Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 06 June 2022
Sec. Public Mental Health

Association Between Social Network and Cognitive Function: A Cross-Sectional Assessment From the Cardiovascular and Metabolic Diseases Etiology Research Center Cohort (2013–2018)

  • 1Department of Public Health, Graduate School, Yonsei University, Seoul, South Korea
  • 2Department of Sociology, Yonsei University, Seoul, South Korea
  • 3Department of Endocrinology and Metabolism, Ajou University School of Medicine, Suwon, South Korea
  • 4Department of Preventive Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea

Background: This study aimed to investigate how social networks are associated with cognitive function in the middle-aged and elderly Korean population.

Methods: A total of 7,704 individuals over the age of 50 were included from the baseline recruitment of the Cardiovascular and Metabolic Diseases Etiology Research Center cohort from the years 2013 to 2018. Egocentric social network characteristics including network size, intimacy, and frequency of face-to-face meetings were measured as exposures, and the Korean version of Mini-Mental State Examination (K-MMSE) score was measured to reflect general cognitive function as an outcome. We also stratified the analysis by income level into tertiles, with income caps of 42,000 thousand won and 72,000 thousand won. A general linear regression model was used, adjusting for age, gender, socioeconomic factors, lifestyle factors, depressive symptoms, and study settings.

Results: Social network properties were positively associated with cognitive function in both men and women. However, the specific estimates varied according to gender and income level. In men, frequency was most significantly associated with cognitive function (standardized β = 0.093, p-value <0 .0001). In women, the strength of the association with cognitive function was found in size (standardized β = 0.055, p-value = 0.001). The effect modification of income level could be seen in the association between frequency and cognitive function. The strongest association between frequency and cognitive function was found in the middle income group in men (standardized β = 0.114, p-value = 0.0063), and the low income group in women (standardized β = 0.076, p-value = 0.0039).

Conclusion: There were positive associations between social network properties (i.e., size, intimacy, and frequency of face-to-face meetings) and cognitive function. The degree of association varied according to social network properties, gender, and income level. Overall, among social network properties, social network size was an important factor in the cognition of women, whereas frequency was important in the cognition of men.

Introduction

Dementia is a chronic disease characterized by the impairment of cognitive functions such as thinking, memory, and reasoning. It is one of the greatest global challenges of the twenty-first century impacting health and social welfare (1). Moreover, the rise in life expectancy seems to have contributed to the increased prevalence of dementia (2). It has been estimated that there were more than 50 million people with dementia worldwide in 2019, and this number is expected to increase to 152 million by 2050 (3). In fact, the prevalence of dementia among Koreans aged 65 years and older was 9.2%, exceeding the estimated prevalence in other Asian countries (4.19–7.63%) (4). Further, the prevalence and incidence of dementia in Korea increased from 2003 to 2015 (5). As dementia requires continuous care and medical expenses, which are higher in cost than other diseases, its economic burden is difficult to ignore (6). The global cost of dementia in 2015 ($818 billion) has increased by 35.4% from that in 2010 ($604 billion) and is anticipated to reach $2 trillion by 2030 (7). Furthermore, in Korea, as of 2015, the elderly with dementia spent 2.4 times more in lifetime medical expenditures ($65,427) than elderly without dementia ($26,439) (6).

Cognitive function has been known to be related to socioeconomic status, activities of daily living, depressive symptoms, and social networks (8). Over the decades, several studies have investigated the relationship between social networks and cognitive function. According to a systematic review of longitudinal studies, a lack of social relationships increased cognitive decline (9). This study classified social relationships by structural (e.g., social network size, social activity) and functional (e.g., social support) aspects, and conducted the meta-analysis independently. Both aspects of social relationships had protective effects on cognitive decline (structural, OR = 1.08; functional, OR = 1.15) (9). However, the effect of social relationships on cognition may have been overestimated due to publication bias (9). Another meta-analysis of 39 articles showed that social activity had positive associations with global cognition, working memory, and processing speed but not with episodic memory or attention (10). Social network size and frequency of contact were associated with better global cognition and episodic memory but not with processing speed or attention (10). However, the results were not consistent between the studies because there were differences in the characteristics of the studies such as target population and study design (10).

Several longitudinal studies have reported that a better social network (e.g., network size and social activities) at baseline was associated with higher initial cognitive performances, but the association with subsequent cognition remains controversial (11, 12). The association with cognition was different depending on the network types (13). Individuals with multiple sources of networks (e.g., friends and relatives) had better cognitive outcomes, while network ties mainly with relatives had poorer cognitive health. A study in community-dwelling Spanish older adults reported that having a larger social network size (i.e., the number of family or friends with whom they had recent contact) decreased the risk of cognitive decline in the elderly (14). Another study in Taiwan suggested that participation in social activities reduced cognitive impairment as an important predictive factor (8). Other factors, such as marital status, living arrangement, emotional support, frequency of contact, and satisfaction, seemed to be associated with cognitive ability in old age, as seen in several studies (1519). This pattern was not considerably different from that observed in studies conducted in South Korea. According to one study based on the Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (KLoSA), older adults who were living with their spouses and had frequent contact with their children had a lower risk of cognitive decline (20). Moreover, other studies using KLoSA data showed that participating in diverse social activities had a positive effect on preventing cognitive impairment (21, 22).

