Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 31 January 2022
Sec. Addictive Disorders

Link Between Perception of Treatment Need and Craving Reports in Addiction

\nLaura Lambert,,Laura Lambert1,2,3Fuschia Serre,,Fuschia Serre1,2,3Berangere ThiriouxBerangere Thirioux4Nematollah Jaafari,Nematollah Jaafari4,5Perrine RouxPerrine Roux6Marie Jauffret-Roustide,,Marie Jauffret-Roustide7,8,9Laurence LalanneLaurence Lalanne10Jean-Pierre Dauloude,Jean-Pierre Daulouède2,11Marc Auriacombe,,,
Marc Auriacombe1,2,3,12*
  • 1University of Bordeaux, SANPSY, USR 3413, Bordeaux, France
  • 2CNRS, SANPSY, USR 3413, Bordeaux, France
  • 3Pôle Interétablissement d'Addictologie, CH Ch. Perrens and CHU de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
  • 4Unité de Recherche Clinique Intersectorielle en Psychiatrie à vocation régionale Pierre Deniker, Centre Hospitalier Henri Laborit, Poitiers, France
  • 5Université de Poitiers, Poitiers, France
  • 6Aix Marseille University, INSERM, IRD, SESSTIM, Sciences Economiques and Sociales de la Santé and Traitement de l'Information Médicale, Marseille, France
  • 7Cermes 3, Inserm U988, CNRS UMR 8236, Université de Paris, EHESS, Paris, France
  • 8British Columbia Centre on Substance Use, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 9Baldy Center for Law and Social Policy, Buffalo University of Social Sciences, New York, NY, United States
  • 10INSERM 1114, Department of Psychiatry and Addictology, University Hospital of Strasbourg, Fédération de Médecine Translationnelle de Strasbourg (FMTS), Strasbourg, France
  • 11Centre de Soins et d'Accompagnement et de Prévention en Addictologie (CSAPA), BIZIA, Médecins du Monde, Centre Hospitalier de la côte Basque, Bayonne, France
  • 12Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, United States

Background: Perception of treatment need (PTN), a component of clinical insight, is associated to negative addiction treatment outcomes when low. Our hypothesis was that lower PTN was associated with less craving when reported retrospectively, the most common measure of craving in clinical settings.

Objective: To explore the association between PTN and craving among a dataset of subjects with severe substance use disorders.

Methods: Participants were recruited from outpatient addiction clinic admissions or harm reduction program services. Good and low PTN were based on consistency between severe addiction (at least six DSM-5 criteria) and self-report need for addiction treatment from the Addiction Severity Index. Craving was retrospectively characterized over the past 30 days. Multiple regression analyses were conducted.

Results: Participants with low PTN (n = 97) retrospectively reported less frequent and intense episodes of craving, compared with participants with good PTN (n = 566) after controlling for sociodemographic factors, addiction type, and severity (p < 0.0001).

Conclusion: Low perception of treatment need among subjects with severe addictions is associated to less retrospective report of craving, which may contribute to reduced efficiency of treatment. Further studies are needed to explore the mechanisms of the association.

Introduction

Addiction is characterized by an impaired control over use of reinforcing substances or behaviors and repeated relapses after attempts to stop (1, 2). Craving is reported as transient, intrusive, and unwanted experiences of urges for use of a substance or an addictive behavior (1, 3). Importantly, craving is a major risk factor for relapse (4, 5) and consequently considered a target for treatment (6, 7). In clinical practice, therapists usually evaluate craving by asking patients to self-report craving episodes that have occurred in the past days/weeks or since the previous interview. Accordingly, the patients' capacity to identify, remember, and self-report craving is paramount for the efficiency of therapies focused on craving management.

Despite the effectiveness and availability of treatments, only a minority of people initiate treatment for addiction [e.g., (810)]. Indeed, according to a global epidemiological survey of more than 70,000 people across all continents, only 39.1% of people with a current substance addiction (excluding tobacco) admit to needing medical care (11). Of these, only 7.1% actually received adequate treatment for their addiction (11). In addition to social and economic barriers, the belief that treatments are not effective (12), or cultural norms (13), there is another fundamental barrier to treatment, which is the lack of perceived need for treatment (11). Lack of perceived treatment need has been documented among adults with substance use disorders (SUD) (14). Perception of treatment need (PTN) is one of the three subdimensions of clinical insight according to the David model (15, 16). Clinical insight is the capability to recognize one's mental illness, to recognize its symptoms and consequences, to perceive need for treatment, and to consent to medical care [for review see (17, 18)]. PTN growth has been hypothesized as the last step of clinical insight growth after problem recognition and symptom identification and relabeling to illness (14).

