Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 13 July 2021
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic Rising Stars in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry View all 5 articles

Gray Matter Volume in the Developing Frontal Lobe and Its Relationship With Executive Function in Late Childhood and Adolescence: A Community-Based Study

\nDajung Sung&#x;Dajung Sung1Bumhee Park,&#x;Bumhee Park2,3Bora KimBora Kim1Hayeon KimHayeon Kim1Kyu-In JungKyu-In Jung1Seung-Yup LeeSeung-Yup Lee1Bung-Nyun KimBung-Nyun Kim4Subin ParkSubin Park5Min-Hyeon Park
Min-Hyeon Park1*
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine, Eunpyeong St. Mary's Hospital, The Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, South Korea
  • 2Department of Biomedical Informatics, Ajou University School of Medicine, Suwon, South Korea
  • 3Office of Biostatistics, Ajou Research Institute for Innovative Medicine, Ajou University Medical Center, Suwon, South Korea
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
  • 5Department of Research Planning, National Center for Mental Health, Seoul, South Korea

Background: During late childhood and adolescence, the frontal lobe undergoes critical developmental changes, affecting a wide range of executive functions significantly. Conversely, abnormality in the maturation of the frontal lobe during this period may result in a limited ability to effectively use various executive functions. However, at present, it is still unclear how the structural development of the frontal lobe is associated with different aspects of executive functions during this developmental period. To fill the gap in evidence, we aimed to elucidate gray matter volume (GMV) in the frontal lobe and its relationship with multiple aspects of executive functions in late childhood and adolescence.

Methods: We recruited our participants aged between 6 and 17 years to assess GMV in the frontal lobe and its relationship with different domains of executive functions in late childhood and adolescence. We used the voxel-based morphometry–DARTEL procedure to measure GMVs in multiple frontal sub-regions and Stroop test and Advanced Test of Attention (ATA) to measure executive functions. We then conducted partial correlation analyses and performed multiple comparisons with different age and sex groups.

Results: Overall, 123 participants took part in our study. We found that many regional GMVs in the frontal lobe were negatively correlated with ATA scores in participants in late childhood and positively correlated with ATA scores in participants in adolescence. Only a few correlations of the GMVs with Stroop test scores were significant in both age groups. Although most of our results did not survive false discovery rate (FDR) correction (i.e., FDR <0.2), considering their novelty, we discussed our results based on uncorrected p-values. Our findings indicate that the frontal sub-regions that were involved in attentional networks may significantly improve during late childhood and become stabilized later in adolescence. Moreover, our findings with the Stroop test may also present the possibility of the later maturation of higher-order executive functioning skills.

Conclusion: Although our findings were based on uncorrected p-values, the novelty of our findings may provide better insights into elucidating the maturation of the frontal lobe and its relationship with the development of attention networks in late childhood and adolescence.

Introduction

The development of the frontal lobe has drawn much attention to neuroimaging and developmental studies because of its strong association with a wide range of executive functions (EFs), from basic motor movements to complex decision making (1). The frontal lobe is known to manage various EFs that are essential for performing complex tasks including the selection and perception of information, inhibitory control, maintenance, working memory, and self-directed behavior (2). A large body of longitudinal neuroimaging studies have exhibited that the frontal lobe is among the last regions of the brain to mature, and that it may not fully maturate until halfway through the third decade of life (3, 4). Especially during childhood and adolescence, the frontal lobe undergoes profound neurodevelopmental changes due to the pruning of excess synapses (5). According to a well-established body of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies (4, 69), there is a decrease in gray matter density with a non-linear pattern in the frontal cortices throughout adolescence (9). A longitudinal study with a wide age range (between 4 and 22 years of age) has demonstrated that gray matter volume (GMV) in the frontal lobe increases during preadolescence, especially in the pre-frontal cortex (8, 10). Within the anatomical structure of the frontal lobe, GMV in the precentral gyrus develops earlier than other frontal sub-regions, whereas the superior and inferior frontal gyri maturate later (9). Furthermore, there has been robust evidence on sex differences in total intracranial volume (TIV) across different ages (11, 12). For example, in the developing brain, GMVs in many regions in the frontal lobe reach maximum volume at around 12 years for males and 11 years for females (10, 13). Following late childhood, the GMV in the frontal lobe gradually decreases in both males, and females (13).

Such structural maturation of the frontal lobe also leads to significant development of EFs throughout childhood and adolescence (14, 15). An extensive body of neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that the main domains (i.e., shifting, working memory, inhibition) of EFs emerge during the first few years of life, and their refinement and stabilization continue throughout adolescence (15, 16). That is, some cognitive abilities may develop early, but a range of higher-order executive functioning skills does not reach their peak until post-adolescence (1719). Higher-order executive functioning skills are defined as multidimensional executive and control processes such as reasoning, planning, organizing, problem-solving, sustained attention, response inhibition, and cognitive flexibility (2022). Anderson et al. (23) examined how children and adolescents aged between 11 and 17 years performed differently on a variety of EF tasks. They observed that there was an improvement in some EF task performances that required selective attention, working memory, and problem-solving in the group of adolescents. They also found that attentional control-processing speed had the most significant development in this age group (23). They speculated that their findings might be linked to the neurobiological processes of pruning and myelination in the frontal lobe, particularly during this developmental period. Moreover, this might reflect that the main domains of EFs have their own developmental trajectories. Similarly, an earlier cognitive study examined EF skills including working memory and inhibition in three age groups (i.e., 6–7, 8–10, and 11–12 years) and found significant differences in the performances of the age groups in EF tasks (24). Explicitly, there was a remarkable peak in the tasks that assessed planning and processing speed in the group of 11 to 12-year-old children (24).

Although there has been a substantial increase in understanding of this area across different ages over the past two decades, yet little is known is about how the neurocognitive development of the frontal lobe is related to the different domains of EFs in children, and adolescents in the general population. Abnormality in the maturation of the frontal lobe in late childhood and adolescence may result in a limited ability to effectively use the domains of EFs (23). This is of particular importance because the transition from late childhood to adolescence is regarded as a time where some externalizing problems [e.g., attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)] can possibly transform into more severe behavioral forms including oppositional defiant disorder, vandalism, theft, physical aggression, delinquency, and bullying (2527). Furthermore, this developmental period is associated with increased risk-taking behaviors (e.g., substance use) and various mental health problems (e.g., depression, schizophrenia, bipolar disorders) (15, 28). Recently, Straub et al. (29) examined GMVs in a relatively large sample of depressed and healthy youth and found greater GMVs in the dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex in the group of depressed youth. Replicating findings from previous studies (3032), they suggested that depressed youth may be more vulnerable to delayed brain maturation.

