memories using seven categories of frequently recalled events (27): (1) major achievements, (2) mundane childhood events, (3) mundane daily life events, (4) missed opportunities, remorse, or events that did not happen, (5) failures or humiliations, (6) traumatic or life threatening events and (7) death-related events. The content of each memory was also rated by the experimenter and by an independent judge, with good inter-reliability (κ = 0.81). Disagreements were resolved by discussion.

Results

Protocol Completion

Only 20 of the 30 patients who began the diary protocol completed it in full. Ten patients gave up the protocol whereas we lost touch with only three control participants. In other words, there were three times as many drop-outs in the patient group as in the control group. The main difficulties and/or complaints reported by the patients were (1) the lack of motivation and autonomy to complete the diary at home over a period of several weeks, (2) their tendency to focus on potentially unpleasant involuntary autobiographical memories and (3) metacognitive impairments that might have affected diary completion. Importantly, none of the individuals with schizophrenia reported discomfort or any increase of psychotic symptoms due to the study.

The Conditions of Elicitation of Involuntary Memories

Attention

Each participant reported 15 involuntary memories classified in two categories according to their associated attentional state. Patients tended to experience more involuntary memories when their attention was diffuse, compared to controls [62.98% in patient group and 51.75% in control group; t(38) = 1.94, p = 0.060].

Cues

A single memory could be elicited by several cues. We calculated a mean score for each type of cue, indicating the proportion of memories for which this cue had been endorsed by the participant. These scores were considered in a 2-by-10 analysis of variance with group (patients with schizophrenia vs. controls) as the between-subjects variable and trigger (person; object; activity; place; feelings; word; sensory experiences; theme; none; other) as a within-subjects variable (see Figure 1, left plot). There was no main effect of group, F(1, 38) = 0.47; p = 0.49; ηp2 = 0.01, as both groups identified on average the same number of cues per memory. There was a main effect of cue, F(9, 342) = 9.93; p < 0.001; ηp2 = 0.21, indicating that overall some cues elicited more frequent involuntary memories (e.g., places, persons) than others (e.g., words). There was also an interaction between group and cue, F(9, 342) = 1.98; p = 0.04; ηp2 = 0.05, reflecting some variations in the types of triggers that elicited involuntary autobiographical memories in the two groups. Post-hoc analyses showed that the cue categories “place,” “theme,” and “sensory experiences” tended to be less frequent in the patient group in comparison to the control group, ps < 0.09.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The left plot shows the proportion of involuntary autobiographical memories triggered by each cue category in patients with schizophrenia and in control participants, in Study 1. The right plot shows the proportion of involuntary autobiographical memories triggered by only external cues, only internal cues, mixed cues, or unidentified cues, in Study 1. $p < 0.15; ¤p < 0.10; *p < 0.05.

We compared the proportion of memories triggered by each category of triggers: external cues (i.e., person, object, activity, place, word, sensory experience, and theme), internal cues (i.e., feeling), mixed cues (both external and internal cues), and unidentified cues (memories for which the participant was not able to detect any relationship to his or her present situation) in both groups, using a four-by-two analysis of variance (see Figure 1, right plot). These categories were defined based on participants' triggers identification, controlling for the description of their ongoing thought. We observed a significant effect of the type of cue, F(1, 38) = 42.69; p < 0.001; ηp2 = 0.53, but no significant effect of the group, F(1, 38) = 1.00; p = 32; ηp2 = 0.03. The interaction between the type of cue and the group was close to significance, F(1, 38) = 2.51; p = 0.062; ηp2 = 0.06, reflecting a lower proportion of memories triggered by external cues in the patient group, compared to the control group, p = 0.04. Conversely, the proportion of memories triggered by internal cues tended to higher in the patient group than in the control group, p = 13.

The comparison, using a two-by-two analysis of variance, of memories triggered by external vs. internal cues, in both groups, showed a significant interaction effect, F(1, 38) = 5.13; p = 0.03; ηp2 = 0.12.

Characteristics of Involuntary and Voluntary Memories

Subjective ratings of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories

We compared involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories in patients with schizophrenia and control participants using a series of two-by-two analyses of variance with group (individuals with schizophrenia vs. control participants) as a between-subjects variable and type of retrieval (involuntary vs. voluntary) as a within-subjects variable (see Table 3). The dependent variables (see Table 2) were mean values, calculated across memories for each participant, or (for the measure of mood impact) the proportion of memories retrieved per participant.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The characteristics of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories in patients with schizophrenia and control participants measured in Study 1.

We observed several main effects of the type of retrieval. Involuntary memories scored higher on negative mood impact, spontaneous rehearsal, intrusiveness and (lack of) feeling of control, and they were rated lower on (positive) emotional valence than voluntary memories, Fs(1, 38) > 7.42; ps < 0.01; ηp2 > 0.16. No significant group effect or interaction effect were observed on any of these subjective ratings, ps > 0.13. Only ratings of autobiographical me-ness tended to be lower in the patients group, in comparison to the control group, F(1, 38)= 3.80; p = 0.097; ηp2 = 0.09.

For both involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories (respectively, n = 512 and n = 533), patient and control groups did not differ with regard to the temporal distribution of the memories, χ2 < 3.10; ps > 0.38.

Content of involuntary and voluntary memories

Consistent with previous studies, a large majority of involuntary and voluntary memories, above 74%, were related to mundane daily life events, in both patient and control groups. Hence, we decided to first compare the proportion of memories related to mundane daily life events to the proportion of memories related to other contents (i.e., all other content categories grouped together), conducting Chi-square analyses separately for involuntary and voluntary memories. The content of both involuntary and voluntary memories did not differ between patients and control participants, respectively, χ2 = 2.94; n = 526; p = 0.23 and χ2 = 6.49; n = 537; p = 0.48 (see Figure 2, upper plots).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The upper plots show the frequencies of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories related to mundane daily life events or other categories of event, in patients with schizophrenia and in control participants, in Study 1. The lower plots show the distribution of these other categories of event, across groups and separately for involuntary and voluntary memories, in Study 1. Error bars represent standard errors.

