ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 09 April 2025

Sec. Public Mental Health

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1547389

This article is part of the Research TopicThe Intersection of Psychology, Healthy Behaviors, and its OutcomesView all 96 articles

The relationship between perceived stress and bedtime procrastination among Chinese college students: a moderated mediation model

  • 1Department of Psychology, School of Medical Humanitarians, Guizhou Medical University, Guiyang, China
  • 2Guizhou Health Development Research Center, Guiyang, China
  • 3Department of Histology and Embryology, School of Basic Medical Sciences, Guizhou Medical University, Guiyang, China
  • 4Key Laboratory for Research on Autoimmune Diseases of Higher Education Schools in Guizhou Province, Guiyang, China

Introduction: the role of stress in inducing bedtime procrastination is a new research topic that has emerged in recent years. This study aimed to explore the psychological mechanism by which perceived stress affects bedtime procrastination in Chinese college students in a life history framework.

Methods: first, we investigated whether life history strategy mediate their relationships. Then, we examined whether distress tolerance moderated the direct effects of perceived stress on bedtime procrastination and/or the indirect effects of them mediated by life history strategy. The data of 1021 college students were analyzed.

Results: the results showed that: (1) perceived stress had a significant predictive effect on bedtime procrastination; (2) life history strategy played a mediating role in the relationship between perceived stress and bedtime procrastination; (3) distress tolerance moderated the indirect effect between them mediated by life history strategy, but not the direct effect between them.

Discussion: the present study suggested that life history-based interventions might be an effective intervention for stress-induced bedtime procrastination. Specifically, it might be helpful for alleviation of bedtime procrastination to develop slow life history strategy, when faced with similar stressful situations in the future. Furthermore, distress tolerance may be an important alternative target.

1 Introduction

Bedtime procrastination is defined as frequently failing to go to bed at the intended time, while no external circumstances prevent a person from doing so (1). The prevalence of bedtime procrastination among college students is higher than that among non-students (2). A survey conducted by China Youth Daily in 2019 among more than 1,000 college students showed that more than 30% of them often stayed up late and slept less than 7 hours a night (3). Bedtime procrastination not only causes sleep insufficiency (4), affects an individual’s learning and working status the next day (5, 6), but also reduces individual well-being and increases risk of physical and mental illness, such as anxiety and depression (7, 8). Given the high prevalence and serious consequences, identifying the influencing factors and psychological mechanisms of bedtime procrastination among Chinese college students is crucial for effective intervention.

Except for a few studies focusing on time perspective, such as chronotype (9, 10) and future time perspective (9, 11), a large number of previous studies have mainly understood bedtime procrastination among college students from the perspective of self-regulation. Bedtime procrastination is often conceptualized as a consequence of poor self-regulation (5) and its associated disorders, such as mobile phone addiction (1219).

However, these studies on college students’ bedtime procrastination did not pay enough attention to the special period of their lives. College students are most in emerging adulthood, which roughly ages 18–25 (20). This period is considered to be a critical and most unstable period of their life span, with many life transitions in terms of living arrangements, relationships, education and employment (21). On the other hand, their ability to deal with problems in life is still immature (22). As a result, college students are believed to have a high level of perceived stress. This is especially true for Chinese college students, because they often the only children in their families and thus are very sensitive to stress. In fact, studies have shown that Chinese college students have high levels of stress (23, 24). A recent qualitative study of a sample of adolescent students found that stress is a highlighted factor associated with bedtime procrastination (25). However, the impact of perceived stress on bedtime procrastination among Chinese college students and its psychological mechanisms are still not fully understood.

A recent study of a Chinese college student sample explored the relationship between academic stress and bedtime procrastination (26). They found that academic stress positively predicted bedtime procrastination, and mobile phone addiction was an important mediator between the two variables. According to the stressor-detachment model, they speculated that mobile phone addiction is a mean of detachment for recovery under academic workload. From this perspective, mobile phone addiction behavior before bedtime is an active strategy, while bedtime procrastination is an active procrastination behavior. In line with this, a qualitative study of new career starters under the pressure of the university-to-work transition found that leisure activities before bedtime is an active behavior for me-time and self-negotiation (27). This is clearly inconsistent with the view that bedtime procrastination is a consequence of self-regulation failure. From the perspective of self-regulation failure, bedtime procrastinator cannot resist the temptation and are more likely to involve in entertaining activities before bedtime at night, then bedtime procrastination seems to be a passive behavior rather than an active procrastination behavior. Therefore, the self-regulation failure model cannot well explain the bedtime procrastination induced by perceived stress.

