Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 14 September 2023
Sec. Social Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Emotions in Neuroscience: Fundamentals and New Discoveries View all 6 articles

The effect of intranasal oxytocin on social reward processing in humans: a systematic review

  • 1Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
  • 2Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, Comenius University in Bratislava, Bratislava, Slovakia
  • 3Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social Studies, Masaryk University, Brno, Czechia
  • 4Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands

Understanding the neurobiology of social reward processing is fundamental, holding promises for reducing maladaptive/dysfunctional social behaviors and boosting the benefits associated with a healthy social life. Current research shows that processing of social (vs. non-social) rewards may be driven by oxytocinergic signaling. However, studies in humans often led to mixed results. This review aimed to systematically summarize available experimental results that assessed the modulation of social reward processing by intranasal oxytocin (IN-OXY) administration in humans. The literature search yielded 385 results, of which 19 studies were included in the qualitative synthesis. The effects of IN-OXY on subjective, behavioral, and (neuro)physiological output variables are discussed in relation to moderating variables—reward phase, reward type, onset and dosage, participants’ sex/gender, and clinical condition. Results indicate that IN-OXY is mostly effective during the consumption (“liking”) of social rewards. These effects are likely exerted by modulating the activity of the prefrontal cortex, insula, precuneus, anterior cingulate cortex, amygdala, and striatum. Finally, we provide suggestions for designing future oxytocin studies.

Systematic review registration: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42021278945, identifier CRD42021278945.

1. Introduction

Rewards in general, and social rewards in particular, are salient stimuli, events, objects, or situations that induce approach and consummatory behavior (1). Over the last decades, research in animals has shown that the processing of rewarding stimuli is characterized by (at least) two main components, with partially different neurochemical regulations: (1) “wanting”, i.e., the motivation to mobilize an effort to obtain an anticipated reward, subtended by the dopaminergic system (2); and (2) “liking”, i.e., the hedonic response evoked by its consumption, subtended, among others, by the opioidergic system (3). While such neurochemical regulation has been extensively documented for primary nonsocial rewards such as food, the neurochemistry of social reward processing is less conclusive, especially in humans. For example, evidence of the involvement of the dopamine and opioids during wanting or liking of social rewards is controversial (46), suggesting that different neurochemical systems may play a bigger role in the processing of rewards of social nature, with the most promising candidate being oxytocin (710).

Oxytocin is a neuropeptide synthesized in the supraoptic and paraventricular (PVN) nuclei of the hypothalamus and is released both as a hormone to the peripheral system and as a neurotransmitter/neuromodulator into the brain (11). The oxytocin-synthesizing magnocellular neurons project to regions relevant for social behavior and reward processing (1215), including the amygdala, striatum, ventral pallidum, and the prefrontal cortex (PFC) (16, 17). Oxytocin strongly modulates sociality in rats (9), mice (18), and monkeys (10). In humans, it has been associated with social attachments, trust facilitation, social memory, and fear reduction (8), and the processing of social rewards (19, 20).

It has been suggested that wanting of social rewards is regulated by an interconnected oxytocin-dopamine pathway in the brain (16, 2123), due to animal evidence of overlapping distribution of oxytocinergic and dopaminergic receptors in both dorsal and ventral striatum, ventral pallidum, and ventral tegmental area (VTA) (24, 25). Furthermore, the oxytocin system seems to interact with the opioid system in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) and arcuate nucleus to modulate the pleasure derived by its consumption (7, 2629).

Overall, these findings indicate an important role of oxytocin in the processing of social rewards, presumably by modulating the action of dopamine and opioids. However, findings in humans have been less consistent than in other species, and recent well-powered studies reported no or negative effects of oxytocin administration on various social behaviors (3032) and, more specifically, on the processing of social rewards (3337).

Apart from the possibility of oxytocin not being involved in humans to the same extent that in other animals, the inconsistencies in the literature might be due to several reasons:

1. Reward phase: Given that wanting and liking (respectively associated with anticipation and consumption) are neurochemically partially distinct components of reward processing, oxytocin modulation can vary depending on the observed phase.

2. Type of stimuli: The definition of socially rewarding stimuli is very heterogeneous between studies, spanning from real social interactions (38, 39) to pictures of smiling faces (40, 41). Additionally, stimuli are usually not controlled for sex/gender effects, even though it has been repeatedly reported that participants might perceive an opposite-sex/gender stimulus as more rewarding (42).

3. Administration route and dosage: Due to the lower side effects compared to intravenous administration and higher efficiency in elevating cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) concentrations (43), oxytocin is mainly administered intranasally (15, 44). The commonly administered dose is 24 international units (IU), but there is little evidence of such dose being the most effective (45). For example, a study by Cardoso et al. used 24 and 48 IU in healthy individuals and found stress-attenuating effects only in the group with 24 IU, suggesting that a higher dose might overstimulate the oxytocin system resulting in no effects (46). Other authors argue for the greatest efficacy of even lower dosages of 8 IU of oxytocin to promote social functioning (45, 47, 48). These findings are broadly consistent with the hypothesized inverted U-shaped dose–response of oxytocin (37, 49). Here, lower levels of administered oxytocin (e.g., 8 or 24 IU) should theoretically move the concentrations of oxytocin in the system closer to the peak, whereas higher doses (e.g., 40 IU) can overstimulate the oxytocin system and result in no effect (50).

4. Participants’ sex/gender: lower baseline levels of endogenous oxytocin in men compared to women have been previously documented (51). Recent findings also suggest sex-dependent regulation of social rewards by oxytocin (52), with some studies observing opposite effects on brain activity during consumption of social rewards (37, 53). The oxytocin effects on women might further be biased by the usage of oral contraceptives (54). Notably, in spite of its relevance, sex/gender effects on the neurochemical regulation of social reward processing in humans, are largely unknown.

5. Onset time: Following single intranasal oxytocin administration, the time the participant has to wait, can also affect oxytocin concentrations in the system (55).

6. Clinical condition: Administration of oxytocin may have different effects on social reward processing in clinical populations and healthy controls. Research for example shows that traumatic experiences [e.g., in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) or depressive states] may affect hormonal systems resulting in reduced production and release of oxytocin (56). In such cases, oxytocin administration could result in more profound effects as compared to participants with normal endogenous levels of oxytocin.

Given the inconsistencies in the effects of oxytocin in modulating social reward processing in humans (31), it is therefore crucial to consider methodological differences. To this aim, we reviewed human empirical studies that utilized intranasal oxytocin to assess social reward processing and systematically summarized their results in relation to the aforementioned points. We also assessed the quality of each individual study, including potential problems to detect small effects due to insufficient sample size. The findings will ultimately help to evaluate the role and the effectiveness of intranasal oxytocin in regulating social reward processes and provide further suggestions for designing oxytocin studies in the field of social reward.

2. Methods

This review is based on the PRISMA guidelines (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) (57) and is publicly pre-registered in PROSPERO (International prospective register of systematic reviews). Pre-registration information is available on the following link.1 The quality of the studies, including reporting, external/internal validity, and power, was assessed with the widely accepted tool for quality assessment developed by Downs and Black (58). The quality assessment was performed by one reviewer, and the decision was checked by a second reviewer. Details on the assessment are provided in the Supplementary material.

