Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychiatry, 23 March 2023
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Causes and Consequences of Solitude in Children and Adolescents View all 10 articles

Positive classroom climate buffers against increases in loneliness arising from shyness, rejection sensitivity and emotional reactivity

  • 1Institute of Psychology, Mykolas Romeris University, Vilnius, Lithuania
  • 2Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Fort Lauderdale, FL, United States

Loneliness is detrimental to well-being, particularly during the transition into and early years of adolescence when peer relations are ascendant. Shy and emotionally sensitive youth, who often spend considerable time alone, have known vulnerabilities to loneliness. Studies of young children suggest that a supportive classroom context may mitigate adjustment risks, reducing victimization and improving a sense of belonging. Herein we extend this work to older students, testing the hypothesis that a positive classroom climate protects temperamentally vulnerable children (i.e., those who are shy, emotionally reactive, or sensitive to rejection) from escalating levels of loneliness across the course of a school year. A community sample of 540 (277 boys, 263 girls) Lithuanian students in grades 5–7 (10–14 years old) completed identical surveys twice, 4–5 months apart. Self-reports assessed shyness, emotional reactivity, and rejection sensitivity, as well as perceived positive classroom climate and loneliness. Path analyses indicated that longitudinal associations from shyness, emotional reactivity, and rejection sensitivity to increased loneliness were mitigated by positive classroom climate. In each case, temperamental vulnerability anticipated greater loneliness for youth reporting low but not high positive classroom climate. The results held after accounting for several potential confounding variables. The findings have practical implications, suggesting that scholars and practitioners redouble efforts to improve classroom support, particularly for temperamentally vulnerable children who are at elevated risk for solitude, loneliness, and attendant mental health challenges.

1. Introduction

Youth are at special risk for loneliness during late childhood and early adolescence, when peer relations are ascendent (1) The debilitating nature of loneliness cannot be overstated: Lonely children and adolescents present a host of short- and long-term mental health problems (2). Not everyone is equally disposed to being lonely. Shy, sensitive to rejection, and emotionally reactive youth have temperamental vulnerabilities that place them at heightened risk for loneliness (3–5). Alert to these risks, investigators have focused on factors that might mitigate loneliness in vulnerable youth. Chief among them is classroom climate, which is known to buffer against adjustment difficulties among children with peer difficulties (6). Using a community sample of Lithuanian youth ages 10 to14, we test the hypotheses that perceived positive classroom climate moderates longitudinal associations from temperamental vulnerabilities (i.e., shyness, emotional reactivity, and rejection sensitivity) to heightened loneliness.

Loneliness is a painful state of unwanted social isolation, often occurring in response to perceived relationship deficits (7, 8). Lonely children report higher rates of solitude and preference for solitude than their nonlonely counterparts (9), not because they enjoy being alone but rather typically to avoid social discomfort (10). Developmental changes conspire to make the transition into adolescence a period of heightened loneliness (1). By some estimates, up to 30% of youth report regularly feeling lonely or very lonely (11). Buffeted by uncertainties about identity, improved perspective taking abilities, and expectations for autonomy, opportunities for social isolation multiply as the social world is transformed from one constructed by adults to one dominated by peers. Lonely youth suffer, particularly from victimization and depression (12, 13).

Some youth have temperamental vulnerabilities that heighten their risk for loneliness. We focus here on children who present elevated levels of shyness, rejection sensitivity, or emotional reactivity, three risk factors with biological origins. Shyness is characterized by conflicting motivations to engage and avoid peers; interactions with agemates are appealing but concerns about negative evaluations can prompt distress and a desire to withdraw (14). Rejection sensitivity is a heightened tendency to “anxiously expect, readily perceive, and overreact” to social rejection (15). Emotional reactivity describes a heightened (often negative) emotional response to affective situations (16). Grounded in temperament, each manifests early in life and is relatively stable across development (17–19). Although modestly correlated [rs range from.300 to.450 (20–22)], the constructs are conceptually and empirically distinct. Shyness stems from a fear of novelty (17), rejection sensitivity has biopsychosocial origins associated with sensitive interpretation of ambiguous early social experiences (23), and emotional reactivity reflects a low emotional threshold for external stimuli (19).

Shy, reactive, and sensitive to rejection youth are assumed to be prone to loneliness because of two underlying mechanisms: An inclination to interpret social situations negatively and social inhibitions that interfere with the creation and maintenance of social ties (3, 24, 25). Solitude may be preferred to the uncertainties and potential pain of social engagement (26, 27), which heightens risks for loneliness (28). Many temperamentally vulnerable youth possess off-putting characteristics, a problem exacerbated by cascading social skills deficits brought on by minimal peer contact (29). A self-fulfilling prophecy unfolds whereby hypervigilance prompts negative interpretations or inaccurate interpretations of social cues and inappropriate emotional reactions to social situations, alienating peers who avoid them as unattractive interaction partners, forcing isolation and fostering loneliness. (30). Finally, temperamentally vulnerable children may find themselves with few friend options aside from other interpersonally challenged agemates, who may be equally unsatisfying partners (31, 32).

Consistent with the above, research indicates that temperamentally vulnerable children are at heightened risk for loneliness. The evidence for shyness is particularly compelling. Shy youth report feeling lonely more frequently than those who are not shy (5). Rejection sensitivity has also been linked to longitudinal increases in adolescent loneliness (4). Less is known about emotional reactivity. Longitudinal links between parent-reported negative reactivity and related constructs have been established. Specifically, among adolescents, emotional intelligence has been linked with loneliness (33) and emotional reactivity has been linked to emotional problems (34).