Lower clinical insight level is often associated with a reduced capacity to recognize, estimate, and self-report symptoms or disorder severity [e.g., (19, 20)] and is associated to lower treatment response (17). It is possible that the capacity to recognize and/or self-report craving episodes may be impaired among people with a low clinical insight and low PTN. In usual clinical practice, if this was the case, such patients would be less likely to benefit from craving management treatments contributing to poorer treatment outcomes.

To date, only few studies have explored the association between some components of clinical insight and craving, and results are inconsistent. In one study, subjects with an “impaired awareness of drug-seeking behavior,” evaluated with task choice preference, reported more cocaine craving (21). In another study, no association was found between “perceived need to change one's own drug use,” also evaluated with task choice preference, and cocaine craving, but the sample was very small (n = 18) (22). In contrast, two studies reported lower or equal level of craving for alcohol among individuals with lower clinical insight evaluated on the self-reported Hanil Alcohol Insight Scale (23, 24). To our knowledge, no study has yet examined the association between PTN and craving.

As individuals with lower clinical insight level are known to underestimate their symptoms severity [e.g., (25)], we hypothesized that lower perception of addiction treatment need (PTN) will be associated to less craving when reported retrospectively. To test this, we examined the correlation between perception of treatment need and craving, self-reported retrospectively, among subjects with a severe addiction, and controlling for addiction and subject characteristics.

Materials and Methods

Cohort Description

Data were extracted from two cohort studies, the Addiction Aquitaine Cohort (ADDICTAQUI) between June 2012 and June 2019 (26), and the Bordeaux subsample of the COhort to identify Structural and INdividual factors associated with drug USe (COSINUS) between November 2016 and May 2018 (27).

ADDICTAQUI cohort participants were recruited from outpatient addiction treatment centers that met the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) (1) criteria for a substance use disorder or behavioral addiction (e.g., pathological gaming) for at least one substance or behavior. Non-inclusion criteria were severe cognitive impairment or illiteracy.

COSINUS cohort participants were recruited from active substance users (illegal substances other than cannabis or diverted psychotropic medications) in harm reduction settings that had injected at least once during the month prior to inclusion. Non-inclusion criteria were to be under judicial protection.

Common inclusion criteria to the two samples were over 18-year-old, French-speaking, regular (2 times per week for the past 6 months) users of psychoactive substances and documented informed consent. All participants received a standard baseline clinical interview with a trained clinical research interviewer using the Addiction Severity Index (ASI), a numerical rating scale to assess craving, and the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI). Both cohorts were approved by French biomedical research regulations and ethical committees (CNIL, CPP, CEEI/IRB).

Selection of Participants for This Study

Participants were selected for the current analyses if they met a current DSM-5 severe intensity diagnosis of a substance use disorder (≥6 criteria) or pathological gambling disorder (≥8 criteria) explored by the MINI.

Participants self-reported their main problematic use (main addiction) during the ASI interview [Drug and Alcohol section, Question 14 (D14)]. When more than one, or no main addiction was reported, the most severe was selected (highest number of criteria). Groups of main addiction with fewer than 40 subjects were excluded because such sample sizes were too small to control for the effects of multiple analyses.

Thus, in total, 663 subjects were included in the study (Figure 1). This final sample was further divided into two groups according to the subjects' PTN status (see Section 2.3.4).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart of selection of subjects among ADDICTAQUI and COSINUS cohorts.

Instruments and Measures

Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview Adapted for DSM 5 Addiction

We adapted the MINI structured interview (28) to explore current DSM 5 addiction diagnosis (over the past 12 months) using the 9 criteria of gambling disorder and the 11 criteria of SUD (1, 2). More than one disorder was qualified as “poly-addiction.” “Diagnostic severity” was defined according to the standard DSM-5 cut-offs (mild: 2–3 criteria, moderate: 4–5 criteria, severe: ≥6 criteria) and “Addiction criteria” was defined according to the number of endorsed DSM-5 criteria.

Addiction Severity Index

The ASI is a semi-structured interview for a multidimensional assessment of problem substance users (29). We used a modified and validated French version of the ASI (m-ASI), adapted to include tobacco and behavioral addictions (30). The m-ASI explores use lifetime and over the past 30 days.