Afterwards, a second run of Chi-square analyses were conducted to analyze the distribution of other content categories across groups, for involuntary and voluntary memories separately. The content of autobiographical memories did not differ significantly across groups, for involuntary recall, χ2 = 13.12; n = 137; p = 0.11, or for voluntary recall, χ2 = 4.30; n = 129; p = 0.12 (see Figure 2, lower plots).

Discussion

The aim of Study 1 was to examine involuntary autobiographical memories of patients with schizophrenia as they spontaneously occur in the course of everyday life. The diary method, used in this study, enabled the collection of critical information about the context in which involuntary memories and spontaneous cognition arise in daily life of people with schizophrenia, which no other method can supply.

With regard to the context in which involuntary autobiographical memories occur, several group differences were identified. First, in comparison to control participants, patients tended to experience more involuntary memories when their attention was diffuse, that is, when not engaged in a current task. This result might be related to those of Elua et al. (18), who showed that patients with schizophrenia, just like control participants, were more likely to report experiencing mind-pops during fairly undemanding activities.

Second, involuntary autobiographical memories were mostly triggered by external environmental cues in both groups, as previously observed (1), but patients reported significantly fewer external cues than controls. Moreover, internal cues (i.e., cues that are only present in thought) tended to trigger involuntary memories more frequently in the patient group, than in the control group. This result might reflect patients' hyper-reflexivity, a symptom related to self-disorders in schizophrenia. The hyper-reflexivity refers to an exaggerated self-consciousness, a (fundamentally non-volitional) tendency to focus on inner processes and phenomena that would normally be experienced tacitly, making them explicit (40, 41).

The present diary study replicated some of the effects of involuntary vs. voluntary retrieval on memory characteristics (for mood impact and spontaneous rehearsal), reported in the literature on non-clinical populations (3, 33, 42). These observations confirm the diary protocol was an effective method for collecting and differentiating between involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories, in the current sample of individuals with schizophrenia and healthy controls.

Considering all memory measures included in Study 1, only autobiographical me-ness tended to be lower in participants with schizophrenia than in control participants, as previously observed for sensory-cued (voluntary) autobiographical memories (29). However, no other group or interaction effects were observed on memory characteristics self-rated by participants, contrary to what was observed in Allé et al. (30) for contextual information and feeling of reliving. The absence of such commonly observed differences [e.g., (28)] might be due to the high dropout rate in the patient group, leading this group to be overrepresented by well-functioning patients. This bias might have attenuated differences between the memory characteristics reported by patients vs. controls.

Quite surprisingly, neither spontaneous rehearsal, nor intrusiveness, nor feeling of control differed between the schizophrenia and control groups. These results could simply reflect the mundane nature of the reported memories in the current study (as reflected in the content coding). Alternatively, these results may reflect patients' active attempt to avoid memories that could be painful or uncomfortable, or their poorer ability to report the most intrusive memories (considering that they might not be able to adequately distinguish them from hallucinations, due to metacognitive deficits).

Several limitations of Study 1 should be acknowledged. The diary method did not allow any control over the type of thought reported in the diary or the proper completion of the questionnaires. Besides, the diary method might have been taxing for participants, as it requires participants to recall the task instructions and hold the memory in mind while they completed the questionnaires.

Study 2 was undertaken to address limitations of Study 1 and replicate and extend its findings. First, the experimental design of Study 2 reduced the cognitive effort required to report involuntary memories, compared with the diary procedure used in Study 1. The task composing Study 2 was less demanding for participants in terms of motivation and autonomy. Besides, the presence of the experimenter during the task made possible a real-time control of the participants' understanding and completion of the questionnaires. Second, Study 2 also enabled to deepen the investigation of involuntary autobiographical memories in schizophrenia, both in terms of qualitative characteristics (including vividness, centrality, emotional intensity, and spatio-temporal context) and conditions in which they are elicited.

Study 2

Methods

Participants

Twenty-five outpatients with schizophrenia (seven women and 18 men) from the Psychiatry Department of Strasbourg University Hospitals (France) took part in the study, as well as a group of 25 matched control participants (see Table 1). Patients' and control participants' recruitment was based on the same criteria as in Study 1.

Design

The design was a 2 (Group: patients with schizophrenia vs. control participants) ×2 (Retrieval: involuntary vs. voluntary) mixed design with Retrieval as within-subject variables.

Materials

A computerized task was designed in E-Prime 2.0 Professional (Psychology Software Tools), consisting of two distinct sessions: the involuntary autobiographical memory elicitation and the voluntary autobiographical memory collection.

Two series of words or phrases, previously recorded by the experimenter and orally presented via a laptop, were used as cues, either to elicit involuntary autobiographical memory in Session 1 or to sample voluntary autobiographical memories in Session 2. Each series of words were composed of 80 items, always presented in the same order, based on words previously used by Berntsen (1) and included various semantic categories (activities, objects, sensory experiences, generic—non personal—themes, life themes, places, wordings, feelings). The two series were randomly assigned to the involuntary vs. voluntary memory sessions.

Procedure

Each participant was individually tested at the Psychiatry Department of the University Hospital of Strasbourg, France. The involuntary and voluntary memory sessions took place at 2 different days (separated by a maximum of 7 days) and were always administered in the same order to avoid the contamination of the involuntary autobiographical memory elicitation with the voluntary autobiographical memory collection.