Several previous studies have explored how social network size, social activities, and social engagement affect cognitive function in the elderly in many countries. However, most studies in Korea have focused more on social activities rather than social network properties, such as size or intimacy. Further, most studies were analyzed without classifying men and women. We expect that the association between social network properties and cognitive function would differ according to gender; however, evidence of this is insufficient. Moreover, research on the effect modification of socioeconomic status on the association is scarce. Therefore, we aimed to investigate the association between social network properties and cognitive function according to gender and income level in middle-aged and elderly South Korean population.

Materials and Methods

Data Collection and Participants

This study was designed as a cross-sectional study and used data from the Cardiovascular and Metabolic Diseases Etiology Research Center (CMERC) cohort. The CMERC cohort was designed to investigate the risk factors for cardiovascular disease (CVD) and produce evidence regarding the preventive treatment of CVD (18). Community-dwelling adults who did not have cardiovascular diseases were recruited by two research clinics (Yonsei University College of Medicine in Seoul and Ajou University School of Medicine Suwon), and hospital-based population participants were recruited at Severance Hospital, Seoul (23). The YUCM Clinic recruited eligible participants from Seoul, and capital areas including Incheon, Goyang, and Gimpo. The AUSM Clinic registered individuals living in Suwon, Yongin, and Hwaseong, which are the southern part of the capital area (23). The participants were recruited through advertisements in newspapers or posters in public places (23). Baseline measurements included socio-demographic factors, medical history, health-related behaviors, psychological health, social network, and support, which were collected through face-to-face interviews (23). Physical examinations including blood analysis and urinalysis were also conducted (23). A total of 8,697 community-based participants aged 30–64 years and 3,267 hospital-based participants aged 30–80 years were included in the baseline assessment between 2013 and 2018 (24). We excluded individuals under the age of 50 (N = 3,659) and those who had missing data on social network properties (N = 151) and Mini-Mental State Examination (N = 450). A total of 7,704 individuals (i.e., 3,027 men and 4,677 women) were included in the final analysis.

Exposure Variables: Social Network Properties

Information about egocentric social networks including network size, density, content, composition, intimacy, the volume of contact, and bridging potential (network member only connected to the respondent), was collected through face-to-face interviews by trained interviewers (23, 25). These factors were assessed using the same questionnaire developed by the US National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (NSHAP) which collected the list of multiple network members associated with the respondents and the relationships with them (23, 26). Social network properties were measured using the name generator module, which had been developed for the General Social Survey in the United States (25). The Korean version was previously carried out in a nationwide social survey and validated to consistently measure key networks in health research (26). The same questionnaires were used in the Korean Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (KSHAP) (23). We used social network size, intimacy and volume of contact to represent the social network status of participants. For network size, we asked participants to list up to six people, including their spouse, with whom they had discussed any important matters during the last 12 months. Since network size members are those who had discussed “important matters” with the participants, this measure captures the type of social bonds that are most likely to provide social support (25). The social network size is the core confidantes and is regarded important when social influence and support are considered (25). Next, they were asked “How close do you feel your relationship is with [name]?” and were asked to respond using a 4-point Likert scale. Possible responses included “not very close” (1), “somewhat close” (2), “very close” (3), and “extremely close” (4). Subsequently, we averaged the total score of all responses for network members and named this value “intimacy.” Finally, participants answered how often they would meet each member of their network, by using an 8-point Likert scale ranging from “less than once a year” (1), “once a year” (2), “more than once a year” (3), “once a month” (4), “once every 2 weeks” (5), “once a week” (6), “more than once a week” (7), and “everyday” (8). Likewise, we averaged the total score of all responses for network members and named this value “frequency.” It did not include the contact by phone or text and only the face-to-face meeting was measured. The social network card used in the CMERC study (24) is described in Supplementary Table 1.

Outcome Variable: Cognitive Function

In this study, the Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (K-MMSE) was conducted on participants over 50 years of age to assess global cognitive function. The MMSE is a widely used examination for screening dementia in both population studies and clinical practice (27). K-MMSE comprises 30 questions on orientation, verbal memory, concentration and calculation, language, praxis, and visuospatial construction. The estimated Cronbach’s alpha of the K-MMSE was 0.84, and that for the normal population was 0.74 (28). Further, the sensitivity was 0.91, and the specificity was 0.76 (28). Although the cut-off score of 24 is conventionally used for screening cognitive deficit, individuals with a score of 24 and under were few among our participants. Considering the statistical power, we used the total score as a continuous variable in the analysis.