Although abnormality in the neurocognitive development of the frontal lobe adversely influences the everyday activities of children and adolescents, most of the previous research was conducted in clinical populations (e.g., ADHD children, schizophrenia patients) or focused on certain age ranges (e.g., 2–5 years of age) or developmental periods (e.g., early childhood) (16, 33, 34). Additionally, there is not enough converging evidence on the relationship between the structural maturation of the frontal lobe and the different domains of EFs during the transition between late childhood and adolescence. This may be mainly due to inconsistency in using neuroimaging techniques (e.g., electroencephalogram) and EF measures. For instance, a wide variety of intelligence quotient (IQ) tests in use today measure inconsistent domains of EFs (35, 36). That said, the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III (WAIS-III), one of the most widely used IQ tests, measures a certain set of four mental abilities such as verbal comprehension, processing speed, perceptual organization, and working memory (35, 37). Considering this, an investigation of EFs by using multiple measures may be more beneficial to elucidate the relationship between the structural maturation of the frontal lobe and the development of the main domains of EFs in this population.

Considering the above concerns, we assessed regional GMVs (rGMVs) in the frontal lobe and its relationship with the main domains of EFs in a community sample of children and adolescents aged between 6 and 17 years. We used the voxel-based morphometry (VBM)–DARTEL procedure to measure GMVs in the frontal sub-regions and several EF tests to measure the different domains of EF. To assess the relationship, we conducted partial correlation analyses with four different age and sex groups (i.e., age ≤ 13 years, male; age ≤ 13 years, female; age ≥14 years, male; age ≥14 years, female) and performed multiple comparisons with these groups.

Materials and Methods

Participants

For participant recruitment, we used flyers that had brief information about the current study in schools and libraries in Seoul and Gyeonggi-do Province. We also recruited our participants from one elementary school and one high school in Seoul, South Korea. For the current study, we excluded participants if they had unstable physical condition or a history of an acquired brain injury, neurological disorders, psychiatric disorders (e.g., schizophrenia), developmental disorders (e.g., autism, intellectual disabilities), learning disabilities, language impairments, or uncorrected sensory impairment. All participants and their legal guardians gave their informed consent before taking part in the present study. Our study was approved by the Institutional Review Board for Human Subjects at Seoul National University Hospital and conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki.

Materials

Korean Educational Developmental Institute–Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children

The Korean Educational Developmental Institute–Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) was a modified and standardized version of the original WISC for Korean children between 5 and 15 years of age (3840). This test includes Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) (i.e., a total score of the sub-tests), verbal IQ, and performance IQ, with 12 sub-tests: information, similarities, arithmetic, vocabulary, comprehension, picture completion, picture arrangement, block design, object assembly, and digital span and mazes (39). For our study, we used FSIQ to measure mental abilities of our participants who were 13 years or younger.

Korean-Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale

We used the Korean version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (K-WAIS), which is the most widely used IQ test with strong validity and reliability (41, 42). The K-WAIS has been widely used for Korean adolescents and adults whose ages are between 16 and 64 years. This test has two categories of 11 sub-tests with a verbal scale and a performance scale. The verbal scale includes information, digit span, vocabulary, arithmetic, comprehension, and similarity (42). The performance scale includes picture completion, picture arrangement, block design, object assembly, and digit symbol (42). For the current study, we used FSIQ to measure the mental abilities of our participants who were 14 years or older.

Stroop Color and Word Test

The Stroop test is one of the most widely used neurological tests to measure selective attention capacity and skills, cognitive flexibility, processing speed, and working memory (4345). Moreover, it has been used to assess dysfunctions of the frontal lobe, as it has good validity and reliability in the pediatric and adolescent populations (44, 46). This test consists of three components such as word task, color task, and color–word task (46). The word task measures basic reading rate and assesses speech motor problems or learning disabilities. The color task assesses speech motor function and colorblindness. The color–word task measures both mental flexibility and inhibitory control. For the word task, participants were asked to name a series of color words (46). For the color task, participants were asked to name the color of cards (e.g., XXX in red or blue ink). For the color–word task, participants were asked to name the color of the ink instead of the word when the names of colors in different ink colors (e.g., the word “red” in blue ink) were shown. Three scores (i.e., word score, color score, color–word score) were calculated based on the number of completed items on each card. Higher scores indicate better performance and less interference. Interference score was also obtained by subtracting the obtained word score from the obtained color–word score (47). This score measures inhibitory control, with smaller scores indicating better control (47).

Advanced Test of Attention

Continuous performance test measures auditory attention, visual processing speed, visual-motor competence, and phonological awareness (48). It has good validity and reliability, with a Cronbach α coefficient of 0.82 (4850). Among different versions of this test, we used the Advanced Test of Attention (ATA) that integrates both visual and auditory sensorial modalities, measuring attention, and response inhibition in Korean children older than 5 years (51). The target rate of stimulus presentation is 22% for the first section, 50% for the middle section, and 78% for the last section, with the stimulus presentation time of 0.1 s and an interval of 2 s between the presentations (39, 52). This test includes both auditory and visual scores of omission errors (i.e., missed targets), commission errors (i.e., incorrect responses to non-targets), the mean of response time (i.e., response speed), response time variability, response criterion, and detectability (i.e., ability to distinguish targets from non-targets) (39). It also provides both auditory and visual scores of ADHD. An omission error reflects sustained attention. A commission error reflects impulsivity, self-regulation, and inhibitory control. The mean of response time reflects the response preparation components of EF. Response time variability reflects the inconsistency in responses. Based on the ATA guidelines, higher scores on both the visual and auditory variables indicate higher probability of ADHD including poor EFs.

The Children's Depression Inventory

The Children's Depression Inventory (CDI), a modified version of Beck's Depression Inventory, measures child and adolescent depression (5355). For our study, we used the Korean version (56) of the CDI, a self-report questionnaire with 27 items that reflect depressive symptoms, with numerical values from 0 to 2. Participants were asked to mark the statement that best described their feelings during the past 2 weeks (57). Higher scores reflect a higher severity of depression.