Our study starts from the premise that perceived support from classmates can help protect temperamentally vulnerable children against loneliness. Research based on social information processing mechanisms [predisposed emotional responses to social cues based on past experiences which impact the interpretation of the situation and the following behaviors] suggests that shy youth are less prone to interpret social situations in a self-defeating manner when interacting with supportive friends (35). Perceiving the classroom as supportive is the postulated mechanism through which the risk of loneliness is reduced. In supportive classes, vulnerable children may interpret social situations as pleasant, lowering the risk of loneliness as children reframe social experiences (36). Additionally, supportive classrooms have higher group cohesion and mutual respect, avoiding situations that elicit mismatched emotional responses that can lead to social exclusion (37). As a consequence, temperamentally vulnerable youth may feel comfortable engaging in interactions that lead to meaningful social connections (38). Additional social opportunities, in turn, help youth improve social skills in ways that diminish the tendency to withdraw (39).

Our study is novel in that we focus on perceptions of classroom climate as an index of support. Positive classroom climate is defined as the perceived tenor of the classroom and the degree to which students feel comfortable and at ease in the classroom and with classmates (40). Perceived classroom climate has been shown to moderate concurrent association between other biologically-linked traits (e.g., effortful control) and depressive symptoms (41). Longitudinal data from college students (42) and concurrent data from young adolescents (3) agree that friend support protects youth against loneliness arising from peer difficulties. Supportive classrooms should operate in a similar manner. For instance, two studies of indicate that school and classroom climate buffered against loneliness among victimized children and young adolescents (43, 44). Similarly, a 3-year study of primary school students indicated that anxious withdrawn children were less likely to be excluded in supportive classrooms than in unsupportive classrooms (37).

The present study utilizes a community sample of Lithuanian primary and middle school students to examine whether perceived positive classroom climate moderates longitudinal associations from shyness, emotional regulation, and rejection sensitivity to increases in loneliness across a four-month period during a single school year. We hypothesized that shyness, rejection sensitivity and emotional reactivity would predict increases in loneliness for youth who perceived low but not high levels of classroom support. Given that temperamental vulnerabilities may have different social outcomes depending on gender (e.g., shy girls face less rejection than shy boys) (45), we compared boys and girls on patterns of association. Because loneliness is associated with emotional problems (34) and the number and quality of friendships (41), each was included as a covariate in supplemental analyses. Victimization has been linked to perceived classroom climate (46) as well as shyness (47), emotional reactivity (48), and rejection sensitivity (49), so peer reports of relational and physical victimization were also included as potential confounders.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants included 540 students in 5th (97 boys, 89 girls; Mage = 10.85, SDage = 0.410), 6th (84 boys, 81 girls; Mage = 11.83, SDage = 0.437), and 7th (96 boys, 93 girls; Mage = 12.73, SDage = 0.457) grades. Nearly all participants were of Lithuanian ethnicity. Most lived with two biological parents (69.3%); the remainder lived in blended (13.7%) or single parent (15.6%) households, or with guardians or grandparents (1.5%). Approximately 9.5% received free meals at school.

2.2. Procedure

All 5-7th graders (attending 33 classrooms in 4 middle schools) in the community were invited to participate. Written parent consent and student assent were required for participation. Trained research assistants administered questionnaires in classes on computer tablets in September 2021 and February 2022. The study was approved by the university ethics committee (Nr. 6/202).

The initial participation rate was 65.2%. Of the 540 students who participated at Time 1, 525 also participated at Time 2. There were no differences in any study or demographic variables between students who did and did not participate at both time points. Item-level missingness ranged from 3.3–22.6% (M = 11.7%, SD = 6.0). Little’s MCAR test indicated that data were missing completely at random, χ2(16,101) = 15,689.330, p = 0.990. Item-level missing data were imputed with an EM algorithm with 25 iterations. Missing wave-level data were handled with FIML.

2.3. Measures

Participants completed the same surveys at both time points. Unless otherwise indicated, items were rated on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (Strongly agree). Scores were averaged. Higher scores indicated greater levels of a variable. Internal reliabilities are presented in Table 1. All items for each variable are listed in Supplementary Table S1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Interclass correlations, means, and standard deviations.

2.3.1. Shyness

Participants completed a 3-item shyness scale from the Motivations for Withdrawal Questionnaire (14) (e.g., “I am shy”).

2.3.2. Perceived positive classroom climate

Participants completed a 3-item positive classroom climate scale adapted from the Peer Context Questionnaire (40) (e.g., “In this class, I feel comfortable”).

2.3.3. Emotional reactivity

Participants completed a 5-item adapted version of emotional reactivity scale (50) (e.g., “My feelings get hurt easily”).

2.3.4. Rejection sensitivity

Participants completed an abbreviated 6-item rejection sensitivity scale adapted from the Rejection Sensitivity Questionnaire (51) (e.g., “How nervous would you feel about whether anyone will choose you?”). Items were rated from 1 (not worried at all) to 5 (very worried). For each of 3 hypothetical social situations, responses to 2 questions were multiplied, then averaged.

2.3.5. Loneliness

Participants completed a 3-item adapted version of loneliness scale (52) (e.g., “I feel alone at school”).

