Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 09 June 2021
Sec. Mood Disorders

Clinical Features, Neuropsychology and Neuroimaging in Bipolar and Borderline Personality Disorder: A Systematic Review of Cross-Diagnostic Studies

  • 1Institute of Neuropsychiatry and Addiction, Parc de Salut Mar, Barcelona, Spain
  • 2Centro Salud Mental Infanto-Juvenil, Parc de Salut Mar, Barcelona, Spain
  • 3Centre Fòrum Research Unit, Institute of Neuropsychiatry and Addiction, Parc de Salut Mar, Barcelona, Spain
  • 4Mental Health Research Group, Hospital del Mar Medical Research Institute (IMIM), Barcelona, Spain
  • 5PhD Progamme, Department of Psychiatry and Forensic Medicine, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 6Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental, Madrid, Spain
  • 7Instituto de Investigación y Tratamiento del Trauma y los Trastornos de la Personalidad (INTRA-TP) Center, A Coruña, Spain
  • 8Departament of Basic, Evolutive and Education Psychology, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 9Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Klinikum der Universität München, Munich, Germany
  • 10Department of Psychiatry and Forensic Medicine, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain

Background: Bipolar Disorder (BD) and Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) have clinically been evolving as separate disorders, though there is still debate on the nosological valence of both conditions, their interaction in terms of co-morbidity or disorder spectrum and their distinct pathophysiology.

Objective: The objective of this review is to summarize evidence regarding clinical features, neuropsychological performance and neuroimaging findings from cross-diagnostic studies comparing BD and BPD, to further caracterize their complex interplay.

Methods: Using PubMed, PsycINFO and TripDataBase, we conducted a systematic literature search based on PRISMA guidelines of studies published from January 1980 to September 2019 which directly compared BD and BPD.

Results: A total of 28 studies comparing BD and BPD were included: 19 compared clinical features, 6 neuropsychological performance and three neuroimaging abnormalities. Depressive symptoms have an earlier onset in BPD than BD. BD patients present more mixed or manic symptoms, with BD-I differing from BPD in manic phases. BPD patients show more negative attitudes toward others and self, more conflictive interpersonal relationships, and more maladaptive regulation strategies in affective instability with separate pathways. Impulsivity seems more a trait in BPD rather than a state as in BD. Otherwise, BD and BPD overlap in depressive and anxious symptoms, dysphoria, various abnormal temperamental traits, suicidal ideation, and childhood trauma. Both disorders differ and share deficits in neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings.

Conclusion: Clinical data provide evidence of overlapping features in both disorders, with most of those shared symptoms being more persistent and intense in BPD. Thus, categorical classifications should be compared to dimensional approaches in transdiagnostic studies investigating BPD features in BD regarding their respective explanatory power for individual trajectories.

Systematic Review Registration: The search strategy was pre-registered in PROSPERO: CRD42018100268.

Introduction

A recent meta-analysis of 42 studies (1) found that 21.6% of patients suffering from Bipolar Disorder (BD) fulfilled also diagnostic criteria for Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD), and conversely, 18.5% of BPD patients for BD. However, these data may not reflect true comorbidity given the overlap of their phenomenological features (24). Of note, researchers have also documented considerable rates of co-occurrence between BPD and Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) or BD ranging from as low as 3% to as high as 48% in clinical samples (58). The high frequency of diagnostic co-occurrence and resemblance of phenomenological features has led some authors to suggest that BPD is part of the bipolar spectrum (913). Other experts in the field have clearly opposed this notion (6, 1417). Furthermore, in the latter line, recent narrative reviews concluded that both are distinct pathologies, even though they can be difficult to distinguish due to a considerable overlap between both conditions (3, 18). Differentiating BPD from BD-I appears relatively straightforward, reflecting the common presence of severe manic episodes, frequently with psychotic features, in BD-I. By contrast, BD-II (alternating depressive and hypomanic episodes) and BPD are frequently less precisely diagnosed, as BD-II patients do not present with psychotic symptoms and both share common clinical features, especially impulsivity and emotional dysregulation (19). Furthermore, in BD-II there is frequently subthreshold symptomatology between episodes instead of full remission. Therefore, misdiagnosis in both directions is common due to the uncertain boundaries between both disorders (13) and shared “transdiagnostic” features (3).

The main objective of this systematic review is to synthesize the evidence of whether clinical features, neuropsychological and neuroimaging data comparing both conditions point toward two distinct clinical entities or to them both belonging on a continuum within the affective spectrum.

Materials and Methods

Study Design

Using PubMed, PsycINFO and TripDataBase, we conducted a systematic literature search of studies published from January 1980 to September 2019 based on PRISMA guidelines (20) which compared clinical features, neuropsychological functions and neuroimaging between BD and BPD. The search terms were selected from the thesaurus of the National Library of Medicine (Medical Subject Heading Terms, MeSH) and the American Psychological Association (Psychological Index Terms) and included the terms “borderline personality disorder,” “BPD,” “bipolar disorder,” “BD,” “mania,” “hypomania,” “clinical features,” “clinical symptoms,” “emotional dysregulation,” “instability,” “temperament,” “mood,” “neuropsycho*,” “neurocognit*,” “cogniti*,” “impairment,” “deficit,” “functioning,” “cognitive function,” “executive function,” “attention,” “memory,” “working memory,” “neuroimaging,” “neuroimage.” The final search equation was defined using the Boolean connectors “AND” and “OR.”

The automatic search was later completed with a manual search of the reference list of previous reviews and meta-analysis. Titles, abstract, methods and results of the articles identified were screened for pertinent information. The search did not include any subheadings or tags (i.e., search fields “All fields”). In accordance with Cochrane Handbook guidelines, due to the significant heterogeneity and small number of the studies, a formal quantitative synthesis (i.e., meta-analysis review) was not possible to conduct (21), therefore a systematic literature review was conducted.

The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale for assessing the quality of non-randomized studies (22) was applied to each study separately by AMR and BH. Discrepancies were resolved by a third researcher, IGS. The completed PRISMA 2009 checklist (20) and the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (22) are included in Supplementary Tables 1, 2.

Inclusion Criteria and Exclusion Criteria

The selection of the articles was carried out using the following inclusion criteria: (i) observational studies published in peer-reviewed journals, (ii) human studies, (iii) adult populations (over 18 years), (iv) comparisons of a non-comorbid BD and non-comorbid BPD group in terms of (v) clinical features, (vi) neuropsychological functions or (vii) neuroimaging performance. The criteria for exclusion were: (i) articles that did not contain original research (i.e., reviews and meta-analyses), (ii) controlled studies, (iii) qualitative designs, (iv) empirical studies with quasi-experimental or single-case designs, (v) unpublished studies, (vi) in youth and child population, (vii) examined only pharmacological treatment trials. AMR and BH conducted the first literature search and selected the articles in an independent way. Discrepancies were resolved by IGS and AVG.

Results

Eligibility of Studies

Using the search terms described above, the initial literature search produced 551 articles. Titles were screened for eligibility with the most relevant articles retained for abstract review. After an initial review of relevance, 359 articles were retained for further review of their titles and abstracts. BH, reviewer two, also screened articles independently from AMR. Discrepancies were resolved by two independent researchers, IG and AVG. 147 full-text articles were assessed for eligibility and a total of 28 studies published since 1980 met the inclusion and exclusion criteria: 19 focused on assessing clinical features (19, 2339), six evaluated neuropsychological functioning (4045) and three assessed similarities and differences in neuroimaging aspects (4648). The study selection process can be seen in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram.

Clinical Features

Table 1 provides an overview of all studies mentioned in this section which evaluate clinical features in BD and BPD.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Clinical features between bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder.

Affective Symptoms

Mood swings are prominent clinical features of both disorders, with affective symptoms of particular interest when comparing BPD and BD-II. In our review, eight clinical studies investigated general psychopathology comparing BPD and BD, in most cases without a healthy control group for comparison. Symptoms of mania and depression were evaluated mainly through clinical judgment, and other symptoms (e.g., dysphoria) through self-report.