Covariates

Covariates were age, gender, socioeconomic factors, lifestyle factors, depressive symptoms, and study settings. Socioeconomic factors included household income, years of education, marital status, living arrangements, and occupation. We classified the household income level into tertiles: a yearly income of 42,000 thousand won and under as a low-income group, over 42,000 thousand won and less than 72,000 thousand won as a middle-income group, and over 72,000 thousand won as a high-income group. Years of education were categorized into four groups according to the final WSA education level: 6 years or lower, 7 years to 9 years, 10 years to 12 years, and more than 12 years. Marital status was classified as married, divorced including separated, widowed, or never married. Living arrangement was dichotomized as living alone or not, and occupation as employed or not. Students and housewives were included in the employed group. Lifestyle factors included cigarette use, alcohol consumption, and physical activity. Cigarette use and alcohol consumption were categorized as current, past, or never used. For physical activity, a Korean version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire-Short Form (29) was used, and participants were asked about the number of days and duration of three levels of exercise: vigorous intensity activities, moderate intensity activities, and walking during the last 7 days. Based on this information, we quantified the total score of physical activity in MET minutes. We weighted 3.3 METs for walking, 4.0 METs for moderate intensity activities, and 8.0 METs for vigorous intensity activities (30). Depressive symptoms during the past 2 weeks were measured using the Beck Depression Inventory-II, which has been verified in the Korean general population (31). Since the data was collected from three different settings in two study centers (23, 32), study setting was included as a covariate.

Statistical Analysis

We analyzed baseline characteristics classified by K-MMSE score and gender to check for differences between the groups using the chi-square test and independent t-test. The association between social networks and cognitive function stratified by gender was analyzed using a general linear regression model. The model for estimating the effect of social network properties is

Y = β 0 + β 1 x 1 + β 2 x 2 + β 3 x 3 + + β 14 x 14 + ε

where Y denotes the K-MMSE score, β0 is the intercept term, and βn is the regression coefficient of the independent variable xn (x1 = social network size; x2 = intimacy; x3 = frequency; x4 = age; x5 = household income; x6 = education; x7 = marital status; x8 = living arrangement; x9 = occupation; x10 = physical activity (MET-minutes); x11 = Beck Depression Inventory-II score; x12 = cigarette use; x13 = alcohol consumption; x14 = study setting). Further, we standardized the estimates to compare the strength of the associations among social network properties. The confounders were selected by reviewing prior studies (8, 33, 34). Model 1 was adjusted for age and model 2 was additionally adjusted for socioeconomic factors. The final model was additionally adjusted for lifestyle factors, depressive symptoms and study settings. As the economic condition can act as an effect modifier, we also stratified the income level into tertiles and conducted the analysis using the same models. As a sensitivity analysis, logistic analysis was performed with K-MMSE score of over 24 as the outcome. All statistical analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, United States).

Results

Characteristics of Study Population

The baseline characteristics of the study population are described in Table 1. We classified individuals into “low cognitive function (case)” for those who had K-MMSE 24 or under; the remaining participants were categorized as “reference” in each gender. “Frequency” in both genders are between 1 and 2.5 which seems low, but this result could be due to the exclusion of contact by phone or text messages. In men, there was no significant age difference between the case and reference groups. In women, the cases were slightly older (59.0 y ± 6.17) than the references (58.33 y ± 5.88). In both men and women, there were higher proportions of low-income and low-education levels (6 years and under) in the case than in the reference group. There were no significant differences in marital status among men. However, in women, the reference showed a higher proportion of married women than the case, while the proportion of bereaved individuals was higher in the case. The proportion of physical activity and depression was higher in the case than in the reference in both men and women. When comparing the characteristics between men and women, there were several noticeable differences. The proportion of the case group was greater in women (22.2%) than in men (14.7%). Also, women were more likely to have lower educational levels, be widowed, live alone, and have a job than men.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. General characteristic of the cardiovascular and metabolic diseases etiology research cohort (CMERC) participants (N = 7,704).

Association Between Social Network Properties and Cognitive Function Stratified by Gender

The gender-stratified associations of social network properties and cognitive function are shown in Table 2. In both men and women, larger social network status was associated with higher cognitive function. In men, frequency showed the greatest association with cognitive function among three social network properties. In the final model, as one standard deviation of frequency increased, the K-MMSE score increased by 0.093 (standardized β = 0.093, standard error [SE] = 0.050, p-value < 0.0001); one standard deviation of size increased the K-MMSE score by 0.058 (standardized β = 0.058, SE = 0.037, p-value = 0.0181), and one standard deviation of intimacy increased it by 0.047 (standardized β = 0.047, SE = 0.074, p-value = 0.0122). In women, among the three properties, size was mostly associated with cognitive function. In the final model, one standard deviation of size increased the K-MMSE score by 0.055 (standardized ß = 0.055, SE = 0.028, p-value = 0.0010); one standard deviation of frequency increased it by 0.051 (standardized β = 0.051, SE = 0.034, p-value = 0.0013), and one standard deviation of intimacy increased it by 0.032 (standardized ß = 0.032, SE = 0.063, p-value = 0.0305). Bonferroni correction was applied for multiple comparisons of the social network properties (significance P-values < 0.0167).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Associations between social network properties and K-MMSE score in cardiovascular and metabolic diseases etiology research cohort (CMERC) participants.