The ASI-D14 item determines the main problematic use according to the subject's opinion. After controlling for the DSM-5 diagnostic status, this substance/behavior was considered the “main addiction.”

The ASI-D30/31 items assess the subject's self-report “Need for [main addiction] treatment” using a 5-point scale: not at all (0), slightly (1), moderately (2), considerably (3), and extremely (4). For the purpose of this study, the variable “Need for (main addiction) treatment” was categorized: slight (scores: 0–1), mild (scores: 2–3), and extreme (score: 4), and only slight and extreme values (0–1 and 4) were considered.

Current use” assesses the number of days of use in the past 30 days.

Duration of regular use” assesses the number of years of regular use, which was defined as at least 2 times per week for 6 months or more.

Craving Evaluation

Craving was evaluated during a face-to-face interview through a self-report assessment covering the past 30 days. “Craving frequency” was defined as the number of days with craving (0–30). “Mean intensity” and “maximal intensity” ever experienced were collected using a numerical rating scale from 0 (no craving) to 10 (extreme craving). Craving was defined as “an intense desire and/or the occurrence of obsessive thoughts centered on the (main addiction).” This single-item method is the most used in clinical and research practices and is effective in assessing craving (6, 31, 32).

Perception of Treatment Need

For the purpose of this study, “Perception of treatment need” was modeled as the consistency between the objective interviewer measurement of “Diagnostic severity” (Severe Use Disorder according to DSM-5 cut-off MINI) and the subjective self-report evaluation of the “need for (main addiction) treatment” by participants (ASI-D30/31). This type of method, which contrasted objective (informant, clinician) vs. subjective (self-report, auto-questionnaire) measures, is often used to assess insight, including in addiction [e.g., (21, 22, 3335)]. As shown in Figure 1, participants were considered having a “Good PTN” when they objectively presented with a severe disorder and subjectively self-reported an extreme need for treatment (ASI-D30/31 = 4). Subjects were considered having a “Low PTN” when they objectively presented with severe disorder but subjectively self-reported only slight need for treatment (ASI-D30/31 = 0 or 1). Analyses were conducted on two groups named “Low PTN” and “Good PTN,” with, respectively, 97 and 566 subjects.

Population

The variable “Population” was defined according to the cohort from which data were extracted: “COSINUS” or “ADDICTAQUI.”

Statistical Analysis

Univariate analyses examined the association between craving variables (frequency, mean and maximal intensities) and PTN group, and with potential confounding factors: age, sex, bachelor's degree, addiction criteria, main addiction, past addiction treatment, current use (days), duration of regular use (years), and population. Additional analyses also compared PTN groups on sociodemographic variables and addiction-related factors, and examined the PTN–craving association in each main addiction groups separately (Supplemental Materials). Because of the non-normal distribution of variance, non-parametric tests were used for all univariate analyses according to variable types [Wilcoxon test (z), Kruskal–Wallis test (χ2), Spearman's correlation (ρ), or Pearson Khi-square test (χ2)].

Multiple generalized linear models (GLM) of Poisson tested the association between craving frequency and PTN and ordinal logistic regressions tested the association between craving intensities and PTN. All models used a backward stepwise selection and controlled for clinically relevant variables (age, sex, bachelor's degree, addiction criteria, population, main addiction), and variables moderately associated with craving in univariate analysis [p (enter) = 0.25; p (exit) = 0.1]. Inter-correlations between independent variables were tested to avoid multicollinearity [variance inflation factor (VIF) <5] before entrance in the same model. GLM produce coefficients that represent the predicted logarithm of counts of the dependent variable and the exponentiation of these coefficients places them on the same scale as the count variable [i.e., number of days of craving (craving frequency)] (36). The value of the exponentiated intercept (null model) represents a multiplicative change in the dependent variable for every one-unit change in the predictor (36).

Statistical analysis was performed using JMP Pro 15.0 (SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina). The level of significance was set at p < 0.05. Bonferroni tests were applied to prevent the probability of a type I error. We conducted n = 54 univariate analyses, so the level of significance was set at p < 0.0009. Multiple generalized linear model and ordinal logistic regressions were FDR corrected.