2.1. Search strategy and exclusion criteria

Three major databases—Web of Science, Scopus, and Pub-Med, and three preprint servers—BioRxiv, MedRxiv, and PsyArXiv, were searched from September 2021 until May 2022 (see Supplementary material for the full search strategy including all the databases and preprint servers utilized). The keywords were combined into command lines for individual databases and ordered to appear in the title or abstract of the empirical papers. The same following search terms were used for all databases combined with Boolean operators AND and OR: oxytocin*, intranasal*, *social*, affilia*, reward*, wanting, incentive*, goal* pursuit, motivat* salien*, desir*, liking, hedonic impact, hedonic value, hedonic react*, pleasur*, and approach* motivat*.

The selection process of the relevant studies is depicted in the PRISMA flow diagram (Figure 1). Only randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled human studies, utilizing intranasal oxytocin and social reward tasks were included in the review. For wanting, the measurement had to occur during the anticipation phase; any behavioral or subjective measures had to reflect motivation/desire to get the reward. For liking, the measurement had to occur during the consumption phase; any behavioral or subjective measures had to reflect positive feelings derived from the reward receipt.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram illustrating the search process.

Exclusion criteria were: (a) social reward stimuli are not distinguishable from non-social rewards; (b) wanting and/or liking components are not assessed [instead, e.g., main focus on learning, i.e., predictive associations and cognitions (59)]; (c) studies including no behavioral/fMRI measures; (d) social rewards are represented by sexual stimuli, as affiliative and sexual reward may be subtended by different neurochemical systems (60, 61). Opposite gender stimuli were not generally considered as sexual rewards unless they were of sexual character or were used to measure some form of sexual arousal.

2.2. Synthesis of outputs and results

The synthesis of results follows a narrative approach. To address the main aim of the review, the relevant outputs of all included studies were first summarized into the following sections (see Table 1): (1) general information (Clinical condition, Sex/Gender, Oral contraceptives, Sample size, and Age), (2) the paradigm and the task used (Task, Task type, Social reward, and Experimental paradigm), and (3) the type of measures and outcome variables (Reward Component, Measures, Analysis, and Results). A more detailed description of each task is reported in Table 2 and brain regions of interest (ROIs) modulated by oxytocin administration in Table 3. Results are discussed based on the following classification criteria: (a) Reward phase (anticipation, consumption), (b) Type of stimuli (simple, i.e., non-interactive/unimodal, interactive), (c) Dosage, (d) Participants sex/gender (female, male); and (e) Clinical condition (clinical, healthy population).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Result of studies included in the systematic review.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Description of tasks used in the included studies.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Region by region significant activations under OXY.

3. Results

Nineteen studies were identified, based on the search strategy and exclusion criteria (see Figure 1). The identified studies encompassed together 984 participants of age ranging from 13 to 43. The sample size varied from 16 to 104. Five of the studies were conducted in China (39, 64, 66, 69, 73), and in the United States (35, 38, 65, 70, 71), three in Germany (36, 42, 63) and in Netherlands (33, 67, 72), one in Norway (68), Australia (62), and France (40). All of the studies have been published within the last decade. The overall risk of bias across all studies was assessed as mainly low (see details on the Quality assessment in the Supplementary material).

3.1. Reward anticipation

Eleven studies measured social reward anticipation (35, 36, 38, 39, 6267, 73), out of which eight used behavioral measures (38, 65), four studies used fMRI measurement (35, 36, 63, 67), and four used self-reported measures (39, 62, 64, 73).

Only one behavioral study (38), and two studies using self-reported measures (62, 64), found significant effect of oxytocin.

On the fMRI level, four studies measured anticipatory social reward, all of which used ROI analysis (35, 36, 63, 67), three used whole-brain analysis (35, 36, 67), and one measured functional connectivity (35). Only one study found a significant effect of oxytocin treatment (63), which was conducted on female participants and precisely, increased VTA activity during social reward anticipation after the treatment.

3.2. Reward consumption

Thirteen studies measured social reward consumption (33, 35, 36, 39, 40, 42, 6773), out of which only one did not use fMRI measurement (68), but used self-reported measures; four studies used both these measurement types (33, 42, 67, 73). Overall, nine out of 13 studies found significant effects of IN-OXY on social reward consumption on at least one level of measurement, and specifically an enhancement of consummatory social reward processing after IN-OXY. Three studies found also decreases in neural processing in female participants (33, 39, 72). In the following paragraphs, results will be summarized according to individual levels of measurement.

For self-reported measurement, three studies used social touch stimulus as a form of social reward, one used smiling faces. Of these, two found significant effects of oxytocin on social reward consumption, which was measured as the level of the pleasantness of a massage (73) or gentle human touch (42) (see Table 1). One study measured self-reported cuteness of the shown infant pictures with null results (33).

Eleven out of 12 fMRI studies found significant effects of IN-OXY on social reward processing, only one reported no oxytocin effects (36). Of the 12 fMRI studies, five studies found effects on the whole-brain level, three of them report effects in PFC (40, 69, 73), two report effects in the insula (39, 73), precuneus (69, 73), and ACC (40, 73). Ten studies used region-of-interest (ROI) analysis with predefined target regions in the brain. 3/4 studies found significant effects in the PFC (40, 54, 72), 1/2 in the insula (67), 2/3 in the ACC (42, 72), 2/4 in the striatum (67, 70), and 2/6 in the amygdala (39, 72). Of the four fMRI studies measuring effective or functional connectivity (35, 39, 71, 72), three found significant effects of IN-OXY (39, 71, 72). Ma et al. (39) between the right insula and left amygdala, Gordon et al. (71) found complex changes in connectivity between the subcortical sites of NAcc and amygdala, and the prefrontal cortices. Finally, Riem et al. (72) found connectivity between the amygdala and vast network of regions including left OFC, hippocampus, precuneus, angular gyrus, MTG, and ACC.

Overall, of the 12 studies measuring the effects of IN-OXY on the neural processing of social reward consumption, changes in the activity or functional connectivity were reported six times in the PFC (40, 42, 69, 7173), four times in the insula (39, 42, 67, 73), precuneus (42, 69, 71, 73), four times in the amygdala (39, 7173), striatum (67, 70, 71, 73), and ACC (40, 42, 72, 73). Specifics of these results are detailed in Table 3.

3.3. Type of stimuli

Seven studies used an interactive task to generate social reward (3840, 62, 6466), and 12 used a positive social stimulus without a preceding social interaction (see Table 1). Of these, three studies used a touch stimulus (42, 68, 73), of which one paired it with a face stimulus (68), and one administered touch in the form of a massage (73). Five studies used pictures of smiling faces (35, 36, 63, 67, 69), of which one used a video of neutral face changing into a smiling face (69). Two studies used pictures of infant faces (33, 70). One study used a stimulus of a happy voice (71) and one study used infant laughter (72).

A significant effect of oxytocin was found in eight out of 12 studies using simple social stimuli (54, 63, 67, 69, 71, 73), where only once the effect was related to reward anticipation (63). A significant effect of oxytocin treatment was further shown in total in five out of seven studies using the interactive task (3840, 62, 64) of which the effect was found in both studies that measured reward consumption (39, 40).

3.4. Dosage

Six studies administered 40 IU IN-OXY (38, 39, 64, 65, 67, 68), of which four found significant effect of OXY (38, 39, 64, 67). Thirteen out of 19 studies used a dose of approximately 24 IU IN-OXY. Specifically one study used approximately 26 IU (63), one study used age-dependent dosing ranging from 12 to 24 IU (71), and 11 studies used exactly 24 IU (33, 35, 36, 40, 54, 62, 66, 69, 70, 72, 73). Only one of these studies did not find a significant effect of treatment of any kind (36).