Methods

MRI Data Acquisition

We used a 3.0-T MRI scanner (Siemens, Magnetom Tim-Trio) to obtain MRI scans. We obtained high-resolution T1-weighted images from each participant with a magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo pulse sequence [repetition time = 1,900 ms, echo time = 3.13 ms, flip angle = 9°, matrix size = 256 × 256, field of view = 230 × 230 mm2, thickness = 0.9 mm]. We used foam pads to minimize head motion-related artifacts during scanning.

Regional VBM Analysis

To estimate rGMV, we conducted a VBM analysis through the SPM12 VBM-DARTEL procedure (SPM12, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/, Wellcome Trust Center for Neuroimaging, London, UK) (58). Comparing with earlier optimized VBM, this procedure serves clearer segmentation and better registration for estimating boundaries between different tissues (58, 59).

Our well-trained physician did not find any abnormalities from motion and/or other artifacts on T1-weighted images. The procedure for preprocessing T1-weighted images included manual reorientation to the anterior commissure, gray matter segmentation based on a standard tissue probability map provided from SPM, creation of the study-specific template, spatial normalization with DARTEL to normalize individual images to the DARTEL template (34, 60, 61), modulation to adjust for volume signal changes during spatial normalization, and spatial smoothing of the gray matter partitions with a Gaussian kernel of 8-mm full width at half maximum. After preprocessing, values of rGMVs were extracted by averaging the values at each frontal region, according to the automated anatomical labeling atlas (62). The frontal sub-regions that we observed were the medial/inferior/superior parts of the orbitofrontal gyrus, the medial/middle/dorsal/superior parts of the frontal gyrus, opercular and triangular parts of the inferior frontal gyrus, and precentral gyrus in each hemisphere.

To conduct our study, we adapted a standard adult SPM template (62). Many neuroimaging studies on the developing brain have used the adult template and demonstrated that using the adult template did not affect the results of their analyses (6367). Moreover, the standard adult SPM template is regarded as a good tool to compare or combine the results from previous studies across different age groups (6870).

Statistical Analysis

Prior to analysis, we divided our sample into four age and sex groups (i.e., age ≤ 13 years, male; age ≤ 13 years, female; age ≥14 years, male; age ≥14 years, female), according to the guidelines of American Academy Pediatrics (71). We then proceeded to conduct partial correlation analyses between rGMVs in the frontal lobe and EF test scores (i.e., Stroop test, visual and auditory ATA) for each group. For the analyses, we controlled for age, sex, FSIQ, and TIV as covariates. We also adjusted for CDI in our final analyses.

Correlation values for each group were transformed into normal distributed values (i.e., Z group 1 = 0.5 × [log (1 + R group 1) – log (1 – R group 1)] and Z group 2 = 0.5 × [log (1 + R group 2) – log (1 – R group 2)] as Fisher r-to-z transformation). After z transformation, we compared them with Z = (Z group 1 – Z group 2) / √[1/(N group 1 – 3-M) + 1/(N group 2 – 3-M)], where N group 1, N group 2, and M each represent the sample size for each group and the number of covariates used in partial correlation analyses. A threshold of false discovery rate (FDR) = 0.2 was used to determine significant correlations and address multiple comparison issues (i.e., FDR = 0.2 or less) (72). FDR thresholding controls the expected proportion of false positives among brain areas exhibiting significance (72). FDR control levels in the range 0.1–0.2 are originally and practically known to be acceptable, as multiple neuroimaging studies have applied (7276). We conducted all statistical analyses using the MATLAB-based custom software (MathWorks, Sherborn, MA, USA) and SPSS 20.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Results

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Participants

Overall, 123 participants aged between 6 and 17 years took part in our study. In our sample, the proportion of males (54.5%) was slightly larger than that of females (45.5%). The proportion of participants (56.9%) whose age was 13 years or younger was slightly larger than that of participants (43.1%) whose age was 14 years or older. The proportions of male and female participants in each year of age were also presented in Table 1. There were no sex differences on IQ and CDI in both age groups. However, sex differences on TIV were observed, as males had relatively larger TIV than females (Table 1). Table 2 shows executive functioning test performances in the four age and sex groups.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Executive functioning test performances across age and sex groups.

Correlation Analysis

As shown in Figure 1, in the age group ≤ 13 years, we found numerous negative correlations and a few positive correlations between the GMVs in the frontal sub-regions and Stroop test and ATA scores (controlled for sex, FSIQ, and TIV). Contrastingly, in the age group ≥14 years, we found many positive correlations between the GMVs and ATA scores, with a few negative correlations of the GMVs with Stroop test scores (Figure 1). Even after FDR correction (i.e., FDR <0.2), we found multiple significant correlations in each age group. However, the FDR-corrected correlations did not withstand adjustment for CDI.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Partial correlations between the GMVs in the frontal lobe and Stroop and ATA test scores in the age groups (age ≤ 13 years, n = 70; age ≥14 years, n = 53). Comparisons of the partial correlations of the age groups. Controlled for sex, FSIQ, and TIV. GMVs, gray matter volumes; FSIQ, Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient; TIV, total intracranial volume; Stroop WT, Stroop Word Test; Stroop CT, Stroop Color Test; Stroop CWT, Stroop Color and Word Test; Stroop I, Stroop inference score; aATA, auditory ATA; vATA, visual ATA.