Results

Sample Description

A total of 663 subjects were included in the analyses. The participants were primarily male (69%), with an average age of 38.9 (SD = 11.1) years, 49% had a bachelor degree, 14.6% (n = 97) had a low PTN, and 85.4% (n = 566) a good PTN. The most frequent main addiction was alcohol (43%), and a minority of subjects (17%) reported no craving episodes over the past 30 days. In ADDICTAQUI population (n = 586), 44 subjects (7.5%) had a low PTN and 542 (92.5%) had a good PTN. In COSINUS population (n = 77), 53 subjects (68.8%) had a low PTN and 24 (31.2%) had a good PTN (Supplementary Table 1).

Univariate Analyses

As described in Table 1 and Figure 2, subjects with a low PTN reported significantly less craving in terms of frequency (Wilcoxon test; z = −7.03; r = −0.27; p < 0.0001), mean intensity (Wilcoxon test; z = −5.02; r = −0.20; p < 0.0001), and maximal intensity (Wilcoxon test; z = −6.18; r = −0.24; p < 0.0001). Compared with those with a good PTN, all correlations had a small size effect. Craving was also slightly associated to a higher number of endorsed DSM-5 diagnostic criteria (Spearman tests; ρ = 0.14 for frequency; ρ = 0.21 for mean intensity; ρ = 0.23 for maximal intensity; p ≤ 0.002 for all) and was slightly less frequent and intense in COSINUS population (Wilcoxon tests; z = −3.73; r = −0.15 for frequency; z = −3.77; r = −0.15 for mean intensity; z = −3.47; r = −0.14 for maximal intensity; p ≤ 0.002 for all).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Univariate analyses between craving parameters, PTN status, and confounding variables (demographic, addiction-related data).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Diagram distribution of subjects according to PTN groups and craving parameters. (A) Frequency (n = 659); (B) mean intensity (n = 660); (C) maximal intensity (n = 662). The density diagram shows how the data are distributed for each craving value: the wider the diagram, the more subjects have that craving value (violin type, equal area size). The whisker boxes show the distributions of the data according to quartiles, range, and median; the isolated points are the outliers.

Compared to those in the good PTN group (Supplementary Table 1), subjects in the low PTN group were less likely to be initiating treatment, more from COSINUS than from ADDICTAQUI population (Khi2 of Pearson; χ2 = 204.91; df = 1; p <0.0001) and more likely to have an opioid use disorder than an alcohol or tobacco use disorder (post hoc comparisons, p < 0.05). PTN groups did not significantly differ on sociodemographic variables or on other addiction-related factors (Supplementary Table 1). Significant small to moderate correlations were found between PTN and craving for alcohol (Wilcoxon test; z = −4.55; r = −0.27 for frequency; z = −4.73; r = −0.28 for maximal intensity; p < 0.0001 for all), opiates (Wilcoxon test; z = −3.44; r = −0.34 for frequency; p = 0.0006), and a tendency in the same direction for all other substances (Supplementary Table 2).

Multiple Regression Models

Multiple regression analyses demonstrated that low PTN was significantly associated with less craving reports (p < 0.05 for the 3 parameters), after adjustment for sociodemographic, population, and addiction-related factors (Table 2). For craving frequency, subjects were predicted to have 17.29 days [B (intercept) = 2.85; e2.85 = 17.29] of craving when all other terms were zero. Thus, a subject with low PTN is expected to self-report, on average, 0.77 times fewer days of craving, i.e., 13.31 days less (17.29 × 0.77) than those with good PTN (p < 0.0001). A subject with low PTN is expected to self-report, on average, a mean intensity of craving 0.74 times lower and a maximal intensity 0.63 lower than those with good PTN (respectively: p < 0.05 and p < 0.001).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Multiple models of predictors of craving frequency and craving mean and maximal intensities.

Lower craving reports (all 3 parameters) were also found associated with fewer days of use over the past month (p < 0.001) and a lower number of DSM-5 criteria endorsed (p < 0.001) (Table 2).

Discussion

Our main objective was to examine the association between perception of treatment need and craving, self-reported retrospectively, among subjects with severe addiction. PTN status was modeled as the consistency between objective addiction severity (severe use disorder according to DSM-5 cut-off) and the ASI self-report of the need for addiction treatment (slight or extreme) by participants. Adjusting for sociodemographic, addiction type, severity, and population, our results show, for the first time, that lower PTN was cross-sectionally associated with less frequent and intense episodes of craving self-reported retrospectively over the past month.