2.3.6. Potential confounding variables

To isolate effects to the main study variables, supplemental analyses were conducted that included, as Time 1 covariates and Time 2 predictors, variables known to correlate with loneliness, shyness, emotional reactivity, and/or rejection sensitivity. Emotional problems, previously linked to shyness and loneliness (53, 54), were measured with 6 items from the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (55) (e.g., “I worry a lot”). Additionally, participants completed a peer assessment questionnaire consisting of a roster on which they identified classmates who best fit a description (56). Unlimited same and other sex nominations were permitted. Nominations received were summed and standardized within classes (57). Two measures of peer liking were included, previously linked to rejection sensitivity (58): (a) rejection (“someone you do not like to spend time with”) and (b) acceptance (“someone you like to spend time with”). Two measures of peer victimization were included, previously linked to loneliness (59). (c) relational victimization (“Someone who is called names or teased by others”) and (d) physical victimization (“someone who is hit or pushed by others”). Finally, the quantity and quality of friendships were assessed, previously linked to loneliness (6) and rejection sensitivity (60). Participants identified up to 5 friends, from which the number of reciprocated friendships (M = 1.90, SD = 1.52) was determined. For the first and second best friends, each participant completed an abbreviated version of the Network of Relationships Inventory (61), with 5 items describing friendship social support (e.g., “My friend and I help each other out”) and 4 items describing friendship negativity (e.g., “My friend and I argue with each other”). Scores for the two best friends were averaged.

2.4. Plan of analysis

Analyses tested the hypothesis that perceived positive classroom climate moderates longitudinal associations from shyness, emotional reactivity, and rejection sensitivity to changes in adolescent loneliness. A two-step procedure for estimating moderated paths was conducted in Mplus 8.4. Figure 1 illustrates the analytic model. The model is akin to a residual change model, such that autoregressive effects represent the stability of a variable. By accounting for stability and within time correlations, cross-lagged paths predict residual change. The COMPLEX function was applied to address potential classroom-level differences; the same pattern of statistically significant results emerged without it, implying minimal variation across classes (56). Intraclass correlations between the main variables, calculated within classrooms, ranged from 0.004 to 0.043 suggesting that classroom nesting accounted for little to no variability (62). Standard model fit indices were applied (63). The chi-squared index should be nonsignificant; the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) should be 0.06 or lower; the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) should be greater than 0.95 (64).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Longitudinal associations from shyness, rejection sensitivity, and emotional reactivity to loneliness: Direct and moderated analytic models. Gray lines represent nonsignificant paths that were trimmed from the final models. Solid lines represent paths that were included in Model 0 and Model 1; the dashed line represents one of three moderator variable paths that were separately included in Models 1A, 1B, and 1C. Concurrent correlations (not depicted) are given in Table 1.

In the first step, a model (Model 0) without any interaction terms was estimated. Model 0 was trimmed by removing nonsignificant cross-lagged paths, provided that doing so did not worsen model fit (64).

In the second step, interaction terms (all measured at Time 1) were added to the model (Model 1). Three moderated models were tested in separate analyses: (1) Shyness predicting changes in loneliness moderated by perceived positive classroom climate (Model 1A); (2) Emotional reactivity predicting changes in loneliness moderated by perceived positive classroom climate (Model 1B); and (3) Rejection sensitivity predicting changes in loneliness moderated by perceived positive classroom climate (Model 1C).

In all models, grade in school was included as a Time 1 covariate to account for mean level differences (see below); the same pattern of statistically significant results emerged when grade was omitted.

Follow-up simple slope analyses probed statistically significant moderated associations at high (1 SD above the mean) and low (1 SD below the mean) levels of the moderator. The procedure estimates slopes at given levels of the predictor, utilizing the entire sample (65).

3. Results

3.1. Preliminary analysis

Table 1 presents interclass correlations (Pearson’s r). At both time points, shyness, rejection sensitivity, emotional reactivity and loneliness were positively correlated with each other and negatively correlated with perceived positive classroom climate.

Separate 2 (gender) × 3 (grade) ANOVAs were conducted for each study variable, with time as a repeated measure. Statistically significant (p < 0.05) main effects of gender emerged for shyness [F (1)=21.055, d = 0.402], emotional reactivity [F (1)=71.757, d = 0.742], rejection sensitivity [F (1)=27.160, d = 0.454], and loneliness [F (1)=18.104, d = 0.375]. In each case, girls scored higher than boys. Statistically significant gender [F (1)=12.212, d = 0.306] and time [F (1)=4.262, d = 0.179] main effects on perceived positive classroom climate were qualified by a gender x time interaction [F (1)=5.030, p = 0.025]. Follow-up t-tests revealed that boys reported decreases in perceived positive classroom climate [F (1)=10.196, d = 0.392], whereas girls did not [F (1)=0.027, d = 0.000]. There was a grade x time interaction for loneliness [F (2)=3.676, p = 0.026]. Loneliness increased among 7th graders [F (1)=4.945, d = 0.326], but not among 5th [F (1)=2.749, d = 0.246] or 6th [F (1)=0.004, d = 0.001] graders.

3.2. Longitudinal associations from initial shyness, rejection sensitivity, and emotional reactivity to subsequent loneliness moderated by initial perceived positive classroom climate

3.2.1. Shyness, rejection sensitivity, and emotional reactivity predicting changes In loneliness

3.2.1 A trimmed version of Model 0 (without interaction terms) fit the data. Five nonsignificant paths were trimmed from the model: Time 1 perceived positive classroom comfort predicting Time 2 shyness (β = 0.029), emotional reactivity (β = −0.044) and rejection sensitivity (β = −0.042); and Time 1 emotional reactivity predicting Time 2 shyness (β = 0.060) and perceived positive classroom climate (β = −0.017). Results are presented in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Longitudinal associations from Time1 shyness, emotional reactivity, rejection sensitivity to Time 2 loneliness moderated by Time 1 perceived classroom climate: Results from path analysis.