As displayed in Figure 3C, we found significant differences between the results age ≤ 13 group and those of age ≥14 male groups (controlled for sex, FSIQ, TIV). Specifically, the GMVs in many frontal sub-regions were negatively correlated with ATA scores in the age group ≤ 13 years (Figure 2A) but were positively correlated with ATA scores, mainly with visual scores, in the age group ≥14 years (Figure 2B). Among these significant correlations, the correlations that survived for FDR correction were the left precentral gyrus and auditory ATA response criterion, the right middle frontal gyrus and visual ATA response criterion, and the left inferior orbitofrontal gyrus and visual ATA response criterion (FDR = 0.1535, 0.0867, and 0.1351, respectively). However, after adjustment for CDI, only the correlation between the right middle frontal gyrus and visual ATA response criterion survived FDR correction (FDR = 0.1857), as shown in Figure 5C. Similarly, we found significant differences between the age ≤ 13 female group and age ≥14 female group (Figure 3D). Even after controlling for CDI, we found multiple significant correlations of the rGMVs with ATA scores, although the magnitude of these correlations did not withstand FDR correction (Figure 5D).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Partial correlations between the GMVs in the frontal lobe and Stroop and ATA test scores in the age and sex groups. Controlled for age, FSIQ, and TIV. (A) Male group (age ≤ 13 years, n = 34). (B) Male group (age ≥14 years, n = 31). (C) Female group (age ≤ 13 years, n = 34). (D) Female group (age ≥14 years, n = 18). GMVs, gray matter volumes; FSIQ, Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient; TIV, total intracranial volume; Stroop WT, Stroop Word Test; Stroop CT, Stroop Color Test; Stroop CWT, Stroop Color and Word Test; Stroop I, Stroop inference score; aATA, auditory ATA; vATA, visual ATA.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Comparisons of partial correlations between the GMVs in the frontal lobe and Stroop and ATA test scores in the age and sex groups. Controlled for age, FSIQ, and TIV. (A) Male group vs. female group (age ≤ 13 years). (B) Male group vs. female group (age ≥14 years). (C) Male group (age ≤ 13 years) vs. male group (age ≥14 years). (D) Female group (age ≤ 13 years) vs. female group (age ≥14 years). GMVs, gray matter volumes; FSIQ, Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient; TIV, total intracranial volume. Stroop WT, Stroop Word Test; Stroop CT, Stroop Color Test; Stroop CWT, Stroop Color and Word Test; Stroop I, Stroop inference score; aATA, auditory ATA; vATA, visual ATA.

Age Group ≤ 13 Years

In male participants, we found that rGMVs in many frontal sub-regions were negatively associated with Stroop test and ATA scores (controlled for age, FSIQ, and TIV) (Figure 2A). The negative correlations that had significant evidence (i.e., p < 0.01 or p < 0.005 or p < 0.001) were observed in the following: the right middle frontal gyrus and visual ATA response criterion (p < 0.001), the left opercular inferior frontal gyrus and visual ATA response criterion (p < 0.001), the left inferior orbitofrontal gyrus and visual ATA response criterion (p < 0.01), the left medial orbitofrontal gyrus and visual ATA response criterion (p < 0.01), and the right medial orbitofrontal gyrus and Stroop Word Test score (p < 0.01). However, as displayed in Figure 4A, adjustment for CDI decreased the magnitude of the following negative correlations: the right middle frontal gyrus and visual ATA response criterion (p < 0.005), the left inferior orbitofrontal gyrus and visual ATA response criterion (p < 0.05), and the right medial orbitofrontal gyrus and Stroop Word Test score (p < 0.05). Adjustment for CDI increased the magnitude of the correlation between the left medial superior frontal gyrus (left) and visual ATA response criterion (p < 0.01). The strength of the correlations between the left opercular inferior frontal gyrus and visual ATA response criterion and the left medial orbitofrontal gyrus and visual ATA response criterion remained the same (p < 0.01). However, such results did not withstand FDR correction.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Partial correlations between the GMVs in the frontal lobe and Stroop and ATA test scores in the age and sex groups. Controlled for age, FSIQ, TIV, and CDI. (A) Male group (age ≤ 13 years, n = 34). (B) Male group (age ≥14 years, n = 31). (C) Female group (age ≤ 13 years, n = 34). (D) Female group (age ≥14 years, n = 18). GMVs, gray matter volumes; FSIQ, Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient; TIV, total intracranial volume; Stroop WT, Stroop Word Test; Stroop CT, Stroop Color Test; Stroop CWT, Stroop Color and Word Test; Stroop I, Stroop inference score; aATA, auditory ATA; vATA, visual ATA.

In female participants, there were several significant correlations (adjusted for age, FSIQ, and TIV), as shown in Figure 2C. Among the correlations that had strong evidence, visual ATA mean of response time was strongly correlated with the right precentral gyrus and the right opercular inferior frontal gyrus (p < 0.01 and p < 0.005, respectively). After adjustment for CDI, the strength of the correlation between the right precentral gyrus and visual ATA mean of response time remained the same, whereas the strength of the correlation between the right opercular inferior frontal gyrus and visual ATA mean of response time was decreased slightly (p < 0.01) (Figure 4C). However, such results did not survive FDR correction.

Additionally, we found sex differences on multiple correlations in this age group (Figure 3A). Most of these correlations were strongly associated with both visual and auditory ATA scores, although the adjustment for CDI slightly reduced the magnitude of the sex differences (Figure 5A). Both the results with CDI and without CDI did not withstand FDR correction.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Comparisons of partial correlations between the GMVs in the frontal lobe and Stroop and ATA test scores in the age and sex groups. Controlled for age, FSIQ, TIV, and CDI. (A) Male group vs. female group (age ≤ 13 years). (B) Male group vs. female group (age ≥14 years). (C) Male group (age ≤ 13 years) vs. male group (age ≥14 years). (D) Female group (age ≤ 13 years) vs. female group (age ≥14 years). GMVs, gray matter volumes; FSIQ, Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient; TIV, total intracranial volume; Stroop WT, Stroop Word Test; Stroop CT, Stroop Color Test; Stroop CWT, Stroop Color and Word Test; Stroop I, Stroop inference score; aATA, auditory ATA; vATA, visual ATA.

Age Group ≥14 Years

After adjustment for age, FSIQ, and TIV, we found strong evidence on many positive correlations between the GMVs and ATA scores, mainly with auditory ATA scores, in male participants (Figure 2B). Among the correlations, the correlations that had significant evidence (i.e., p < 0.01, p < 0.005, or p < 0.001) were identified in the following: the left precentral gyrus and visual ATA detectability (p < 0.01), the left precentral gyrus and auditory ATA mean of response time (p < 0.01), the left precentral gyrus and auditory ATA response criterion (p > 0.005), the right precentral gyrus and auditory ATA mean of response time (p < 0.01), the right precentral gyrus and auditory ATA response criterion (p < 0.001), and the left middle orbitofrontal gyrus and auditory ATA score (p < 0.01). After adjusting for CDI, all the correlations remained significant, and the positive correlation between the right medial orbitofrontal gyrus and visual ATA response criterion became significant (Figure 4B). There were no significant correlations of the rGMVs with Stroop test scores.