Our results are consistent with a previous study, which showed that lower clinical insight level was related to lower craving for alcohol self-reported retrospectively on the Obsessive-Compulsive Drinking Scale (OCDS) (23). Interestingly, some studies in addiction suggest that low clinical insight relates to earlier relapse (24, 37) or worse outcomes (e.g., drinking behavior) (38) after addiction treatment. Based on our results, we make the hypothesis that low clinical insight may negatively impact the ability to self-report craving retrospectively (the most common way craving is assessed in standard clinical practice) that in turn may interfere with participant's ability to seek treatment, and control craving when in treatment, and in turn be associated with a greater risk of relapse.

Although it is beyond the goal of this study to determine the underlying mechanisms of the association between PTN and craving, it is interesting to note that neuroimaging studies report that both are sustained by functional abnormalities in the same brain regions, i.e., anterior insula, anterior cingulate, and ventro-medial prefrontal cortices (vmPFC), that are involved in interoception (perception and processing of internal body signals) and in evaluating the personal relevance of stimuli (22, 25, 39).

Over-representation of subjects with AUD in our sample (43%) may have influenced the results as the toxicological features of alcohol are known to impair cognitive functions including memory (40, 41). However, subjects with AUD were mostly in the good PTN group compared with those with opioid use disorders (Supplementary Table 1). Moreover, correlation between PTN and craving was observed not only for alcohol (p < 0.001) but also for opiates (craving frequency; p < 0.001), and a trend in the same direction was also observed for all substances, except for tobacco craving mean intensity (Supplementary Table 2). It is also worth noting that the association persisted after adjustment for main addiction, past month consumption and duration of regular use, suggesting that it could be a common feature across addiction types and not just limited to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol.

Surprisingly, a large majority of our sample (85%) had a good PTN, whereas previous studies reported that individuals with addictions have mainly low clinical insight: 57% (42) and low PTN: 72% (14). One explanation is that our sample was mostly composed of subjects who self-initiated treatment (88% from ADDICTAQUI population), which may require a certain level of perception of treatment need. However, in our study, low PTN was also found in smaller proportions than in the literature in those not seeking treatment (from COSINUS cohort). Also, the overall good level of PTN in our sample could also be explained by the level of addiction severity, as only subjects with a “severe” use disorder were included in the analyses.

Contrary to our study, a study conducted among cocaine users showed that subjects with low “awareness of drug seeking behavior,” another sub-dimension of clinical insight, reported more craving (21). A possible reason to explain this result, in addition to the fact that this previous study was only among cocaine users and examined a different sub-dimension of clinical insight, may be the way craving was assessed. In our study, craving was assessed over a past period (retrospectively) and not currently (when it occurs), as in this other study. Indeed, low clinical insight in addiction has been previously linked to lower cognitive performances, especially executive functions, learning and episodic memories (43, 44), and could therefore be correlated with more difficulty to recollect past craving episodes.

The current study has several limitations that need to be acknowledged. First, our measurement of PTN was originally developed for the present study and is not yet validated. However, to assess PTN objectively and indirectly is less likely to be biased by social desirability compared with a direct (e.g., self-report scale) assessment (45, 46). Second, our sample was composed only of subjects with a severe addiction, thus results cannot be generalized to all individuals with addictions, especially those who have a less severe addiction. In the current study, we chose to exclude participants with mild to moderate addiction severity to contrast the objective severity and subjective report of PTN. Further studies should involve individuals with a wider range of addiction severity by evaluating the PTN differently. Third, we chose to compare only the extreme values for “Need for treatment” for which the differences should, in theory, be the most noticeable. However, we also tested the craving–PTN association with the full range of “Need for treatment” (0–4) evaluations and the results (data not shown) were consistent with those reported. Finally, PTN and craving were only evaluated once, cross-sectionally. Some theories suggest that clinical insight could fluctuate over time [e.g., (14, 33, 34)]. As craving is also a fluctuating phenomenon (47), it would be interesting to study how the fluctuations of one interact with the fluctuations of the other in daily life. Further studies should explore the effect of cognitive capacities on deficits in PTN or clinical insight and how they impact symptom perception and their retrospective report, and compare results when craving is assessed prospectively.