3.5. Participants’ sex/gender

Six studies included only men in their sample (36, 38, 42, 66, 69, 73), four studies measured only women (33, 63, 70, 72).2 The other 10 studies included both men and women; but one of these included only one female participant out of 20 (40), another study included only two women out of 28 (35), and another only three women out of 13 (71); therefore, we will further refer to these studies as if they were conducted solely on men. Finally, one study did not report the exact number of women in the sample for social reward measurement (62). Thus, in total, seven studies included a significant portion of both male and female participants. The study by Ma et al. (39) reported a significant difference between men and women where social reward processing was downregulated by oxytocin in women, while upregulated in men. Another study reported a significant oxytocin effect for men while no effect for women (64). Two studies reported significant effects of oxytocin regardless of gender (62, 67), and two studies reported no effects of treatment regardless of gender (65, 68). Finally four studies used a solely female sample and found significant oxytocin effects (33, 63, 70, 72), however two of these studies found also an effect in opposite direction (decrease in activity), and this was mainly in the amygdala (33, 72).

Eight studies reported information about the usage of oral contraceptives (33, 39, 6264, 67, 68, 72), three of these reported that no women in their sample used oral contraceptives (39, 63, 64), three studies controlled for the effect (62, 68, 72), one study enrolled only participants using oral contraceptives (33), and one other study reported that the number of women using and not using oral contraceptives was balanced between the patient groups (67).

3.6. Onset time

Five studies set the onset of the experiment 45 min after the IN-OXY administration (39, 62, 64, 69, 73), all reporting significant effects of OXY against placebo at least on one level of analysis (subjective, behavioral, and fMRI). Three studies used shorter onset time of 40 min (36, 68, 72), one of which showed no significant results (36). Another study used an unspecified onset within 40–45 min after administration, with significant findings on brain activity (40). Six studies had onsets around 30 min (38, 54, 63, 65, 66, 70), of which one was of 35 min (66). Four of these studies reported significant effect of IN-OXY manipulation (38, 54, 63, 70). Two studies utilized an onset of around 50 min, both reporting significant findings but only on brain activity (33, 67). Lastly, one study did not report the onset time at all (71).

3.7. Clinical condition

Twelve studies used only healthy participants (see Table 1), four studies used patients with autism (35, 36, 39, 71), of which one compared them with healthy controls (36), two studies compared schizophrenia patients with healthy controls (38, 65), and one study compared PTSD patients with healthy trauma-exposed participants (67). Overall, 12 out of 16 studies using healthy participants found an effect of oxytocin treatment (33, 35, 39, 42, 6773), three out of four studies using autistic patients found a significant effect (35, 40, 71), and the only study using PTSD patients found a significant effect as well (67).

4. Discussion

The current review employed a systematic narrative approach to identify the effects of intranasal oxytocin on social reward processing. Importantly, this review differentiated between the anticipatory and consummatory components of social reward and considered different aspects that may affect the action of OXY on social reward processing. The results provide evidence that intranasal oxytocin can effectively modulate social reward processing, even though its effects are mainly detectable on the neural level. A consistent pattern was found for the effects of oxytocin during the consumption phase; however, the effects during anticipation were less prominent.

4.1. Social reward anticipation

For the anticipatory phase, half of the studies using self-reported measurement found a significant IN-OXY effect (62, 64). However, in the study by Xu et al. oxytocin significantly increased the desire (i.e., wanting social reward) to play the game again with the excluders and not the includes, which was considered to generate social reward (64). The plausible explanation is that this study was conducted in a collectivistic culture (i.e., community prioritizing the group over the individual), where restoring relationships with the community might be more relevant than individual pleasure. It is also of interest that both of these studies utilized the same type of paradigm (cyberball task) as compared to the studies using social touch and social sharing paradigms (39, 73) that found no such effects on self-reports. Nearly none of the behavioral studies found an effect of oxytocin. Apart from oxytocin not being effective, there is also another possible explanation that all behavioral studies in this review started the measurement after 30–50 min. Even though CSF levels of oxytocin are already elevated 35 min post-administration, behavioral effects of oxytocin on social behavior in rhesus macaques have been observed after 2 h post-administration (74). Therefore, a lack of positive results might not only mean that oxytocin does not affect behavior, but also that the effects possibly could not yet be measured. However, most of the studies here did not report the task/experiment duration, making it impossible to assess this line of thought. On the neural level, only one out of four fMRI studies found an effect of IN-OXY during the anticipatory phase of social reward processing, reporting an increase in VTA activity (63), a reward-related brain area mainly regulated by dopamine (3). Nonetheless, all these fMRI studies used a similar task of low ecological validity, providing a social reward in the form of a stranger’s happy face image. Still, modulation of social reward anticipation by IN-OXY administration is expected, especially in light of the known co-expression of oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) with various dopaminergic gene sets (75). Overall, both behavioral and fMRI studies did not provide convincing evidence for such an effect.

4.2. Social reward consumption

During social reward consumption, studies utilized predominantly the fMRI measurement and to some extent the self-reported measurement. In the self-reported measurement domain, only two (42, 73) out of five studies found an effect (see Table 1). However, conscious reward-related feelings might require additional neural mechanisms, and be undetected by the individual, while unconscious hedonic reactions can still be manifested in behavior and neural processing (3).

In contrast to the self-reports, most fMRI assessments found an effect. The results provide information about the regions that are most commonly modulated by IN-OXY during social reward consumption. The prefrontal cortex, insula, precuneus, ACC, amygdala, and striatum were identified as the six key regions modulated by IN-OXY at least in four fMRI studies (see Table 3). The ventromedial PFC including the OFC has been repeatedly associated with hedonic experiences (3, 7678), the OFC is believed to signal reward value (7983), especially, its medial parts (84). Kennerley et al. (85) have studied the role of ACC in reward processing and indicated its role in positive valence-specific reward prediction error coding, with ACC signaling an unexpectedly good reward outcome, but not unexpectedly bad outcome. ACC together with the ventrolateral PFC is further involved in emotion regulation (86). Striatum is an important node in the reward system (3), with its subregions to be dissociable in their contributions to the motivational versus the hedonic component of the affective processing of reward (87). The results on striatum are further in line with research examining human mRNA expression in the brain of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) and the oxytocin secretion gene (CD38). This research found increased expression of OXTR and CD38 specifically in the caudate and putamen, along with the pallidum, thalamus and the olfactory region (75). It is however worth noting that overall the studies included in this review did not show increased activity in the pallidum, the thalamus nor in the olfactory region, in response to IN-OXY during social reward tasks. Next, previous studies indicated a role of the insula in emotional and salience processing (8890) as well as its anterior aspect linked to processing of rewards (91), and the precuneus is suggested to be a part of the brain’s social and self-referential circuitry (92). These brain areas are hubs of large-scale neural networks including the salience network, comprising the insula, ACC, and amygdala (93); and default mode network, which involves medial PFC and precuneus. Oxytocin therefore appears to increase the salience of consumed social rewards, self-referential processing and the perceived value of received rewards. Moreover, studies using connectivity measures also found increased coupling of the insula with amygdala (39), the amygdala with regions such as the OFC, precuneus, ACC, and MTG (72), ventral striatum with the amygdala and the precuneus (71), and ventral striatum with the middle frontal gyrus (71). These brain regions entail the so-called hedonic hotspots, which are regulated, among others, by opioidergic activity (3). Interestingly, there is evidence showing a co-expression of OXTR mRNA and the opioid receptor mRNA (10), a finding recently limitedly supported by research on oxytocin pathway gene networks in the human brain (75, 94), suggesting an interaction between the oxytocin and the opioid pathways.