Firstly, Berrocal et al. (23) found both disorders showed elevated lifetime mood symptomatology as compared to Healthy Controls (HC) but no significant difference between them.

Four studies compared both disorders in terms of manic symptoms (2326). Berrocal et al. (23) found the differences in manic symptoms between bipolar patients and BPD were not statistically significant. Conversely, three of these found that these symptoms could differentiate the two disorders (2426). Specifically, Pauselli et al. (24) found in an acute inpatient sample that manic-psychotic symptoms differentiated BD-I from BPD. Vöhringer et al. (25) in an outpatient sample, found that elevated mood, increased goal-directed activities, racing thoughts, reduced need for sleep, increased self-confidence and episodicity predicted BD but not BPD. Similarly, di Giacomo et al. (26) found that BD patients in mixed or manic phases showed significantly more manic symptoms than BPD patients.

At the other end of the mood spectrum, six studies compared the two disorders in terms of depressive symptoms (19, 23, 24, 2628). Of these, two studies (23, 24) which did not state the BD phase, found no significant differences in depressive symptoms between the two disorders, including symptoms of inhibited depression or mixed features with anxiety, depression and suicidality. In contrast, di Giacomo et al. (26) found fewer anxiety and depressive symptoms in BD patients in manic or mixed states, while BPD patients had a wider range of anxiety and depressive symptoms. Similarly, two studies found BPD was characterized by a significantly earlier onset of depressive episodes than BD (19, 27), but BPD patients had fewer overall episodes and less history of psychotic symptoms during a mood episode. Furthermore, Saunders et al. (28) found that BPD patients had more depressive symptoms than euthymic BD patients and HC.

Finally, one study (29) compared the two disorders in terms of dysphoria. This study demonstrated that total score for dysphoria, and subscales of irritability and interpersonal resentment, were significantly higher in BPD patients than in BD.

Thus, in brief, manic features, including psychotic symptoms, can indicate a diagnosis of BD-I and distinguish BD from BPD. Otherwise, anxiety, depressive symptoms and dysphoria can be found in both disorders but may be more persistent, intensive and with an earlier onset of depressive symptoms in BPD than in BD.

Affective Temperament

The affective temperament is described as the set of behavioral traits, stable throughout the life course and with a biological basis, that reflect styles of affective reactivity including activity levels, mood rhythms and patterns of cognitive functioning (5052). Assessed by the studies in this review by self-report measures, it has been discussed as a neurobiological underpinning of both BD and BPD (29).

Two studies compared the affective temperament of BPD with that of BD (30, 31). Eich and colleagues (30) suggested that BD and BPD share a common temperamental diathesis, including similar levels of abnormal cyclothymic temperament, reactive instability, anxious-dependent and avoidant attitudes, and impulsive reactive behavior. The other study (31) found that, while these disorders share abnormal temperamental traits, BPD patients showed a greater severity and scored significantly higher in terms of cyclothymic, depressive, irritable and anxious temperament than BD patients, whereas BD patients scored significantly higher in the hyperthymic temperament.

In conclusion, both conditions share high scores in various temperamental traits, but with a greater severity in cyclothymic, depressive, irritable and anxious temperament in patients with BPD, while the BD profile is characterized more by a hyperthymic temperament.

Emotion Dysregulation

Emotion dysregulation or affective instability is defined as the inability to flexibly respond and manage emotions (53) which interfere with appropriate goal-directed activity (54). Many researchers have characterized BPD and BD separately in terms of heightened reactivity and instability of negative emotion (5558). In this review, seven studies reviewed affective instability/emotional dysregulation, with a set of structured diagnostic interview and self-rating scales, comparing BPD with BD patients (26, 3237).

Although findings from four studies showed similarly high levels of affective lability comparing BPD to a mixed sample of BD-I and BD-II (26, 3234), the results revealed also some different pattern in both disorders. Accordingly, Mneimne et al. (32) found that, while emotional instability is a shared trait, in terms of heightened instability of anger and irritability and heightened inertia of irritability, only BPD is characterized by emotional reactivity to interpersonal challenges, heightened for guilt, shame and excitement, irritability and happiness. As well, shifts from anger and anxiety to euthymia were associated with BPD, whereas shifts from euthymia to depression and elation, and shifts from depression to elation, were characteristic of BD patients (33, 34). Furthermore, BPD patients showed higher affective intensity (33, 34) and higher affective instability than BD patients in a mixed or manic state (26).

Two other studies analyzed similarities and differences between emotion regulation strategies in both disorders (35, 36). Both studies found that BPD subjects displayed a higher number of maladaptive emotion regulation strategies than BD patients, such as deficits in non-acceptance of emotional responses, limited access to emotion regulation strategies, lack of clarity around emotions and emotional awareness, difficulties in controlling impulsive behavior and a tendency to self-blame, catastrophize and blame others. BPD patients were also less likely to use adaptive strategies such as planning, reappraisal and putting things into perspective compared with those with a BD disorder. However, neither of these two studies compared patients to HC.

Coping, as a specific operationalization of affect regulation, was reviewed in one study (37). Findings showed that while both BPD and BD patients both had difficulties in the coping domains of competence and resources, BPD patients have a specific lack of adaptive autonomy coping patterns, such as negotiation and accommodation, as compared to BD patients.

In summary, the results show that emotional dysregulation or affective instability appear to be an overlapping symptom, but it is more pronounced in BPD and pathways are different. Furthermore, BPD have a higher number of maladaptive emotion regulation strategies than BD patients.

Impulsivity

Impulsivity or impulsiveness is a core feature of BPD and BD and may represent a way of managing negative emotions (9). It reflects the tendency to act on a whim, displaying behavior characterized by little or no forethought, reflection, or consideration of the consequences (59). Nine studies investigated impulsiveness in BPD and BD patients using clinical interviews and self-report measures (24, 26, 28, 31, 33, 35, 36, 38, 49).

Five of these studies directly compared impulsivity between BD and BPD and all of them found that BPD patients had elevated impulsivity compared to BD patients (24, 26, 33, 34, 36). Otherwise, four studies comparing BD and BPD patients with HC found that while BD patients had lower impulsivity than BPD, they showed significantly higher impulsivity than HC (28, 31, 38, 49). Further analysis of impulsivity profiles found that BPD patients demonstrated high levels of impulsivity as a trait, unrelated to current mood state, while impulsivity in the BD-II group is present as both a trait and strongly related to current depressive mood state (49). BPD patients had higher scores in all impulsivity dimensions except in novelty seeking (38). Both Bøen et al. (49) and Richard-Lepouriel et al. (38) found that impulsivity was correlated with levels of childhood trauma in BD, while in BPD it was not.

Similarly, three studies compared both disorders in terms of aggression, closely related to impulsivity (26, 28, 33). Two studies showed that BPD patients are more hostile and aggressive than euthymic BD patients and HC (28, 33). However, a further study found that BD patients showed low “anger-control” but high “irritability” and “disruptive-aggressive behavior” during manic or mixed phases (26).

Along these lines, two studies carried out into behaviors related to suicide and self-harm found that BPD patients show a significantly higher rate of self-injury, suicidal and parasuicidal behaviors than BD (19, 26).

In summary, there is strong evidence for a higher impulsiveness in BPD than in BD, which is again more pronounced when compared to HC. Whereas, in BPD high levels of impulsivity were described as a trait, in BD impulsiveness was potentially more related to affective episodes. Both disorders were related with aggressive behavior, which was again more intense in BPD.

Childhood Trauma

History of childhood trauma has been reported among the etiological factors of BD (60) and BPD (61). Five retrospective studies were carried out to investigate different forms of traumatic childhood experiences in both disorders using structured clinical interview and self-report questionnaires.