Conclusion

This study explored the link between perception of treatment need, one of the three main sub-dimensions of clinical insight, and self-report of craving assessed retrospectively in a large sample of individuals with various addictions. Interestingly, based on a retrospective self-report, individuals with low perception of treatment need self-reported less frequent and intense episodes of craving, even after adjustment for addiction severity, and sociodemographic and population characteristics. This was not the case in individuals with a similar severity but a higher perception of treatment need. These results have important clinical implications when addiction treatment is focused on the management of craving. Future studies are needed to further investigate and better understand how PTN and more generally clinical insight impact craving, relapse, and treatment adherence.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Bordeaux. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

LLam, FS, and MA: conception, design of the study, writing—review, and editing the article. LLam: organized the database, performed the statistical analysis, and wrote the first draft of the article. MA, FS, LLam, and J-PD: data collection. MA, FS, NJ, BT, MJ-R, PR, and LLal: funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by Nouvelle-Aquitaine region (No. 2017-1R30114-00013238). Funding for COSINUS was supported by MILDECA Grant 2016. Funding for ADDICTAQUI was supported by Grant PHRC (2006- 2014) from the French Ministry of Health, Grant MILDT/MILDECA 2010, 2016 and office and staff support from Hospital Charles Perrens.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank Charlotte Kervran for supervision of COSINUS data collection from Bordeaux site. The authors are grateful to medical staff of the Bordeaux recruitment sites of ADDICTAQUI and COSINUS for facilitating recruitment of participants (Saman Sarram, Jacques Dubernet, Bérengère Gelot, Marie Chivé, Bénédicte Apert, Victor Harly, Nathalie Ramdjee, Louise Jakubiec, Jean-Michel Delile, Véronique Latour, Jean-Pierre Daulouède). A very special thanks to all the interviewers of the ADDICTAQUI Bordeaux site team 2012-2019 (Jean-Marc Alexandre, Romain Debrabant, Hortense Hurmic, Charlotte Kervran, Manon Chevalier, Sarah Moriceau, Lucie Fournet, Marion Desset and their colleagues), and Bayonne site team 2012-2019 (Virginie Beltran, Cécilia Maitre and their colleagues) and the interviewers of the COSINUS Bordeaux site team 2016-2018 (Charlotte Kervran).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.790203/full#supplementary-material

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders- Fifth Edition- DSM-5. Washington, DC; Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

2. Hasin DS, O'Brien CP, Auriacombe M, Borges G, Bucholz K, Budney A, et al. DSM-5 criteria for substance use disorders: recommendations and rationale. Am J Psychiatry. (2013) 170:834–51. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12060782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Auriacombe M, Serre F, Denis C, Fatseas M. Diagnosis of addictions. In: Pickard H, Ahmed S, editors. The Routledge Handbook of the Philosophy Science of Addiction. London: Routledge (2018). p. 132–44.

Google Scholar

4. Tavares H, Zilberman ML, Hodgins DC, El-Guebaly N. Comparison of craving between pathological gamblers and alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2005) 29:1427–31. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000175071.22872.98

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Serre F, Fatseas M, Swendsen J, Auriacombe M. Ecological momentary assessment in the investigation of craving and substance use in daily life: a systematic review. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2015) 148:1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.12.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Tiffany ST, Wray JM. The clinical significance of drug craving. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2012) 1248:1–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06298.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Auriacombe M, Serre F, Fatséas M. Le craving : marqueur diagnostic et pronostic des addictions? In: Reynaud M, Benyamina A, Karila L, Aubin H-J, editors. 2 éd. Traité d'addictologie. Paris: Lavoisier (2016). p. 78–83.

Google Scholar

8. Rehm J, Allamani A, Elekes Z, Jakubczyk A, Manthey J, Probst C, et al. Alcohol dependence and treatment utilization in Europe - a representative cross-sectional study in primary care. BMC Fam Pract. (2015) 16:90. doi: 10.1186/s12875-015-0308-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Grant BF, Saha TD, Ruan WJ, Goldstein RB, Chou SP, Jung J, et al. Epidemiology of DSM-5 drug use disorder: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions-III. JAMA Psychiatry. (2016) 73:39–47. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Substance Abuse Health Services Administration. Key Substance Use and Mental Health Indicators in the United States: Results from the 2018 National Survey on Drug Use and Health. N.S.H.-. HHS Publication No. PEP19-5068. Rockville, MD : Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (2019).