We focus first on the longitudinal paths of interest. There were positive associations from Time 1 shyness, Time 1 rejection sensitivity and Time 1 emotional reactivity to Time 2 loneliness. There also was a negative association from Time 1 perceived positive classroom climate to Time 2 loneliness.

Several additional cross-lagged paths were statistically significant. Time 1 rejection sensitivity was negatively associated with Time 2 perceived positive classroom climate, and positively associated with Time 2 emotional reactivity and Time 2 shyness. Time 1 shyness was positively associated with Time 2 emotional reactivity and Time 2 rejection sensitivity, and negatively associated with Time 2 perceived positive classroom climate. Time 1 emotional reactivity was positively associated with Time 2 rejection sensitivity. All stability coefficients were statistically significant.

3.2.2. Shyness predicting changes in loneliness, moderated by perceived positive classroom climate

Model 1A (with the shyness x perceived positive classroom climate interaction term) fit the data. The interaction term predicted changes in loneliness from Time 1 to Time 2. Figure 2 presents results from simple slope analyses. There was a significant positive association from Time 1 shyness to Time 2 loneliness at low (−1 SD) but not high (+1 SD) levels of perceived positive classroom climate. For youth reporting low perceived positive classroom climate, higher initial shyness was associated with increased loneliness across the school year.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Time 1 Shyness (A), Emotional reactivity (B) and Rejection sensitivity (C) predicting Time 2 Loneliness at low and high levels of perceived positive classroom climate. N = 540; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.001.

3.2.3. Emotional reactivity predicting changes in loneliness, moderated by perceived positive classroom climate

Model 1B (with the emotional reactivity x perceived positive classroom climate interaction term) fit the data. The interaction term predicted changes in loneliness from Time 1 to Time 2. Figure 2 presents results from simple slope analyses. There was a significant positive association from Time 1 emotional reactivity to Time 2 loneliness at low (−1 SD) but not high (+1 SD) levels of perceived positive classroom climate. For youth reporting low perceived positive classroom climate, higher initial emotional reactivity was associated with increased loneliness across the school year.

3.2.4. Rejection sensitivity predicting changes in loneliness, moderated by perceived positive classroom climate

Model 1C (with the rejection sensitivity x perceived positive classroom climate interaction term) fit the data. The interaction term predicted changes in loneliness from Time 1 to Time 2. Figure 2 presents results from simple slope analyses. There was a significant positive association from Time 1 rejection sensitivity to Time 2 loneliness at low (−1 SD) but not high (+1 SD) levels of perceived positive classroom climate. For youth reporting low perceived positive classroom climate, higher initial rejection sensitivity was associated with increased loneliness across the school year.

3.2.5. Supplemental analyses

Additional analyses were conducted to rule out the possibility that associations were driven by unobserved variables previously linked to the predictor, moderator, and dependent variables. With one exception, the same pattern of statistically significant results emerged when potential confounding variables (i.e., gender, self-reports of emotional problems, number of reciprocated friendships, friendship social support, friendship negativity, peer reports of relational victimization and physical victimization, and peer reports of rejection and acceptance) were separately included as Time 1 covariates and as predictors of Time 2 outcomes. When emotional problems were included in the model 0, the Time 1 shyness to Time 2 loneliness path became marginally significant (β = 0.080; p = 0.085).

Multiple group contrasts identified only one gender difference in cross-lagged associations (Δχ2 = 8.92, p = 0.002): Time 1 rejection sensitivity was positively associated with Time 2 loneliness for boys (β = 0.051, p = 0.001) but not for girls (β = 0.011, p = 0.341).

4. Discussion

We followed a community sample of pre- and early adolescents over the course of a single school year to examine the mitigating role of perceived positive school climate in the development of loneliness among temperamentally vulnerable (i.e., shy, sensitive to rejection, or emotionally reactive) children. The results indicated that perceived positive classroom climate moderates longitudinal associations. In each case, temperamental vulnerabilities anticipated greater loneliness for youth reporting low but not high positive classroom climate.

The findings replicate and extend insights into the antecedents of loneliness. In terms of replication, the findings add to the long list of studies indicating that shy children are especially vulnerable to loneliness (5). Others have also reported that rejection sensitivity increases loneliness (25). Far fewer longitudinal studies have examined whether emotionally reactive children are similarly at risk (3). Taken together with results from previous research, our findings underscore the painful and potentially debilitating costs of loneliness that confront emotionally vulnerable children.

We are not the first to find that a positive classroom climate buffers against conditions that might otherwise have an adverse impact on development. Perceptions of classroom support protect against the untoward consequences of victimization (46, 66), and mitigate the effects of low effortful control on conduct problems (67) and depressive symptoms (36). As such, the findings align with social information processing theory (35), which posits that temperamentally vulnerable youth in classrooms perceived as supportive tend to interpret challenging social situations as benign and nonthreatening. Different processes may be at work depending on whether risks for loneliness have origins in overly sensitive perceptions of social situations (3) or relationship difficulties caused by unattractive traits (31, 32). Shy and sensitive youth may perceive supportive classrooms as a safe place where temperamental characteristics are not a social liability, providing confidence to build ties with classmates. Supportive classrooms are characterized by high engagement and positive peer and teacher interactions (68). Teachers and classmates may work to minimize the time the emotionally vulnerable spend alone and avoid activities that exclude or marginalize members. Finally, supportive classrooms are known to embrace prosocial norms (43), which may disrupt the self-fulfilling prophecy cycle among sensitive children or counteract incipient loneliness among youth so inclined.

