Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 14 January 2021
Sec. Addictive Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Problematic Internet Technology Use: Assessment, Risk Factors, Comorbidity, Adverse Consequences and Intervention View all 13 articles

Self-Reported Screen Time on Social Networking Sites Associated With Problematic Smartphone Use in Chinese Adults: A Population-Based Study

  • 1School of Nursing, University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China
  • 2School of Public Health, University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China
  • 3Aberdeen Kai-fong Welfare Association Social Service, Hong Kong, China

Background: Problematic smartphone use (PSU) has been associated with screen time in general, but little is known about the effect of different screen-based activities. We examined the associations of self-reported time spent on overall and specific screen-based activities with PSU and its addictive symptoms in Hong Kong Chinese adults.

Methods: We analyzed data from 562 smartphone owners (56.5% female; 82.1% aged 25–64 years) in a population-based telephone survey in 2017. PSU was measured using Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version (range 10–60) which includes symptoms of daily-life disturbance, withdrawal, cyberspace-oriented relationship, overuse, and tolerance. Screen time was self-reported as average hours per day spent on the internet, online book/newspaper/magazine, online video, and social networking sites (SNS). Multivariable linear regression analyzed the associations of self-reported screen time with PSU severity and symptoms. Interaction effects of sex, age group, educational attainment, and monthly household income were examined.

Results: Self-reported time spent on overall screen-based activities was associated with PSU severity (β = 1.35, 95% CI 0.15, 2.55) and withdrawal and overuse symptoms, after adjusting for sociodemographic and health-related variables. Independent association was observed for self-reported SNS time with PSU severity (β = 1.42, 95% CI 0.35, 2.49) and symptoms of withdrawal and cyberspace-oriented relationship, after mutually adjusting for time on other activities. The strongest association between self-reported SNS time and PSU severity was observed in younger than older adults (β = 4.36, 95% CI 2.58, 6.13; P for interaction = 0.004).

Conclusions: The independent association of self-reported SNS time with PSU and core addictive symptoms highlighted the addiction potential of SNS use, particularly in younger users.

Introduction

Excessive use of the internet has raised concerns about its behavioral addiction potential (i.e., internet use disorders), among which online gaming disorder has been added in ICD-11 (1). Problematic smartphone use (PSU) is suggested as a subtype of internet use disorder (2) showing the similar neurobiological pathway of structural and functional brain abnormalities with addictive behaviors (e.g., gambling disorder) (3, 4). PSU could represent symptoms of salience, mood modification, withdrawal, tolerance, conflict, and relapse as posited in the behavioral addiction components model (5, 6). Such symptoms have a spectrum of severity that can be less or more significant to indicate pathological addiction (7). Core criteria of PSU were proposed to include symptoms related to withdrawal, tolerance, and functional impairments (of physical/psychological health, social relationship, or workplace/school performance) (7, 8). The diagnostic accuracy of the criteria has been shown in studies to distinguish online gaming disorder from non-pathological use (9). However, it should be noted that PSU is not an official diagnosis in ICD-11 or DSM-5 and may not be comparable to the established heroin and tobacco addictions on severity and/or associated health problems (7). Debates are ongoing on whether PSU qualifies for an addiction (10, 11). We used the term “PSU” as recommended to avoid over-pathologizing of everyday-life behaviors (1214).

PSU is found to be associated with self-reported overall screen time (15, 16). The association was supported in studies objectively measuring smartphone use on different devices (Android and iOS) (1719). The screen time can involve various activities differing in using motives categorized into process and social use (2023). Process use is for news consumption, entertainment, or other nonsocial motives, such as surfing the internet/websites, reading online book/newspaper/magazine, and watching online videos, while social use is for social interaction motives, such as using social networking sites (SNS; e.g., Facebook, Twitter, WhatsApp, WeChat) (21).

Results are mixed on associations between different screen-based activities and PSU. Social-oriented activity was associated with PSU (15), particularly in females who might have greater sociality than males and younger people who might be more active on SNS (24). Exposure to social comparison information on SNS, such as a large amount of “Likes” and comments, perfect body images, and idealized lives, might reduce self-esteem and increase depression (25), which are consistently associated with PSU (2628). Other research showed stronger associations of process-oriented activities with PSU adjusting for sex and age (20). Spending more time on process-oriented activities could be a mechanism for individuals with higher anxiety to deflect negative stressors (29), while anxiety and dysfunctional emotion regulation could lead to PSU (20). Little is known about the potential moderating role of educational attainment and income, despite that electronic literacy and pattern of engagement in screen-based activities may differ by socioeconomic status (30).