4.3. Type of stimuli

It is of interest that during social reward anticipation, studies that found an effect of IN-OXY were using an interactive task of higher ecological validity. This is in line with a study by Declerck et al. (30) reporting that effects of IN-OXY on cooperative behavior were not present when the interaction partner was anonymous. These findings together indicate that interactive tasks with real (or believed to be) people might be more effective in oxytocin studies rather than tasks using simple, i.e., non-interactive/unimodal stimuli with faces of strangers. Regarding reward consumption, the majority of studies, on the other hand, utilized simple stimuli instead of interactive tasks. As reward consumption studies used mainly imaging and the reward anticipation studies used mainly behavioral measures, it may be that overall the potential IN-OXY effects of such doses are detected more easily on the (neuro)physiological level. At any rate, it is possible that utilizing interactive tasks could lead to more valid and robust findings across reward phases and measurement types. However, more data are still needed to specifically test the effects of IN-OXY on social reward processing stemming from any kind of social interaction.

4.4. Dosage and remarks on the administration route

Comparing the effects of IN-OXY between different dosages was another aim of this review. However, we cannot provide an answer to the question of whether the higher or lower doses are more effective, as the different dosages appear to be similarly effective in modulating social reward processing. Most of the included studies used the common dose of 24 IU, one of them used age-dependent dosing with the highest dose of 24 IU, and one study used a dose of approximately 26 IU. The six remaining studies used a dose of 40 IU. Nonetheless, the results of studies using different dosages are affected by other methodological aspects, which precludes identifying the effectivity of the doses as such.

This review investigated only studies that administered oxytocin intranasally. The decision was due to the lower side effects and higher efficiency in elevating cerebrospinal cerebrospinal fluid concentrations than the intravenous route of administration (15, 43, 44). Here, several studies suggest that intranasally administered oxytocin primarily exerts its central effects via the nose-to-brain route, instead of crossing the blood–brain barrier (47, 95, 96). However, it needs to be mentioned that these studies still do not provide direct confirmation of the nose-to-brain oxytocin administration route. Even though some recent animal studies using novel oxytocin receptor radiotracer demonstrated an increased detection of the radiotracer in the olfactory bulbs of rats after intranasal administration (vs. intravenous administration) (97), a direct confirmation on humans is still lacking mainly due to the lack of an identified human oxytocin ligand (98).

4.5. Participants’ sex/gender

We also aimed to examine the sex-differences in effects of IN-OXY on social reward processing. Unfortunately, because of the complicated sex-specific brain responses to IN-OXY, oxytocin research tends to focus mostly on men (89). This is also the case for studies in this review. Most of them were conducted solely on male samples, or included only a small proportion of female participants, so the effects could not be compared between the sexes/genders. Only six studies involved a substantial portion of both men and women in their sample. Of these, only two found significant differences between them. Xu et al. (64) found an effect of IN-OXY only for men, and Ma et al. (39) found that in men social reward processing was increased while in women it was decreased after IN-OXY. These findings are consistent with previous studies reporting increases in reward-related neural processing in men and decreases in women (53, 73, 99, 100). These studies support the sex-dependent inverted U-shaped dose–response hypothesis of oxytocin, first proposed by Rilling et al. (37) and then fully fleshed out by Borland et al. (52), which states that women have higher baseline oxytocin levels in their CSF than men, so exogenous oxytocin administration might overstimulate their oxytocin system, and decrease the reward-related neural activity relative to baseline (37, 52). These findings might suggest that individuals with higher baseline CSF levels of oxytocin, which could also include some patient groups, might need a lower dose of oxytocin to benefit from the treatment. Four out of six studies with samples of mixed male and female participants did not find any difference between the sexes/genders (62, 65, 67, 68) and, additionally, one study conducted solely on a female sample even reported increase in reward-related activity in anticipation (63). This, however, might not stand completely against the hypothesis, because, for example, Nawijn et al. reported that they did not find any baseline (i.e., placebo) differences between the sexes/genders (67), which the studies reporting opposite effects of IN-OXY for women did (39, 53, 100). These findings indicate that there might be other important factors beside the participants’ sex/gender that affect the baseline levels of oxytocin among different individuals.

Three studies investigated the potential changes during the consumption phase in women (33, 70, 72). All of these studies had in common the utilization of the infant stimuli, and two of them assessed also the amygdala reactivity (33, 72). It is of interest that both of these studies, together with the study by Ma et al. report a decreased (and not increased) reactivity of the amygdala in women in response to the IN-OXY treatment (33, 39, 72). This is in contrast to findings in men that systematically showed an upregulation of the amygdala activity (39, 71, 73). These studies together indicate a possibility of a sex-dependent reactivity in response to oxytocin that might be specific to the amygdala. The other explanation may lie in the nature of the stimuli presented. Studies by Bos et al. (33) and Riem et al. (72) both utilized infant stimuli that are generally arousing and induce caretaking motivation tendencies (101). Oxytocin is considered to be the primal neurotransmitter associated with infant caretaking and bond formation (102, 103). At the same time the amygdala generally reacts to salient stimuli (20, 104) and has potentially a high density of oxytocin receptors as shown in other mammals (105). It is thus possible that an already higher concentration of oxytocin in the system due to the IN-OXY administration gives a feedback signal to the amygdala, to no longer signal the salience of the caretaking-relevant stimuli, as the oxytocin implied in caretaking is already upregulated in the system. Nevertheless, more studies are necessary in order to address the relevance of these preliminary contrasting findings in the amygdala.

4.6. Onset time

Even though a well calibrated onset (and the subsequent task length) seems to be crucial for maximizing any drug effects, the studies included in this review used various onset times for their experimental task after intranasal oxytocin administration. This was varying from 30 to 50 min and was often without a justification for such a decision. The inconsistent results of the reported studies however do not allow us to draw any conclusions about the efficiency of different utilized onsets. There is no clear consensus about the optimal dose-test latency to allow the greatest concentration of oxytocin in CSF. A study by Spengler et al. (106) used a sample of 116 healthy men and found, that the greatest effectivity of oxytocin was during 45–70 min post-administration. Paloyelis et al. (107) measured cerebral blood flow in 32 healthy men and reported peak response at 39–51 min post-administration. Striepens et al. (43) measured CSF concentrations of oxytocin on a sample of 15 subjects and found that the increase was significant no earlier than after 75 min post-administration. Chang et al. (74) used six rhesus macaques to measure CSF levels of oxytocin and found significant increase after 35 min, however, the prosocial behavioral effects were manifested no earlier than 2 h post-administration. Direct comparison between shorter (e.g., 35 min) and longer (e.g., 2 h) onsets in the IN-OXY effectiveness are, however, still lacking.