The odds ratios (ORs) for K-MMSE score of over 24 are shown in Supplementary Table 2. In men, as one unit of frequency increases, the OR for K-MMSE score of over 24 was 1.213 (95% CI = 1.062–1.384). In women, as one unit of network size increases, the OR for K-MMSE score of over 24 was 1.088 (95% CI = 1.027–1.154). These results correlates with our main results (Table 2) that the frequency is more significantly associated with K-MMSE in men, and size is more significantly associated with K-MMSE in women.

Association Between Social Network Properties and Cognitive Function Stratified by Income Level

The income-stratified associations between social network properties and cognitive function are shown in Table 3. First, we tested whether there were interaction effects between social networks and income. The interaction effect was found only between frequency and income (men, p-interaction = 0.0461; women, p-interaction = 0.0183). In men, middle income group showed the strongest association between frequency and cognitive function (low income, standardized β = 0.107, p-value = 0.0211; middle income, standardized β = 0.114, p-value = 0.0063; high income, standardized β = 0.070, p-value = 0.0726). In women, frequency and cognitive function had the greatest association in low income group (low income, standardized β = 0.076, p-value = 0.0039; middle income, standardized β = 0.072, p-value = 0.0116; high income, standardized β = −0.017, p-value = 0.5699). Bonferroni correction was applied for multiple comparisons of the social network properties and income level (significance P-values < 0.0056).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Associations between social network properties and K-MMSE score stratified by income level in cardiovascular and metabolic diseases etiology research cohort (CMERC) participants.

Discussion

These analyses of social network and cognitive function in the Korean elderly showed that the associations between social network size, intimacy, frequency and cognitive function vary according to gender and income level. In men, frequency was the most important factor associated with cognitive function, whereas in women, network size had the greatest association with cognitive function. These associations persisted after adjusting for potential confounders. Furthermore, there was an interaction effect of frequency and income on cognitive function. However, when stratified by income level, under Bonferroni correction, none of the income groups showed significant association between (8) frequency and cognitive function except low income group in women.

Although our results showed slight differences in specific estimates, in general, expanded social network size and higher frequency were positively associated with cognitive function. Few studies have been conducted in Korea on topics similar to ours. One study investigated which type of social activity affected cognitive decline using the KLoSA (21). Among 1,568 participants aged 65 years of age or older who participated in senior citizen clubs, who had frequent contact by phone or letters with offspring reduced cognitive decline. However, face-to-face contact with offspring showed a positive association with cognitive decline, which contradicts our results. This difference can be explained by the different study designs and demographic characteristics of participants. This study was a longitudinal study, and the association mentioned above was significant in adults aged 70 years or older. Face-to-face contact might not have a positive effect on cognitive function in the elderly and the specific individuals one meets may be a major factor influencing cognitive abilities.

The term “social network” refers to the total set of connections between all members of a particular individual, and networks can be classified into different dimensions (35). There are additional studies that have measured different social network variables. A study based on the Kungsholmen Project—a longitudinal population-based study on aging and dementia in 1987 in Sweden—included 1,203 participants who were 75 years or older without a diagnosis of dementia (16). They reported that frequent but unsatisfactory contact with children increased the incidence of dementia. They had fewer and older participants than our study. Unlike our study, they also measured the satisfaction of contact, which, therefore, could allow for investigation of the effect of both the quantitative network and qualitative network. Furthermore, some studies used an integrated index, such as the Lubben Social Network Scale, to assess the size and perceived support confidant network and claimed that a larger social network might benefit cognitive function (18, 36). Although there are several studies about the relationship between social network and cognition, more studies measuring diverse dimensions of social networks are necessary to examine the influence of different social network resources.

In terms of outcomes, various indicators were used to assess cognitive function. Some studies cited questions from the Short Portable Mental Status Questionnaire (8, 14, 37); other studies assessed cognitive function by the Moray House Test, Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status-modified, or used different tests for different sectors of cognitive ability (17, 18, 38). A study examining the association between social engagement and cognitive decline in African American adults, measured cognitive function by assessing several cognitive domains; the associations for each domain have been reported (39). Therefore, further study in Korea is needed to investigate the effect on specific sectors of cognitive function that are typically impaired in dementia.