Participants were seated in front of the laptop displaying a black screen. Each series of words or phrases was played aloud.

Involuntary autobiographical memory session

During the first session, participants were presented the series of words (also referred as cues in the results section) played every 5 s, and were instructed to do nothing but listening carefully to those words. If, at some point, they thought about a past situation during the task, they had to press the spacebar to interrupt the task. The button press aimed to record retrieval time for each involuntary memory elicited. Immediately after the task interruption, a written description of the memory was collected, and the experimenter made sure that the spontaneous thought reported was an actual memory and not another kind of thoughts (i.e., general statement on his/her life, future projection, prospective memory, etc.), by asking the following question: “Are you reporting a memory of past situation you lived?” The number of false alarms was recorded.

When 10 involuntary autobiographical memories had been elicited, the audio-recorded reading of words was terminated. Hence, the number of cues used to elicit the 10 involuntary autobiographical memories varied according to participants' proneness to experience involuntary memories. Participants self-rated memory characteristics using 7-point Likert scales. Some measures were common to Study 1 and additional measures were included to assess vividness, emotional intensity, or context [questions were derived or modified from previous work; (4345) see Table 2].

Voluntary autobiographical memory session

The second session consisted of exactly the same setup, except that participants were specifically asked to strategically retrieve a memory in response to each word or phrase orally presented. In this condition, the series of words was presented with a 60-s delay between each word, to give participant time to think about a personal event and find a memory. Similar to the first session, participants were told to immediately report when they remembered a past event, by pressing the spacebar. Once 10 voluntary memories were collected, the word reading was stopped and participants were asked to answer the same questions as in the involuntary condition to assess the characteristics of their voluntary autobiographical memories (see Table 2).

Results

Protocol Completion

All participants (patients and controls) fully completed the protocol. There is no dropout to report in Study 2.

Experimental Validity: Effects of Voluntary vs. Involuntary Retrieval

We first checked whether the thoughts reported in involuntary and voluntary conditions were actual autobiographical memories. First, in the involuntary condition, the total number of button presses (i.e., task interruptions to report a memory) was recorded. The number of false alarm (i.e., pressing a button to report a thought different from a memory) was compared between both patient and control groups. Patients (M = 2.95; SD = 3.43) committed more false alarms than control participants (M = 1.08; SD = 1.70; t = 2.33; p = 0.02; d = −0.19).

In the voluntary condition, non-autobiographical memories were counted from participants descriptions of their memories, but other thought occurrence was extremely rare (1 for 1 patient and for 2 control participants), meaning that participants successfully completed the voluntary condition of the protocol.

Second, we examined how involuntary retrieval differs from voluntary retrieval (see Table 4 for statistics). In order to validate the new paradigm we developed, we needed to observe robust differences between the two within-subjects conditions.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Retrieval characteristics of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories in patients with schizophrenia and control participants, in Study 2.

First, as expected, the retrieval time was significantly higher in the voluntary condition, strongly supporting the distinction between the two types of retrieval. Second, the number of memories elicited was significantly higher in the voluntary condition compared to the involuntary condition. Third, the number of cues (i.e., words or phrases) necessary to elicit autobiographical memories was significantly higher in the involuntary condition than in the voluntary condition, reflecting that involuntary memories required, on average, five times more cues than voluntary memories. These findings are consistent with previous work on involuntary vs. voluntary memories in healthy individuals, using an experimental approach (46). Participants also assessed their cognitive effort associated with memory retrieval as being higher in the voluntary condition compared to the involuntary condition.

Emotional intensity and spontaneous rehearsal were higher in involuntary autobiographical memories in comparison to voluntary autobiographical memories, Fs(1, 48) > 4.21; ps < 0.03; ηp2 > 0.08 (see Table 5), whereas the surprise effect associated with retrieval did not differ between involuntary and voluntary retrieval. In contrast, belief in occurrence was lower in involuntary autobiographical memories in comparison to voluntary autobiographical memories, F(1, 48) = 11.14; p = 0.01; ηp2 = 0.20 (see Table 5).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. The characteristics of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories in patients with schizophrenia and control participants in Study 2.

Overall, these findings support the validity of the current experimental paradigm by showing that this paradigm reliably distinguished involuntary from voluntary memories.

Effects of Patient vs. Control Group and Interaction Effects

The conditions of elicitation of involuntary memories (see Figure 3)

The category of memory triggers (i.e., activities, objects, sensory experiences, generic—non-personal—themes, life themes, places, wordings or feelings) did not differ between patient and control groups, χ2 = 10.35; n = 391; p = 0.17 (see Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The plot shows the proportion of involuntary autobiographical memories triggered by each cue category in patients with schizophrenia and in control participants, in Study 2. Error bars represent standard errors.

Self-rated qualitative characteristics of autobiographical memory

We compared involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories in individuals with schizophrenia and control participants using a series of two-by-two analyses of variance with group (schizophrenia vs. control) as a between-subjects variable and type of retrieval (involuntary vs. voluntary) as a within-subjects variable (see Table 5). The dependent variables were mean values, calculated across memories for each participant, or (for mood impact) the proportion of memories retrieved per participant.

However, we observed several main effects of group, showing a pattern of lower ratings in the patient group, in comparison to the control group, for auditory and visual vividness, Fs(1, 48) > 16.38; ps < 0.001; ηp2 > 0.26, temporal context, F(1, 48) = 7.92; p = 0.007; ηp2 = 0.14, personal location, F(1, 48) = 4.43; p = 0.04; ηp2 = 0.09, event content, F(1, 48) = 10.89; p = 0.002; ηp2 = 0.19, and event layout, F(1, 48) = 10.29; p = 0.002; ηp2 = 0.18. Trends were also observed for the frequency of spontaneous rehearsal and the visual perspective, Fs(1, 48) > 3.38; ps < 0.07; ηp2 > 0.07. Centrality of events was rated higher in patient group in comparison to control group, F(1, 48) = 5.48; p = 0.02; ηp2 = 0.10.