Why bedtime procrastinators actively choose non-adaptive behaviors over adaptive behaviors is a puzzling and critical question that urgently needs to be explained. One scholar has tried to use the perspective of resource conflict to explain the tension between leisure and health in bedtime procrastination, that is, individuals want to satisfy both pleasure or leisure needs and health needs, but one of the needs is at the expense of the other, and they are competing for the same time resource (28). While bedtime procrastinators appear to focus on the present rather than the future in their decision-making about handling this conflict. For example, the future time perspective negatively predicts bedtime procrastination among college students sample (9, 11). Performance goal orientation, a future time orientation disposition, also negatively predicted bedtime procrastination (3). The viewpoints of resource competition, future time perspective and active strategy are highly consistent with the life history strategy (LHS) perspective in evolutionary psychology, suggesting that the life history strategy perspective may help integrate these perspectives and better understand this phenomenon, especially for college students who are in emerging adulthood, a period of heightened instability.

1.1 The mediating role of life history strategies in the relationship between perceived stress and bedtime procrastination

LHS focuses on how individuals allocate their limited resources to adapt to stressful and challenging environments based on an assessment of the environmental constraints (29). When the environment is predictable, planning and working towards higher future returns is cost-effective. Therefore, humans and animals’ cognition and behavior tend to be more future- than present-oriented. That is, they prefer actions that may yield high future rewards but have no or little immediate benefits. This is called the slow LHS. Within the same framework, people with slow LHS are expected to be future-oriented and not procrastinate (30). Conversely, when the future is uncertain and difficult to predict, the likelihood of an investment paying off in the future is slim. Present orientation has greater practical significance for individual survival than future orientation. Therefore, organisms will pay more attention to the present and discount the future. This is called the fast LHS. Since investing in the present is most profitable compared to the future, people who adopt a fast LHS are expected to procrastinate (30).

Environmental stress is a key antecedent factor in inducing a fast life history strategy (31). In human life history strategies, subjective perceived stress can more accurately reflect an individual’s stress state than objective environmental stress and can more effectively induce a fast life history strategy. Perceived stress refers to the imbalance between perceived environmental demands and their ability to cope with those demands (32). When an individual believes that his or her abilities are adequate to cope with environmental demands, perceived stress is low. Conversely, when an individual believes that his or her abilities are insufficient to cope with environmental demands, perceived stress is high. Chinese college students, who are often the only children in their families, are very sensitive to stress. They face multiple pressures, such as changes of environmental and lifestyle, academic workload, and interpersonal relationships. In fact, studies have shown that Chinese college students have high levels of stress (23, 24). Existing evidences showed that perceived stress is closely related to bedtime procrastination among Chinese college students. For example, perceived stress was significantly positively correlated with bedtime procrastination (33). Self-efficacy, the subjective feeling of ability to handle stress, an important antecedent of perceived stress, was negatively associated with bedtime procrastination (34).

After experiencing stress during the day, college students are faced with a dilemma between leisure and health (28), which involves how to optimally allocate limited time resources to maximize adaptation to the stressful environment. According to the concept of perceived stress, higher perceived stress often means a greater lack of ability to cope with environmental demands, which not only means more resource depletion, but also indicates a loss of control over future outcomes and increased uncertainty about the future. In this context, for those with higher perceived stress, their future is uncertain and difficult to predict, and the possibility of getting a return on investment in the future is smaller. Therefore, it is more important for them to recover from the individual fatigue induced by excessive stress. Detachment from stressful events might be the best way to recover. The increase in the demand for me-time and self-negotiation may reflect this need for detachment. That might be a need for work-life balance, specifically the balance between “what I want to do” and “what I am expected or should do”. This balance is critical to the well-being of college students (35, 36) and a predictor of college student anxiety and depression (37). That is, under stressful situations, a fast life history strategy that focuses on the present and discounts the future is more adaptive to the environment. Therefore, it is reasonable to hypothesize that LHS mediates the effects of perceived stress on bedtime procrastination (H1).