Researchers have suggested possible mechanisms by which social networks may affect cognitive function. One possible pathway is related to stress. Stress and depression are known to affect brain function (18). Social support enhances a clear understanding of stress situations, helps to develop reasonable behaviors to deal with problems, and strengthens positive self-feelings (40). Through these processes, social support weakens the impact of stress which could affect cognition (40). Low social support is also known to be related to neuroendocrine and physiological factors of elevated stress reactivity (41). Social support may mitigate genetic and environmental vulnerabilities and strengthen resilience to stress via the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical system, noradrenergic system, and central oxytocin pathways (41). Thus, it affects neuroendocrine measures of stress, which may be linked to cognition (42). Another possible pathway is by improving healthy behaviors. By facilitating access to health care and healthy behaviors, social networks can indirectly reduce or prevent brain pathologies and other conditions related to cognition (18). Individuals with larger social networks have greater access to various resources or health-related information, which may lead to positive, healthy behaviors and prevent cognitive decline (43).

Social network size is also known to be related to social activity (33). According to a study examining the effect of social networks and activity on cognitive function, social activity participation showed a stronger association with cognition than social network resources which included social network size and frequency (44). However, the positive association of social activity attenuated as social network resources increased (44). In other words, for people who did not have close social ties, engaging in diverse social activities helped to strengthen positive correlation with cognitive recall. Thus, this finding indicates that social activities are beneficial to those with limited social network resources. A randomized-control trial in Finland reported the effect of social group intervention on cognitive function in elderly people (45). The participants participated in one of three types of group activities–writing, exercise, and art- once a week for 3 months (45). As the participants chose which social activity group to attend, they could form friendships with people of similar interests. They shared their feelings and experiences and received social support from one another. The intervention group showed more improved cognition than the control group (45). This result indicates that cognition is interrelated with social networks and activities. When the effect of productive activities (e.g., quilt, photo) and receptive activities (e.g., movies, games) on cognition was compared, episodic memory was more improved in the former group than in the latter group (46). Productive engagement can strengthen oneself and encourage continuous learning and intellectual stimulation (46). These results provide evidence that learning new things and concentrating one’s mind can be an important key to preventing cognitive decline.

Possible explanations for our finding that the degree of associations among social network properties differ based on gender and income may be found in previous studies. Evidence shows gender differences in social characteristics—men rely on their partners more exclusively for social support; women have greater social networks of friends and relatives from whom they can receive support (47). This supports our finding on the importance of frequency in men, and size in women. Another gender-stratified study in Korea reported that social network size and social activity had significant associations with cognition only in the female group (48). Although the participants were recruited in one rural area, the result that emphasizes the importance of social network size in women shows the same direction as our findings. One study that examined the effect of socioeconomic status on social networks reported that socioeconomic status—particularly education and occupation—affects network structure differentially among men and women (49). For example, men have more opportunities to form social connections in the workplace than women because women usually have lower-wage jobs in poor, isolated working conditions. The difference in socioeconomic level (e.g., income, occupation and education) between men and women may influence the composition of social networks and might lead to different effects of social networks on mental health, including cognitive function. However, it is difficult to clarify the mechanism of effect modification of income in our results because socioeconomic status is complexly correlated with each other, and gender might also play a significant role in this relationship. Therefore, further studies are needed to investigate the effect of socioeconomic status on the association between social networks and cognitive function.

Several limitations should be considered when interpreting our results. First, as the study design is cross-sectional, the result cannot be interpreted as a causal relationship, and the possibility of reverse causality should not be overlooked. Social network properties and K-MMSE score were measured in the same survey at the baseline of the cohort. Thus, there may be a possibility of changes in social networks as a consequence of cognitive decline. A longitudinal study is required to examine the effects of social network properties on cognitive function. Second, it is hard to say that our results are generalizable to the entire population since the participants were recruited from relatively urban areas. Also, the fact that social network properties can be affected by urbanicity might have influenced our results (50). A nationwide study of social networks could help investigate the effect of social networks and its association with cognitive function. Third, the MMSE only reflects cognitive status, not the diagnosis or incidence of dementia. Nor the past diagnosis of dementia could be known in our data source. Moreover, we could not determine the association with specific cognitive domains such as orientation, memory, concentration, and visuospatial construction. Fourth, social network properties were measured by individuals’ self-reported data, which can involve a risk of recall bias, and only egocentric network was measured. Also, a possibility of unmeasured close relationships exists. Since the network size was measured to be up to six people, social relationships with people beyond the network members were not included. However, since only 1.39% of our participants answered they had more than 6 network members, the potential for unmeasured close relationships might be small. Frequency was measured for only face-to-face meetings, so it did not reflect the case of phone calls or text messages. Finally, there might be an influence of unmeasured confounders due to the lack of data sources.