We used the Uses and Gratifications Theory (UGT) (31) and the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors (32) to theoretically conceptualize the study. The UGT posits that individual differences motivate people to increasingly use specific types of electronic media to satisfy underlying needs (31). UGT was proposed for traditional media but has been widely applied to the advanced screen-based activities (20, 21, 26). Individual differences in the UGT can include sociodemographic and psychological characteristics such as anxiety and depression symptoms (21). The more comprehensive I-PACE model can be used to explain associations between specific screen-based activities and PSU (23, 33). Specific using motives plus general characteristics (e.g., sociodemographic and psychological characteristics) could predispose individuals to the onset and maintenance of PSU as posited in the I-PACE model (32).

Hong Kong, the most developed city in China, has one of the highest smartphone penetration rates worldwide (91.5% in 2019) (34). Our previous study showed a high prevalence of PSU (38.5%) in Hong Kong (35). We have also found that PSU was associated with impaired mental health and family well-being (36, 37). This study took advantage of a population-based survey in Hong Kong Chinese adults to examine the associations of time spent on overall and specific types of screen-based activities with PSU and its addictive symptoms. We also examined the potential interaction effects of sex, age group, educational attainment, and monthly household income on these associations.

Materials and Methods

Design and Participants

The Hong Kong Family and Health Information Trends Survey (FHInTS) was a periodic territory-wide telephone survey on the general public's behaviors and views regarding information use, individual health, and family well-being, under the project “FAMILY: A Jockey Club Initiative for a Harmonious Society.” We conducted five waves of FHInTS since 2009. The present landline telephone survey was part of the fifth wave of FHInTS from February to August 2017. Details of the study design have been reported elsewhere (30, 35). The target population was Cantonese-speaking Hong Kong residents aged 18 years or above. A two-stage probability-based sampling method was used to minimalize sampling bias. In the first stage of random-digit-dialing, telephone numbers were randomly generated using known prefixes assigned to telecommunication service providers by the Government Office of the Communications Authority, which covered nearly all households in Hong Kong. Invalid numbers were removed according to the computer and manual dialing records. Telephone numbers of respondents from previous waves were also filtered. In the second stage of within household sampling, once a household was successfully reached, an eligible family member whose next birthday was the closest to the interview day was invited for the survey. All telephone interviews were conducted by trained interviewers from Public Opinion Programme (POP) at the University of Hong Kong. Of 5,773 eligible subjects, 4,054 responded to the survey (response rate = 70.2%). Six hundred eighty-six respondents were randomly selected to answer questions on screen time and PSU. The final study sample comprised 562 smartphone owners, after excluding smartphone nonowners.

Measures

PSU was measured using the ten-item Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version (SAS-SV), with each item scoring on a six-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 6 = strongly agree) (38). A higher total SAS-SV score (range 10–60) indicates a higher PSU severity (38). The addictive symptoms of cyberspace-oriented relationship, overuse, and tolerance each have one item; symptoms of daily-life disturbance have three items; and withdrawal symptoms have four items (38). The average score of these multi-item symptoms was calculated (range 1–6). The Chinese version of SAS-SV was found reliable and valid in our previous study (35). Cronbach's alpha was 0.84 in the present sample. SAS-SV scores could be dichotomized into “non-PSU” and “PSU” using suggested cutoffs (male 31; female 33) (38). However, the cutoffs were established by receiver-operating characteristic analyses on adolescents (38), which may be less applicable to adults in the present study. Note that PSU has not been an official diagnosis but a behavior ranging from unproblematic to problematic (7). We hence used continuous SAS-SV scores for all analyses.

Respondents were asked that “In the past month, how many hours did you spend on…per day? [If <1 h, please probe how long, i.e., half an hour (0.5), 15 min (0.25), no (0)].” Process-oriented screen-based activities included surfing the internet; reading online book/newspaper/magazine; watching online video (e.g., YouTube); and social-oriented activity included using SNS (e.g., Facebook, Twitter, WhatsApp, WeChat). We categorized the amount of time as “0,” “>0 to <1 hours per day,” “≥1 to <2 hours per day,” “≥2 to <3 hours per day,” and “≥3 hours per day.” The similar categorization was used in large-scale longitudinal studies on self-reported screen time (25, 39). Overall screen time was calculated by summing the amount of time spent on all four screen-based activities measured.