Google Scholar

11. Degenhardt L, Glantz M, Evans-Lacko S, Sadikova E, Sampson N, Thornicroft G, et al. Estimating treatment coverage for people with substance use disorders: an analysis of data from the world mental health surveys. World Psychiatry. (2017) 16:299–307. doi: 10.1002/wps.20457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Mojtabai R, Crum RM. Perceived unmet need for alcohol and drug use treatments and future use of services: results from a longitudinal study. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2013) 127:59–64. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Gopalkrishnan N, Babacan H. Cultural diversity and mental health. Australas Psychiatry. (2015) 23:6–8. doi: 10.1177/1039856215609769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Moeller SJ, Platt JM, Wu M, Goodwin RD. Perception of treatment need among adults with substance use disorders: longitudinal data from a representative sample of adults in the United States. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2020) 209:107895. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.107895

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. David AS. On insight and psychosis: discussion paper. J R Soc Med. (1990) 83:325–9. doi: 10.1177/014107689008300517

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. David AS, Bedford N, Wiffen B, Gilleen J. Failures of metacognition and lack of insight in neuropsychiatric disorders. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2012) 367:1379–90. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Thirioux B, Harika-Germaneau G, Langbour N, Jaafari N. The relation between empathy and insight in psychiatric disorders: phenomenological, etiological, neuro-functional mechanisms. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 10:966. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Jaafari N, Marková IS. Le concept d'insight en psychiatrie. Ann Méd Psychol. (2011) 169:2. doi: 10.1016/j.amp.2011.06.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Dackis CA, O'Brien CP. Cocaine dependence: a disease of the brain's reward centers. J Subst Abuse Treat. (2001) 21:111–7. doi: 10.1016/S0740-5472(01)00192-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Lyu KY, Lee K, Bejerano IL. Factors related to internalization of stigma for alcohol dependence among Korean men. Soc Behav Person Int J. (2017) 45:127–42. doi: 10.2224/sbp.5328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Moeller SJ, Konova AB, Parvaz MA, Tomasi D, Lane RD, Fort C, et al. Functional, structural, and emotional correlates of impaired insight in cocaine addiction. JAMA Psychiatry. (2014) 71:61–70. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Moeller SJ, Kundu P, Bachi K, Maloney T, Malaker P, Parvaz MA, et al. Self-awareness of problematic drug use: preliminary validation of a new fMRI task to assess underlying neurocircuitry. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2020) 209:107930. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.107930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Dandaba M, Ebrahimighavam S, Langbour N, Chatard A, Jaafari N. Échelle d'évaluation de l'insight dans l'alcoolisme : validation de la version française du questionnaire hanil alcohol insight scale (HAIS). Psychotropes. (2020) 26:87–113. doi: 10.3917/psyt.264.0087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Dandaba M, Serra W, Harika-Germaneau G, Silvain C, Langbour N, Solinas M, et al. Predicting relapse in patients with severe alcohol use disorder: the role of alcohol insight and implicit alcohol associations. Addict Behav. (2020) 107:106433. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Goldstein RZ, Craig AD, Bechara A, Garavan H, Childress AR, Paulus MP, et al. The neurocircuitry of impaired insight in drug addiction. Trends Cogn Sci. (2009) 13:372–80. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Auriacombe M,. ADDICTAQUI - Aquitaine Addiction Cohort: Trajectories of People With Addiction (substances or behaviour) in Contact With Health-Care System. Medical, Neurobiological, Sociological Psychological Characteristics. Prospective Multicentric, Multidisciplinary Study [Online]. epidemiologie-france.aviesan.fr: Portail Epidemiologie France (2019). Available online at: https://epidemiologie-france.aviesan.fr/en/epidemiology/records/cohorte-addiction-aquitaine-trajectoires-de-personnes-presentant-une-addiction-aux-substances-ou-une-addiction-comportementale-en-contact-avec-le-dispositif-de-soins.-caracteristiques-medicales-neurobiologiques-sociologiques-et-psychologiques.-etude#tab_1 (accessed September, 2021).

Google Scholar

27. Auriacombe M, Roux P, Briand Madrid L, Kirchherr S, Kervran C, Chauvin C, et al. Impact of drug consumption rooms on risk practices and access to care in people who inject drugs in France: the COSINUS prospective cohort study protocol. BMJ Open. (2019) 9:e023683. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. (1998) 20(Suppl. 59):22–33;quiz 34–57.