Despite these limitations, our study is meaningful and has several strengths. First, we considered a sufficient number of middle-aged and elderly participants. Unlike most studies on cognitive function and dementia, whose participants were 65 years or older, we included individuals aged 50 years and older. Second, we compared the associations by gender. Previous studies have suggested gender differences in the perception and relation of social networks and the influence of social ties on cognition. They reported that engagement with friends and social participation showed a protective effect on cognitive decline and mental health in women but not in men (14, 51). In another study, women had more close persons and reported greater satisfaction with their relationships than men, and men answered they receive higher levels of support from the closest person in their relationship (52). Our results support those previous studies that the relationship between social network and cognitive function have different characteristics according to gender, and offer a new perspective on this association. Third, we investigated the effect modification of income level on the association between social networks and cognitive function. As income is one of the most important factors related to cognitive function (53, 54), our results can serve as a basis for future research.

Conclusion

Our study showed the association between three social network properties and cognitive function among middle-aged and elderly Koreans, which differed based on gender and income level. This study indicated the importance of social network size in women and the frequency of contact among men. It would be beneficial to encourage social relationships in older populations by supporting a variety of activities in community centers or establishing a space where the elderly can interact with each other. The individuals will be able to choose the group they are interested in, and maintain a stronger relationship with people with the same interests. Also, as our findings reported that frequency is important in men and network size is important in women, sufficient participants should be recruited and social meetings should be held frequently. Further studies are needed to investigate the effect of other social properties on cognitive function, and the causal relationship between them.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Yonsei University Health System, Seoul, Korea (4-2013-0661). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JK: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, data curation, writing—original draft, writing—review, and editing. JSY: data curation, writing—review, and editing. YY, DJK, and HCK: writing—review and editing. SJJ: supervision, conceptualization, data curation, writing—original draft, writing—review, and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the Basic Science Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) funded by the Ministry of Science and Information and Communication Technology (2020R1C1C1003502) awarded to SJJ.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank the Basic Science Research Program of the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) which was funded by the Ministry of Science and Information and Communication Technology (2020R1C1C1003502).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.893290/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, Ames D, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. (2017) 390:2673–734.

Google Scholar

2. Prince MJ, Wimo A, Guerchet MM, Ali GC, Wu Y-T, Prina M. World Alzheimer Report 2015-The Global Impact of Dementia: An Analysis of Prevalence, Incidence, Cost and Trends. London: Alzheimer’s Disease International (2015).

Google Scholar

3. Alzheimer’s Disease Internationa. World Alzheimer Report 2019: Attitudes to Dementia. London: Alzheimer’s Disease International (2019).

Google Scholar

4. Kim YJ, Han JW, So YS, Seo JY, Kim KY, Kim KW. Prevalence and trends of dementia in Korea: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Korean Med Sci. (2014) 29:903–12. doi: 10.3346/jkms.2014.29.7.903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Jang J-W, Park JH, Kim S, Lee S-H, Lee S-H, Kim Y-J. Prevalence and incidence of dementia in South Korea: a Nationwide Analysis of the National Health Insurance Service Senior Cohort. J Clin Neurol. (2021) 17:249. doi: 10.3988/jcn.2021.17.2.249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Moon S, Park HJ, Sohn M. The impact of long-term care service on total lifetime medical expenditure among older adults with dementia. Soc Sci Med. (2021) 280:114072. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Wimo A, Guerchet M, Ali GC, Wu YT, Prina AM, Winblad B, et al. The worldwide costs of dementia 2015 and comparisons with 2010. Alzheimers Dement. (2017) 13:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.07.150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Glei DA, Landau DA, Goldman N, Chuang YL, Rodriguez G, Weinstein M. Participating in social activities helps preserve cognitive function: an analysis of a longitudinal, population-based study of the elderly. Int J Epidemiol. (2005) 34:864–71. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyi049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kuiper JS, Zuidersma M, Zuidema SU, Burgerhof JG, Stolk RP, Oude Voshaar RC, et al. Social relationships and cognitive decline: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Int J Epidemiol. (2016) 45:1169–206.

Google Scholar

10. Kelly ME, Duff H, Kelly S, McHugh Power JE, Brennan S, Lawlor BA, et al. The impact of social activities, social networks, social support and social relationships on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review. Syst Rev. (2017) 6:259. doi: 10.1186/s13643-017-0632-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Stoykova R, Matharan F, Dartigues J-F, Amieva H. Impact of social network on cognitive performances and age-related cognitive decline across a 20-year follow-up. Int Psychogeriatr. (2011) 23:1405–12. doi: 10.1017/S1041610211001165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Nie Y, Richards M, Kubinova R, Titarenko A, Malyutina S, Kozela M, et al. Social networks and cognitive function in older adults: findings from the HAPIEE study. BMC Geriatr. (2021) 21:570. doi: 10.1186/s12877-021-02531-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Cohn-Schwartz E, Levinsky M, Litwin H. Social network type and subsequent cognitive health among older Europeans. Int Psychogeriatr. (2021) 33:495–504. doi: 10.1017/S1041610220003439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Zunzunegui M-V, Alvarado BE, Del Ser T, Otero A. Social networks, social integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in community-dwelling Spanish older adults. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2003) 58:S93–100. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.2.s93