Two significant interactions were observed between group and type of retrieval for belief in occurrence and emotional intensity, Fs(1, 48) > 4.18; ps < 0.04; ηp2 > 0.08. Post-hoc analyses showed that patients' involuntary memories were rated as emotionally less intense than controls' involuntary memories, p < 0.001, whereas no difference was observed for voluntary memories. The belief in occurrence was significantly lower for patients' involuntary autobiographical memories in comparison to their voluntary memories, p = 0.002, whereas this difference was not present in the control group.

In addition, a trend was observed for the proportion of memories with no mood impact, F(1, 48) = 3.94; p = 0.053; ηp2 = 0.08, reflecting that in the patient group the proportion of memories with no mood impact was higher in the involuntary condition in comparison to the voluntary condition, while the reverse pattern being observed in the control group.

No group effect was observed on the retention time of the autobiographical memories, F(1, 48) = 0.60; p = 0.44; ηp2 = 0.01.

Content of involuntary and voluntary memories

Similar to Study 1, we first compared the proportion of memories related to mundane daily life events to the proportion of memories related to other contents (i.e., all other content categories grouped together), conducting Chi-square analyses separately for involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories. The content of both involuntary and voluntary memories differed between patients and control participants, respectively, χ2 = 10.49; n = 383; p = 0.001 and χ2 = 7.14; n = 487; p = 0.007 (see Figure 4, upper plots), reflecting that patients recorded more memories classified as “other.”

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The upper plots show the frequencies of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories related to mundane daily life events or other categories of event, in patients with schizophrenia and in control participants, in Study 2. The lower plots show the distribution of these other categories of event, across groups and separately for involuntary and voluntary memories, in Study 2. Error bars represent standard errors. ¤p < 0.10; *p < 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01.

Afterwards, a series of Chi-square analyses was conducted to analyze the distribution of other content categories across groups, for involuntary and voluntary memories separately (respectively, n = 54 and n = 77).

With regard to involuntary autobiographical memory, the thematic content of memories was highly similar between patients and control participants, a comparison across all event categories using Fisher's exact test showed do significant differences (all ps > 0.10)

On contrary, the thematic content of voluntary autobiographical memories differed between patient and control groups for “major achievement” category, χ2 = 4.16; n = 25; p = 0.04 and “death” category, χ2 = 9.33; n = 14; p = 0.002, which were less frequently observed in patients' memories than in controls' memories. The other categories of thematic content were similarly represented across groups; comparisons across all event categories using Fisher's exact test showed do significant differences (all ps > 0.10).

Discussion

Study 2 was conducted to further pursue the mechanisms underlying the activation of involuntary memories and to explore other qualitative characteristics of involuntary memories in schizophrenia, using an experimental paradigm. Importantly, the new experimental set-up showed to be efficient in eliciting involuntary memories and distinguishing them from voluntary memories, and thereby provided a methodological advancement to address limitations observed in Study 1.

Study 2 replicated and extended the findings of Study 1. First, similarly to Study 1 results, Study 2 showed no significant group differences on me-ness, spontaneous rehearsal, visual perspective, emotional component, and on memories content—the majority referring to mundane, daily life events, in both patient and control groups.

Second, with regard to the mechanisms of memories activation, the experimental setup of Study 2 allowed us to observe that patients with schizophrenia reported involuntary autobiographical memories more frequently than control participants, using overall fewer cues. In other words, involuntary autobiographical memories, but not their voluntary counterpart, were more easily triggered in the patient group, compared to the control group. This may reflect patients' sensitivity to triggers, and their specific proneness to involuntary autobiographical memories. These results can be related to those of Study 1 showing that patients had difficulties identifying memories' triggers, probably reflecting an overwhelming sensitivity to triggers.

In addition to their involuntary memory proneness, patients with schizophrenia reported significantly more other types of thoughts in the involuntary condition, compared to controls and to the voluntary condition. This finding likely reflects patients' difficulties in scanning and monitoring their stream of consciousness in order to report only involuntary memories (and not spontaneous future projections, personal reflections or prospective memories, for instance), as instructed. This relates to patients' metacognitive deficits (4750) or internal source-monitoring deficits (51), that is, their poorer ability to distinguish between different internal sources of mental representation, such as memories from imagined events (52, 53).

These results are in agreement with research on thought disorders in schizophrenia, showing higher semantic association (54, 55), more frequent cognitive intrusions (16), and more frequent semantic mind-pops (17, 18) in schizophrenia patients, in comparison to control groups.

Going further, the new measures added to Study 2 showed significant group differences, reflecting that patients' memories (both involuntary and voluntary) were less vivid, had less spatial and temporal context information, but were rated as more central to identity than those of controls. No interaction effects were observed on those variables, meaning that involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories were similarly impaired in terms of vividness and scene measures, but enhanced with regard to their centrality. The findings on impaired vividness and contextual information are in full agreement with existing literature on voluntary [e.g., (28, 34, 35)] and involuntary (30) autobiographical memory in schizophrenia.

Nonetheless, the higher centrality of autobiographical memory in the patient group contradicts previous results showing no differences between patients and control participants regarding centrality for very important personal events (56), or studies showing a deficit in self-integration of important autobiographical memories in schizophrenia (5658). Surprisingly, in the current study, while most of the reported memories were related to mundane events in both groups (see content coding), and not particularly emotional, patients attributed a higher degree of centrality to their memories in comparison to control participants. This finding is consistent with an enhanced self-focus observed in most clinical disorders (59). In addition, the higher centrality of autobiographical memories in the patient group, in spite of their mundane content, could relate to the assignment of inappropriate salience usually observed in schizophrenia, leading certain precepts and ideas to be assigned an exaggerated importance [for a review see (60)]. The aberrant salience to non-relevant stimuli has been thought to relate to difficulties in differentiating self-relevant from self-irrelevant information, which might be explained by dysfunctional self-referential processes (61). Therefore, impairment in self-referential processes in schizophrenia might explain the higher centrality of patients' memories in comparison to controls.