Classroom climate has a downstream influence on solitude. Start from the premise that perceptions of a positive classroom climate are joined with perceptions of positive peer experiences (69). Children and adolescents who enjoy spending time with classmates may leap at opportunities to spend time together out of class, accepting and making social invitations, and enrolling in clubs and after-school activities. Additional social experiences may provide temperamentally vulnerable children with much needed social skills practice, bolstering confidence in abilities and reducing withdrawal tendencies (39, 70). Unsupportive classrooms, in contrast, may increase the likelihood that shy and emotionally sensitive children seek to be alone when out of school (37). Discouraged by interpersonal missteps and fearful of replicating painful peer experiences, temperamentally vulnerable children may learn that solitude is safer and preferable (27). In the process, children who most need the company of others instead fall further behind in social skills, developing a (sometimes well-deserved) reputation for awkwardness, which can make successful social integration more difficult in the future.

Other replicated results should bolster confidence in our novel findings. Consistent with previous reports (21, 71), shyness, emotional reactivity, and rejection sensitivity were interrelated longitudinally, such that higher levels of one begat increases in another. We also found that shy and rejection sensitive children were least likely to report that classroom climate improved over the course of the school year, recalling findings from other studies in which less empathetic children (who are lower in emotion regulation) and children with more behavioral problems perceived declining levels of school climate (72, 73).

Our study is not without limitations. First, reliance on self-report variables increases the risk of bias arising from shared-reporter variance. This problem lacks an easy solution, however, because many of the variables of interest focus on child feelings and perceptions, which are not reliably gaged by teachers or parents (74) and whose impact may vary as a function of the discrepancy between actual and ideal self-perceptions (75). Observational data could help distinguish between the impact of perceived and actual classroom climate. Second, it was not possible, with two waves of data, to conduct a random intercept model. In its absence, conclusions about within-individual cross-lagged associations must be tempered, because changes in loneliness may be a product of between-person effects (76).Person-oriented analyses may be better suited to identifying constellations of interpersonal and individual factors tied to adaptive and maladaptive outcomes (77). Third, perceived positive classroom climate is tied to the child’s perceptions of relationships with friends and teachers (78, 79). The same pattern of results emerged when we controlled for friendship quality, suggesting that classroom climate captures more than just getting along with friends. Unfortunately, we lacked data on teacher-child relationships and so cannot make conclusions about the degree to which climate is distinct from getting along with teachers. Finally, the participants lived in a small, homogeneous Northern European community. Those unfamiliar with Lithuania may be hesitant to generalize from its populace. Once involuntarily situated in the Soviet Union, Lithuania is currently a member of the European Union. Students in Lithuania resemble those in other Western European nations with regard to adolescent norms, values, and development (80). Of course, it remains to be seen whether findings from this sample generalize to other, dissimilar contexts.

The results emphasize the importance of perceptions of classroom climate for the well-being of temperamentally vulnerable youth. Future research could investigate the processes through which positive classroom climate protects vulnerable adolescents (67) or classroom level characteristics which determine it being perceived as positive by vulnerable youth (37). This research has implications for classroom oriented interventions that extend well beyond loneliness, with the potential to change the lives of many youths who might otherwise develop a snowballing cascade of interpersonal and mental health challenges.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Mykolas Romeris University, Institute of Psychology, Committee of Psychological Research Ethics. DECISION Nr. 6/−202. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

This project has received funding from European Social Fund (project no 09.3.3-LMT-K-712-17-0009) under grant agreement with the Research Council of Lithuania (LMTLT).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1081989/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Laursen, B, and Hartl, AC. Understanding loneliness during adolescence: developmental changes that increase the risk of perceived social isolation. J Adolesc. (2013) 36:1261–8. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Loades, ME, Chatburn, E, Higson-Sweeney, N, Reynolds, S, Shafran, R, Brigden, A, et al. Rapid systematic review: the impact of social isolation and loneliness on the mental health of children and adolescents in the context of COVID-19. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2020) 59:1218–1239.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Qualter, P, Brown, SL, Rotenberg, KJ, Vanhalst, J, Harris, RA, Goossens, L, et al. Trajectories of loneliness during childhood and adolescence: predictors and health outcomes. J Adolesc. (2013) 36:1283–93. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Zhou, J, Li, X, Tian, L, and Huebner, ES. Longitudinal association between low self-esteem and depression in early adolescents: the role of rejection sensitivity and loneliness. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract. (2020) 93:54–71. doi: 10.1111/papt.12207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Mahon, NE, Yarcheski, A, Yarcheski, TJ, Cannella, BL, and Hanks, MM. A meta-analytic study of predictors for loneliness during adolescence. Nurs Res. (2006) 55:308–15. doi: 10.1097/00006199-200609000-00003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Clark, KN, Strissel, D, Demaray, MK, Ogg, J, Eldridge, MA, and Malecki, CK. Victimization and depressive symptoms in early adolescence: the role of perceived school climate. Sch Psychol. (2022) 37:355–66. doi: 10.1037/spq0000511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Perlman, D, and Peplau, LA. Toward a social psychology of loneliness. Pers Relat. (1981) 3:31–56.