Five studies explored childhood trauma rates in BD and BPD patients (19, 27, 28, 39, 49). While both patient groups had experienced significantly more trauma than HC (28, 39), results point to severity of childhood trauma being more marked in BPD patients (19, 27, 28, 39, 49). In detail, Bayes, McClure et al. (19) found BPD patients were significantly more likely to report childhood sexual abuse, parental indifference, maternal abuse and over-control, developmental trauma, childhood depersonalization, distant/rejecting parenting. Similarly, Mazer et al. (39) found that emotional abuse, emotional neglect and physical neglect predominated in BPD, differentiating it from BD. Otherwise, Bøen et al. (49) found more severe childhood maltreatment in BPD. Conversely, Saunders et al. (28) found no significant difference between physical and sexual abuse suffered between both groups, although rates were higher in the BPD group.

Interestingly, Fletcher et al. (36) showed that maladaptive strategies and maladaptive emotion regulation were linked to parental style in both disorders, particularly in BPD, but with differences in terms of the type of trauma and its consequences. Therefore, dysfunctional maternal relationships characterize BPD, whereas dysfunctional paternal relationships led to emotion regulation deficits in both disorders. Furthermore, maternal abuse was associated with increased self-blame, while maternal over-control was associated with an increased tendency to catastrophize and blame others in BPD. Otherwise, paternal abuse was associated with a reduced use of acceptance strategies in BPD, while paternal over-control was negatively associated with the use of positive refocusing and to put things into perspective and a reduced impulsive control behavior in patients with BD-II. Finally, paternal relationships characterized by indifference were relevant to both groups, but in BPD this was associated with an increased tendency to blame others, whereas in BD-II it was associated with an increased tendency to ruminate.

Therefore, to summarize, both BD and BPD patients are more likely to have suffered childhood trauma than HC, but with BPD patients being more affected than BD.

Relationships With Self and Others

Three clinical studies compared BD and BPD in terms of different aspects of relationships with others (19, 28, 31) using interviews and self-report measures. Two studies found that BPD patients had significantly more difficulties in interpersonal relationships as compared to BD patients (19, 28). Specifically, Bayes, McClure et al. (19) found that two personality characteristics, “relationship difficulties” and “sensitivity to criticism by others,” distinguished BPD patients from BD patients and were the most consistent predictors of BPD. Saunders et al. (28) found that BPD patients showed reduced cooperation compared to BD patients and HC, and had difficulties in establishing and maintaining reciprocally cooperative relationships. In contrast, the results for euthymic BD patients showed they were capable of developing cooperative relationships and were marginally more cooperative than HC.

Additionally, Nilsson et al. (31) found that BPD patients endorse negative and distressing beliefs about themselves and their relationships with others compared to HC and BD group.

In summary, the evidence shows that BPD patients have more negative attitudes toward others and self, and more conflictive interpersonal relationships, which may distinguish BPD from BD.

Neuropsychological Functions

Six studies investigated differences in executive functioning, attention and impulsivity between BD and BPD patients (4045) and compared to HC data (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Neuropsychological differences between bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder.

Akbari et al. (40) found that euthymic patients with BD-II and BPD had a poorer performance than HC in most neurocognitive domains, specifically in cognitive flexibility and set-shifting, decision-making, sustained and selective attention and problem-solving. In addition, BPD patients had more elevated response inhibition deficits than BD-II patients, which may contribute to greater impulsivity and poor affect regulation.

Another study compared errors made on the Continuous Performance Test-II (CPT-II) related to impulsivity and attention in both disorders (41). The authors found both clinical groups displayed neuropsychological deficits as compared to HC, but that the pattern differed according to each disorder. Processing speed was a key differentiator, with BPD patients showing significantly faster processing speed which was related to impulsivity, and a reduced ability to discriminate stimuli, probably related to selective attention. On the other hand, BD patients showed greater overall cognitive impairment, with slower processing speeds and deficits in sustained attention. These results support the findings from studies into clinical features indicating impulsivity as a trait marker of BPD. BPD patients are characterized by high levels of motor impulsivity and non-planning impulsivity, while BD patients have increased levels of attentional impulsiveness (41).

Gvirts et al. (42) used the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery to compare sustained attention, problem-solving, planning, strategy formation, cognitive flexibility and working memory. Similar to the previous study, the authors found deficits in both disorders which followed divergent patterns. BPD patients showed greater deficits in planning compared to BD and HC, and in problem-solving only with the HC. Otherwise, BD patients showed significant deficits in strategy formation and increased execution time compared to BPD patients and HC. Both BPD and BD patients showed deficits in sustained attention as compared to HC.

Another study compared interference control between BPD, BD and HC assessing the ability to exert control over interference arisen from semantic memory or from distracting perceptual information (43). They found that BD and BPD shared a common impairment in the first task, but both disorders retained intact control over perceptual interference, leading the authors to conclude that similar cognitive functioning may underlie different disorders and symptomatology.

Saunders et al. (44) carried out a study to investigate the speed and accuracy of sensorimotor performance in BD patients, BPD patients and HC. They found no significant differences between any group in any task. However, BPD patients showed transitory post-error slowing.

Finally, Saunders et al. (45) carried out another study to evaluate decision-making in BPD patients, euthymic BD patients and HC. They found that BPD is associated with impaired decision-making, specifically in problems attending and using reinforcement cues to identify negative outcomes. They paid significantly less attention to prospective losses; thus BPD is related to engagement in harmful and risky behaviors. The BD sample showed no impairments in decision-making as compared to the HC participants, but were intermediate between the other groups in their sensitivity to high-loss risks. However, in this study the potential presence of comorbid Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a possible confounder.

To conclude, evidence from the six neuropsychological studies shows that both disorders overlap and differ in neuropsychological deficits. BPD and BD-II patients had in common poorer performance in their planning and problem-solving performance, when compared to HC. Whilst deficits related to motor and non-planning impulsivity, planning, and difficulty attending to specific cues are more prevalent in BPD patients, deficits related to processing speed and strategy formation and increased levels of attentional impulsiveness are indicative of BD patients.

Neuroimaging

Neuroimaging studies are increasingly used to characterize psychiatric disorders since they provide precise and direct information on the structure and functioning of the brain, which can be compared in different psychiatric disorders. Three studies revised neuroimaging features in BPD and BD as compared to HC (4648) (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Neuroimaging results between bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder.

Rossi et al. (46) carried out a structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) study to explore volumetric differences in hippocampal subdivisions in BPD and BD as compared with HC using a three-dimensional mapping method. Results showed that both clinical groups had smaller hippocampal volumes as compared to HC; however, two distinct patterns of gray matter (GM) loss were found in each of the clinical conditions. More specifically, the hippocampal surface maps showed that the CA1 region and the subiculum were bilaterally atrophic in BPD, whereas in bipolar subjects there was a significant alteration in the right dentate gyrus.

In order to extend the previous analysis, the same research team carried out another structural study using a Voxel-Based-Morphometry (VBM) and Regional Volumes (RV) analysis (47). The VBM analysis showed again distinct patterns of GM alterations for each condition. BD subjects presented smaller GM volume in the temporal and frontal lobes, and the precuneus, cerebellum and thalami regions compared to the HC. Otherwise, BPD subjects showed smaller GM volume in the hippocampus, the amygdala and the prefrontal, frontal, parietal and occipital lobes. However, the RV analysis showed that both disorders presented smaller global GM regional volumes compared with HC. The results also provided evidence that regions selectively observed in BPD are strongly correlated with deficits in emotional processing (hippocampus, middle and inferior temporal gyrus). Furthermore, the RV analysis showed that BD had less GM volume in the frontal, limbic, parietal and cerebellar regions compared with BPD. The authors hypothesized that BD might present a relatively diminished pre-frontal modulation of subcortical and medial temporal structures within the limbic system (amygdala and thalamus), resulting in dysregulation of mood.