Two significant interactions between group and type of retrieval were observed in Study 2. First, involuntary autobiographical memories were emotionally more intense than their voluntary counterpart in the control group, which is in line with some previous findings (62, 63), whereas this difference was not significant in the patient group.

Second, a significant interaction was also observed for belief in occurrence, showing that patients' involuntary autobiographical memories were associated with a lower belief in occurrence in comparison to their voluntary memories, whereas no differences were observed in the control group. Because involuntary memories are unintentional and uncontrolled, information about the cognitive operations necessary to reconstruct these memories are lacking, potentially making them less easily recognized as self-generated memories (64). In addition, patients' internal source monitoring deficit affects their ability to distinguish reality (e.g., memories of past events) from imagination (e.g., future projection), which might impact even more severely their involuntary autobiographical memories, compared to voluntary autobiographical memories.

General Discussion

Studies 1 and 2 were undertaken to explore involuntary remembering in schizophrenia and the conditions in which it occurs in those patients, both in daily life and in an experimental context. Studies 1 and 2 assessed similar characteristics of autobiographical memories but were also designed to investigate different aspects of involuntary remembering in schizophrenia. Study 1 provided information on involuntary remembering in patients' daily life, while Study 2 allowed us to measure quantitative parameters of involuntary retrieval, not accessible with a diary method (e.g., the retrieval time or the propensity to experience involuntary memories in response to experimenter generated cues).

Studies 1 and 2 provided a body of convergent evidence on the mechanisms underlying the activation of involuntary autobiographical memories in schizophrenia. Patients with schizophrenia were more prone to experience involuntary autobiographical memories and more sensitive to triggers (Study 2). In particular, they seemed to be more sensitive to internal cues than controls (Study 1). The higher proneness to experiencing involuntary autobiographical memory in patients might be caused by deficits in intentional inhibition, which are well-described in schizophrenia (65, 66), making spontaneous memories difficult to control and avoid.

The findings from both studies might be related to patients' impairments in self-reflective processes (including metacognition, self-referential, and internal source-monitoring). More precisely, patients' difficulties with identifying their involuntary memories per se (Study 2) are likely to be caused by patients' metacognitive deficits (4750). The lower proportion of external cues and higher proportion of internal cues in the patient group, compared to the control group in Study 1, might be related to an aberrant salience of inner stimuli (40, 41). Relatedly, despite the mundane nature of both patients and controls' involuntary memories, patients rated their memories as being more central to their identity in comparison to controls, thus allocating a higher salience to their mental events. Together, these findings might reflect deficits in self-referential processes in schizophrenia. Finally, patients' involuntary autobiographical memories were associated with a lower belief in occurrence, compared to their voluntary memories and compared to both involuntary and voluntary memories in the control group, possibly reflecting source monitoring disorders in schizophrenia. All together, these results suggest that self-disorders may alter how patients involuntarily remember personal events and the associated experience of control.

To sum up, the difficulty of patients in identifying their mental states and thoughts, associated with a lack of cognitive control, could make them more sensitive to triggers (in particular internal triggers) that elicit involuntary autobiographical memories. Hence, patients' involuntary memories, mostly related to mundane events with low emotional load, are experienced more frequently. Besides, although involuntary and voluntary memories were vague, poorly contextualized and associated with a lower belief in occurrence, patients considered them as more central to the self.

Recent works highlighted the relationship between involuntary autobiographical memory and hallucination within the psychosis continuum including people with schizophrenia (26, 27). Hallucinations are characterized by a failure of self-recognition leading to the externalization of self-generated events [for a review see (67)]. Among the various candidates thought to explain hallucinations (inner speech, semantic mind-pops, memory, etc.), memories of personal events have the potential to account for the wide phenomenological diversity of hallucinations in schizophrenia as ~60% of patients also report non-verbal hallucinations (i.e., not voices), such as environmental noises or animal sounds, in addition to regular verbal hallucinations (68, 69). Moreover, a majority of patients report to know the identity of their voices, and about half agree with the idea that the content of their voices could be the reproduction of speech that they have heard in the past (70).

Although indirectly and speculatively, the present studies on involuntary remembering in patients with schizophrenia provided new evidence suggesting parallels between hallucination and involuntary autobiographical memories [see also (26)]. Concerning the conditions in which involuntary memories occur, we showed that, similarly to hallucinations (68, 71), they were more likely to pop-up during diffuse attentional states (Study 1). Besides, we observed that patients' involuntary autobiographical remembering was also characterized by a deficit of self-reflective processes (impaired triggers identification and more internal triggers in Study 1, and impaired involuntary autobiographical memories identification, higher centrality and lower belief in occurrence in Study 2).

With regard to the emotional component of autobiographical memory, results were mixed. No differences were observed between groups, in terms of emotional valence or mood impact, in any of the two studies. However, in Study 2, patients' memories tended to be less associated with physical reaction or mood impact, than did memories of controls, but these tendencies were not seen in Study 1. Finally, emotional intensity was similar in patients' involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories, whereas the control group's involuntary autobiographical memories were more emotionally intense, in comparison to their voluntary autobiographical memories. This result differs from previous findings, showing higher emotional intensity associated with involuntary autobiographical memories of individuals with attenuated psychotic symptoms (26, 27) and also higher proportion of trauma-related involuntary memories in the psychosis continuum (26). However, these prior studies focused only on examples of selected involuntary autobiographical memories that were retrospectively rated, which may have led to a biased collection of salient involuntary memories. On the contrary, words or phrases describing everyday life activities and objects were used in the present study to trigger involuntary memories, leading the memories to mostly refer to mundane events.