Similarly, we found that the rGMVs in some frontal sub-regions were positively associated with visual ATA scores; Stroop test and auditory ATA variables were not strongly correlated with the rGMVs (Figure 2D). The correlations that had significant evidence include the left dorsolateral superior frontal gyrus and ATA omission errors (p < 0.01), the left middle frontal gyrus and ATA omission errors (p < 0.01), the right opercular inferior frontal gyrus and ATA mean of response time (p < 0.005), the right opercular inferior frontal gyrus and ATA response time variability (p < 0.005), and the left medial superior frontal gyrus and ATA omission errors (p < 0.005). Even after adjusting for CDI, most of the correlations remained strong and significant (Figure 4D).

The correlations that showed sex differences in this age group are shown in Figure 3B. Specifically, among those, the correlation between the right opercular inferior frontal gyrus and auditory ATA mean of response time and the correlation between right opercular inferior frontal gyrus and auditory ATA response time variability had significant evidence (p < 0.01 and p < 0.001, respectively). Only the correlation between the right opercular inferior frontal gyrus and auditory ATA response time variability survived FDR correction (FDR = 0.0992). The sex difference on this correlation remained strong even after we controlled for CDI and performed FDR correction (FDR = 0.0912) (Figure 5B). After adjusting for CDI, the magnitude of sex differences was increased in the correlation between the left medial superior frontal gyrus and visual ATA commission errors (p < 0.005) and the correlation between the right medial superior frontal gyrus and visual ATA commission errors (p < 0.01) (Figure 5B).

Discussion

At present, although the development of the frontal lobe has been extensively investigated, it is still unclear about how the structural development of the frontal lobe is associated with different executive functioning skills in late childhood and adolescence. To fill the gap in evidence in this specific area, we examined the relationship between GMVs in the frontal sub-regions and the main domains of EFs in late childhood and adolescence. Interestingly, we found that most of the visual and auditory ATA scores that mainly measured attentional control were negatively correlated with the rGMVs in participants in late childhood and positively correlated with the rGMVs in participants in adolescence. Only a few correlations of the GMVs with Stroop test scores that assessed various higher-order EFs were strong and significant in both age groups (i.e., age ≤ 13 years, age ≥14 years). Even after adjusting for CDI, we observed the similar results.

Moreover, in both age groups, sex differences were shown in the majority of the correlations with ATA scores, whereas only a few correlations with Stroop test scores showed sex differences. The only correlation that survived FDR correction even after controlling for CDI was the right opercular inferior frontal gyrus and auditory ATA response time variability [FDR = 0.0992 (without CDI), FDR = 0.0912 (with CDI)] in participants in adolescence. Based on such findings, we suggest that the sex differences might reflect the earlier structural maturation of the frontal lobe in females, and specifically, attentional control, which is one of the main forms of EFs, might develop earlier in females. Strictly, although most of our findings were no longer significant after FDR correction (i.e., FDR <0.2), considering the potentially significant novelty of our study, here we present and discuss our findings based on uncorrected p-values (i.e., p ≤ 0.05).

The results of our correlation analyses showed a statistically significant developmental trend in several domains of attention (i.e., selective attention, sustained attention), as there were numerous negative correlations of the rGMVs in the frontal sub-regions with ATA test performances in participants in late childhood in the current study. We speculate that this might possibly reflect ongoing significant maturation of the frontal sub-regions involved in attention, as GMV reduction is one of the major characteristics of adolescent brain maturation (77). On the other hand, a few Stroop test scores that assessed the domains of higher-order EFs (e.g., working memory) were strongly correlated with the GMVs in the frontal sub-regions, which may possibly present the possibility of the later maturation of higher-order executive functioning skills.

The important roles of attention include alertness, set, spatial attention, sustained attention, and interference control (9). These roles seem to develop gradually toward full maturity at 12 years of age (9, 78). In line with this, our results revealed the peak of the development of these domains in late childhood, and presented the possibility that the development of attention might already have passed the peak and become stable during this developmental period. Furthermore, attention is known to have distinctive functions (78). Posner and Rothbart (79) proposed a model of attentional networks. In this model, there are three distinctive networks such as the alerting network, the orienting network, and the executive attention network (7981). In line with this model, all frontal sub-regions had strong evidence with the executive skills of visual and auditory attention in our participants. Considering this, we suggest that the GMVs in the frontal sub-regions that are implicated with the attentional networks may dramatically develop during late childhood.

Among the frontal sub-regions we assessed, the superior frontal gyrus, the orbitofrontal gyrus, the middle frontal gyrus, and the inferior frontal gyrus are involved in the executive attention network. The executive attention network is primarily responsible for error detection and the goal-directed control of attention and behavior associated with novelty and interference (81, 82). This network is active when a conflict is present and inhibitory attentional processes are necessary (81, 83). It is known that while the development of executive attention (e.g., alertness from external cues) occurs since early infancy, the development of orienting, and maintaining the level of alertness seems to develop through late childhood (78). This was well-demonstrated in our study, as our findings revealed the structurally developmental changes in the frontal sub-regions and the growth spurt in the domains of attentional control in participants in late childhood.

We also observed the structurally significant development of the alerting network in the GMV in the precentral gyrus, which has a main role in executing voluntary movements (78). The alerting network is mainly involved in achieving and maintaining a state of high sensitivity to external stimuli (78). This network is considered as one of the prerequisites of conscious perception and other attentional operations (78, 83). The development of this network continues beyond 14 years of age (83). Our findings are consistent with this notion, as there was a large number of positive correlations between the GMV in the precentral gyrus and both visual and auditory attentional task performances in our participants who were 14 years or older. More importantly, there was also statistically strong evidence (FDR = 0.01535; controlled for sex, TIV, and FSIQ) on the correlation between the left precentral gyrus and auditory attentional response criterion when we compared the result of male participants in late childhood with that of male participants in adolescence. That is, male participants in the age group ≥14 years whose GMV in the precentral gyrus was more developed responded less impulsively than male participants in age ≤ 13 male group while they performed the auditory attentional tasks.

Combined with previous neuroimaging evidence and the model of attentional networks, we suggest that GMVs in most of the frontal sub-regions that are associated with the attentional networks might significantly develop in late childhood and become stabilized in early adolescence. Furthermore, it is possible that the maturation of higher-order executive functioning skills might occur later in adolescence, as we did not find any strong evidence on the relationship between structural maturation of the frontal sub-regions and Stroop test task performances.