Likewise, Das et al. (48) carried out a study in order to understand how impaired functional connectivity correlated with emotion dysregulation in both disorders. Results demonstrated that BD and BPD can be differentiated on the basis of resting state functional connectivity among networks involved in detecting social salience, self-referential processing and emotional regulation. Specifically, BD patients displayed increased connectivity, compared to both BPD and HC, in coupling of social salience (SS)-ventral medial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and default mode (DM)-precuneus networks. Conversely, BPD patients displayed decreased connectivity as compared to the rest of the groups in the coupling of SS-precuneus and SS-right fronto-parietal (RFP) networks that were responsible for self-referencing information and which were related to the failure to integrate information from the environmental stimuli and internal representation, linked to impulsive behaviors and self-harm.

In conclusion, both structural and functional neuroimaging findings suggest that a number of areas of overlap exist; however, structural and functional findings also suggest neuroanatomical differences between BPD and BD which could explain the clinical symptomatology presented in both disorders, especially related with emotional processing and emotional dysregulation. Nevertheless, the sample size for significance of these studies are limited, meaning a firm confirmation about neuroanatomic differences still cannot be drawn.

A summary of the specific and shared features of both disorders can be seen in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Specific and shared features of bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first systematic review of clinical and neurobiological studies which directly compare patients with BPD and BD. A total of 28 studies met our inclusion criteria, 19 of them comparing clinical features, six comparing neuropsychological performance and three comparing neuroimaging abnormalities. Our review shows that both disorders can be distinguished in terms of distinct clinical variables, but also share a variety of psychiatric symptoms. Interestingly, most of the shared symptoms seem more persistent and intense in BPD. Furthermore, while neurobiological data suggest differences and similarities between both disorders, the data from the neuropsychological and especially neuroimaging studies are currently limited. This means we cannot definitively answer our research question of whether both conditions belong to the same affective continuum, or whether they are separate nosological entities, with BD a mood disorder and BPD a personality disorder.

Most clinical variables investigated were present and common in both disorders, but there exists a clear tendency that various symptoms are more intense in BPD and some variables differ in further phenomenological and state/trait aspects. For instance, BD patients present more mixed or manic symptoms than BPD patients. A manic state, related to euphoric mood with psychotic symptoms, increased goal-directed activities and mood episodicity, seems specific to distinguish BD-I from BPD (3, 24, 25). Of note here, affective instability in BPD shifts from anger and anxiety to euthymia, whereas in BD it shifts from euthymia to depression and elation, and from depression to elation (33, 34, 62). What is more, the temporal course of emotional dysregulation differs between the two disorder: in BPD mood tends to change over hours, usually in response to interpersonal conflict, while in BD it requires at least a 4-day course to be described as hypomania and the triggers are less clear (63). This distinct pattern might represent a relevant underpinning for the differentiation between BD-I and BPD. Interestingly, patients with BPD show more maladaptive regulation strategies in their affective instability than BD patients. Unlike BPD, impulsivity is not a diagnostic criterion of BD, but it could be related to disinhibition, present during manic or mixed episodes (64, 65) and also persistent during euthymia (66) yielding problematic behaviors in both disorders such as self-injury, suicidality or substance abuse (66, 67). Results showed differences in profile impulsivity between both disorders, and in this regard our neuropsychological findings are of note. As stated, BPD patients are characterized by high levels of motor impulsivity and non-planning impulsivity while BD patients have increased levels of attentional impulsiveness (41). This finding could be related with cognitive execution. Along these lines, BPD patients are characterized by a faster processing speed and a reduced ability to discriminate stimuli and to use cues to identify negative outcomes, while in BD there are more deficits in sustained attention. Therefore, impulsivity can be considered more as a trait in BPD, while in BD it can be considered more as a state and related to affective episodes. Interestingly, impulsivity in BPD tends to decrease over time while affective characteristics and relationship problems are more likely to continue, unlike BD. Further evidence which appears to support two different disorders might be that BPD patients have an emotionally noxious sense of self, more negative attitudes toward others and self, and more conflictive interpersonal relationships than bipolar patients. There is an extensive literature that has attempted to clarify the nosological controversy between bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder. While older articles support the vision of a mood spectrum (913), others, including more recent ones, tend toward the idea that they are two different entities (6, 1417). Our findings to this point represent a tendency so far toward a more dichotomous approach to both disorders, especially to BD-I and BPD. Putting our findings into the context of reviews which included data from studies which investigated the disorders separately rather than directly comparing them, our findings that mood lability, emotional dysregulation and impulsivity are symptoms common to both disorders (although their presentation and time course may be different), but that there are differences in terms of manic and psychotic symptoms and past abuse, are in line with recent reviews (3, 18). Again using data from studies that were not a direct comparison, previous authors have also found differences in parasuicidal self-harm, incidence of suicide, and treatment response, and more data to support a difference in cognitive deficits, as well as differences in bipolar family history suggesting a stronger genetic component in BD (3, 16). Indeed, the heritability rate in bipolar disorder has been shown to be greater than in BPD (between 0.69 and 0.80 (68) compared to between 0.35 and 0.80 (69, 70). Finally, a recent narrative review by Sanches (18) also summarizes that the two conditions are separate, but also states: “It is possible that some forms of bipolar disorder are virtually identical to BPD from a phenotypical standpoint, making both conditions difficult to distinguish at times, particularly given the absence of well-established biomarkers from both conditions.”

In this sense, we detected various clinical variables which are prevalent and common in both disorders and differ in intensity only. This observation could serve as an argument to place both conditions along a continuum of the affective spectrum, as proposed by Akiskal (9), or following Sanches (18) who suggested that some forms of BD are identical to BPD from a phenotypical standpoint. Clinical variables common to both are aggressive behavior and suicidal ideation. Furthermore, symptoms in both disorders include depressive and anxious symptoms, dysphoria, and hypomanic mixed states. The latter are defined by high and low symptoms concurrently present in the same episode (10, 71). Whilst we have evidence to be able to differentiate BPD from BD-I, the difference between BPD and BD-II is much more challenging and often confusing in clinical settings. Especially in cases of a pronounced affective instability or rapid cycling (four or more affective episodes a year), both conditions present with the aforementioned affective symptoms. An important neurobiological underpinning might hereby be the underlying abnormal temperamental traits, which are considered as genetically stable and are hypothesized as etiological risk factors for both BPD and BD (29). We found evidence that BD-II and BPD share higher scores in all affective temperamental traits, with a greater severity in cyclothymic, depressive, irritable and anxious temperaments in patients with BPD and, conversely, the hyperthymic temperament being more specific to BD. Akiskal (9) hypothesized hereby that BPD belongs to the affective spectrum as “the dysphoric facet of cyclothymia” due to the extreme emotional reactivity in BPD, which is based on abnormal affective temperament and cyclothymia (with brief alternating hypomanic and depressive symptoms) in both disorders.

Childhood trauma, such as emotional abuse and neglect, is associated with both BPD and BD and, specifically, with emotional regulation difficulties and propensity to impulsiveness (38, 72, 73). While both patient groups had significantly experienced trauma, results point to the severity of abuse being more marked in BPD patients. In our review, preliminary neuroimaging evidence shows that BPD and BD share overlapping functional and structural neuroanatomical abnormalities, especially involving temporal lobe and related limbic structures associated with mood lability and rapid cycling. These abnormalities could be associated with childhood maltreatment and, in turn, with high sensitivity to rejection, and with impulsiveness, all characteristic factors of both BPD (38, 66, 72, 73) and BD (49, 61, 66, 73, 74). Based on these results, impulsivity could be conceptualized as a consequence of malfunctioning emotion regulation mechanisms or even as a facet of emotional dysregulation, rather than an expression of impulsivity as a primary trait. However, results diverge here as well. Plasma cortisol levels and sexual abuse were correlated in both diagnostic-related groups, but in opposite ways. In BPD, the level of cortisol was positively correlated with sexual abuse, while in BD the correlation was negative. Thus, in BPD patients the history of sexual abuse could stimulate the functioning of the HPA axis while in BD patients a more inhibitory response is observed. Additionally, a negative correlation was identified between emotional neglect and physical neglect, and plasma cortisol levels in BPD patients.