Google Scholar

8. Kaniušonytė, G, Truskauskaitė-Kunevičienė, I, Žukauskienė, R, and Crocetti, E. Knowing who you are for not feeling lonely? A longitudinal study on identity and loneliness. Child Dev. (2019) 90:1579–88. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Goossens, L, Lasgaard, M, Luyckx, K, Vanhalst, J, Mathias, S, and Masy, E. Loneliness and solitude in adolescence: a confirmatory factor analysis of alternative models. Pers Individ Dif. (2009) 47:890–4. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.07.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Corsano, P, Grazia, V, and Molinari, L. Solitude and loneliness profiles in early adolescents: a person-centred approach. J Child Fam Stud. (2019) 28:3374–84. doi: 10.1007/s10826-019-01518-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Twenge, JM, Haidt, J, Blake, AB, McAllister, C, Lemon, H, and Le Roy, A. Worldwide increases in adolescent loneliness. J Adolesc. (2021) 93:257–9. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2021.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Acquah, EO, Topalli, PZ, Wilson, ML, Junttila, N, and Niemi, PM. Adolescent loneliness and social anxiety as predictors of bullying victimisation. Int J Adolesc Youth. (2016) 21:320–31. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2015.1083449

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Vanhalst, J, Luyckx, K, Teppers, E, and Goossens, L. Disentangling the longitudinal relation between loneliness and depressive symptoms: prospective effects and the intervening role of coping. J Soc Clin Psychol. (2012) 31:810–4. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2012.31.8.810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Barstead, MG, Smith, KA, Laursen, B, Booth-LaForce, C, King, S, and Rubin, KH. Shyness, preference for solitude, and adolescent internalizing: the roles of maternal, paternal, and best-friend support. J Res Adolesc. (2018) 28:488–4. doi: 10.1111/jora.12350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Downey, G, and Feldman, S. Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate relationships. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1996) 70:1327–43. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Silvers, JA, McRae, K, Gabrieli, JD, Gross, JJ, Remy, KA, and Ochsner, KN. Age-related differences in emotional reactivity, regulation, and rejection sensitivity in adolescence. Emotion. (2012) 12:1235–47. doi: 10.1037/a0028297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Chen, X. Culture and shyness in childhood and adolescence. New Ideas Psych. (2019) 53:58–66. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2018.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Gao, S, Assink, M, Cipriani, A, and Lin, K. Associations between rejection sensitivity and mental health outcomes: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2017) 57:59–74. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Carlo, G, Crockett, LJ, Wolff, JM, and Beal, SJ. The role of emotional reactivity, self-regulation, and puberty in adolescents' prosocial behaviors. Soc Dev. (2012) 21:667–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2012.00660.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Wang, Y, and Chung, MC. Linking rejection sensitivity, shyness and unsociability with posttraumatic stress disorder and psychiatric co-morbidity among Chinese adolescents. Psych Q. (2020) 91:309–9. doi: 10.1007/s11126-019-09701-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hassan, R, Willoughby, T, and Schmidt, LA. Development of shyness across adolescence: reactivity, regulation, or both? Dev Psychol. (2021) 57:421–31. doi: 10.1037/dev0001159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Preti, E, Casini, E, Richetin, J, De Panfilis, C, and Fontana, A. Cognitive and emotional components of rejection sensitivity: independent contributions to adolescent self-and interpersonal functioning. Assessment. (2020) 27:1230–41. doi: 10.1177/1073191118817866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Romero-Canyas, R, Downey, G, Berenson, K, Ayduk, O, and Kang, NJ. Rejection sensitivity and the rejection–hostility link in romantic relationships. J Pers. (2010) 78:119–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00611.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Zhao, J, Song, F, Chen, Q, Li, M, Wang, Y, and Kong, F. Linking shyness to loneliness in Chinese adolescents: the mediating role of core self-evaluation and social support. Pers Ind Diff. (2018) 125:140–4. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. London, B, Downey, G, Bonica, C, and Paltin, I. Social causes and consequences of rejection sensitivity. J Res Adolesc. (2007) 17:481–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2007.00531.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Wang, JM, Rubin, KH, Laursen, B, Booth-LaForce, C, and Rose-Krasnor, L. Preference-for-solitude and adjustment difficulties in early and late adolescence. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2013) 42:834–2. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.794700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Coplan, RJ, Bowker, JC, and Nelson, LJ. Alone again: revisiting psychological perspectives on solitude In: RJ Coplan, JC Bowker, and LJ Nelson, editors. The Handbook of Solitude: Psychological Perspectives on Social Isolation, Social Withdrawal, and Being Alone. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc (2021)

Google Scholar

28. Molinari, L, Grazia, V, and Corsano, P. School relations and solitude in early adolescence: a mediation model involving rejection sensitivity. J Early Adolesc. (2020) 40:426–8. doi: 10.1177/0272431619847523