1.2 The moderation of distress tolerance

If the aforementioned hypothesis 1 is true, how to alleviate perceived stress-induced bedtime procrastination based on this model is a topic worthy of further exploration. Perceived stress is often accompanied by negative physical and emotional states. The ability to effectively withstand these aversive internal experiences, termed distress tolerance (DT), has long been of interest in psychotherapy (3840). Distress intolerance is an important motivator of maladaptive avoidance-based coping strategies (41). Low DT has been associated with maladaptive avoidance behaviors, such as procrastination behavior (42), substance use (43, 44), stock addiction (45), depression and anxiety (46, 47).

On the one hand, individuals with low distress tolerance have been reported to have greater responses to distress and are more likely to exceed their tolerance threshold and experience stress overload because of their low distress tolerance threshold (48). On the other hand, distress intolerance may motivate these self-defeating behaviors, serving as powerful proximal reinforcers to quickly relieve distressing states interpreted as dangerous or unmanageable (41). These active choices by individuals may arise from their belief that they cannot or do not want to endure present pain in the future (49). In this context, individuals with low distress tolerance are expected to have a stronger need to relieve negative emotions and be more likely to initiate the fast LHS and/or indulge in relaxing activities before going to bed, which can lead to delayed bedtime. Therefore, distress tolerance appears to moderate the relationship between perceived stress and LHS (H2) and the relationship between perceived stress and bedtime procrastination (H3).

In summary, this study aimed to explore the psychological mechanism by which perceived stress affects bedtime procrastination in Chinese college students. First, we investigated whether life history strategy mediates their relationships. Then, we examined whether distress tolerance moderated the direct effects of perceived stress on bedtime procrastination and/or the indirect effects of them mediated by life history strategy. Figure 1 shows the hypothesized overall model.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The moderated mediation model hypothesized in the present study. Notes of Figure  In this model, we hypothesized that LHS may mediate the relationship between perceived stress and bedtime procrastination (H1); and that distress tolerance may moderate both the relationship of perceived stress and life history strategy (H2) and that of perceived stress and bedtime procrastination (H3).

2 Methods

2.1 Participants and procedure

We recruited Chinese college students from three universities in Guizhou, Shandong, and Fujian provinces, which are located in the southwest, north and east of China respectively. Prior to participation, all students signed informed consent forms and were informed that their responses would be anonymous with the option of withdrawal at any time.1021 students were included in the data analysis. Among them were 335 males and 686 females; 612 were ethnic minorities and 409 were Han Chinese; 247 were from towns and 774 were from villages; 423 had left-behind experience; 598 had no left-behind experience. The participants ranged in age from 18 to 22 years old, and the average age was 18.97 ± 0.96 years. This research was approved by the ethics committee of Guizhou Medical University.

2.2 Measures

2.2.1 Bedtime procrastination

The Bedtime Procrastination Scale (50) was used to measure bedtime procrastination. The Chinese version of the Bedtime Procrastination Behavior Scale has good reliability and validity in college students. It consists of nine items, such as “I sleep later than I expected.” and the responses of participators were rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Items 2, 3, 7, and 9 were scored in reverse. Higher scores indicated more bedtime procrastination behaviors (51). Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was 0.80 in the present study.

2.2.2 Perceived stress

The Perceived Scale (52) was used to measure stress. The Chinese version of the Perceived Stress Scale has been demonstrated to have good reliability and validity. It contains 14 items, such as “I feel out of control of the important things in my life” and the responses of participators were rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Higher scores indicated higher perceived stress (53). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.75 in the present study.

2.2.3 Life history strategy

LH strategy was measured using the Mini-K Life History Strategy Scale (54). The Chinese version of the Mini-K Life History Strategy Scale demonstrated good reliability and validity among Chinese College Students. It consists of 19 items, such as “I try to understand how I got into a situation to figure out how to handle it.” The responses of participators were rated on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Higher scores indicated a slower life history strategy (55). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.80 in the present study.

2.2.4 Distress tolerance

The Distress Tolerance Scale was used to measure distress tolerance (56). The Chinese vision of distress tolerance has good reliability and validity among Chinese adults. The scale consists of 13 items, such as “For me, it’s intolerable to feel pain or discomfort.” The responses of participators were rated on a 5-Likert point scale ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree). Higher scores indicated higher Distress tolerance (57). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.82 in this study.