These results highlight the need to research further the role of childhood trauma in the nosology of these disorders and are in line with the previous data on the etiology of these disorders. Recent reviews suggest that BD could be the result of a gene x environment interaction, supporting the evidence from this review that environmental factors such as childhood trauma and adverse life events play a significant role (7577), while a recent review by Cattane et al. (61) suggests that BPD is a result of a combination of biological vulnerabilities and environmental factors including the exposure to traumatic experiences during childhood. Early life stress and childhood trauma have been seen to be major risk factors for the development and persistence of BPD, contributing to the emergence of hippocampal abnormalities. In this sense, the subiculum modulates the response to stress, influencing the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and being the primary locus of hippocampal interaction with this axis (61). On the other hand, atrophy in the dentate gyrus is associated with deficits in the GABAergic system which have been implicated in the etiology of BD (46). Additionally, the impairment in the former network was related to deficits in conscious self-representation and in turn with impaired social understanding, while impairment in the second network explained emotional clarity deficits and depressive rumination (48). In this line, these results could explain why bipolar patients have heightened awareness and increased sensitivity and receptivity to social inputs, but because of diminished emotional clarity, they are unable to process the meaning of these inputs with respect to internal emotional milieu. Nevertheless, few studies have been carried out in this direction, so the results must be interpreted with caution and are not robust enough to draw firm conclusions. Greater understanding of the etiology of the two disorders in the future may help answer our research question, especially in light of the fact that debate over the clinical similarities and differences has gone on for a long time without a decisive consensus being reached.

Other variables should be also mentioned here, even though they have not been targeted in this review. One is longitudinal data. A review by Parker (17) regarding whether BPD can be classified as a mood disorder argues that longitudinal data helps clarify the distinction between BPD and BD-II. On the other hand, a recent article suggests the course of BPD frequently changes over time, with symptoms fluctuating to such an extent that they no longer fulfill the diagnostic criteria later in the life course (78), unlike BD which tends to be a lifelong disorder. As Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) criteria state that traits in personality disorders are chronic and pervasive (63), this puts into doubts the nature of BPD as a personality disorder, and could be considered as a further argument for summarizing BPD within a broad affective spectrum with a shift from an Axis II to Axis I disorder. In our review, all the studies which met the inclusion criteria were cross-sectional, and it would be interesting to see longitudinal research directly comparing the two disorders.

Furthermore, pharmacological treatment and psychotherapy in BPD is worth mentioning due to the nosological nature of our review and its frequent use in clinical practice. Ghaemi et al. (3) argue certain treatment effects may be diagnostically specific. They support there being a strong consensus that psychotherapies alone are not effective in BD but they may be effective adjunctively with medications while, in contrast, psychotherapy is central to the treatment for BPD and there exists controversy about the benefits of psychopharmacotherapy (3). As well, a recent review of pharmacological treatment and psychotherapy in BPD recommends in general terms the use of both strategies (79). The authors highlight the need for further large randomized controlled pharmacological trials across classes of medication (especially in antidepressants) in BPD, but state that the available evidence suggests that anticonvulsants (mainly valproate and lamotrigine), atypical antipsychotics (especially olanzapine) are therapeutic options in treating impulsivity, anger, and affective instability in BPD patients. All these agents are indicated and frequently used in BD. This is of interest as core symptoms of both BPD and BD disorders can improve with medication (80).

However, the different psychotherapeutic treatment options appear to favor a more dichotomous approach to both conditions. The disorders differ in terms of the triggers of mood change. In BPD, most of the mood swings are associated with interpersonal triggers, while in BD they are related more often to biological factors linked to sleeping habits, self-care and changes of season. Thus, BD psychotherapy would address risk factors and environmental triggers to reduce emotional dysregulation (81) while BPD psychotherapy would be focused on social coping and trouble resolution strategies in the form of Dialectic Behavior Therapy (DBT) to improve emotional regulation (82). This would again support the idea that in BPD, the mood tends to change over time and in response to interpersonal conflict, whereas in BD the change lasts for a few days and the triggers are less clear. On the other hand, and based on the similarities observed in cognitive performance in both conditions, some studies have included cognitive rehabilitation and cognitive remediation techniques in their intervention. A systematic review carried out by Bellani et al. (83) states that there is evidence of functional and cognitive improvements in patients with BD who have undergone cognitive remediation treatment. Pascual et al. (84) carried out a controlled study of cognitive rehabilitation in patients with BPD, observing also an improvement in the psychosocial functioning of these patients. These results have led the authors to propose the possible efficacy of broader interventions which might help both disorders and include the elements of the psychoeducational model, DBT emotional regulation techniques and, in turn, cognitive remediation strategies.

Strengths of this work include that this is, to the best of our knowledge, the first systematic review that involves only studies which directly compare both disorders and review clinical and neuropsychological features and neuroimaging data. Due to the comparative lack of data of these last two categories, most of the discussion focuses on the clinical features. This review comprises a total sample of 2,631 participants which provides interesting preliminary evidence and directions for future research. Another strength of the review was the search of three databases for articles over an extended timeframe, repeated by an independent reviewer with a third to resolve discrepancies.

Limitations of our work need to be taken into account and include a lack of longitudinal studies and that several studies had no HC group, meaning an estimation of the degree of impairment or clinical severity of the patients is difficult. Several studies did not confirm the diagnosis of one or both clinical groups or confirm the lack of psychopathology in the control group. Additionally, studies differed in how the BD group was defined, with the majority not distinguishing between BD-I and BD-II in the statistical analysis, while others focused on BD-II or cyclothymia, making generalizations difficult, as well as differences in whether BD samples were euthymic or in an acute phase. Many studies did not compare BD and BPD during depressive episodes, when there are many overlapping symptoms (85). Three studies did not confirm diagnosis as part of the study (24, 26, 37), while four studies did not screen the control group for psychopathology (23, 31, 46, 47). There could have been comorbidity or a BD/BPD diagnosis in these groups which would have confounded results. A large variety of diagnostic tools and measures and scales were employed between the studies. Some studies relied on self-report measures for some or all outcome variables. All childhood maltreatment data was retrospective and self-report, meaning they may be subject to recall bias and an individual's subjective interpretation of events (86), which may have limited validity (87). Pharmacological treatment, the first-line treatment for BD and often used to stabilize mood in BPD (71), may have confounded results and was controlled for only in a minority of studies. Additionally, some studies used only female samples, meaning findings may not be generalizable to males. As well, two studies (19, 35) appeared to use the same sample, as did the group of Saunders et al. (44, 45) and the group of Rossi et al. (46, 47), meaning findings may be distorted. Finally, only three imaging studies are included in the review. One study (48) was performed with fMRI at resting state in a sample of 16 BD patients and 14 BPD, and two studies by almost the same authors (46, 47) were performed with sMRI in the same small sample of patients and controls. The sample size for significance of both functional and structural studies was not reached, meaning these studies were underpowered, thus the conclusions of these studies cannot be generalized to the general population of BD and BDP patients.

Conclusion

In conclusion, based on current evidence directly comparing the two disorders, there is not sufficient data to either confirm the distinction as separate nosological entities, or to support both disorders being part of a shared affective spectrum continuum, or BPD being a bipolar sub-type. Although the results obtained in this exhaustive review may lead us to support the view of BD and BPD as two different categories, in clinical practice the differences between both are often very subtle, especially between BD-II and BPD. leading us to contemplate a dimensional vision.