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Seeman TE, Lusignolo TM, Albert M, Berkman L. Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: macArthur studies of successful aging. Health Psychol. (2001) 20:243–55. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.20.4.243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Fratiglioni L, Wang H-X, Ericsson K, Maytan M, Winblad B. Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: a community-based longitudinal study. Lancet. (2000) 355:1315–9. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02113-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Gow AJ, Corley J, Starr JM, Deary IJ. Which social network or support factors are associated with cognitive abilities in old age? Gerontology. (2013) 59:454–63. doi: 10.1159/000351265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Crooks VC, Lubben J, Petitti DB, Little D, Chiu V. Social network, cognitive function, and dementia incidence among elderly women. Am J Public Health. (2008) 98:1221–7. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2007.115923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Siette J, Georgiou A, Brayne C, Westbrook JI. Social networks and cognitive function in older adults receiving home-and community-based aged care. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. (2020) 89:104083. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2020.104083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Kim YB, Lee SH. Social network types and cognitive decline among older Korean adults: a longitudinal population-based study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2019) 34:1845–54. doi: 10.1002/gps.5200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Lee SH, Kim YB. Which type of social activities may reduce cognitive decline in the elderly?: a longitudinal population-based study. BMC Geriatr. (2016) 16:165. doi: 10.1186/s12877-016-0343-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hwang J, Park S, Kim S. Effects of participation in social activities on cognitive function among middle-aged and older adults in Korea. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2018) 15:2315. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15102315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Shim JS, Song BM, Lee JH, Lee SW, Park JH, Choi DP, et al. Cohort Profile: the cardiovascular and metabolic diseases etiology research center cohort in Korea. Yonsei Med J. (2019) 60:804–10. doi: 10.3349/ymj.2019.60.8.804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kim K, Jung SJ, Baek JM, Yim HW, Jeong H, Kim DJ, et al. Associations between social network properties and metabolic syndrome and the mediating effect of physical activity: findings from the Cardiovascular and Metabolic Diseases Etiology Research Center (CMERC) Cohort. BMJ Open Diabetes Res Care. (2020) 8:e001272. doi: 10.1136/bmjdrc-2020-001272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Cornwell B, Schumm LP, Laumann EO, Graber J. Social Networks in the NSHAP Study: rationale, Measurement, and Preliminary Findings. J Gerontol B. (2009) 64B:i47–55. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Youm Y, Laumann EO, Ferraro KF, Waite LJ, Kim HC, Park Y-R, et al. Social network properties and self-rated health in later life: comparisons from the Korean social life, health, and aging project and the national social life, health and aging project. BMC Geriatr. (2014) 14:102. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-14-102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Helkala EL, Kivipelto M, Hallikainen M, Alhainen K, Heinonen H, Tuomilehto J, et al. Usefulness of repeated presentation of Mini-Mental State Examination as a diagnostic procedure–a population-based study. Acta Neurol Scand. (2002) 106:341–6. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0404.2002.01315.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Jeong SK, Cho KH, Kim JM. The usefulness of the Korean version of modified Mini-Mental State Examination (K-mMMSE) for dementia screening in community dwelling elderly people. BMC Public Health. (2004) 4:31. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-4-31

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Chun MY. Validity and reliability of Korean version of international physical activity questionnaire short form in the elderly. Korean J Fam Med. (2012) 33:144. doi: 10.4082/kjfm.2012.33.3.144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Cho SMJ, Lee H, Shim JS, Youm Y, Jung SJ, Kim DJ, et al. Association between social network structure and physical activity in middle-aged Korean adults. Soc Sci Med. (2021) 282:114112. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Lim S-Y, Lee E-J, Jeong S-W, Kim H-C, Jeong C-H, Jeon T-Y, et al. The validation study of Beck Depression Scale 2 in Korean version. Anxiety Mood. (2011) 7:48–53.