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Wang, JM, Hartl, AC, Laursen, B, and Rubin, KH. The high costs of low agreeableness: low agreeableness exacerbates interpersonal consequences of rejection sensitivity in US and Chinese adolescents. J Res Personal. (2017) 67:36–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2016.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Herd, T, and Kim-Spoon, J. A systematic review of associations between adverse peer experiences and emotion regulation in adolescence. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (2021) 24:141–3. doi: 10.1007/s10567-020-00337-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Bowker, JC, Thomas, KK, Norman, KE, and Spencer, SV. Mutual best friendship involvement, best friends’ rejection sensitivity, and psychological maladaptation. J Youth Adolesc. (2011) 40:545–5. doi: 10.1007/s10964-010-9582-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Rubin, KH, Wojslawowicz, JC, Rose-Krasnor, L, Booth-LaForce, C, and Burgess, KB. The best friendships of shy/withdrawn children: prevalence, stability, and relationship quality. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2006) 34:139–3. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-9017-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Wols, A, Scholte, RH, and Qualter, P. Prospective associations between loneliness and emotional intelligence. J Adolesc. (2015) 39:40–8. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Dollar, JM, Perry, NB, Calkins, SD, Shanahan, L, Keane, SP, Shriver, L, et al. Longitudinal associations between specific types of emotional reactivity and psychological, physical health, and school adjustment. Dev Psychopathol. (2022) 17:1–5. doi: 10.1017/S0954579421001619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Burgess, KB, Wojslawowicz, JC, Rubin, KH, Rose-Krasnor, L, and Booth-LaForce, C. Social information processing and coping strategies of shy/withdrawn and aggressive children: does friendship matter? Ch Dev. (2006) 77:371–3. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00876.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Gazelle, H. Class climate moderates peer relations and emotional adjustment in children with an early history of anxious solitude: a child× environment model. Dev Psychol. (2006) 42:1179–92. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Avant, TS, Gazelle, H, and Faldowski, R. Classroom emotional climate as a moderator of anxious solitary children's longitudinal risk for peer exclusion: a child× environment model. Dev Psychol. (2011) 47:1711–27. doi: 10.1037/a0024021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Loukas, A, and Robinson, S. Examining the moderating role of perceived school climate in early adolescent adjustment. J Adolesc. (2004) 14:209–3. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2004.01402004.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Wang, MT, and Eccles, JS. Social support matters: longitudinal effects of social support on three dimensions of school engagement from middle to high school. Child Dev. (2012) 83:877–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01745.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Boor-Klip, HJ, Segers, E, Hendrickx, MM, and Cillessen, AH. Development and psychometric properties of the classroom peer context questionnaire. Soc Dev. (2016) 25:370–9. doi: 10.1111/sode.12137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Woods, S, Done, J, and Kalsi, H. Peer victimisation and internalising difficulties: the moderating role of friendship quality. J Adolesc. (2009) 32:293–8. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Tian, Y, Chen, P, Meng, W, Zhan, X, Wang, J, Wang, P, et al. Associations among shyness, interpersonal relationships, and loneliness in college freshmen: a longitudinal cross-lagged analysis. Scand J Psychol. (2019) 60:637–5. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Schacter, HL, and Juvonen, J. You've got a friend (ly school): can school prosocial norms and friends similarly protect victims from distress? Soc Dev. (2018) 27:636–1. doi: 10.1111/sode.12281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Troop-Gordon, W, Chambless, K, and Brandt, T. Peer victimization and well-being as a function of same-ethnicity classmates and classroom social norms: revisiting person× environment mismatch theory. Dev Psychol. (2021) 57:2050–66. doi: 10.1037/dev0001161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Doey, L, Coplan, RJ, and Kingsbury, M. Bashful boys and coy girls: a review of gender differences in childhood shyness. Sex Roles. (2014) 70:255–6. doi: 10.1007/s11199-013-0317-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Sullivan, K, Zhu, Q, Wang, C, and Boyanton, D. Relations among peer victimization, aggression, and school climate among elementary school students in China. School Psych Rev. (2021) 7:1–3. doi: 10.1080/2372966X.2021.1898290

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Chen, BB, and Santo, JB. The relationships between shyness and unsociability and peer difficulties: the moderating role of insecure attachment. Int J Behav Dev. (2016) 40:346–8. doi: 10.1177/0165025415587726

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Rosen, PJ, Milich, R, and Harris, MJ. Dysregulated negative emotional reactivity as a predictor of chronic peer victimization in childhood. Aggr Behav. (2012) 38:414–7. doi: 10.1002/ab.21434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Zimmer-Gembeck, MJ, Trevaskis, S, Nesdale, D, and Downey, GA. Relational victimization, loneliness and depressive symptoms: indirect associations via self and peer reports of rejection sensitivity. J Youth Adolesc. (2014) 43:568–2. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-9993-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Nock, MK, Wedig, MM, Holmberg, EB, and Hooley, JM. The emotion reactivity scale: development, evaluation, and relation to self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. Behav Ther. (2008) 39:107–6. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2007.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Downey, G, Lebolt, A, Rincón, C, and Freitas, AL. Rejection sensitivity and children's interpersonal difficulties. Child Dev. (1998) 69:1074–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06161.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Parker, JG, and Asher, SR. Friendship and friendship quality in middle childhood: links with peer group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Dev Psychol. (1993) 29:611–21. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.29.4.611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Lund, I. ‘I just sit there’: shyness as an emotional and behavioural problem in school. J Res Spec Educ Needs. (2008) 8:78–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-3802.2008.00105.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Schwartz-Mette, RA, Shankman, J, Dueweke, AR, Borowski, S, and Rose, AJ. Relations of friendship experiences with depressive symptoms and loneliness in childhood and adolescence: a meta-analytic review. Psych Bulletin. (2020) 146:664–700. doi: 10.1037/bul0000239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Goodman, R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. Child Psychol Psychiatry. (1997) 38:581–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Yoho, M, Faur, S, and Laursen, B. Conflict moderates the longitudinal association between aggression with classmates and popularity: leveraging disagreements into peer status. Pers Ind Diff. (2022) 190:111538. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2022.111538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Velásquez, AM, Bukowski, WM, and Saldarriaga, LM. Adjusting for group size effects in peer nomination data. Soc Dev. (2013) 22:845–3. doi: 10.1111/sode.12029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Wang, J, McDonald, KL, Rubin, KH, and Laursen, B. Peer rejection as a social antecedent to rejection sensitivity in youth: the role of relational valuation. Pers Ind Diff. (2012) 53:939–2. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Storch, EA, Brassard, MR, and Masia-Warner, CL. The relationship of peer victimization to social anxiety and loneliness in adolescence. Child Stud J. (2003) 33:1–9.