2.3 Statistical analysis

The data were analyzed in SPSS version 25.0 and PROCESS V3. 4.1. First, we tested the common method biases; Then, we conducted the descriptive statistics and correlation analysis to examine the demographic characteristics and the association between various variables; Next, we tested the mediation effect of life history strategy by PROCESS macro model 4; Finally, we tested simultaneously all the study hypotheses by model 8. Data were calculated based on average score of items. Bootstrapped confidence interval (CI; 5,000 bootstrap samples) for the effect was obtained. If zero is not included in the confidence interval, effects are significant. Moreover, we transformed gender and left-behind experience into dummy variables and included them as control variables in subsequent analyses. gender: 1= male, 0= female; left-behind experience:1=have left-behind experience; 0= have no left-behind experience.

3 Results

3.1 Common method bias test

To assess and rule out common method bias, Harman’s single-factor test was employed. The analysis identified 14 factors with eigenvalues greater than 1, and the first factor accounted for 15.98% of the total variance. This proportion was lower than 40% recommended by Podsakoff (58), indicating that there was no the interpretation confusion induced by common method bias in the data analysis of this study.

3.2 Descriptive statistics and correlation analysis

Because the sample size difference between genders was too large, we used Welch’s t -test, a method more suitable for analyzing differences in unequal samples (59), to analyze gender differences in each variable. The result showed that compared with men, women had higher bedtime procrastination (t = -3.25, p< 0.01), perceived stress (t = -3.75, p< 0.001), and lower distress tolerance (t = 2.04, p< 0.05). At the same time, the independent sample T-test showed that the experience of being left behind significantly reduced the score of life history strategy (t = -2.74, p< 0.01). Further, gender and the experience of being left behind has been reported to be related with sleep problem (14, 60). Therefore, these two variables were selected as covariates in the subsequent analysis using PROCESS macro for SPSS (61).

We tested the data for normal distribution and the results showed that it obeyed a normal distribution (see Figure 2). Then, to examine relationships among the main variables, Pearson’s correlation coefficients were calculated (see Table 1). Results showed that perceived stress was positively correlated with bedtime procrastination (r = 0.32, p < 0.001), negatively correlated with life history strategy (r = -0.40, p < 0.001) and distress tolerance (r = -0.46, p < 0.001). Distress tolerance was negatively correlated with bedtime procrastination (r = -0.25, p < 0.001), positively corrected with Life history strategy (r = 0.31, p < 0.001). Life history strategy was negatively correlated with bedtime procrastination (r = -0.33, p < 0.001).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Tests for normal distribution of variables.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations between variables.

3.3 Testing for mediation effect

As Model 4 is suitable for simple mediation effect tests, it allows for comprehensive effect estimation and effective inclusion of control variables in the analysis, making the results more precise (62), we ran PROCESS macro-Model 4 to test hypothesis 1. Results showed that after controlling for covariates (gender and left-behind experience), life history strategy partly mediates the relationship between perceived stress and bedtime procrastination(R2 = 0.16, F = 46.79, p < 0.001). Specifically, the results showed that perceived stress negatively predicted on life history strategy (β = -0.57, p < 0.001. see Model 1 in Table 2A), while a lower LHS score indicates a tendency to use a faster life strategy. Thus, it means that the higher level of students’ perceived stress, the faster life history strategy was initiated. Perceived stress positively predicted on bedtime procrastination (β = 0.31, p <.001. see Model 2 in Table 2A), while life history strategy negatively predicted on bedtime procrastination (β = -0.25, p < 0.001.see Model 2 in Table 2A), suggesting that the faster life history strategy was initiated, the more bedtime procrastination. In summary, higher perceived stress induced faster life history strategy, which in turn led to higher bedtime procrastination. Moreover, the biased-corrected percentile bootstrap method was used to show that the indirect effect of perceived stress on bedtime procrastination through life history strategy was significant (ab=0.14, SE=0.02, and the 95%CI = [0.10, 0.19]), the direct effect of perceived stress on bedtime procrastination was significant (c’=0.31, SE=0.05, and the 95%CI = [0.22, 0.40]). Therefore, life history strategy partly mediates the relationship between perceived stress and bedtime procrastination. This means that perceived stress affect bedtime procrastination not only directly, but also indirectly through life history strategy. The percentage of this mediation effect of the total effect was 31.72%. These results support Hypothesis 1 (see Tables 2A, B).

Table 2A
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2A. Testing the mediation effect of Life History Strategy on Bedtime Procrastination.

Table 2B
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2B. The bootstrapping analysis of the mediating effects.