These results agree with the mixed findings in the literature on emotion and autobiographical memory in schizophrenia and the difficulty to draw any firm conclusions with regard to the emotional component of memory in schizophrenia [see (56, 57, 7275)]. Future research on emotion and autobiographical memory in schizophrenia could benefit from the use of objective measures of physiological arousal [e.g., (76)] in patients, as these measures have been shown to correlate to some extent with the subjective ratings of emotion in the general population (77).

Studies 1 and 2 both share the limitation that only self-reports were used to assess the qualitative characteristics of autobiographical memories. Insofar as it is well-known that patients also suffer from several impairments of awareness of their own state and of insight [for a review see (78)], the question of patients' abilities to correctly self-assess and report their mental experiences therefore remains. However, in the present studies, we were primarily interested in the contrast of the subjective experience of patients while they were having involuntary vs. voluntary memories. Hence, capturing the subjective experience, regardless of its objective accuracy, is of prime importance with regard to patients' clinical symptoms, such as hallucinations. As a perspective, future research should look at correlations between autobiographical memory measures and cognitive functions in larger sample of participants.

In addition, the design of the present studies might not have allowed us to capture distressing involuntary memories in patients with schizophrenia whereas intrusive thoughts are well-described in literature on thought disorders in schizophrenia (15, 16). Further studies might specifically address stress-related involuntary memories in schizophrenia.

In conclusion, the present studies investigated involuntary remembering in schizophrenia using both a structured diary methodology in participants' everyday environments and in an experimental method in a laboratory context. The results demonstrated that both the conditions under which involuntary memories occur, and the involuntary remembering experience itself, are altered in schizophrenia. Hence, involuntary autobiographical memory presents a specific pattern of alterations in schizophrenia. Viewed together, the findings suggest some parallels between involuntary remembering in schizophrenia and patients' hallucination. Future research should investigate the nature of this relationship, focusing more specifically on distressing involuntary autobiographical memories.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Comité de Protection des Personnes Sud Méditerranée IV. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants, and where applicable, the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

MA and DB designed the studies. MA collected the data, conducted the analyses, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. DB, FB, and J-MD provided resources to conduct the research and provided substantial contributions to the manuscript. DB and FB supervised the study. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Danish National Research Foundation [DNRF89] and Independent Research Fund, Denmark [9037-00015B].

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Berntsen D. Voluntary and involuntary access to autobiographical memory. Memory. (1998) 6:113–41. doi: 10.1080/741942071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Berntsen D. The unbidden past: involuntary autobiographical memories as a basic mode of remembering. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2010) 19:138–42. doi: 10.1177/0963721410370301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Berntsen D. Involuntary Autobiographical Memories: An Introduction to the Unbidden Past. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2009).

Google Scholar

4. Singer J. L. (1966). Daydreaming: An Introduction to the Experimental Study of Inner Experience. New York, NY: Random House.

Google Scholar

5. Berntsen D. Involuntary autobiographical memories. Appl Cogn Psychol. (1996) 10:435–54. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199610)10:5<435::AID-ACP408>3.0.CO;2-L

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Hall SA, Rubin DC, Miles A, Davis SW, Wing EA, Cabeza R, et al. The neural basis of involuntary episodic memories. J Cogn Neurosci. (2014) 26:2385–99. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Berntsen D. Spontaneous recollections: involuntary autobiographical memories are a basic mode of remembering. In: Understanding Autobiographical Memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2012).

Google Scholar

8. Suddendorf T, Addis DR, Corballis MC. Mental time travel and the shaping of the human mind. Philos Trans Roy Soc B Biol Sci. (2009) 364:1317–24. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol. (2002) 53:1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Pillemer DB. Directive functions of autobiographical memory: the guiding power of the specific episode. Memory. (2003) 11:193–202. doi: 10.1080/741938208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Rasmussen AS, Berntsen D. Emotional valence and the functions of autobiographical memories: positive and negative memories serve different functions. Mem Cognit. (2009) 37:477–92. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.4.477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th ed. American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

13. Rachman S. Obsessional ruminations. Behav Res Ther. (1971) 9:229–35. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(71)90008-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Rachman S, de Silva P. Abnormal and normal obsessions. Behav Res Ther. (1978) 16:233–48. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(78)90022-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Morrison AP, Haddock G, Tarrier N. Intrusive thoughts and auditory hallucinations: a cognitive approach. Behav Cogn Psychother. (1995) 23:265–80. doi: 10.1017/S1352465800015873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Morrison AP, Baker CA. Intrusive thoughts and auditory hallucinations: a comparative study of intrusions in psychosis. Behav Res Ther. (2000) 38:1097–106. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00143-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Elua I, Laws KR, Kvavilashvili L. From mind-pops to hallucinations? A study of involuntary semantic memories in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. (2012) 196:165–70. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Elua I, Laws KR, Kvavilashvili L. Increased frequency of involuntary semantic memories or mind-pops in schizophrenia: a diary study. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (2015) 20:502–11. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2015.1092431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. David AS. The cognitive neuropsychiatry of auditory verbal hallucinations: an overview. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (2004) 9:107–23. doi: 10.1080/13546800344000183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