Common criteria such as emotional dysregulation are easily identified but focusing on differential symptoms is a more complex task. BD-I differs clearly from BPD in the sense of manic/mixed phases which require hospitalization and affective instability has different pathways, being especially common interpersonal triggers in BPD. Furthermore, impulsivity seems more a trait in BPD, whereas a state in BD, and decreases over time in BPD, unlike in BD, supported by some neuropsychological findings. On the other hand, most symptoms, especially depressive, anxious, and hypomanic mixed symptoms are shared in both conditions (with a greater emphasis on BD-II than BD-I), even though they may be more intense in BPD. The latter might indicate that BPD is on the upper end of the affective spectrum as a temporal (as diagnosis of BPD changes over time), but more severe condition, whereas BD tends to be a lifelong disorder. Central shared parts of both conditions are affective temperament, even though some are more specific for one or the other entity, and adverse experiences in childhood, which have an especially important etiological role that is objectified in structural and functional brain alterations, which can explain symptomatic deficits. The noxious sense of self and difficulties in interpersonal relationships are characteristic of BPD. Neuropsychological, but especially neuroimaging, data are inconclusive so far to decide on the nosological discussion between both disorders. However, a recent Genome Wide Association Study by Witt and colleagues (88) revealed that “rather than being a discrete entity,” BPD has an etiological overlap with major psychosis, including BD. We must not forget other factors that can help us in the distinction as the family history of BD, the incidence of suicide and suicide attempts, the pharmacology response, the form of mood cycling or psychotic episodes or the incidence of early sexual abuse.

Future lines of investigation should focus on clinical and neuobiological variables with larger samples and hetero-applied scales, translating this information into effective diagnostic tests and treatment techniques for both disorders. It would be necessary to carry out more clinical trials focused mainly on neuropsychological and neuroimaging aspects directly comparing BD and BPD to increase the understanding of common and differentiated aspects between these two clinical conditions. In particular, prospective, longitudinal cross-diagnostic studies are needed to clarify the controversy. Some authors advocate for a multivariate approach that goes beyond the categorical distinction and uses dimensional information as a better way to understand the relationship between BPD and BD-II (89). Current diagnostic systems demonstrate a move toward dimensional rather than categorical approaches to classifying personality pathology and specially to consider personality traits in the management of all patients with BD irrespective of whether criteria for a categorical BPD are met (90). In this line, the alternative model proposed by the 5th edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (63) and the 11th edition of International Classification of Diseases for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics (91) point to dimensions in BPD. Thus, another approach would investigate personality disorder dimensions in the BD spectrum.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

AM and BH separately carried out the search and the primary assessment of the quality of non-randomized studies and wrote the first draft of the manuscript, supervised by AM-A. Two researchers, IG-S and AV-G, resolved discrepancies. AM-A, BA, VP, FC, FP, and DM, experts within the investigated field and contributed with the interpretation of data. AG-E and AT participated with contributions to the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

BA receives two grants (PI/15/02242 and PI18/00009) from the Instituto de Salud Carlos III-Subdirección General de Evaluación y Fomento de la Investigación, Plan Nacional 2008–2011 and 2013–2016, a NARSARD Independent Investigator Grant from the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation (24397) and a grant within the Pla estratègic de recerca i innovació en salut (PERIS; G60072253) by the Catalan Government. He acknowledges also the continuous support by the CIBERSAM (Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental). BH thanks the support and funding of the Instituto de Salud Carlos III through a PFIS grant (FI10/00017). FC thanks the support and funding of the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, through a FIS (PI15/00588; PI19/00009), and the Secretaria d'Universitats i Recerca del Departament d'Economia i Coneixement de la Generalitat de Catalunya GOVERNMENT OF CATALONIA (2017_SGR_134). The funding institutions had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

FP is a member of the European Scientific Advisory Board of Brainsway Inc., Jerusalem, Israel, and has received speaker's honoraria from Mag & More GmbH and the neuroCare Group. His lab has received support with equipment from neuroConn, Ilmenau, Germany, and Mag & More GmbH and Brainsway Inc., Jerusalem, Israel.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.681876/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Fornaro M, Orsolini L, Marini S, De Berardis D, Perna G, Valchera A, et al. The prevalence and predictors of bipolar and borderline personality disorders comorbidity: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2016) 195:105–18. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.01.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Benazzi F. Does temperamental instability support a continuity between bipolar II disorder and major depressive disorder? Eur Psychiatry. (2006) 21:274–9. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2006.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Ghaemi SN, Dalley S, Catania C, Barroilhet S. Bipolar or borderline: a clinical overview. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2014) 130:99–108. doi: 10.1111/acps.12257

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Gunderson JG, Phillips KA. A current view of the interface between borderline personality disorder and depression. Am J Psychiatry. (1991) 148:967–75. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.8.967

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Brieger P, Ehrt U, Marneros A. Frequency of comorbid personality disorders in bipolar and unipolar affective disorders. Compr Psychiatry. (2003) 44:28–34. doi: 10.1053/comp.2003.50009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Paris J, Gunderson J, Weinberg I. The interface between borderline personality disorder and bipolar spectrum disorders. Compr Psychiatry. (2007) 48:145–54. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Fan AH, Hassell J. Bipolar disorder and comorbid personality psychopathology: a review of the literature. J Clin Psychiatry. (2008) 69:1794–803. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v69n1115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Zimmerman M, Morgan TA. Problematic boundaries in the diagnosis of bipolar disorder: the interface with borderline personality disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2013) 15:422. doi: 10.1007/s11920-013-0422-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Akiskal HSS. Demystifying borderline personality: critique of the concept and unorthodox reflections on its natural kinship with the bipolar spectrum. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2004) 110:401–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2004.00461.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Benazzi F. Borderline personality-bipolar spectrum relationship. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2006) 30:68–74. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.06.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Deltito J, Martin L, Riefkohl J, Austria B, Kissilenko A, Corless P, et al. Do patients with borderline personality disorder belong to the bipolar spectrum? J Affect Disord. (2001) 67:221–8. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00436-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Perugi G, Toni C, Travierso MC, Akiskal HS. The role of cylothymia in atypical depression: toward a data-based reconceptualization of the borderline-bipolar II connection. J Affect Disord. (2003) 73:87–98. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00329-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Zimmerman M, Ruggero CJ, Chelminski I, Young D. Psychiatric diagnoses in patients previously overdiagnosed with bipolar disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. (2010) 71:26–31. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Paris J. Differential diagnosis of borderline personality disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am. (2018) 41:575–82. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2018.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Magill CA. The boundary between borderline personality disorder and bipolar disorder: current concepts and challenges. Can J Psychiatry. (2004) 49:551–6. doi: 10.1177/070674370404900806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Basset D. Borderline personality disorder and bipolar affective disorder. Spectra o spectre? A review. Aust N Z J Psichiatry. (2012) 46 327–39. doi: 10.1177/0004867411435289

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Parker G. Is borderline personality disorder a mood disorder? Br J Psychiatry. (2014) 204:252–3. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.136580

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Sanches M. The limits between bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder: a review of the evidence. Diseases. (2019) 7:49. doi: 10.3390/diseases7030049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Bayes AJ, McClure G, Fletcher K, Román Ruiz del Moral YE, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Stevenson JL, et al. Differentiating the bipolar disorders from borderline personality disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2016) 133:187–95. doi: 10.1111/acps.12509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. BMJ. (2009) 339:b2535. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b2535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Ryan R, Cochrane Consumers and Communication Review Group. Cochrane Consumers and Communication Review Group: Data Synthesis and Analysis. (2013). Available online at: http://cccrg.cochrane.org (accessed May, 2021).

22. Wells G, Shea B, O'Connell D, Peterson J, Welch V, Losos M, et al. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for Assessing the Quality of Nonrandomised Studies in Meta-Analysis. (2011). Available online at: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp (accessed May, 2021).