Google Scholar

29. McLellan AT, Kushner H, Metzger D, Peters R, Smith I, Grissom G, et al. The fifth edition of the addiction severity index. J Subst Abuse Treat. (1992) 9:199–213. doi: 10.1016/0740-5472(92)90062-S

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Denis C, Fatséas M, Beltran V, Serre F, Alexandre JM, Debrabant R, et al. Usefulness and validity of the modified addiction severity index: a focus on alcohol, drugs, tobacco, and gambling. Subst Abus. (2016) 37:168–75. doi: 10.1080/08897077.2015.1036334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Sayette MASS, Tiffany ST, Niaura RS, Martin CS, Shadel WG. The measurement of drug craving. Addiction. (2000) 95:S189–210. doi: 10.1080/09652140050111762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Enkema MC, Hallgren KA, Larimer ME. Craving is impermanent and it matters: investigating craving and cannabis use among young adults with problematic use interested in reducing use. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2020) 210:107957. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.107957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Marková IS. L'Insight en Psychiatrie. Rueil-Malmaison: Doin (2011).

Google Scholar

34. Verdejo-García A, Pérez-García M. Substance abusers' self-awareness of the neurobehavioral consequences of addiction. Psychiatry Res. (2008) 158:172–80. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Le Berre AP, Pinon K, Vabret F, Pitel A.-L., et al. Study of metamemory in patients with chronic alcoholism using a feeling-of-knowing episodic memory task. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2010) 34:1888–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01277.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Coxe S, West Sg Fau - Aiken LS, Aiken LS. The analysis of count data: a gentle introduction to poisson regression and its alternatives. J Pers Assess. (2009) 91:121–36. doi: 10.1080/00223890802634175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kim JS, Park BK, Kim GJ, Kim SS, Jung JG, Oh MK, et al. The role of alcoholics' insight in abstinence from alcohol in male Korean alcohol dependents. J Korean Med Sci. (2007) 22:132–7. doi: 10.3346/jkms.2007.22.1.132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Willems PJ, Letemendia JJ, Arroyave F. A two-year follow-up study comparing short with long stay in-patient treatment of alcoholics. Br J Psychiatry. (1973) 122:637–48. doi: 10.1192/bjp.122.6.637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Moeller SJ, Goldstein RZ. Impaired self-awareness in human addiction: deficient attribution of personal relevance. Trends Cogn Sci. (2014) 18:635–41. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Zahr NM, Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV. Perspectives on fronto-fugal circuitry from human imaging of alcohol use disorders. Neuropharmacology. (2017) 122:189–200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2017.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Nunes PT, Kipp BT, Reitz NL, Savage LM. Aging with alcohol-related brain damage: critical brain circuits associated with cognitive dysfunction. Int Rev Neurobiol. (2019) 148:101–68. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2019.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Raftery D, Kelly PJ, Deane FP, Baker AL, Ingram I, Goh MCW, et al. Insight in substance use disorder: a systematic review of the literature. Addict Behav. (2020) 111:106549. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Rinn W, Desai N, Rosenblatt H, Gastfriend DR. Addiction denial and cognitive dysfunction: a preliminary investigation. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2002) 14:52–57. doi: 10.1176/jnp.14.1.52

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Poncin M, Neumann A, Luminet O, Vande Weghe N, Philippot P, de Timary P. Disease recognition is related to specific autobiographical memory deficits in alcohol-dependence. Psychiatry Res. (2015) 230:157–64. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.08.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Kimberlin CL, Winterstein AG. Validity and reliability of measurement instruments used in research. Am J Health System Pharmacy. (2008) 65:2276–84. doi: 10.2146/ajhp070364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Zemore SE. The effect of social desirability on reported motivation, substance use severity, treatment attendance. J Subst Abuse Treat. (2012) 42:400–12. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2011.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Serre F, Fatseas M, Debrabant R, Alexandre JM, Auriacombe M, Swendsen J. Ecological momentary assessment in alcohol, tobacco, cannabis and opiate dependence: a comparison of feasibility and validity. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2012) 126:118–23. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.04.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: perception of treatment need, clinical insight, substance use disorder, craving, addiction

Citation: Lambert L, Serre F, Thirioux B, Jaafari N, Roux P, Jauffret-Roustide M, Lalanne L, Daulouède J-P and Auriacombe M (2022) Link Between Perception of Treatment Need and Craving Reports in Addiction. Front. Psychiatry 12:790203. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.790203

Received: 06 October 2021; Accepted: 29 November 2021;
Published: 31 January 2022.

Edited by:

Scott J. Moeller, Stony Brook Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

Ramesh Nagarajappa, Siksha O Anusandhan University, India
Muhammad A. Parvaz, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, United States

Copyright © 2022 Lambert, Serre, Thirioux, Jaafari, Roux, Jauffret-Roustide, Lalanne, Daulouède and Auriacombe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marc Auriacombe, marc.auriacombe@u-bordeaux.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.