4.7. Clinical condition

Oxytocin is being studied as a potential treatment for social deficits in various clinical conditions. In the current review we summarized results of seven studies, which attempted to measure the effects of IN-OXY treatment on social reward processing in clinical samples. Overall, four of these studies reported significant effect of oxytocin on social reward processing (38, 40, 67, 71). Some of these studies also included a healthy control group, but most of them did not find a significant difference between the groups, indicating that IN-OXY treatment might be similarly effective for healthy people and patient groups with social deficits. However, the study by Nawijn et al. (67) found that IN-OXY increased social reward processing in PTSD patients and decreased it in healthy controls. Moreover, several of these studies indicate an influence of individual characteristics, for example Wang and Ma (66) report that IN-OXY selectively increased desire to know positive social evaluation (i.e., social reward) in individuals with higher depression scores. Groppe et al. (63) found that behavioral performance on anticipatory social reward was affected by differences in sociability with enhanced performance in individuals scoring low on self-reported measures of agreeableness. Bradley et al. compared the effect of IN-OXY between schizophrenia patients and healthy controls, and even though the difference did not reach statistical significance, the effect was more pronounced in the patient group. Similar findings have been reported previously, indicating that less socially adaptive individuals might benefit the most of oxytocin treatment (38). Bartz et al. suggested that oxytocin can only boost social capacities up to a certain limit beyond which the treatment has not further effect (108). The lack of measurement of individual differences in social proficiency might be one possible explanation for the inconsistent results among previous studies.

4.8. Future directions

As the oxytocin studies using simple social stimuli appear to be less effective in modulating social reward processing, future studies should opt for social rewards derived from interactive tasks involving real, or believed to be real, interaction partners and social situations that commonly happen in real-world social environments.

Oxytocin research is currently largely investigating male participants while ignoring the necessity of female samples, even though the effects seem to be to a great extent sex-dependent. It has been suggested that these sex-differences might be due to differential baseline oxytocin functioning caused by hormonal differences (109, 110); however, as it appears that participants sex/gender might not be the only relevant cause of baseline differences, researchers first need to identify which other individual characteristics might be associated with oxytocin system functioning. One of these might be the general social abilities or social adaptivity, which has been reported as a characteristic correlating with the effects of IN-OXY in the previous studies (63, 108). To generate consistent findings, the oxytocin research also needs more dose–response studies to identify which doses are the most effective for specific groups of individuals. More information about the most adequate onset necessary for the greatest concentration of oxytocin in CSF is also still lacking. In line with this, it would also be of interest to experimentally explore the appropriate onset-based time window of the possible oxytocin effects to be manifested on the subjective and the behavioral level.

5. Limitations

5.1. Limitations of studies

Detailed information about limitations for each reported study is provided in the Quality assessment form in the Supplementary material. One limitation which applies especially to the studies using a simple, i.e., unimodal/non-interactive social stimulus is the low external validity, as the design used to elicit social reward was often somewhat artificial and far from real-world social situations.

Further, samples consisted mainly of healthy male participants. It is, however, important to focus on determining whether oxytocin can also improve social reward processing in women in response to various rewards and specific patient groups that display social reward impairments. Additionally, important information such as peak MNI coordinates, exact p values, or exact proportion of female participants, were missing in some studies.

Most studies also did not provide enough information about their sampling methods and source population. This could complicate the ability to compare the results of individual studies and it could also indicate a risk of potential bias in results of these studies. Notably, earlier studies with oxytocin might have suffered from being under-powered, and therefore able to detect only a limited range of medium-to-large effect sizes (45). On the other hand, most reported meta-analysis summary effect size estimates are small [i.e., d = 0.2 (45)]. Thus, by identifying and utilizing the smallest effect size of interest for sample size estimation, wider range of effect sizes could reliably be detected (98).

5.2. Limitations of the review process

Although three major databases and three preprint servers have been used for searches in this review, some relevant papers might have been missed due to not being available in these databases. Next, in contrast to our initial preregistration, we decided to include also studies utilizing infant stimuli, given our re-assessment of their primal relevance as social rewards, and especially in the context of oxytocin manipulation. As the task designs that can measure social reward may be here even more heterogeneous, some relevant studies might have been missed due to lacking keywords we used to create the command lines for database searches. Further studies could have been overlooked due to being written in other languages than English, as only studies written in English have been searched for. Additionally, when studies did not report information about some of the inclusion criteria, we asked the authors for additional information. However, when unreachable by email correspondence, we excluded them from further analysis. Nonetheless, some of these might have, in fact, be eligible but were excluded due to the lack of information. Further, we did not assess publication bias, therefore some studies with insignificant results which were left unpublished may cause additional bias in our results. It is worth noting that out of the original 385 studies first collected and screened only 19 could be included for analysis in this review, suggesting that cumulative evidence is still limited. Lastly, given that the design of this paper was conceptualized as a systematic review and not a meta-analysis, the results obtained from the empirical studies were compared with each other in terms of significance and summarized in a narrative format. We do not provide any quantitative summarization of the data.

6. Conclusion

This systematic review summarizes results of randomized double-blind placebo-controlled studies examining the effects of IN-OXY on non-sexual social reward processing in humans, with a special focus on the anticipatory- and consummatory-related reward components. The results suggest that IN-OXY is effective in modulating social reward-related brain activity. Its effects during the consumption of the social reward are exerted likely through modulating activity in brain regions, including the PFC, insula, precuneus, ACC, amygdala, and striatum. In contrast, not much support has been found for the effects of IN-OXY on the anticipatory phase. Future research should focus on female (vs. male) samples to determine the possible sex-differences in the effect of oxytocin, but studies should also measure individual characteristics related to social behavior to address individual differences. Importantly, future research should opt for interactive tasks with real or believed to be real interaction partners, as it appears to be crucial for oxytocin effects.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Author contributions

JK: conceptualization of the study, data obtainment and synthesis, tables and figures preparation, supervision, writing original draft, and review and editing. EV: methodology, data obtainment and synthesis, tables and figures preparation, writing original draft, and review. GS: supervision, writing original draft, and review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1244027/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1. ^https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42021278945

2. ^Sex/gender are not distinguishable here as we did not assess either or, and thus are used interchangeably.

References

1. Schultz, W. Reward In: AW Toga, editor. Brain Mapping Elsevier. United Kingdom: Academic Press: Elsevier (2015). 643–51.

Google Scholar

2. Berridge, KC. Food reward: brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (1996) 20:1–25. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(95)00033-B

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Berridge, KC, and Kringelbach, ML. Pleasure Systems in the Brain. Neuron. (2015) 86:646–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Chelnokova, O, Laeng, B, Eikemo, M, Riegels, J, Løseth, G, Maurud, H, et al. Rewards of beauty: the opioid system mediates social motivation in humans. Mol Psychiatry. (2014) 19:746–7. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Korb, S, Götzendorfer, SJ, Massaccesi, C, Sezen, P, Graf, I, Willeit, M, et al. Dopaminergic and opioidergic regulation during anticipation and consumption of social and nonsocial rewards. elife. (2020) 9:e55797. doi: 10.7554/eLife.55797

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Løseth, GE, Eikemo, M, and Leknes, S. Effects of opioid receptor stimulation and blockade on touch pleasantness: a double-blind randomised trial. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2019) 14:411–22. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bethlehem, R, Baron-Cohen, S, van Honk, J, Auyeung, B, and Bos, P. The oxytocin paradox. Front Behav Neurosci. (2014) 8:48. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Campbell, A. Oxytocin and human social behavior. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. (2010) 14:281–95. doi: 10.1177/1088868310363594