Google Scholar

23. Berrocal C, M RM, Rando MA, Benvenuti A, Cassano GB. Borderline personality disorder and mood spectrum. Psychiatry Res. (2008) 159:300–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Pauselli L, Verdolini N, Santucci A, Moretti P, Quartesan R. Bipolar and borderline personality disorders: a descriptive comparison of psychopathological aspects in patients discharged from an Italian Inpatient Unit using PANSS and BPRS. Psychiatr Danub. (2015) 27(Suppl. 1):S170−6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

25. Vöhringer PA, Barroilhet SA, Alvear K, Medina S, Espinosa C, Alexandrovich K, et al. The International Mood Network (IMN) Nosology Project: differentiating borderline personality from bipolar illness. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2016) 134:504–10. doi: 10.1111/acps.12643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. di Giacomo E, Aspesi F, Fotiadou M, Arntz A, Aguglia E, Barone L, et al. Unblending borderline personality and bipolar disorders. J Psychiatr Res. (2017) 91:90–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Perroud N, Zewdie S, Stenz L, Adouan W, Bavamian S, Prada P, et al. Methylation of serotonin receptor 3A in ADHD, borderline personality, and bipolar disorders: link with severity of the disorders and childhood maltreatment. Depress Anxiety. (2016) 33:45–55. doi: 10.1002/da.22406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Saunders KEA, Goodwin GM, Rogers RD. Borderline personality disorder, but not euthymic bipolar disorder, is associated with a failure to sustain reciprocal cooperative behaviour: implications for spectrum models of mood disorders. Psychol Med. (2015) 45:1591–600. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714002475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Bachetti MC, Brufani F, Spollon G, Moretti P. Dysphoria dimensions: a preliminary inpatients study to diffrentiate borderline personality and bipolar disorder spectrum. Psychiatr Danub. (2019) 31(Suppl. 3):490–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

30. Eich D, Gamma A, Malti T, Vogt Wehrli M, Liebrenz M, Seifritz E, et al. Temperamental differences between bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder, and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: some implications for their diagnostic validity. J Affect Disord. (2014) 169:101–4. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Nilsson AKK, Jorgensen CR, Straarup KN, Licht RW. Severity of affective temperament and maladaptive self-schemas differentiate borderline patients, bipolar patients, and controls. Compr Psychiatry. (2010) 51:486–91. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Mneimne M, Fleeson W, Arnold EM, Furr RM. Differentiating the everyday emotion dynamics of borderline personality disorder from major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder. Person Disord Theory Res Treat. (2017) 9:192–6. doi: 10.1037/per0000255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Henry C, Mitropoulou V, New A, Koenigsberg H, Silverman J, Siever L. Affective instability and impulsivity in borderline personality and bipolar II disorders: similarities and differences. J Psychiatr Res. (2001) 35:307–12. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3956(01)00038-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Reich DB, Zanarini MC, Fitzmaurice G. Affective lability in bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder. Compr Psychiatry. (2012) 53:230–7. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Bayes A, Parker G, McClure G. Emotional dysregulation in those with bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder and their comorbid expression. J Affect Disord. (2016) 204:103–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.06.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Fletcher K, Parker G, Bayes A, Paterson A, McClure G. Emotion regulation strategies in bipolar II disorder and borderline personality disorder: differences and relationships with perceived parental style. J Affect Disord. (2014) 157:52–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kramer U. Observer-rated coping associated with borderline personality disorder: an exploratory study. Clin Psychol Psychother. (2014) 21:242–51. doi: 10.1002/cpp.1832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Richard-Lepouriel H, Kung AL, Hasler R, Bellivier F, Prada P, Gard S, et al. Impulsivity and its association with childhood trauma experiences across bipolar disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and borderline personality disorder. J Affect Disord. (2019) 244:33–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.07.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Mazer AK, Cleare AJ, Young AH, Juruena MF. Bipolar affective disorder and borderline personality disorder: differentiation based on the history of early life stress and psychoneuroendocrine measures. Behav Brain Res. (2019) 357–8:48–56. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Akbari V, Rahmatinejad P, Mohammadi SD. Comparing neurocognitive profile of patients with borderline personality and bipolar–II disorders. Iran J Psychiatry. (2019) 14:113. doi: 10.18502/ijps.v14i2.990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Feliu-Soler A, Soler J, Elices M, Pascual JC, Perez J, Martin-Blanco A, et al. Differences in attention and impulsivity between borderline personality disorder and bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2013) 210:1307–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Gvirts HZ, Braw Y, Harari H, Lozin M, Boche Y, Fefer K, et al. Executive dysfunction in bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder. Eur Psychiatry. (2015) 30:959–64. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Lozano V, Soriano MF, Aznarte JI, Gomez-Ariza CJ, Bajo MT. Interference control commonalities in patients with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and borderline personality disorder. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2016) 38:238–50. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2015.1102870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Saunders KEA, Goodwin GM, Rogers RD. Borderline personality disorder, but not euthymic bipolar I disorder, is associated with prolonged post-error slowing in sensorimotor performance. J Affect Disord. (2016) 198:163–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.03.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Saunders KEA, Goodwin GM, Rogers RD. Insensitivity to the magnitude of potential gains or losses when making risky choices: women with borderline personality disorder compared with bipolar disorder and controls. J Pers Disord. (2016) 30:530–44. doi: 10.1521/pedi_2015_29_216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Rossi R, Lanfredi M, Pievani M, Boccardi M, Beneduce R, Rillosi L, et al. Volumetric and topographic differences in hippocampal subdivisions in borderline personality and bipolar disorders. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2012) 203:132–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Rossi R, Pievani M, Lorenzi M, Boccardi M, Beneduce R, Bignotti S, et al. Structural brain features of borderline personality and bipolar disorders. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2013) 213:83–91. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Das P, Calhoun V, Malhi GS. Bipolar and borderline patients display differential patterns of functional connectivity among resting state networks. Neuroimage. (2014) 98:73–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Bøen E, Hummelen B, Elvsåshagen T, Boye B, Andersson S, Karterud S, et al. Different impulsivity profiles in borderline personality disorder and bipolar II disorder. J Affect Disord. (2015) 170:104–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.08.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Akiskal HS. The temperamental foundations of affective disorders. In: Mundt C, Goldstein MJ, Hahlweg K, Fiedler P, editors. Interpersonal Factors in the Origin and Course of Affective Disorders. London: Gaskill (1996). p. 3–30.

Google Scholar

51. Romero E, Holtzman JN, Tannenhaus L, Monchablon R, Rago CM, Lolich M, et al. Neuropsychological performance and affective temperaments in Euthymic patients with bipolar disorder type II. Psychiatry Res. (2016) 238:172–80. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.02.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Vázquez GH, Baldessarini RJ, Tondo L. Co-occurrence of anxiety and bipolar disorders: clinical and therapeutic overview. Depress Anxiety. (2014) 31:196–206. doi: 10.1002/da.22248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Thompson RA. Emotion dysregulation: a theme in search of definition. Dev Psychopathol. (2019) 31:805–15. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419000282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Beauchaine TP. Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth psychopathology. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2015) 44:875–96. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1038827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Broome MR, He Z, Iftikhar M, Eyden J, Marwaha S. Neurobiological and behavioural studies of affective instability in clinical populations: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 51:243–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Coifman KG, Berenson KR, Rafaeli E, Downey G. From negative to positive and back again: polarized affective and relational experience in borderline personality disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. (2012) 121:668–79. doi: 10.1037/a0028502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Tsanas A, Saunders KEAEA, Bilderbeck ACC, et al. Daily longitudinal self-monitoring of mood variability in bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder. J Affect Disord. (2016) 205:225–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.06.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Wessa M, Linke J. Emotional processing in bipolar disorder: behavioural and neuroimaging findings. Int Rev Psychiatry. (2009) 21:357–67. doi: 10.1080/09540260902962156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. VandenBos GR, (editor). APA Dictionary of Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association (2007).