Despite the novelty of our findings, our study has several limitations that merit attention. First, our study was exploratory, and our findings were based on uncorrected p-values, as most of our findings did not survive FDR correction. However, this may possibly be due to the limitation of the relatively low sample size, which is one of the common limitations of neuroimaging studies. We therefore recommend future studies to obtain larger sample sizes with more balanced proportions of male and female participants to better understand how rGMVs in the frontal lobe contributes to a wide range of EFs in late childhood and adolescence. Moreover, measuring a single executive skill is difficult, as all aspects of EFs are inter-twined (84). It is also possible that other potential covariates (e.g., stress) might have contributed to the strength of the statistically significant correlations we found, although we controlled for the well-known covariates in our analyses (85, 86). Lastly, the nature of our study (i.e., the cross-sectional design) does not provide any further evidence on the causal relationship between the associations found. All cross-sectional studies may be subject to the bias of the selection of people of various ages. Considering this, it may be beneficial to longitudinally investigate how the structural maturation of the frontal lobe is linked to the different domains of executive functioning skills in late childhood and adolescence. Although our findings were based on uncorrected p-values, our findings may provide better insights into elucidating the structural maturation of the frontal lobe and its relationship with the development of attention networks in late childhood and adolescence.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by The Institutional Review Board for Human Subjects at Seoul National University Hospital. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

M-HP designed the study and wrote the protocol. DS, BK, B-NK, SP, K-IJ, HK, and S-YL conducted the literature searches and analyses. M-HP, BP, and DS performed the statistical analysis. DS prepared Tables 1 and 2. BP prepared figures. DS and BP wrote the first and final draft of the manuscript. All authors have approved the final manuscript.

Funding

The present study was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korean Government (NRF-2014R1A1A2057866, NRF-2017R1D1A1B03031680, NRF-2020R1F1A1070581), and the Catholic Medical Center Research Foundation made in the program year of 2019. The funders had no roles in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all our study participants for their participation and dedication.

References

1. RoschK S, Mostofsky S. Development of the frontal lobe. Handb Clin Neurol. (2019) 163:351–67. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-804281-6.00019-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Tulio O, Lauren B. The Frontal Lobes and Executive Functioning. New York, NY: Handbook of Executive Functioning (2014). doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Johnson SB, Blum RW, Giedd JN. Adolescent maturity and the brain: the promise and pitfalls of neuroscience research in adolescent health policy. J Adolesc Health. (2009) 45:216–21 doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Holmes CJ, Toga AW. In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions. Nat Neurosci. (1999) 2:859–61. doi: 10.1038/13154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Oldham S, Fornito A. The development of brain network hubs. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2019) 36:100607. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Leonard CM, Welcome ES, Kan E, Toga AW. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J Neurosci. (2004) 24:8223–31. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1798-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Lenroot R, Giedd J. Brain development in children and adolescents: Insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2006) 30:718–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Gogtay N, Gieldd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2004) 101:8174–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Uytun MC. Development period of prefrontal cortex. Prefrontal Cortex. (2018) 3–22. doi: 10.5772/intechopen.78697

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. (1999) 2:861–3. doi: 10.1038/13158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Sanchis-Segura C, Victoria Ibañez-Gual M, Adrián-Ventura J, Aguirre N, Javier Gómez-Cruz Á, Avila C, et al. Sex differences in gray matter volume: how many and how large are they really? Biol Sex Differ. (2019) 10:32 doi: 10.1186/s13293-019-0245-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Sanchis-Segura C, Victoria Ibañez-Gual M, Aguirre N, Javier Cruz-Gómez Á, Forn C. Effects of different intracranial volume correction methods on univariate sex differences in grey matter volume and multivariate sex prediction. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:12953. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-75522-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Blakemore SJ, Choudhury S. Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. (2006) 47:296–312. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Moriguchi Y. The early development of executive function and its relation to social interaction: a brief review. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:1–6 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Murty VP, Calabro F, Luna B. The role of experience in adolescent cognitive development: Integration of executive, memory, and mesolimbic systems. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 70:46–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Best JR, Miller PH. A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev. (2010) 81:1641–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Davidson MC, Amso D, Anderson LC, Diamond A. Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia. (2006) 44:2037–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Luna B, Garver KE, Urban TA, Lazar NA, Sweeney JA. Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Dev. (2004) 75:1357–72. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00745.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Zelazo PD, Craik F, Booth L. Executive function across the life span. Acta Psychol. (2004) 115:167–83. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Paz-Alonso PM, Bunge SA, Ghetti S. Emergence of higher cognitive functions: reorganization of large-scale brain networks during childhood and adolescence. Oxford Handbooks Online. (2014) 1–19. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199935291.013.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Mller E, Wallis J. Executive function and higher-order cognition: definition and neural substrates. Encyclop Neurosci. (2009) 4:99–104. doi: 10.1016/B978-008045046-9.00418-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Blanco-Campala A, Diaz-Oruetab U, Navarro-Pradosc AB, Burke T, Libone DJ, Lamarf M. Features and psychometric properties of the Montreal cognitive assessment: review and proposal of a process-based approach version (MoCA-PA). Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 34:83–109. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2019.1681996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Anderson VA, Anderson P, Northam E, Jacobs R, Catroppa C. Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Dev Neuropsychol. (2001) 20:385–406. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2001_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Pureza JR, Gonçalves HA, Branco L, Grassi-Oliveira R, Paz Fonseca R. Executive functions in late childhood: age differences among groups. Psychol Neurosci. (2013) 6:79–88. doi: 10.3922/j.psns.2013.1.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Hughes C, White A, Sharpen J, Dunn J. Antisocial, angry, and unsympathetic: “hard-to-manage” preschoolers' peer problems and possible cognitive influences. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2000) 41:169–79. doi: 10.1017/S0021963099005193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Laubach M, Lammers F, Zacharias N, Feinkohl I, Pischon T, Borchers F, et al. Size matters: Grey matter brain reserve predicts executive functioning in the elderly. Neuropsychologia. (2018) 119:172–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Atherton OE, Ferrer E, Robins RW. The development of externalizing symptoms from late childhood through adolescence: a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth. Dev Psychol. (2018) 54:1135–47 doi: 10.1037/dev0000489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. LeMoult J, Humphreys KL, Tracy A, Hoffmeister JA, Ip E, Gotlib IH. Meta-analysis: exposure to early life stress and risk for depression in childhood and adolescence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2020) 59:842–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Straub J, Brown R, Malejko K, Bonenberger M, Grön G, Plener PL, et al. Adolescent depression and brain development: evidence from voxel-based morphometry. J Psychiatry Neurosci. (2019) 44:237–45. doi: 10.1503/jpn.170233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Pagliaccio D, Alqueza KL, Marsh R, Auerbach RP. Brain volume abnormalities in youth at high risk for depression: adolescent brain and cognitive development study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2020) 59:1178–88. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.09.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Schmaal L, Hibar DP, Sämann PG, Hall GB, Baune BT, Jahanshad N, et al. Cortical abnormalities in adults and adolescents with major depression based on brain scans from 20 cohorts worldwide in the ENIGMA major depressive disorder working group. Mol. Psychiatry. (2017) 22:900–9 doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.60