Sociodemographic characteristics included sex, age group, marital status, employment status, educational attainment (primary or below, secondary, or tertiary), and monthly household income (≤HK$ 19,999, 20,000–29,999, or ≥30,000; US $1 = HK $7.8) (the median household income was HK$ 24,900 in Hong Kong in 2016). Cigarette smoking (never smoker, former smoker, or current smoker) and alcohol drinking (never drinker, former drinker, occasional drinker, or monthly or more drinker) were examined, given the co-occurrence of substance use and addictive behaviors (40). History of doctor-diagnosed chronic diseases (e.g., cardiovascular diseases, respiratory diseases, liver diseases, allergies, and others) was dichotomized into none or any. Psychological characteristics were measured using the validated four-item Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-4) (41). Each item scores on a Likert scale (0 = not at all to 3 = nearly every day), with a higher total score (range 0–12) indicating higher symptom severity (41). Cronbach's α was 0.83 in the present sample. Cigarette smoking, alcohol drinking, chronic disease, and PHQ-4 score were grouped as health-related characteristics.

Statistical Analysis

All data were weighted according to sex, age, and educational attainment distributions of the Hong Kong general population to increase the sample representativeness. We examined the associations of time spent on overall and each of four specific screen-based activities with PSU severity and symptoms using bivariate and multivariable linear regression analyses adjusting for sociodemographic and health-related characteristics (Model 1). In Model 2, we repeated Model 1 with mutually adjusting time spent on other types of screen-based activities. The variance inflation factors (range 1.51–2.34, <10 acceptable) suggested the minimal multicollinearity among time spent on different types of screen-based activities in Model 2 (42). Dichotomized SAS-SV scores by suggested cutoffs were used for testing robustness of results in sensitivity analyses. We further tested whether any observed association of time spent on specific screen-based activity with PSU severity in Model 2 differed by sex, age group, educational attainment, and monthly household income by adding multiplicative interaction terms. An omnibus P for interactor was calculated using adjusted Wald tests. Missing data were handled by available case analyses as missing values for all variables were minimal (<2.0%). All analyses were conducted using STATA version/MP 15.1 (StataCorp., TX, USA). A two-side P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

The weighted sample (N = 562; 56.5% female; 82.1% aged 25–64 years) had a mean SAS-SV score of 27.8 [standard deviation (SD) 10.2], with the highest symptom score was observed for cyberspace-oriented relationship (mean 3.5, SD 1.6) (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and SAS-SV total and symptom score (N = 562).

Nearly three-quarters of respondents (71.5%) reported spending over 3 h per day on overall screen-based activities (Table 2). The most prevalent activity (spent over 3 h per day) was surfing the internet, followed by using SNS, reading online book/newspaper/magazine, and watching online video.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Hours per day spent on overall and specific types of screen-based activities (N = 562).

Multivariable analyses showed that each hour of increase in time spent on overall screen-based activities was associated with higher symptom severity of withdrawal (adjusted β = 0.19, 95% CI 0.05, 0.34) and overuse (adjusted β = 0.20, 95% CI 0.02, 0.38) and higher PSU severity (adjusted β = 1.35, 95% CI 0.15, 2.55), after adjusting for sociodemographic and health-related characteristics (Model 1) (Table 3). Associations with higher PSU severity were also observed for each hour of increase in time spent on surfing the internet, reading online book/newspaper/magazine, watching online video, and using SNS in Model 1. After mutually adjusting for time spent on other types of screen-based activities (Model 2), these associations were attenuated and became nonsignificant except for time spent on using SNS. Each hour of increase in time spent on using SNS was associated with higher PSU severity (adjusted β = 1.42, 95% CI 0.35, 2.49) and higher symptom severity of withdrawal (adjusted β = 0.18, 95% CI 0.05, 0.32) and cyberspace-oriented relationship (adjusted β = 0.38, 95% CI 0.23, 0.53). The association of SNS time with PSU was found robust in sensitivity analyses (adjusted odds ratio = 1.40, 95% CI 1.08, 1.83) (Supplementary Table 1). Each hour of increase in time spent on surfing the internet was associated with higher symptom severity of daily-life disturbance (adjusted β = 0.13, 95% CI 0.02, 0.24). Each hour of increase in time spent on watching online video was associated with higher symptom severity of withdrawal (adjusted β = 0.13, 95% CI 0.004, 0.26).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Associations of time spent on overall and specific screen-based activities with PSU and its addictive symptoms (N = 562).