Google Scholar

32. Shim J-S, Song BM, Lee JH, Lee SW, Park JH, Choi DP, et al. Cardiovascular and Metabolic Diseases Etiology Research Center (CMERC) cohort: study protocol and results of the first 3 years of enrollment. Epidemiol Health. (2017) 39:e2017016. doi: 10.4178/epih.e2017016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Tang Y, Arnold SE, Wilson RS. The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer’s disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Neurol. (2006) 5:406–12. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70417-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Litwin H, Shaul A. The effect of social network on the physical activity-cognitive function nexus in late life. Int Psychogeriatr. (2019) 31:713–22. doi: 10.1017/S1041610218001059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Mitchell RE, Trickett EJ. Task force report: social networks as mediators of social support. Commun Ment Health J. (1980) 16:27–44. doi: 10.1007/BF00780665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Evans IE, Llewellyn DJ, Matthews FE, Woods RT, Brayne C, Clare L, et al. Social isolation, cognitive reserve, and cognition in healthy older people. PLoS One. (2018) 13:e0201008. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0201008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Li CL, Hsu HC. Cognitive function and associated factors among older people in Taiwan: age and sex differences. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. (2015) 60:196–200. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2014.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Sorman DE, Ronnlund M, Sundstrom A, Norberg M, Nilsson LG. Social network size and cognitive functioning in middle-aged adults: cross-sectional and longitudinal associations. J Adult Dev. (2017) 24:77–88. doi: 10.1007/s10804-016-9248-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Pugh E, De Vito A, Divers R, Robinson A, Weitzner DS, Calamia M. Social factors that predict cognitive decline in older African American adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2021) 36:403–10. doi: 10.1002/gps.5435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Krause N. Social support, stress, and well-being among older adults. J Gerontol. (1986) 41:512–9. doi: 10.1093/geronj/41.4.512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Ozbay F, Johnson DC, Dimoulas E, Morgan IIIC, Charney D, Southwick S. Social support and resilience to stress: from neurobiology to clinical practice. Psychiatry. (2007) 4:35.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

42. Dori D, Casale G, Solerte SB, Fioravanti M, Migliorati G, Cuzzoni G, et al. Chrono-neuroendocrinological aspects of physiological aging and senile dementia. Chronobiologia. (1994) 21:121–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

43. Cohen S. Social relationships and health. Am Psychol. (2004) 59:676. doi: 10.1136/sextrans-2018-053935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Litwin H, Stoeckel KJ. Social network, activity participation, and cognition: a complex relationship. Res Aging. (2016) 38:76–97. doi: 10.1177/0164027515581422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Pitkala KH, Routasalo P, Kautiainen H, Sintonen H, Tilvis RS. Effects of socially stimulating group intervention on lonely, older people’s cognition: a randomized, controlled trial. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2011) 19:654–63. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181f7d8b0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Park DC, Lodi-Smith J, Drew L, Haber S, Hebrank A, Bischof GN, et al. The impact of sustained engagement on cognitive function in older adults: the synapse project. Psychol Sci. (2014) 25:103–12. doi: 10.1177/0956797613499592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Najar J, Aakre JA, Vassilaki M, Wetterberg H, Rydén L, Zettergren A, et al. Sex difference in the relation between marital status and dementia risk in two population-based cohorts. J Alzheimers Dis. (2021) 83:1269–79. doi: 10.3233/JAD-210246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Lee S, Lee S, Lee E, Youm Y, Cho HS, Kim WJ. Gender differences in social network of cognitive function among community-dwelling older adults. Geriatr Gerontol Int. (2020) 20:467–73. doi: 10.1111/ggi.13906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Ajrouch KJ, Blandon AY, Antonucci TC. Social networks among men and women: the effects of age and socioeconomic status. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2005) 60:S311–7. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.6.s311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Kim JM, Stewart R, Shin IS, Yoon JS, Lee HY. Lifetime urban/rural residence, social support and late-life depression in Korea. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2004) 19:843–51. doi: 10.1002/gps.1175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Takagi D, Kondo K, Kawachi I. Social participation and mental health: moderating effects of gender, social role and rurality. BMC Public Health. (2013) 13:701. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Fuhrer R, Stansfeld SA. How gender affects patterns of social relations and their impact on health: a comparison of one or multiple sources of support from “close persons”. Soc Sci Med. (2002) 54:811–25. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(01)00111-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Mani A, Mullainathan S, Shafir E, Zhao J. Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science. (2013) 341:976–80.

Google Scholar

54. Lee S, Buring JE, Cook NR, Grodstein F. The relation of education and income to cognitive function among professional women. Neuroepidemiology. (2006) 26:93–101. doi: 10.1159/000090254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social network size, social network intimacy, frequency of meeting, cognitive function, public health

Citation: Kim J, Yang JS, Youm Y, Kim DJ, Kim HC and Jung SJ (2022) Association Between Social Network and Cognitive Function: A Cross-Sectional Assessment From the Cardiovascular and Metabolic Diseases Etiology Research Center Cohort (2013–2018). Front. Psychiatry 13:893290. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.893290

Received: 10 March 2022; Accepted: 18 May 2022;
Published: 06 June 2022.

Edited by:

Christos Theleritis, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Greece

Reviewed by:

Haewon Byeon, Inje University, South Korea
Kevin Mccarroll, St. James’s Hospital, Ireland

Copyright © 2022 Kim, Yang, Youm, Kim, Kim and Jung. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sun Jae Jung, SUNJAEJUNG@yuhs.ac

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.