Google Scholar

60. McDonald, KL, Bowker, JC, Rubin, KH, Laursen, B, and Duchene, MS. Interactions between rejection sensitivity and supportive relationships in the prediction of adolescents’ internalizing difficulties. J Youth Adolesc. (2010) 39:563–4. doi: 10.1007/s10964-010-9519-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Furman, W, and Buhrmester, D. Children's perceptions of the personal relationships in their social networks. Dev Psychol. (1985) 21:1016–24. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.21.6.1016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Hedges, LV, and Hedberg, EC. Intraclass correlation values for planning group-randomized trials in education. Ed Eval Pol Anal. (2007) 29:60–87. doi: 10.3102/0162373707299706

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Hu, LT, and Bentler, PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. (1999) 6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kline, RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, Guilford publications: (2015).

Google Scholar

65. Kaniušonytė, G, and Laursen, B. Perceptions of positive parenting predict increases in resilience for low neurotic and high agreeable adolescents. Pers Ind Diff. (2022) 185:111272. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Cava, MJ, Musitu, G, Buelga, S, and Murgui, S. The relationships of family and classroom environments with peer relational victimization: an analysis of their gender differences. Span J Psychol. (2010) 13:156–5. doi: 10.1017/S1138741600003747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Loukas, A, and Murphy, JL. Middle school student perceptions of school climate: examining protective functions on subsequent adjustment problems. J Sch Psychol. (2007) 45:293–309. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2006.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Khalfaoui, A, García-Carrión, R, and Villardón-Gallego, L. A systematic review of the literature on aspects affecting positive classroom climate in multicultural early childhood education. Early Child Educ J. (2021) 49:71–81. doi: 10.1007/s10643-020-01054-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Wang, MT, Degol, JL, Amemiya, J, Parr, A, and Guo, J. Classroom climate and children’s academic and psychological wellbeing: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev Rev. (2020) 57:100912. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2020.100912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Wentzel, KA, Russell, SR, and Baker, S. Peer relationships and positive adjustment at school. In handbook of positive psychology in schools. Milton Park: Routledge (2014).

Google Scholar

71. Gardner, AA, Zimmer-Gembeck, MJ, and Modecki, K. A longitudinal model of rejection sensitivity and internalizing symptoms: testing emotion regulation deficits as a mechanism and outcome of symptoms. J Pers. (2020) 88:1045–57. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Berg, JK, and Aber, JL. A multilevel view of predictors of children’s perceptions of school interpersonal climate. J Educ Psychol. (2015) 107:1150–70. doi: 10.1037/edu0000027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Way, N, Reddy, R, and Rhodes, J. Students’ perceptions of school climate during the middle school years: associations with trajectories of psychological and behavioral adjustment. Am J Community Psychol. (2007) 40:194–3. doi: 10.1007/s10464-007-9143-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Geukens, F, Maes, M, Cillessen, AH, Colpin, H, Van Leeuwen, K, Verschueren, K, et al. Spotting loneliness at school: associations between self-reports and teacher and peer nominations. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:971. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18030971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Ferguson, GM, Hafen, CA, and Laursen, B. Adolescent psychological and academic adjustment as a function of discrepancies between actual and ideal self-perceptions. J Youth Adolesc. (2010) 39:1485–97. doi: 10.1007/s10964-009-9461-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Hamaker, EL, Kuiper, RM, and Grasman, RP. A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychol Methods. (2015) 20:102–16. doi: 10.1037/a0038889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Hiatt, C, Laursen, B, Mooney, KS, and Rubin, KH. Forms of friendship: a person-centered assessment of the quality, stability, and outcomes of different types of adolescent friends. Pers Ind Diff. (2015) 77:149–5. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Koth, CW, Bradshaw, CP, and Leaf, PJ. A multilevel study of predictors of student perceptions of school climate: the effect of classroom-level factors. J Educ Psychol. (2008) 100:96–104. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.96

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Nelson, RM, and DeBacker, TK. Achievement motivation in adolescents: the role of peer climate and best friends. J Exp Educ. (2008) 76:170–9. doi: 10.3200/JEXE.76.2.170-190

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Kaniušonytė, G, and Žukauskienė, R. Relationships with parents, identity styles, and positive youth development during the transition from adolescence to emerging adulthood. Emerg Adulthood. (2018) 6:42–52. doi: 10.1177/216769681769097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: loneliness, classroom climate, shyness, rejection sensitivity, emotional reactivity

Citation: Katulis G, Kaniušonytė G and Laursen B (2023) Positive classroom climate buffers against increases in loneliness arising from shyness, rejection sensitivity and emotional reactivity. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1081989. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1081989

Received: 27 October 2022; Accepted: 07 March 2023;
Published: 23 March 2023.

Edited by:

Louis A. Schmidt, McMaster University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Melanie Zimmer-Gembeck, Griffith University, Australia
Xuechen Ding, Shanghai Normal University, China
Junsheng Liu, East China Normal University, China

Copyright © 2023 Katulis, Kaniušonytė and Laursen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Brett Laursen, laursen@fau.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.