We also examined the interaction effects of sex, age group, educational attainment, and monthly household income on the association of SNS time with PSU severity in Model 2 (Table 4). Younger respondents aged 18–24 years had the strongest association (adjusted β = 4.36, 95% CI 2.58, 6.13), compared with older respondents (25–44 years: adjusted β = 0.97, 95% CI −0.76, 2.69; 45–64 years: adjusted β = 1.69, 95% CI 0.35, 3.04; ≥65 years: adjusted β = 2.87, 95% CI 0.95, 4.80; P for interaction = 0.004). Sex appeared to have no interaction effect on the association of SNS time with PSU severity (male: adjusted β = 1.52, 95% CI 0.03, 3.02; female: adjusted β = 1.98, 95% CI 0.46, 3.51). Respondents with higher educational attainment tended to have the stronger association of SNS time with PSU severity (secondary: adjusted β = 1.46, 95% CI 0.11, 2.82; tertiary: adjusted β = 2.11, 95% CI 0.90, 3.33), but the interaction effect did not reach significance level (P for interaction = 0.06).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Adjusted associations of time spent on using social networking sites with PSU by sex, age, educational attainment, and monthly household income (N = 562)a.

Discussion

In a random sample of Hong Kong Chinese adults, self-reported time spent on overall screen-based activities was positively associated with PSU severity. This finding was consistent with studies showing that longer smartphone usage time was associated with higher PSU severity using the same SAS-SV in Swiss, Spanish, and Belgian populations (15, 16).

Our study added to the literature by exploring the associations of self-reported time spent on overall and specific types of screen-based activities with PSU and its addictive symptom. Specifically, we found that overall screen time was associated with overuse (i.e., “Using my smartphone longer than I had intended.”). The self-awareness of excessive use was also reported in a qualitative interview in users with higher PSU severity (8). However, overuse has been identified as a necessary but insufficient condition of pathological addiction (43). Tolerance symptoms were proposed in the core criteria (7), but we observed no association of overall screen time with tolerance. This might be attributable to the failure of the single-item measure of tolerance in SAS-SV (i.e., “The people around me tell me that I use my smartphone too much.”) to capture other aspects, including increases in financial costs, feelings of gratification, and achievement (e.g., game score/level and SNS “Like”/comment) (44). Overall screen time was also not associated with core addictive symptoms of daily-life disturbance or cyberspace-oriented relationship. In contrast, impairments of workplace/school performance and offline interactions were consistently observed in research into online gaming disorder, a specific form of internet use disorder (9).

Self-reported time spent on using SNS was independently associated with PSU and its core addictive symptoms, including withdrawal and cyberspace-oriented relationship. This association was observed after adjusting for time spent on reading online book/newspaper/magazine, watching online video, and surfing the internet. The findings showed that social- and process-oriented activity were not equally associated with PSU, which supported the UGT (31) and I-PACE (32). Some SNS has features to predispose users to excessive use to meet such higher levels of online social demands; Snapchat is an example that delivers messages only available for a short time after having been viewed by recipients (45). The ephemeral nature of Snapchat can risk users to PSU to avoid missing out the time-limited social connections (46). Our observed association of SNS time with withdrawal symptoms complemented the findings from an experimental study, which found that imagining no access to SNS for 48 h led to dysregulated emotions, depression, and stress (47). A potential mediator on this association might be social anxiety about missing out real-time posts, events, and interactions on SNS (48). The observed association of SNS time with cyberspace-oriented relationship suggested a trade-off between online and offline interactions in the displacement hypothesis (49), which was also supported by impaired family relationships associated with increased SNS time (50). The independent associations of self-reported SNS time with PSU highlighted the needs of specific measurements on problematic SNS use (51) and interventions to prevent and reduce SNS time.

Younger adults appeared to be the most susceptible to the association of self-reported SNS time with PSU in our subgroup analyses. A similar finding was reported in a cross-generation study showing the predictive effect of SNS time on PSU only in the younger group (52). Younger adults tend to be more active but have lower self-control than older adults in using SNS, which might explain the observed association (24). Early adulthood is a developmental stage of emotion change, self-control underdevelopment, and reward sensitivity, which are known risk factors for PSU (6). Neuroscience studies supported the notion by identifying the lateral orbitofrontal gray matter abnormalities in young people with PSU and particularly in those spent time on SNS, and the lateral orbitofrontal cortex is important in regulatory control and reward-related decision-making (3). The stronger association of SNS time with PSU was also observed in those with higher education in our study, although the interaction effect was not significant possibly due to insufficient sample size. Higher SES group may have more social capitals, but greater needs to develop and maintain social connections have been associated with increased SNS time (53).