3.4 Testing for the moderated mediation model

We ran the PROCESS macro model 8 to examine hypothesis 2 and hypothesis 3. Overall testing models are presented in Figure 3, and the specific indirect effects are presented in Table 3A. The results showed that perceived stress negatively predicted on life history strategy (β = -0.48, p < 0.001.see Model 3 in Table 3A), and the interaction effect of perceived stress and distress tolerance on life history strategy was significant (β = 0.16, p < 0.05.see Model 3 in Table 3A). Therefore, the effects of perceived stress on life history strategy are moderated by distress tolerance, and Hypothesis 2 is supported. Perceived stress positively predicted bedtime procrastination (β = 0.25, p < 0.001. see Model 4 in Table 3A), but the interaction effect of perceived stress and distress tolerance on bedtime procrastination was not significant (β = 0.02, p > 0.05.see Model 4 in Table 3A). That is to say, the direct effects of perceived stress on bedtime procrastination are not moderated by distress tolerance, the Hypothesis 3 was not supported. Therefore, our hypothetical model was partially supported (see Figure 3), and the index of moderated mediation was -0.038, SE = 0.018, 95% CI = [-0.073, -0.003] (see Model 5 in Table 3A).

Table 3A
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3A. Testing for the moderated mediation model.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Path coefficients of the moderated mediation model. Notes of Figure In this model, We examined the significance of each path and labeled its coefficients, *p< 0.05, ***p< 0.001. β is the unstandardized coefficient.

We further conducted a simple slope analysis to explore the pattern of the moderating effect. The results indicated that perceived stress was negatively correlated with life history strategy for both adolescents with higher distress tolerance (Bsimple = -0.39, p < 0.001) and those with lower distress tolerance (Bsimple = -0.56, p < 0.001). However, bias-corrected percentile bootstrap analysis revealed that the relationship between perceived stress and life history strategy was stronger under low distress tolerance (β = 0.13, SE = 0.03, 95% CI = [0.09, 0.19]) than at the high level of distress tolerance (β = 0.09, SE = 0.02, 95% CI = [0.06, 0.13], as shown in Table 3B. As shown in Figure 4, as an individual moves from low to high perceived stress, the lower distress tolerance is associated with lower life history strategy score than higher distress tolerance level. Given that lower life history strategy scores indicate faster life history strategy, our data suggest that subjects with lower distress tolerance tend to use faster life history strategy as the perceived stress increase.

Table 3B
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3B. Conditional indirect effect of distress tolerance when life history strategy between perceived stress and bedtime procrastination.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Interaction effect of distress tolerance and perceived stress on life history strategy. Notes of Figure High and low scores for perceived stress and distress tolerance are one standard deviation above and below the mean, respectively. A higher life strategy score indicates a tendency to use a slower life strategy; conversely, a lower score indicates a tendency to use a faster life strategy. A higher distress tolerance score indicates a higher distress tolerance; conversely, a lower distress tolerance score indicates a lower distress tolerance.

4 Discussion

The role of stress in inducing bedtime procrastination is a new research topic that has emerged in recent years. However, the underlying psychological mechanism of this phenomenon is still unclear. Perceived stress refers to the imbalance between perceived environmental demands and their ability to cope with those demands (32). When an individual believes that his or her abilities are adequate to cope with environmental demands, perceived stress is low. Conversely, when an individual believes that his or her abilities are insufficient to cope with environmental demands, perceived stress is high. A previous study found that self-efficacy, a subjective experience of the ability to cope with stress, was negatively correlated with bedtime procrastination, and harm avoidance mediated the relationship between self-efficacy and bedtime procrastination (34). In fact, when environmental demands exceed an individual’s ability to cope, harm avoidance behavior has important survival adaptation significance. Therefore, bedtime procrastination may be a positive and proactive adaptive behavior under stressful situations, rather than a maladaptive behavior. From the perspective of life history strategy in evolutionary psychology, this is a fast life history strategy. The present study further explored this idea and found that life history strategies mediated the effect of perceived stress on bedtime procrastination. Furthermore, distress tolerance affected the indirect effect of perceived stress on bedtime procrastination, altering life history strategies by interacting with perceived stress.

4.1 The mediating role of life history strategies in the relationship between perceived stress and bedtime procrastination

The college students are in emerging adulthood. On the one hand, they are in the transition from school to society, with heavy study tasks and confusion about the future; on the other hand, their emotion regulation abilities are immature, despite struggling to hone their emotion regulation abilities. Thus, they often report high levels of perceived stress (63), and may be at risk for mental health problems (64). Consistently, university students had a higher incidence of bedtime procrastination than non-students in Polish (2); the present study found that perceived stress was significantly positively correlated with bedtime procrastination among college students in a Chinese sample. Further, our study extended this idea, finding that life history strategy mediates the relationship between these two variables.