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Lukas, M, Toth, I, Reber, SO, Slattery, DA, Veenema, AH, and Neumann, ID. The neuropeptide oxytocin facilitates pro-social behavior and prevents social avoidance in rats and mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2011) 36:2159–68. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Monte, OD, Piva, M, Anderson, KM, Tringides, M, Holmes, AJ, and Chang, SWC. Oxytocin under opioid antagonism leads to supralinear enhancement of social attention. PNAS. (2017) 114:5247–52. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1702725114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. MacDonald, K, and MacDonald, TM. The peptide that binds: a systematic review of oxytocin and its prosocial effects in humans. Harv Rev Psychiatry. (2010) 18:1–21. doi: 10.3109/10673220903523615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Bethlehem, RAI, van Honk, J, Auyeung, B, and Baron-Cohen, S. Oxytocin, brain physiology, and functional connectivity: a review of intranasal oxytocin fMRI studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2013) 38:962–74. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Dölen, G, Darvishzadeh, A, Huang, KW, and Malenka, RC. Social reward requires coordinated activity of nucleus accumbens oxytocin and serotonin. Nature. (2013) 501:179–84. doi: 10.1038/nature12518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Grinevich, V, and Neumann, ID. Brain oxytocin: how puzzle stones from animal studies translate into psychiatry. Mol Psychiatry. (2021) 26:265–79. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-0802-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Quintana, DS, Lischke, A, Grace, S, Scheele, D, Ma, Y, and Becker, B. Advances in the field of intranasal oxytocin research: lessons learned and future directions for clinical research. Mol Psychiatry. (2021) 26:80–91. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-00864-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Love, TM. Oxytocin, motivation and the role of dopamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2014) 119:49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Skuse, DH, and Gallagher, L. Dopaminergic-neuropeptide interactions in the social brain. Trends Cogn Sci. (2009) 13:27–35. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Sgritta, M, Dooling, SW, Buffington, SA, Momin, EN, Francis, MB, Britton, RA, et al. Mechanisms underlying microbial-mediated changes in social behavior in mouse models of autism Spectrum disorder. Neuron. (2019) 101:246–259.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Itskovich, E, Bowling, DL, Garner, JP, and Parker, KJ. Oxytocin and the social facilitation of placebo effects. Mol Psychiatry. (2022) 27:2640–9. doi: 10.1038/s41380-022-01515-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Shamay-Tsoory, SG, and Abu-Akel, A. The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol Psychiatry. (2016) 79:194–202. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Kohli, S, King, MV, Williams, S, Edwards, A, Ballard, TM, Steward, LJ, et al. Oxytocin attenuates phencyclidine hyperactivity and increases social interaction and nucleus accumben dopamine release in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2019) 44:295–305. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0171-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Krach, S, Paulus, F, Bodden, M, and Ti, K. The rewarding nature of social interactions. Front Behav Neurosci. (2010) 4:22. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Strathearn, L, Fonagy, P, Amico, J, and Montague, PR. Adult attachment predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2009) 34:2655–66. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Baskerville, TA, and Douglas, AJ. Dopamine and oxytocin interactions underlying behaviors: potential contributions to behavioral disorders. CNS Neurosci Ther. (2010) 16:e92–e123. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00154.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Smeltzer, MD, Curtis, JT, Aragona, BJ, and Wang, Z. Dopamine, oxytocin, and vasopressin receptor binding in the medial prefrontal cortex of monogamous and promiscuous voles. Neurosci Lett. (2006) 394:146–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Csiffáry, A, Ruttner, Z, Tóth, Z, and Palkovits, M. Oxytocin nerve fibers innervate β-endorphin neurons in the arcuate nucleus of the rat hypothalamus. NEN. (1992) 56:429–35. doi: 10.1159/000126259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Gu, X-L, and Yu, L-C. Involvement of opioid receptors in oxytocin-induced Antinociception in the nucleus Accumbens of rats. J Pain. (2007) 8:85–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2006.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kohls, G, Chevallier, C, Troiani, V, and Schultz, RT. Social ‘wanting’ dysfunction in autism: neurobiological underpinnings and treatment implications. J Neurodev Disord. (2012) 4:10. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-4-10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Vuong, C, Van Uum, SHM, O’Dell, LE, Lutfy, K, and Friedman, TC. The effects of opioids and opioid analogs on animal and human endocrine systems. Endocr Rev. (2010) 31:98–132. doi: 10.1210/er.2009-0009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Declerck, CH, Boone, C, Pauwels, L, Vogt, B, and Fehr, E. A registered replication study on oxytocin and trust. Nat Hum Behav. (2020) 4:646–55. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0878-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Maltese, F, and Papaleo, F. Oxytocin discrepancies in social dynamics. Neuron. (2020) 107:591–3. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.07.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Trilla, I, Drimalla, H, Bajbouj, M, and Dziobek, I. The influence of reward on facial mimicry: no evidence for a significant effect of oxytocin. Front Behav Neurosci. (2020) 14:88. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Bos, PA, Spencer, H, and Montoya, ER. Oxytocin reduces neural activation in response to infant faces in nulliparous young women. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2018) 13:1099–109. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Domes, G, Heinrichs, M, Gläscher, J, Büchel, C, Braus, DF, and Herpertz, SC. Oxytocin attenuates amygdala responses to emotional faces regardless of valence. Biol Psychiatry. (2007) 62:1187–90. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Greene, RK, Spanos, M, Alderman, C, Walsh, E, Bizzell, J, Mosner, MG, et al. The effects of intranasal oxytocin on reward circuitry responses in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodev Disord. (2018) 10:12. doi: 10.1186/s11689-018-9228-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Mayer, AV, Preckel, K, Ihle, K, Piecha, FA, Junghanns, K, Reiche, S, et al. Assessment of reward-related brain function after a single dose of oxytocin in autism: a randomized controlled trial. Biol Psychiatry Global Open Sci. (2022) 2:136–46. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsgos.2021.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Rilling, JK, DeMarco, AC, Hackett, PD, Chen, X, Gautam, P, Stair, S, et al. Sex differences in the neural and behavioral response to intranasal oxytocin and vasopressin during human social interaction. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2014) 39:237–48. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Bradley, ER, Brustkern, J, de Coster, L, van den Bos, W, McClure, SM, Seitz, A, et al. Victory is its own reward: oxytocin increases costly competitive behavior in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. (2020) 50:674–82. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719000552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Ma, X, Zhao, W, Luo, R, Zhou, F, Geng, Y, Xu, L, et al. Sex- and context-dependent effects of oxytocin on social sharing. NeuroImage. (2018) 183:62–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Andari, E, Richard, N, Leboyer, M, and Sirigu, A. Adaptive coding of the value of social cues with oxytocin, an fMRI study in autism spectrum disorder. Cortex. (2016) 76:79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Clark-Elford, R, Nathan, PJ, Auyeung, B, Voon, V, Sule, A, Müller, U, et al. The effects of oxytocin on social reward learning in humans. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. (2014) 17:199–209. doi: 10.1017/S1461145713001120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Scheele, D, Kendrick, KM, Khouri, C, Kretzer, E, Schläpfer, TE, Stoffel-Wagner, B, et al. An oxytocin-induced facilitation of neural and emotional responses to social touch correlates inversely with autism traits. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2014) 39:2078–85. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Striepens, N, Kendrick, KM, Hanking, V, Landgraf, R, Wüllner, U, Maier, W, et al. Elevated cerebrospinal fluid and blood concentrations of oxytocin following its intranasal administration in humans. Sci Rep. (2013) 3:3440. doi: 10.1038/srep03440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Born, J, Lange, T, Kern, W, McGregor, GP, Bickel, U, and Fehm, HL. Sniffing neuropeptides: a transnasal approach to the human brain. Nat Neurosci. (2002) 5:514–6. doi: 10.1038/nn0602-849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Quintana, DS. Most oxytocin administration studies are statistically underpowered to reliably detect (or reject) a wide range of effect sizes. Compreh Psychoneuroendocrinol. (2020) 4:100014. doi: 10.1016/j.cpnec.2020.100014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Cardoso, C, Ellenbogen, MA, Orlando, MA, Bacon, SL, and Joober, R. Intranasal oxytocin attenuates the cortisol response to physical stress: a dose–response study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2013) 38:399–407. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Quintana, DS, Westlye, LT, Alnæs, D, Rustan, ØG, Kaufmann, T, Smerud, KT, et al. Low dose intranasal oxytocin delivered with breath powered device dampens amygdala response to emotional stimuli: a peripheral effect-controlled within-subjects randomized dose-response fMRI trial. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2016) 69:180–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.04.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Quintana, DS, Westlye, LT, Hope, S, Nærland, T, Elvsåshagen, T, Dørum, E, et al. Dose-dependent social-cognitive effects of intranasal oxytocin delivered with novel breath powered device in adults with autism spectrum disorder: a randomized placebo-controlled double-blind crossover trial. Transl Psychiatry. (2017) 7:e1136–6. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Benelli, A, Bertolini, A, Poggioli, R, Menozzi, B, Basaglia, R, and Arletti, R. Polymodal dose-response curve for oxytocin in the social recognition test. Neuropeptides. (1995) 28:251–5. doi: 10.1016/0143-4179(95)90029-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Hecht, EE, Robins, DL, Gautam, P, and King, TZ. Intranasal oxytocin reduces social perception in women: neural activation and individual variation. NeuroImage. (2017) 147:314–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Altemus, M, Jacobson, KR, Debellis, M, Kling, M, Pigott, T, Murphy, DL, et al. Normal CSF oxytocin and NPY levels in OCD. Biol Psychiatry. (1999) 45:931–3. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00263-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Borland, JM, Aiani, LM, Norvelle, A, Grantham, KN, O’Laughlin, K, Terranova, JI, et al. Sex-dependent regulation of social reward by oxytocin receptors in the ventral tegmental area. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2019) 44:785–92. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0262-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Feng, C, Hackett, PD, DeMarco, AC, Chen, X, Stair, S, Haroon, E, et al. Oxytocin and vasopressin effects on the neural response to social cooperation are modulated by sex in humans. Brain Imag Behav. (2015) 9:754–64. doi: 10.1007/s11682-014-9333-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Scheele, D, Plota, J, Stoffel-Wagner, B, Maier, W, and Hurlemann, R. Hormonal contraceptives suppress oxytocin-induced brain reward responses to the partner’s face. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2016) 11:767–74. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Gossen, A, Hahn, A, Westphal, L, Prinz, S, Schultz, RT, Gründer, G, et al. Oxytocin plasma concentrations after single intranasal oxytocin administration – a study in healthy men. Neuropeptides. (2012) 46:211–5. doi: 10.1016/j.npep.2012.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Donadon, MF, Martin-Santos, R, Osório, FL, and de Osório, FL. The associations between oxytocin and trauma in humans: a systematic review. Front Pharmacol. (2018) 9:154. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2018.00154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Page, MJ, McKenzie, JE, Bossuyt, PM, Boutron, I, Hoffmann, TC, Mulrow, CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. (2021) 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Downs, SH, and Black, N. The feasibility of creating a checklist for the assessment of the methodological quality both of randomised and non-randomised studies of health care interventions. J Epidemiol Community Health. (1998) 52:377–84. doi: 10.1136/jech.52.6.377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Berridge, KC, Robinson, TE, and Aldridge, JW. Dissecting components of reward: ‘liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Curr Opin Pharmacol. (2009) 9:65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Paredes, RG. Opioids and sexual reward. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2014) 121:124–31. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.11.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Rademacher, L, Krach, S, Kohls, G, Irmak, A, Gründer, G, and Spreckelmeyer, KN. Dissociation of neural networks for anticipation and consumption of monetary and social rewards. NeuroImage. (2010) 49:3276–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Alvares, GA, Hickie, IB, and Guastella, AJ. Acute effects of intranasal oxytocin on subjective and behavioral responses to social rejection. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. (2010) 18:316–21. doi: 10.1037/a0019719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Groppe, SE, Gossen, A, Rademacher, L, Hahn, A, Westphal, L, Gründer, G, et al. Oxytocin influences processing of socially relevant cues in the ventral tegmental area of the human brain. Biol Psychiatry. (2013) 74:172–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Xu, X, Yao, S, Xu, L, Geng, Y, Zhao, W, Ma, X, et al. Oxytocin biases men but not women to restore social connections with individuals who socially exclude them. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:40589. doi: 10.1038/srep40589