Self-reported time spent on surfing the internet was associated with daily-life disturbance in our study. Internet has an array of contents for information seeking, shopping, gaming, pornography viewing, gambling, or aimless browsing, and some of which have been identified with addiction potential (e.g., gaming and gambling disorders) and were associated with impairments of school/work performance (54, 55). We found that time spent on watching online video was associated with withdrawal symptoms, which was consistent with a study showing anxiety symptoms associated with excessive use of process-oriented activity (20). The popular video-sharing and live-streaming SNS (e.g., TikTok, Twitch, and Facebook Live) suggested that social anxiety about missing out connections may also be a mediator in the pathway from online video time to withdrawal symptoms (56). Future studies would benefit from examining more detailed contents that are used on the internet and online video.

Our results have theoretical and practical implications. The different associations between time spent on social- and process-oriented activities and PSU informed future research to distinguish between using motives. This fits with the UGT (31) and I-PACE (32) which posit specific motives could predispose individuals to PSU. Prevention and intervention programs, particularly in younger adults, might target limiting SNS time to reduce PSU severity and symptoms. Technology providers could consider incorporate time limit setting into mobile devices or applications.

Our study has several limitations. The cross-sectional data restricted the inference of the temporal sequence of self-reported screen time and PSU. Reverse direction of the association is possible as people with PSU tend to have longer screen time to deflect negative emotions induced by PSU (57). Longitudinal, experimental, and intervention studies are warranted to clarify causal relations and potential mechanisms. Although we adjusted for several potential confounders consistently reported in the literature (24), residual or unmeasured confounders such as personalities of low self-control and social anxiety might explain the observed associations between screen time and PSU (6, 48). Landline survey excluding mobile phone-only group might not be representative for the entire population. Weighting can reduce the non-coverage bias but may not fully compensate for differences in screen time and PSU between landline and mobile surveys. The exclusion of individuals younger than 18 years might underestimate the findings as this age group tended to have longer screen time and higher risks for PSU (24). Self-reported screen time was subject to recall bias and social desirability bias and with uncertain reliability. Future research can use the reliable screen-time questionnaire (58) or objective smartphone use by longitudinal and repeated measures (1719) to validate the results. The lacking data on other popular social- (e.g., email) and process-oriented activities (e.g., gaming, shopping, online banking) warranted more comprehensive measures in future research.

Conclusions

Self-reported screen time was positively associated with PSU severity in Chinese adults in Hong Kong, but the associations varied by types of screen-based activities. The independent associations of self-reported SNS time with PSU severity and core addictive symptoms of withdrawal and cyberspace-oriented relationship highlighted recent warnings about excessive SNS use. Younger adults were the most susceptible to the association of SNS time with PSU severity. Our study provided reminders to smartphone users of the addiction potential of screen time in particular on using SNS. The study could help SNS providers and policymakers to develop regulations to prevent excessive SNS use and PSU.

Data Availability Statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: The data that support the findings of this study are available from the FAMILY project but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for the current study, and so are not publicly available. Data are however available from the authors upon reasonable request and with permission of the Hong Kong Jockey Club Charities Trust. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to FAMILY project, https://www.family.org.hk/en/about-us/contact-us-en/.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Hong Kong/Hospital Authority Hong Kong West Cluster. All respondents provided verbal informed consent. Telephone interviews were audio-recorded for quality checking with respondents' consent. Interview records were then erased 6 months after completing the survey. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author Contributions

NG: formal analysis, data curation, writing—original draft, and writing—review and editing. TTL: writing—review and editing. MPW: conceptualization, methodology, and writing—review and editing, supervision. SYH: conceptualization, methodology, writing—review and editing, and supervision. DYTF: writing—review and editing and supervision. AW: project administration. SSC: conceptualization and methodology. THL: conceptualization, methodology, writing—review and editing, and funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Hong Kong Jockey Club Charities Trust as part of the project: FAMILY: a Jockey Club Initiative for a Harmonious Society (https://www.family.org.hk/en).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the respondents who completed the telephone surveys and the Public Opinion Programme (HKU) for conducting the interviews.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.614061/full#supplementary-material

References

1. World Health Organization. Gaming Disorder, Predominantly Online. (2019). Available online at: https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en#/http%3a%2f%2fid.who.int%2ficd%2fentity%2f338347362 (accessed November 18, 2020).