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Bora E, Fornito A, Pantelis C, Yücel M. Gray matter abnormalities in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of voxel based morphometry studies. J Affect Disord. (2012) 138:9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Isquith P, Gioia G, Espy K. Executive function in preschool children: examination through everyday behavior. Dev Neuropsychol. (2004) 26:403–22. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2601_3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Haynes L, Ip A, Cho IYK, Dimond D, Rohr CS, Bagshawe M, et al. Grey and white matter volumes in early childhood: a comparison of voxel-based morphometry pipelines. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2020) 46:100875 doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ganuthula VRR, Sinha S. The looking glass for intelligence quotient tests: the interplay of motivation, cognitive functioning, and affect. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:02857. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Sternberg RJ. The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press (2018). doi: 10.1017/9781316817049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Wechsler D. WAIS-III Administration and Scoring Manual: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation (1997).

Google Scholar

38. Chae PK, Ji-Hye K, Kyung-Sun N. Diagnosis of ADHD among gifted children in relation to KEDI-WISC and T.O.V.A. performance. Gift Child Q. (2003) 47:192–201. doi: 10.1177/001698620304700303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Koh SH, Shin MS, Hong MK. The study of the attentional problems and cognitive characteristics of children with mental disorder by KEDI-WISC and TOVA. Korean J Clin Psychol. (1996) 15:165–78.

Google Scholar

40. Shin MS, Oh KJ, Hong MK. Cognitive characteristics of ADHD children assessed by KEDI-WISC. Korean J Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (1990) 1:55–64.

Google Scholar

41. Choi W-J. Verification of Homogeneity of K-WAIS-IV and K-WAIS Intelligence Quotients: Focusing on Normal Male and Female Adults in Their 20s. Seoul: Graduate School Duksung Women's University (2015).

Google Scholar

42. Kyungja O, Taeho Y, Youngsook P, Junggyu K, Youngho L. Age-intelligence relationship in adulthood : a cross-sectional analysis of K-WAIS standardization data. Korean J Clin Psychol. (1992) 11:22–30.

Google Scholar

43. Grandjean J, D'Ostilio K, Phillips C, Balteau E, Degueldre E, Luxen A, et al. Modulation of brain activity during a stroop inhibitory task by the kind of cognitive control required. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e41513. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Malek A, Hekmati I, Amiri S, Pirzadeh J, Gholizadeh H. The standardization of Victoria Stroop color-word test among Iranian bilingual adolescents. Arch Iran Med. (2013) 16:380–4. doi: 10.1155/2013/165193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Lavoie M, Charlebois P. The discriminant validity of Stroop color and word test: toward a cost-effective strategy to distinguish subgroups of disruptive preadolescents. Psychol. Sch. (1994) 31:98–107. doi: 10.1002/1520-6807(199404)31:2<98::AID-PITS2310310203>3.0.CO;2-F

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Homack S, Riccio CA. A meta-analysis of the sensitivity and specificity of the Stroop color and word test with children. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2004) 19:725–43. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2003.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Paap KR, Anders-Jefferson R, Zimiga B, Mason L, Mikulinsky R. Interference scores have inadequate concurrent and convergent validity: should we stop using the flanker, Simon, and spatial Stroop tasks? Cogn Res Princ Implic. (2020) 5:7. doi: 10.1186/s41235-020-0207-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Lasee MJ, Choi H. Evidence of reliability and validity for a children's auditory continuous performance test. SAGE Open. (2013) 3:1–10. doi: 10.1177/2158244013511828

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. McGee RA, Clark SE, Symons D. Does the Conners' continuous performance test aid in ADHD diagnosis? J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2000) 28:415–24. doi: 10.1023/A:1005127504982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Rodríguez C, Areces D, García T, Cueli M, González-Castro P. Comparison between two continuous performance tests for identifying ADHD: traditional vs. virtual reality. Int J Clin Heal Psychol. (2018) 18:254–63. doi: 10.1016/j.ijchp.2018.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Park MH, Kweon YS, Lee SJ, Park EJ, Lee C, Lee CU. Differences in performance of ADHD children on a visual and auditory continuous performance test according to IQ. Psychiatry Investig. (2011) 8:227–33. doi: 10.4306/pi.2011.8.3.227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Fujioka T, Takiguchi S, Yatsuga C, Hiratani M, Hong K-EM, Shin M-S, et al. Advanced test of attention in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Japan for evaluation of methylphenidate and atomoxetine effects. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci. (2016) 14:79–87. doi: 10.9758/cpn.2016.14.1.79

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Dohyun K, Ho-Jang K, Mina H, Myung Ho L, Kyoung Min K. Network analysis for the symptom of depression with children's depression inventory in a large sample of school-aged children. J Affect Disord. (2021) 281:256–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Kovacs M. Manual For the Children's Depression Inventory. North Tonawanda, NJ: Multi-Health Systems (1992).

Google Scholar

55. Twenge JM, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Age, gender, race, socioeconomic status, and birth cohort difference on the children's depression inventory: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Psychol. (2002) 111:578. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.111.4.578

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Cho SC, Lee Y. Development of the Korean form of the Kovac's children's depression inventory. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc. (1990) 29:943–56.