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Fulford, J, Milton, F, Salas, D, Smith, A, Simler, A, Winlove, C, et al. The neural correlates of visual imagery vividness – an fMRI study and literature review. Cortex. (2018) 105:26–40. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Wang, D, and Ma, Y. Oxytocin facilitates valence-dependent valuation of social evaluation of the self. Commun Biol. (2020) 3:433–12. doi: 10.1038/s42003-020-01168-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Nawijn, L, van Zuiden, M, Koch, SBJ, Frijling, JL, Veltman, DJ, and Olff, M. Intranasal oxytocin increases neural responses to social reward in post-traumatic stress disorder. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2017) 12:212–23. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Ellingsen, D-M, Wessberg, J, Chelnokova, O, Olausson, H, Laeng, B, and Leknes, S. In touch with your emotions: oxytocin and touch change social impressions while others’ facial expressions can alter touch. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2014) 39:11–20. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Hu, J, Qi, S, Becker, B, Luo, L, Gao, S, Gong, Q, et al. Oxytocin selectively facilitates learning with social feedback and increases activity and functional connectivity in emotional memory and reward processing regions. Hum Brain Mapp. (2015) 36:2132–46. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Gregory, R, Cheng, H, Rupp, HA, Sengelaub, DR, and Heiman, JR. Oxytocin increases VTA activation to infant and sexual stimuli in nulliparous and postpartum women. Horm Behav. (2015) 69:82–8. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Gordon, I, Jack, A, Pretzsch, CM, Vander Wyk, B, Leckman, JF, Feldman, R, et al. Intranasal oxytocin enhances connectivity in the neural circuitry supporting social motivation and social perception in children with autism. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:35054. doi: 10.1038/srep35054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Riem, MME, van Ijzendoorn, MH, Tops, M, MAS, B, SARB, R, and Bakermans-Kranenburg, MJ. No laughing matter: intranasal oxytocin administration changes functional brain connectivity during exposure to infant laughter. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2012) 37:1257–66. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Chen, Y, Becker, B, Zhang, Y, Cui, H, Du, J, Wernicke, J, et al. Oxytocin increases the pleasantness of affective touch and orbitofrontal cortex activity independent of valence. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2020) 39:99–110. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2020.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Chang, SWC, Barter, JW, Ebitz, RB, Watson, KK, and Platt, ML. Inhaled oxytocin amplifies both vicarious reinforcement and self reinforcement in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2012) 109:959–64. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1114621109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Quintana, DS, Rokicki, J, van der Meer, D, Alnæs, D, Kaufmann, T, Córdova-Palomera, A, et al. Oxytocin pathway gene networks in the human brain. Nat Commun. (2019) 10:668. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-08503-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Castro, DC, and Berridge, KC. Opioid and orexin hedonic hotspots in rat orbitofrontal cortex and insula. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2017) 114:E9125–34. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1705753114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Elliott, R, Agnew, Z, and Deakin, JFW. Hedonic and informational functions of the human orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. (2010) 20:198–204. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Kringelbach, ML. The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2005) 6:691–702. doi: 10.1038/nrn1747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Azzi, JCB, Sirigu, A, and Duhamel, J-R. Modulation of value representation by social context in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2012) 109:2126–31. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1111715109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. FitzGerald, THB, Seymour, B, and Dolan, RJ. The role of human orbitofrontal cortex in value comparison for incommensurable objects. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:8388–95. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0717-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. O’Doherty, J, Critchley, H, Deichmann, R, and Dolan, RJ. Dissociating valence of outcome from behavioral control in human orbital and ventral prefrontal cortices. J Neurosci. (2003) 23:7931–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-21-07931.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Rolls, ET, Cheng, W, and Feng, J. The orbitofrontal cortex: reward, emotion and depression. Brain. Communications. (2020) 2:fcaa196. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcaa196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Wallis, JD, and Kennerley, SW. Contrasting reward signals in the orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2011) 1239:33–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06277.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Heather Hsu, C-C, Rolls, ET, Huang, C-C, Chong, ST, Zac Lo, C-Y, Feng, J, et al. Connections of the human orbitofrontal cortex and inferior frontal gyrus. Cereb Cortex. (2020) 30:5830–43. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Kennerley, SW, Behrens, TEJ, and Wallis, JD. Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons. Nat Neurosci. (2011) 14:1581–9. doi: 10.1038/nn.2961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Steinberg, L. Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci. (2005) 9:69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Pool, ER, Tord, DM, Delplanque, S, Stussi, Y, Cereghetti, D, Vuilleumier, P, et al. Differential contributions of ventral striatum subregions to the motivational and hedonic components of the affective processing of reward. J Neurosci. (2022) 42:2716–28. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1124-21.2022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Striepens, N, Scheele, D, Kendrick, KM, Becker, B, Schäfer, L, Schwalba, K, et al. Oxytocin facilitates protective responses to aversive social stimuli in males. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2012) 109:18144–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208852109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Wigton, R, Radua, J, Allen, P, Averbeck, B, Meyer-Lindenberg, A, McGuire, P, et al. Neurophysiological effects of acute oxytocin administration: systematic review and meta-analysis of placebo-controlled imaging studies. J Psychiatry Neurosci. (2015) 40:E1–E22. doi: 10.1503/jpn.130289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Yao, S, Zhao, W, Geng, Y, Chen, Y, Zhao, Z, Ma, X, et al. Oxytocin facilitates approach behavior to positive social stimuli via decreasing anterior insula activity. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. (2018) 21:918–25. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyy068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Haber, SN, and Knutson, B. The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2010) 35:4–26. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Amft, M, Bzdok, D, Laird, AR, Fox, PT, Schilbach, L, and Eickhoff, SB. Definition and characterization of an extended social-affective default network. Brain Struct Funct. (2015) 220:1031–49. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0698-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Menon, V, and Uddin, LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. (2010) 214:655–67. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Rokicki, J, Kaufmann, T, de Lange, A-MG, van der Meer, D, Bahrami, S, Sartorius, AM, et al. Oxytocin receptor expression patterns in the human brain across development. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2022) 47:1550–60. doi: 10.1038/s41386-022-01305-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Quintana, DS, Westlye, LT, Rustan, ØG, Tesli, N, Poppy, CL, Smevik, H, et al. Low-dose oxytocin delivered intranasally with breath powered device affects social-cognitive behavior: a randomized four-way crossover trial with nasal cavity dimension assessment. Transl Psychiatry. (2015) 5:e602–2. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.93