Google Scholar

2. Montag C, Wegmann E, Sariyska R, Demetrovics Z, Brand M. How to overcome taxonomical problems in the study of Internet use disorders and what to do with “smartphone addiction”? J Behav Addict. (2019). doi: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.59. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Lee D, Namkoong K, Lee J, Lee BO, Jung Y-C. Lateral orbitofrontal gray matter abnormalities in subjects with problematic smartphone use. J Behav Addict. (2019) 8:404–11. doi: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Moccia L, Pettorruso M, De Crescenzo F, De Risio L, di Nuzzo L, Martinotti G, et al. Neural correlates of cognitive control in gambling disorder: a systematic review of fMRI studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 78:104–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Griffiths M. A “components” model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. J Subst Use. (2005) 10:191–7. doi: 10.1080/14659890500114359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Billieux J, Maurage P, Lopez-Fernandez O, Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Can disordered mobile phone use be considered a behavioral addiction? An update on current evidence and a comprehensive model for future research. Curr Addict Rep. (2015) 2:156–62. doi: 10.1007/s40429-015-0054-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Panova T, Carbonell X. Is smartphone addiction really an addiction? J Behav Addict. (2018) 7:252–9. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.49

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lopez-Fernandez O. Generalised versus specific internet use-related addiction problems: a mixed methods study on internet, gaming, and social networking behaviours. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2018) 15:2913. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15122913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Ko C-H, Lin H-C, Lin P-C, Yen J-Y. Validity, functional impairment and complications related to Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5 and gaming disorder in the ICD-11. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. (2019) 54:707–18. doi: 10.1177/0004867419881499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Sussman S, Rozgonjuk D, Eijnden RJJM van den. Substance and behavioral addictions may share a similar underlying process of dysregulation. Addiction. (2017) 112:1717–8. doi: 10.1111/add.13825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kardefelt-Winther D, Heeren A, Schimmenti A, van Rooij A, Maurage P, Carras M, et al. How can we conceptualize behavioural addiction without pathologizing common behaviours? Addiction. (2017) 112:1709–15. doi: 10.1111/add.13763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