Google Scholar

57. Smucker MR, Craighead WE, Craighead LW, Green BJ. Normative and reliability data for the children's depression inventory. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (1986) 14:25–39. doi: 10.1007/BF00917219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. J Neuroimage. (2007) 38:95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Hidemasa T, Osamu A, Hidenori Y, Shigeki A, Kiyoto K, Kuni O. Cerebral asymmetry in patients with schizophrenia: a voxel-based morphometry (VBM) and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) study. J Magn Reson Imaging. (2010) 31:221–6. doi: 10.1002/jmri.22017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Goto M, Kamatgata K, Saito A, Abe O, Motoi Y, Miyajima M, et al. Detection of lesions depends on the DARTEL template used in group comparisons with voxel-based morphometry. Acta Radiol. (2020) 61:236–43. doi: 10.1177/0284185119855735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Sartorius A, Demirakca T, Böhringer A, Clemm von Hohenberg C, Aksay SS, Bumb JM, et al. Electroconvulsive therapy increases temporal gray matter volume and cortical thickness. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2016) 26:506–17. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.12.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau D, Papathanassiou F, Crivello O, Etard N, Delcroix B, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. (2002) 15:273–89. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Burgund ED, Kang HC, Kelly JE, Buckner RL, Synder AZ, Peterson SE, et al. The feasibility of a common stereotactic space for children and adults in fMRI studies of development. Neuroimage. (2002) 17:184–200. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kang HC, Burgund ED, Lugar H, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. Comparison of functional activation foci in children and adults using a common stereotactic space. Neuroimage. (2003) 19:16–28. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00038-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Muzik O, Chugani DC, Juhász C, Shen C, Chugani HT. Statistical parametric mapping: assessment of application in children. Neuroimage. (2000) 12:538–49. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Poldrack RA. Interpreting developmental changes in neuroimaging signals. Hum Brain Mapp. (2010) 31:872–8. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Richards JE, Sanchez C, Phillips-Meek M, Xie W. A database of age-appropriate average MRI templates. Neuroimage. (2017) 124:1254–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Ross P, de Gelder B, Crabbe F, Grosbras M-H. Emotion modulation of the body-selective areas in the developing brain. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2019) 38:100660. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Verdejo-Román J, Björnholm L, Muetzel RL, Torres-Espínola FJ, Lieslehto J, Jaddoe V, et al. Maternal prepregnancy body mass index and offspring white matter microstructure: results from three birth cohorts. Int J Obes. (2019) 43:1995–2006. doi: 10.1038/s41366-018-0268-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Weissa Y, Boothb RJ. Neural correlates of the lexicality effect in children. Brain Lang. (2017) 175:64–70. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2017.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. American Academy of Pediatrics. (2019). Stages of Adolescence. Available online at: https://www.healthychildren.org/English/ages-stages/teen/Pages/Stages-of-Adolescence.aspx (accessed May 10, 2021).

Google Scholar

72. Genovese CR, Lazar NA, Nichols T. Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage. (2002) 15:870–8. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Gordon BA, McCullough A, Mishra S, Blazey TM, Su Y, Christensen J, et al. Cross-sectional and longitudinal atrophy is preferentially associated with tau rather than amyloid β positron emission tomography pathology. Alzheimers Dement. (2018) 10:245–52. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2018.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Jung K-I, Park M-H, Park B, Kim S-Y, Kim YO, Kim B-N, et al. Cerebellar gray matter volume, executive function, and insomnia: gender differences in adolescents. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:855. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-37154-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Molteni E, Rocaa MA, Strazzer S, Pagani E, Colombo K, Arrigoni F, et al. A diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging study of paediatric patients with severe non-traumatic brain injury. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2017) 59:199–206. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Yu Q, He Z, Zubkov D, Huang S, Kurochkin I, Yang X, et al. Lipidome alterations in human prefrontal cortex during development, aging, and cognitive disorders. Mol Psychiatry. (2020) 25:2952–69. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0200-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Cservenka A, Nagel BJ. Neuroscience of alcohol for addiction medicine: Neurobiological targets for prevention and intervention in adolescents. Prog Brain Res. (2016) 223:215–35. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.07.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Rueda MR, Posner MI. Development of Attention Networks. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2013). doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199988693.013.0015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annu Rev Psychol. (2007) 58:1–23. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Rueda MR, Fan J, McCandliss BD, Halparin JD, Gruber DB, Lercari LP, et al. Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia. (2004) 42:1029–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Posner M, Rueda MR. Development of Attentional Networks, Vol. 1. Oxford: The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology (2013). p. 683–705. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199958450.013.0024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Tiego J, Bellgrove MA, Whittle S, Pantelis C, Testa R. Common mechanisms of executive attention underlie executive function and effortful control in children. Dev Sci. (2020) 23:1–25. doi: 10.1111/desc.12918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Max JE, Manes FF, Robertson BAM, Mathews K, Fox PT, Lancaster J. Prefrontal and executive attention network lesions and the development of attention-deficit/hyperactivity symptomatology. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2005) 44:443–450. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000156661.38576.0f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Akyurek G. Executive functions and neurology in children and adolescents. In: Occupational Therapy - Therapeutic and Creative Use of Activity (2018). doi: 10.5772/intechopen.78312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Reising MM, Bettis AH, Dunbar JP, Watson KH, Gruhn M, Hoskinson KR, et al. Stress, coping, executive function, and brain activation in adolescent offspring of depressed and nondepressed mothers. Child Neuropsychol. (2017) 24:638–56.doi: 10.1080/09297049.2017.1307950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Leskin LP, White PM. Attentional networks reveal executive function deficits in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychol. (2007) 21:275–84. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.3.275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: frontal lobe, executive functions, late childhood, adolescence, attentional networks

Citation: Sung D, Park B, Kim B, Kim H, Jung K-I, Lee S-Y, Kim B-N, Park S and Park M-H (2021) Gray Matter Volume in the Developing Frontal Lobe and Its Relationship With Executive Function in Late Childhood and Adolescence: A Community-Based Study. Front. Psychiatry 12:686174. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.686174

Received: 26 March 2021; Accepted: 07 June 2021;
Published: 13 July 2021.

Edited by:

Li Yang, Peking University Sixth Hospital, China

Reviewed by:

Grace Eugenia Sameve, UNICEF Indonesia, Indonesia
Xiaoyan Ke, Nanjing Brain Hospital Affiliated to Nanjing Medical University, China

Copyright © 2021 Sung, Park, Kim, Kim, Jung, Lee, Kim, Park and Park. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Min-Hyeon Park, neominnie00@daum.net

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.