As mentioned above, college students are in a period of heavy study tasks, confusion about the future, and immature emotion regulation skills. In this context, the future is usually full of uncertainty and difficult to predict. That is to say, it is uncertain whether investing in the future will pay off. For college students, it is crucial to resolve excessive psychological pressure from uncertainty about the future and low self-efficacy in stressful situations promptly. Therefore, they focus more on the present, less on the future, manifesting by immersing oneself in relaxing activities before bed, despite knowing there will be side effects on the longer time scale. Ultimately, these behaviors lead to bedtime procrastination. From the perspective of life history strategy, this fast life history strategy may help them to escape the aforementioned excessive emotional distress, is more adaptive. Many previous studies have shown the role of immersive activities before bed in bedtime procrastination (17, 65, 66). Our study provides a possible explanation for why they are prone to engage in these activities, which ultimately leads to bedtime procrastination.

4.2 The moderation of distress tolerance

Distress tolerance is one of the dispositions that underlie individual differences in emotion regulation abilities. Low distress tolerance is characterized by not accepting distress, perceiving it as unbearable and lower abilities to cope with distress than others. Individuals with low distress tolerance use the quickest and easiest methods to boost their mood, regardless of the side effects of their actions (56). Low distress tolerance works in favor of avoidance tendency that prevail in form of procrastination. Therefore, distress tolerance might regulate bedtime procrastination by interacting with perceived stress through two pathways: (1) the indirect effect of perceived stress on the bedtime procrastination through the life history strategy; (2) the direct effect of perceived stress on the bedtime procrastination. Our results support the former hypothesis, but not the latter. This is consistent with previous findings that distress tolerance modulates perceived stress and life history strategies in other forms of disorders (67). The results of the present study further illustrate the role of life history strategy in perceived stress leading to bedtime procrastination. Distress tolerance interacts with perceived stress to further exacerbate bedtime procrastination by altering life history strategy. Therefore, distress tolerance may be an effective potential intervention target for bedtime procrastination. On the other hand, to our knowledge, research on how distress tolerance affects bedtime procrastination is still very rare, their relationship still needs further in-depth exploration.

The present study has the following theoretical and practical contributions. First, it theoretically extends our understanding about the psychological mechanism underling perceived stress inducing bedtime procrastination in a life history framework. Bedtime procrastination might be an active adaptive behavior rather than a passive result of a failure of self-control, particularly in the stressful situations. Distress tolerance did not directly moderate the relationship between perceived stress and bedtime procrastination, but rather indirectly through life history strategies. Secondly, practically the present study suggested that life history-based interventions might be an effective intervention for stress-induced bedtime procrastination. Specifically, it might be helpful for alleviation of bedtime procrastination to develop slow life history strategy, when faced with similar stressful situations in the future. Further, the present study also suggested that distress tolerance skills training might be another potential effective method to relieve bedtime procrastination triggered by perceived stress indirectly by developing slow life history strategy.

4.3 Limitations of this study

Several limitations need to be considered when interpreting the findings. First, our cross-sectional data limited causal inferences. It is necessary to use longitudinal designs to obtain stronger empirical evidence of causal evidence in future research. Second, a questionnaire was used to measure the subjects’ bedtime procrastination, the self-reported method is somewhat subjective and can play an impact on the results of the study. Future studies should enrich the measurement methods to increase objective indicators, for example, by using a bracelet to record the subjects’ sleep circumstances. Finally, although the model hypothesized in this study is partially supported, the effect size is not large. This may be due to few control variables in this study. In addition, it suggests that there are other important factors besides perceived stress that have an impact on bedtime procrastination and future research should attempt to control for them.

5 Conclusion

In summary, we revealed the relationship between perceived stress and bedtime procrastination of college students in the framework of life history strategy and found that life history strategy serves as a mediating role between them. Further, distress tolerance moderated the indirect effect between them mediated by life history strategy, but not the direct effect between them.