20. Larøi F, Woodward TS. Hallucinations from a cognitive perspective. Harv Rev Psychiatry. (2007) 15:109–17. doi: 10.1080/10673220701401993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med. (2009) 39:179–95. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Jones V, Steel C. Schizotypal personality and vulnerability to involuntary autobiographical memories. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2012) 43:871–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Eysenck HJ, Barrett P. The nature of schizotypy. Psychol Rep. (1993) 73:59–63. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1993.73.1.59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Holmes EA, Steel C. Schizotypy: a vulnerability factor for traumatic intrusions. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2004) 192:28–34. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000105997.28152.4b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Jones V, Steel C. Examining vulnerability to involuntary memories in schizophrenia comorbid with post-traumatic stress disorder. Schizophrenia Res. (2014) 152:487–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.11.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Allé MC, Berna F, Berntsen D. Individuals with psychotic-like experiences exhibit enhanced involuntary autobiographical memories. Psychiatry Res. (2019) 273:281–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.01.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Allé MC, Berna F, Berntsen D. Involuntary autobiographical memory and future thought predicting hallucination proneness. Clin Psychol Sci. (2018) 6:891–98. doi: 10.1177/2167702618785618

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Berna F, Potheegadoo J, Aouadi I, Ricarte JJ, Allé MC, Coutelle R, et al. A meta-analysis of autobiographical memory studies in schizophrenia spectrum disorder. Schizophr Bull. (2016) 42:56–66. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Allé MC, Berna F, Danion J-M, Berntsen D. Seeing or hearing one's memories: Manipulating autobiographical memory imagery in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. (2020) 286:112835. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Allé MC, Berna F, Danion J-M, Berntsen D. Unraveling the role of retrieval deficits in autobiographical memory impairment in schizophrenia: a comparison of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories. Under Review. (2020).

31. Berna F, Huron C, Kazès M, Offerlin-Meyer I, Willard D, Verry P, et al. Chronic persecutory delusion and autobiographical memories in patients with schizophrenia: a diary study. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci. (2014) 51:25–33.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

32. Pernot-Marino E., Schuster C., Hedelin G., Berna F., Zimmermann M. A., Danion J. M. (2010). True and false autobiographical memories in schizophrenia: Preliminary results of a diary study. Psychiatry Res. 179:1–5.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

33. Berntsen D, Jacobsen AS. Involuntary (spontaneous) mental time travel into the past and future. Conscious Cogn. (2008) 17:1093–104. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Danion J-M, Cuervo C, Piolino P, Huron C, Riutort M, Peretti CS, et al. Conscious recollection in autobiographical memory: an investigation in schizophrenia. Conscious Cogn. (2005) 14:535–47. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2005.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Raffard S, D'Argembeau A, Bayard S, Boulenger J-P, Van der Linden M. Scene construction in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology. (2010) 24:608–15. doi: 10.1037/a0019113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (1987) 13:261–76. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri W. Comparison of beck depression inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess. (1996) 67:588–97. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Addington D, Addington J, Maticka-Tyndale E. Assessing depression in schizophrenia: the calgary depression scale. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. (1993) 22:39–44. doi: 10.1192/S0007125000292581

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Berntsen D, Hall NM. The episodic nature of involuntary autobiographical memories. Mem Cognit. (2004) 32:789–803. doi: 10.3758/BF03195869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Sass LA. The ego and its fate. An “archaeologic” study of the psychoanalytic avant garde theory (I). Psyche. (1992) 46:52–90.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