12. Billieux J, Schimmenti A, Khazaal Y, Maurage P, Heeren A. Are we overpathologizing everyday life? A tenable blueprint for behavioral addiction research. J Behav Addict. (2015) 4:119–23. doi: 10.1556/2006.4.2015.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Starcevic V, Billieux J, Schimmenti A. Selfitis and behavioural addiction: a plea for terminological and conceptual rigour. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. (2018) 52:919–20. doi: 10.1177/0004867418797442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Brand M, Rumpf H-Jü, Demetrovics Z, MÜller A, Stark R, King DL, et al. Which conditions should be considered as disorders in the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) designation of “other specified disorders due to addictive behaviors”? J Behav Addict. (2020). doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.00035. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Haug S, Castro RP, Kwon M, Filler A, Kowatsch T, Schaub MP. Smartphone use and smartphone addiction among young people in Switzerland. J Behav Addict. (2015) 4:299–307. doi: 10.1556/2006.4.2015.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Lopez-Fernandez O. Short version of the Smartphone Addiction Scale adapted to Spanish and French: towards a cross-cultural research in problematic mobile phone use. Addict Behav. (2017) 64:275–80. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Elhai JD, Tiamiyu MF, Weeks JW, Levine JC, Picard KJ, Hall BJ. Depression and emotion regulation predict objective smartphone use measured over one week. Personal Individ Differ. (2018) 133:21–8. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Rozgonjuk D, Levine JC, Hall BJ, Elhai JD. The association between problematic smartphone use, depression and anxiety symptom severity, and objectively measured smartphone use over one week. Comput Hum Behav. (2018) 87:10–7. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.05.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Randjelovic P, Stojiljkovic N, Radulovic N, Stojanovic N, Ilic I. Problematic smartphone use, screen time and chronotype correlations in university students. Eur Addict Res. (2020). doi: 10.1159/000506738. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Elhai JD, Levine JC, Dvorak RD, Hall BJ. Non-social features of smartphone use are most related to depression, anxiety and problematic smartphone use. Comput Hum Behav. (2017) 69:75–82. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Wolniewicz CA, Tiamiyu MF, Weeks JW, Elhai JD. Problematic smartphone use and relations with negative affect, fear of missing out, and fear of negative and positive evaluation. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 262:618–23. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Rozgonjuk D, Elhai JD, Täht K, Vassil K, Levine JC, Asmundson GJG. Non-social smartphone use mediates the relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and problematic smartphone use: evidence from a repeated-measures study. Comput Hum Behav. (2019) 96:56–62. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.02.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Rozgonjuk D, Elhai JD. Emotion regulation in relation to smartphone use: process smartphone use mediates the association between expressive suppression and problematic smartphone use. Curr Psychol. (2019) 1−10. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00271-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. De-Sola Gutiérrez J, Rodríguez de Fonseca F, Rubio G. Cell-phone addiction: a review. Front Psychiatry. (2016) 7:175. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Boers E, Afzali MH, Newton N, Conrod P. Association of screen time and depression in adolescence. JAMA Pediatr. (2019) 173:853–9. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.1759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Hussain Z, Griffiths MD, Sheffield D. An investigation into problematic smartphone use: the role of narcissism, anxiety, and personality factors. J Behav Addict. (2017) 6:378–86. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wolniewicz CA, Rozgonjuk D, Elhai JD. Boredom proneness and fear of missing out mediate relations between depression and anxiety with problematic smartphone use. Hum Behav Emerg Technol. (2020) 2:61–70. doi: 10.1002/hbe2.159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Mitchell L, Hussain Z. Predictors of problematic smartphone use: an examination of the integrative pathways model and the role of age, gender, impulsiveness, excessive reassurance seeking, extraversion, and depression. Behav Sci. (2018) 8:74. doi: 10.3390/bs8080074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Moccia L, Mazza M, Nicola MD, Janiri L. The experience of pleasure: a perspective between neuroscience and psychoanalysis. Front Hum Neurosci. (2018) 12:359. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Wang MP, Chu JT, Viswanath K, Wan A, Lam TH, Chan SS. Using information and communication technologies for family communication and its association with family well-being in Hong Kong: FAMILY Project. J Med Internet Res. (2015) 17:e207. doi: 10.2196/jmir.4722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Blumler JG, Katz E. The Uses of Mass Communications: Current Perspectives on Gratifications Research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage (1974).

32. Brand M, Wegmann E, Stark R, Müller A, Wölfling K, Robbins TW, et al. The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2019) 104:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hussain Z, Wegmann E, Yang H, Montag C. Social networks use disorder and associations with depression and anxiety symptoms: a systematic review of recent research in China. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:211. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Census and Statistics Department. Thematic Household Survey Report No. 69 - Personal Computer and Internet Penetration. (2019). Available online at: https://www.statistics.gov.hk/pub/B11302692020XXXXB0100.pdf (accessed November 18, 2020).

Google Scholar

35. Luk TT, Wang MP, Shen C, Wan A, Chau PH, Oliffe J, et al. Short version of the Smartphone Addiction Scale in Chinese adults: psychometric properties, sociodemographic, and health behavioral correlates. J Behav Addict. (2018) 7:1157–65. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Guo N, Wang MP, Luk TT, Ho SY, Fong DYT, Chan SS, et al. The association of problematic smartphone use with family well-being mediated by family communication in Chinese adults: a population-based study. J Behav Addict. (2019) 8:412–9. doi: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Guo N, Luk TT, Ho SY, Lee JJ, Shen C, Oliffe J, et al. Problematic smartphone use and mental health in Chinese adults: a population-based study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:844. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17030844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Kwon M, Kim D-J, Cho H, Yang S. The smartphone addiction scale: development and validation of a short version for adolescents. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e83558. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Khouja JN, Munafò MR, Tilling K, Wiles NJ, Joinson C, Etchells PJ, et al. Is screen time associated with anxiety or depression in young people? Results from a UK birth cohort. BMC Public Health. (2019) 19:82. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-6321-9

CrossRef Full Text

40. Di Nicola M, Ferri VR, Moccia L, Panaccione I, Strangio AM, Tedeschi D, et al. Gender differences and psychopathological features associated with addictive behaviors in adolescents. Front Psychiatry. (2017) 8:256. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Löwe B. An ultra-brief screening scale for anxiety and depression: the PHQ−4. Psychosomatics. (2009) 50:613–21. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.50.6.613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Miles J. Tolerance and variance inflation factor. In: Balakrishnan N, Colton T, Everitt B, Piegorsch W, Ruggeri E, Teugels JL, editors. Wiley StatsRef: Statistics Reference Online. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. (2014).