Google Scholar

60. Aas M, Henry C, Andreassen OA, Bellivier F, Melle I, Etain B. The role of childhood trauma in bipolar disorders. Int J Bipolar Disord. (2016) 4:1–10. doi: 10.1186/s40345-015-0042-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Cattane N, Rossi R, Lanfredi M, Cattaneo A. Borderline personality disorder and childhood trauma: exploring the affected biological systems and mechanisms. BMC Psychiatry. (2017) 17:221. doi: 10.1186/s12888-017-1383-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Bayes A, Parker G, Paris J. Differential diagnosis of bipolar II disorder and borderline personality disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2019) 21:1–11. doi: 10.1007/s11920-019-1120-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA (2013).

Google Scholar

64. Lewis M, Scott J, Frangou S. Impulsivity, personality and bipolar disorder. Eur Psychiatry. (2009) 24:464–9. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2009.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Swann AC, Steinberg JL, Lijffijt M, Moeller FG. Impulsivity: differential relationship to depression and mania in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord. (2008) 106:241–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Etain B, Aas M, Andreassen OA, Lorentzen S, Dieset I, Gard S, et al. Childhood trauma is associated with severe clinical characteristics of bipolar disorders. J Clin Psychiatry. (2013) 74:991–8. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13m08353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Oquendo MA, Perez-Rodriguez MM, Poh E, Sullivan G, Burke AK, Sublette ME, et al. Life events: a complex role in the timing of suicidal behavior among depressed patients. Mol Psychiatry. (2014) 19:902–9. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Goodwin F, Jamison K. Manic-depressive Illness: Bipolar Disorders and Recurrent Depression. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2007). p. 411–62.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

69. Torgersen S, Lygren S, Oien PA, SkrE I, Onstad S, Edvardsen J, et al. A twin study of personality disorders. Compr Psychiatry. (2000) 41:416–25. doi: 10.1053/comp.2000.16560

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Torgersen S, Czajkowski N, Jacobson K, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Røysamb E, Neale MC, et al. Dimensional representations of DSM-IV cluster B personality disorders in a population-based sample of Norwegian twins: a multivari-ate study. Psychol Med. (2008) 38:1617–25. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708002924

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Goodwin GM. Evidence-based guidelines for treating bipolar disorder: revised second edition-recommendations from the British association for psychopharmacology. J Psychopharmacol. (2009) 23:346–88. doi: 10.1177/0269881109102919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Foxhall M, Hamilton-Giachritsis C, Button K. The link between rejection sensitivity and borderline personality disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Clin Psychol. (2019) 58:289–326. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Alloy LB, Abramson LY, Walshaw PD, Neeren AM. Cognitive vulnerability to unipolar and bipolar mood disorders. J Soc Clin Psychol. (2006) 25:726–54. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2006.25.7.726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Leverich GS, McElroy SL, Suppes T, Keck PE, Denicoff KD, Nolen WA, et al. Early physical and sexual abuse associated with an adverse course of bipolar illness. Biol Psychiatry. (2002) 51:288–97. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01239-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Jansen K, Cardoso TA, Fries GR, Branco JC, Silva RA, Kauer-Sant'Anna M, et al. Childhood trauma, family history, and their association with mood disorders in early adulthood. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2016) 134:281–6. doi: 10.1111/acps.12551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Misiak B, Stramecki F, Gaweda Ł, Prochwicz K, Sasiadek MM, Moustafa AA, et al. Interactions between variation in candidate genes and environmental factors in the etiology of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a systematic review. Mol Neurobol. (2018) 55:5075–100. doi: 10.1007/s12035-017-0708-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Bortolato B, Köhler CA, Evangelou E, León-Caballero J, Solmi M, Stubbs B, et al. Systematic assessment of environmental risk factors for bipolar disorder: an umbrella review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Bipolar Disord. (2017) 19:84–96. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Temes CM, Zanarini MC. The longitudinal course of borderline personality disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am. (2018) 41:685–94. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2018.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Bozzatello P, Rocca P, De Rosa ML, Bellino S. Current and emerging medications for borderline personality disorder: is pharmacotherapy alone enough? Exp Opin Pharmacother. (2020) 21:47–61. doi: 10.1080/14656566.2019.1686482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Crawford MJ, MacLaren T, Reilly JG. Are mood stabilisers helpful in treatment of borderline personality disorder? BMJ. (2014) 349:g5378. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g5378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Colom F, Vieta E. A perspective on the use of psychoeducation, cognitive-behavioral therapy and interpersonal therapy for bipolar patients. Bipolar Disord. (2004) 6:480–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00136.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Linehan MM. Cognitive Behavioural Treatment of Borderline Personality Disorder. New York, NY: Guildford Press (1993).

Google Scholar

83. Bellani M, Biagianti B, Zovetti N, Rossetti MG, Bressi C, Perlini C, et al. The effects of cognitive remediation on cognitive abilities and real-world functioning among people with bipolar disorder: a systematic review. J Affect Disord. (2019) 257:691–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.07.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Pascual JC, Palomares N, Ibáñez Á, Portella MJ, Arza R, Reyes R, et al. Efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation on psychosocial functioning in borderline personality disorder: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry. (2015) 15:255. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0640-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Socada JL, Söderholm JJ, Rosenström T, Ekelund J, Isometsä E. Presence and overlap of bipolar symptoms and borderline features during major depressive episodes. J Affect Disord. (2021) 280 (Pt A):467–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.11.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Frissa S, Hatch SL, Fear NT, Dorrington S, Goodwin L, Hotopf M. Challenges in the retrospective assessment of trauma: comparing a checklist approach to a single item trauma experience screening question. BMC Psychiatry. (2016) 16:20. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-0720-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Danese A, Widom CS. Objective and subjective experiences of child maltreatment and their relationships with psychopathology. Nat Hum Behav. (2020) 4:811–8. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0880-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Witt SH, Streit F, Jungkunz M, Frank J, Awasthi S, Reinbold CS, et al. Genome-wide association study of borderline personality disorder reveals genetic overlap with bipolar disorder, major depression and schizophrenia. Transl Psychiatry. (2017) 7:e1155. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Villarroel J, Salinas V, Silva H, Herrera L, Montes C, Jerez S, et al. Beyond the categorical distinction between borderline personality disorder and bipolar ii disorder through the identification of personality traits profiles. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:552. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Saunders KEA, Jones T, Perry A, Di Florio A, Craddock N, Jones I, et al. The influence of borderline personality traits on clinical outcomes in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. (2020). doi: 10.1111/bdi.12978. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. World Health Organization. International Classification of Diseases for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics. 11th Revision. (2018). Available online at: https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en (accessed May, 2021).

Keywords: bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder, clinical features, cognitive functions, neuroimaging, affective continuum

Citation: Massó Rodriguez A, Hogg B, Gardoki-Souto I, Valiente-Gómez A, Trabsa A, Mosquera D, García-Estela A, Colom F, Pérez V, Padberg F, Moreno-Alcázar A and Amann BL (2021) Clinical Features, Neuropsychology and Neuroimaging in Bipolar and Borderline Personality Disorder: A Systematic Review of Cross-Diagnostic Studies. Front. Psychiatry 12:681876. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.681876

Received: 17 March 2021; Accepted: 14 May 2021;
Published: 09 June 2021.

Edited by:

Maj Vinberg, University Hospital of Copenhagen, Denmark

Reviewed by:

Joel Paris, McGill University, Canada
Steven Marwaha, University of Birmingham, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2021 Massó Rodriguez, Hogg, Gardoki-Souto, Valiente-Gómez, Trabsa, Mosquera, García-Estela, Colom, Pérez, Padberg, Moreno-Alcázar and Amann. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Benedikt Lorenz Amann, benedikt.amann@gmail.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.