41. Sass LA, Parnas J. Schizophrenia, consciousness, and the self. Schizophr Bull. (2003) 29:427–44. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Johannessen KB, Berntsen D. Current concerns in involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories. Conscious Cogn. (2010) 19:847–60. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Berntsen D, Bohn A. Remembering and forecasting: the relation between autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking. Mem Cognit. (2010) 38:265–78. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.3.265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Rubin DC, Schrauf RW, Greenberg DL. Belief and recollection of autobiographical memories. Mem Cognit. (2003) 31:887–901. doi: 10.3758/BF03196443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Rubin DC, Berntsen D. The frequency of voluntary and involuntary autobiographical memories across the life span. Mem Cognit. (2009) 37:679–88. doi: 10.3758/37.5.679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Berntsen D, Staugaard SR, Sørensen LMT. Why am I remembering this now? Predicting the occurrence of involuntary (spontaneous) episodic memories. J Exp Psychol Gen. (2013) 142:426–44. doi: 10.1037/a0029128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Hasson-Ohayon I, Avidan-Msika M, Mashiach-Eizenberg M, Kravetz S, Rozencwaig S, Shalev H, et al. Metacognitive and social cognition approaches to understanding the impact of schizophrenia on social quality of life. Schizophrenia Res. (2015) 161:386–91. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Lysaker PH, Dimaggio G. Metacognitive capacities for reflection in schizophrenia: implications for developing treatments. Schizophr Bull. (2014) 40:487–91. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Massé M, Lecomte T. Metacognitive profiles in individuals with a first episode of psychosis and their relation to social functioning and perceived social support. Schizophrenia Res. (2015) 166:60–64. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Vohs JL, Lysaker PH. Metacognitive mastery and intrinsic motivation in schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2014) 202:74–7. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Sapara A, Ffytche DH, Cooke MA, Williams SCR, Kumari V. Is it me? Verbal self-monitoring neural network and clinical insight in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. (2015) 234:328–35. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Brébion G, Ohlsen RI, Pilowsky LS, David AS. Visual hallucinations in schizophrenia: confusion between imagination and perception. Neuropsychology. (2008) 22:383–9. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.3.383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Mintz S, Alpert M. Imagery vividness, reality testing, and schizophrenic hallucinations. J Abnorm Psychol. (1972) 79:310–6. doi: 10.1037/h0033209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Moritz S, Mersmann K, Kloss M, Jacobsen D, Wilke U, Andresen B, et al. “Hyper-priming” in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients. Psychol Med. (2001) 31:221–9. doi: 10.1017/S0033291701003105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Wentura D, Moritz S, Frings C. Further evidence for “hyper-priming” in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients using repeated masked category priming. Schizophrenia Res. (2008) 102:69–75. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Allé MC, Potheegadoo J, Köber C, Schneider P, Coutelle R, Habermas T, et al. Impaired coherence of life narratives of patients with schizophrenia. Sci Rep. (2015) 5:12934. doi: 10.1038/srep12934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Berna F, Bennouna-Greene M, Potheegadoo J, Verry P, Conway MA, Danion J-M. Impaired ability to give a meaning to personally significant events in patients with schizophrenia. Conscious Cogn. (2011) 20:703–11. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Raffard S, D'Argembeau A, Lardi C, Bayard S, Boulenger J-P, Van der Linden M. Narrative identity in schizophrenia. Conscious Cogn. (2010) 19:328–40. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Ingram RE. Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: review and a conceptual model. Psychol Bull. (1990) 107:156–76. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Kapur S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. (2003) 160:13–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Pankow A, Katthagen T, Diner S, Deserno L, Boehme R, Kathmann N, et al. Aberrant salience is related to dysfunctional self-referential processing in psychosis. Schizophr Bull. (2016) 42:67–76. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Palacio-Gonzalez A, del Berntsen D, Watson LA. Emotional intensity and emotion regulation in response to autobiographical memories during dysphoria. Cognit Ther Res. (2017) 41:530–42. doi: 10.1007/s10608-017-9841-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Watson LA, Berntsen D, Kuyken W, Watkins ER. The characteristics of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories in depressed and never depressed individuals. Conscious Cogn. (2012) 21:1382–92. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Johnson MK, Hashtroudi S, Stephen D. Source monitoring. Psychol Bull. (1993) 114:3–28. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Badcock JC, Waters FAV, Maybery MT, Michie PT. Auditory hallucinations: failure to inhibit irrelevant memories. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (2005) 10:125–36. doi: 10.1080/13546800344000363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Waters FAV, Badcock JC, Michie PT, Maybery MT. Auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia: intrusive thoughts and forgotten memories. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (2006) 11:65–83. doi: 10.1080/13546800444000191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Waters F, Allen P, Aleman A, Fernyhough C, Woodward TS, Badcock JC, et al. Auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia and nonschizophrenia populations: a review and integrated model of cognitive mechanisms. Schizophr Bull. (2012) 38:683–93. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Nayani TH, David AS. The auditory hallucination: a phenomenological survey. Psychol Med. (1996) 26:177–89. doi: 10.1017/S003329170003381X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Woods A, Jones N, Alderson-Day B, Callard F, Fernyhough C. Experiences of hearing voices: analysis of a novel phenomenological survey. Lancet Psychiatry. (2015) 2:323–31. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00006-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. McCarthy-Jones S, Trauer T, Mackinnon A, Sims E, Thomas N, Copolov DL. A new phenomenological survey of auditory hallucinations: evidence for subtypes and implications for theory and practice. Schizophr Bull. (2014) 40:231–5. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Tarrier N. An investigation of residual psychotic symptoms in discharged schizophrenic patients. Br J Clin Psychol. (1987) 26:141–3. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1987.tb00740.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Ben Malek H, D'Argembeau A, Allé MC, Meyer N, Danion J-M, Berna F. Temporal processing of past and future autobiographical events in patients with schizophrenia. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:13858. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50447-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Cuervo-Lombard C, Lemogne C, Gierski F, Béra-Potelle C, Tran E, Portefaix C, et al. Neural basis of autobiographical memory retrieval in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. (2012) 201:473–80. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.099820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Neumann A, Philippot P, Danion J-M. Impairment of autonoetic awareness for emotional events in schizophrenia. Can J Psychiatry. (2007) 52:450–6. doi: 10.1177/070674370705200707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Raffard S, D'Argembeau A, Lardi C, Bayard S, Boulenger J-P, Van Der Linden M. Exploring self-defining memories in schizophrenia. Memory. (2009) 17:26–38. doi: 10.1080/09658210802524232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Garbarino M., Lai M., Bender D., Picard R. W., Tognetti S. Empatica E3 – A wearable wireless multi-sensor device for real-time computerized biofeedback and data acquisition. In Proceedings of the 2014 4th International Conference on Wireless Mobile Communication and Healthcare – Transforming Healthcare through Innovations in Mobile and Wireless Technologies (MOBIHEALTH) (Athens: IEEE) (2015) 39–42. doi: 10.1109/MOBIHEALTH.2014.7015904

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Salgado S, Kingo OS. How is physiological arousal related to self-reported measures of emotional intensity and valence of events and their autobiographical memories? Conscious Cogn. (2019) 75:102811. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2019.102811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Raffard S, Bayard S, Capdevielle D, Garcia F, Boulenger J-P, Gely-Nargeot M-C. [Lack of insight in schizophrenia: a review]. L'Encephale. (2008) 34:511–6. doi: 10.1016/j.encep.2007.10.012

CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: diary study, involuntary autobiographical memory, schizophrenia, self-reflection, triggers

Citation: Allé MC, Berna F, Danion J-M and Berntsen D (2020) Involuntary Autobiographical Memories in Schizophrenia: Characteristics and Conditions of Elicitation. Front. Psychiatry 11:567189. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.567189

Received: 07 July 2020; Accepted: 08 September 2020;
Published: 09 October 2020.

Edited by:

Błażej Misiak, Wroclaw Medical University, Poland

Reviewed by:

Mohamad El Haj, Université de Nantes, France
John Mace, Eastern Illinois University, United States

Copyright © 2020 Allé, Berna, Danion and Berntsen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mélissa C. Allé, melissa.alle@univ-lille.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.