Google Scholar

43. Charlton JP, Danforth IDW. Distinguishing addiction and high engagement in the context of online game playing. Comput Hum Behav. (2007) 23:1531–48. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2005.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Petry NM, Rehbein F, Gentile DA, Lemmens JS, Rumpf H-J, Mößle T, et al. An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach: internet gaming disorder. Addiction. (2014) 109:1399–406. doi: 10.1111/add.12457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Utz S, Muscanell N, Khalid C. Snapchat elicits more jealousy than Facebook: a comparison of Snapchat and Facebook use. Cyberpsychology Behav Soc Netw. (2015) 18:14146. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Social networking sites and addiction: ten lessons learned. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2017) 14:311. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Elhai JD, Hall BJ, Erwin MC. Emotion regulation's relationships with depression, anxiety and stress due to imagined smartphone and social media loss. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 261:28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.12.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Przybylski AK, Murayama K, DeHaan CR, Gladwell V. Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Comput Hum Behav. (2013) 29:1841–8. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Kraut R, Patterson M, Lundmark V, Kiesler S, Mukophadhyay T, Scherlis W. Internet paradox: a social technology that reduces social involvement and psychological well-being? Am Psychol. (1998) 53:1017. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.9.1017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Sharaievska I, Stodolska M. Family satisfaction and social networking leisure. Leis Stud. (2017) 36:231–43. doi: 10.1080/02614367.2016.1141974

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Chen I-H, Pakpour AH, Leung H, Potenza MN, Su J-A, Lin C-Y, et al. Comparing generalized and specific problematic smartphone/internet use: longitudinal relationships between smartphone application- based addiction and social media addiction and psychological distress. J Behav Addict. (2020) 9:410–9. doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.00023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Kuss DJ, Kanjo E, Crook-Rumsey M, Kibowski F, Wang GY, Sumich A. Problematic mobile phone use and addiction across generations: the roles of psychopathological symptoms and smartphone use. J Technol Behav Sci. (2018) 3:141–9. doi: 10.1007/s41347-017-0041-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Salehan M, Negahban A. Social networking on smartphones: when mobile phones become addictive. Comput Hum Behav. (2013) 29:2632–9. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Gainsbury SM. Online gambling addiction: the relationship between internet gambling and disordered gambling. Curr Addict Rep. (2015) 2:185–93. doi: 10.1007/s40429-015-0057-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Li W, Garland EL, Howard MO. Family factors in Internet addiction among Chinese youth: a review of English- and Chinese-language studies. Comput Hum Behav. (2014) 31:393–411. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.11.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Hasan MR, Jha AK, Liu Y. Excessive use of online video streaming services: impact of recommender system use, psychological factors, and motives. Comput Hum Behav. (2018) 80:220–8. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.11.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Elhai JD, Yang H, Montag C. Cognitive- and emotion-related dysfunctional coping processes: transdiagnostic mechanisms explaining depression and anxiety's relations with problematic smartphone use. Curr Addict Rep. (2019) 6:410–7. doi: 10.1007/s40429-019-00260-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Vizcaino M, Buman M, DesRoches CT, Wharton C. Reliability of a new measure to assess modern screen time in adults. BMC Public Health. (2019) 19:1–8. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-7745-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: screen time, screen-based activities, problematic smartphone use, addictive symptoms, social networking sites

Citation: Guo N, Luk TT, Wang MP, Ho SY, Fong DYT, Wan A, Chan SS-c and Lam TH (2021) Self-Reported Screen Time on Social Networking Sites Associated With Problematic Smartphone Use in Chinese Adults: A Population-Based Study. Front. Psychiatry 11:614061. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.614061

Received: 05 October 2020; Accepted: 02 December 2020;
Published: 14 January 2021.

Edited by:

Dmitri Rozgonjuk, University of Ulm, Germany

Reviewed by:

Zaheer Hussain, Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom
Marco Di Nicola, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Guo, Luk, Wang, Ho, Fong, Wan, Chan and Lam. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Man Ping Wang, mpwang@hku.hk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.