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Quintana, DS, Westlye, LT, Alnæs, D, Kaufmann, T, Mahmoud, RA, Smerud, KT, et al. Low-dose intranasal oxytocin delivered with breath powered device modulates pupil diameter and amygdala activity: a randomized controlled pupillometry and fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2019) 44:306–13. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0241-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Beard, R, Singh, N, Grundschober, C, Gee, AD, and Tate, EW. High-yielding 18F radiosynthesis of a novel oxytocin receptor tracer, a probe for nose-to-brain oxytocin uptake in vivo. Chem Commun. (2018) 54:8120–3. doi: 10.1039/C8CC01400K

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Winterton, A, Westlye, LT, Steen, NE, Andreassen, OA, and Quintana, DS. Improving the precision of intranasal oxytocin research. Nat Hum Behav. (2021) 5:9–18. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00996-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Rilling, JK, DeMarco, AC, Hackett, PD, Thompson, R, Ditzen, B, Patel, R, et al. Effects of intranasal oxytocin and vasopressin on cooperative behavior and associated brain activity in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2012) 37:447–61. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Rilling, JK, Chen, X, Chen, X, and Haroon, E. Intranasal oxytocin modulates neural functional connectivity during human social interaction. Am J Primatol. (2018) 80:e22740. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Glocker, ML, Langleben, DD, Ruparel, K, Loughead, JW, Gur, RC, and Sachser, N. Baby Schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology. (2009) 115:257–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01603.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Feldman, R. The neurobiology of human attachments. Trends Cogn Sci. (2017) 21:80–99. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Finkenwirth, C, Martins, E, Deschner, T, and Burkart, JM. Oxytocin is associated with infant-care behavior and motivation in cooperatively breeding marmoset monkeys. Horm Behav. (2016) 80:10–8. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Cunningham, WA, and Brosch, T. Motivational salience: amygdala tuning from traits, needs, values, and goals. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2012) 21:54–9. doi: 10.1177/0963721411430832

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Insel, TR, and Shapiro, LE. Oxytocin receptor distribution reflects social organization in monogamous and polygamous voles. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (1992) 89:5981–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.13.5981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Spengler, FB, Schultz, J, Scheele, D, Essel, M, Maier, W, Heinrichs, M, et al. Kinetics and dose dependency of intranasal oxytocin effects on amygdala reactivity. Biol Psychiatry. (2017) 82:885–94. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.04.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Paloyelis, Y, Doyle, OM, Zelaya, FO, Maltezos, S, Williams, SC, Fotopoulou, A, et al. A spatiotemporal profile of in vivo cerebral blood flow changes following intranasal oxytocin in humans. Biol Psychiatry. (2016) 79:693–705. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Bartz, JA, Zaki, J, Bolger, N, and Ochsner, KN. Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends Cogn Sci. (2011) 15:301–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Gabor, CS, Phan, A, Clipperton-Allen, AE, Kavaliers, M, and Choleris, E. Interplay of oxytocin, vasopressin, and sex hormones in the regulation of social recognition. Behav Neurosci. (2012) 126:97–109. doi: 10.1037/a0026464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Insel, TR. The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior. Neuron. (2010) 65:768–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: oxytocin, social reward, anticipation, consumption, social motivation, wanting, liking, fMRI

Citation: Kraus J, Výborová E and Silani G (2023) The effect of intranasal oxytocin on social reward processing in humans: a systematic review. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1244027. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1244027

Received: 21 June 2023; Accepted: 30 August 2023;
Published: 14 September 2023.

Edited by:

Clare Margaret Eddy, Birmingham and Solihull Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Johnathan Michael Borland, University of Minnesota, United States
Mary Rachel Lee, United States Department of Veterans Affairs, United States

Copyright © 2023 Kraus, Výborová and Silani. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jakub Kraus, jakub.kraus@univie.ac.at; Giorgia Silani, giorgia.silani@univie.ac.at

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.