Data availability statement

All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in the article/Supplementary Material. For further inquiries, please contact the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the ethics committee of Guizhou Medical University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

QH: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology. HW: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. XM: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing, Funding acquisition, Supervision. CL: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by the Guizhou Medical University start-up fund for doctoral talent (J-[2021]050; J-[2021]051), NSFC Cultivation Project (21NSFCP49; 22NSFCP40), Science and Technology Fund Project of Guizhou Provincial Health Commission (gzwkj2024-130).

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge all participants involved in our research and those who helped in recruiting.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1547389/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Kroese FM, Evers C, Adriaanse MA, de Ridder DT. Bedtime procrastination: A self-regulation perspective on sleep insufficiency in the general population. J Health Psychol. (2014) 21:853–62. doi: 10.1177/1359105314540014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Herzog-Krzywoszanska R, Krzywoszanski L. Bedtime procrastination, sleep-related behaviors, and demographic factors in an online survey on a polish sample. Front Neurosci. (2019) 13:963. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00963

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Yang Y, Gong S, Yao L, Qiu Y, Wang J, Xu X. The relationship between performance goal orientation and bedtime procrastination: the mediation roles of academic stress and self-control. Curr Psychol. (2024) 43:25923–33. doi: 10.1007/s12144-024-06260-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kroese FM, Nauts S, Kamphorst BA, Anderson JH, de Ridder DT. Bedtime procrastination: a behavioral perspective on sleep insufficiency. In: Procrastination, health, and well-being. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier (2016). p. 93–119.

Google Scholar

5. Kadzikowska Wrzosek R. Self-regulation and bedtime procrastination: The role of self-regulation skills and chronotype. Pers Individ Dif. (2018) 128:10–5. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.02.015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Sirois FM, Nauts S, Molnar DS. Self-compassion and bedtime procrastination: an emotion regulation perspective. Mindfulness. (2019) 10:434–45. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-0983-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Cox RC, Olatunji BO. A systematic review of sleep disturbance in anxiety and related disorders. J Anxiety Disord. (2016) 37:104–29. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.12.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Irwin MR. Why sleep is important for health: A psychoneuroimmunology perspective. Annu Rev Psychol. (2015) 66:143–72. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115205

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Meng D, Zhao Y, Guo J, Xu H, Fu Y, Ma X, et al. Time perspective and bedtime procrastination: the role of the Chronotype–time perspective relationship. Nat Sci Sleep. (2021) 13:1307–18. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S313157

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Zhu Y, Huang J, Yang M. Association between chronotype and sleep quality among Chinese college students: the role of bedtime procrastination and sleep hygiene awareness. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 20:197. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20010197

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Mao B, Chen S, Wei M, Luo Y, Liu Y. Future time perspective and bedtime procrastination: the mediating role of dual-mode self-control and problematic smartphone use. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:10334. doi: 10.3390/ijerph191610334

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. An Y, Zhang MX. Relationship between problematic smartphone use and sleep problems: The roles of sleep-related compensatory health beliefs and bedtime procrastination. Digit Health. (2024) 10:20552076241283338. doi: 10.1177/20552076241283338

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Caninsti R, Rahayu SAT. The role of smartphone addiction on bed procrastination and mindful eating behavior in adolescents. Indigen: Jurnal Ilmiah Psikologi. (2024) 9:1–13. doi: 10.23917/indigenous.v9i1.2804

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Chung SJ, An H, Suh S. What do people do before going to bed? A study of bedtime procrastination using time use surveys. Sleep. (2020) 43:zsz267. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz267

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Geng Y, Gu J, Wang J, Zhang R. Smartphone addiction and depression, anxiety: The role of bedtime procrastination and self-control. J Affect Disord. (2021) 293:415–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.06.062

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Jeoung S, Suh S. Smartphone application usage patterns in individuals with high bedtime procrastination: A preliminary study. J Sleep Med. (2020) 17:49–57. doi: 10.13078/jsm.200002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Meng S, Zhang Y, Tan L, Zhang M, Tang W, Onyebuchi N, et al. The effects of mobile phone addiction on bedtime procrastination in university students: The masking effect of physical activity and anxiety. BMC Psychol. (2024) 12:395. doi: 10.1186/s40359-024-01899-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Yang Z, Huang J, Li Z, Xu H, Guo C. The effect of smartphone addiction on the relationship between psychological stress reaction and bedtime procrastination in young adults during the COVID-19 pandemic. BMC Psychiatry. (2023) 23:813. doi: 10.1186/s12888-023-05276-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Zhu Y, Liu J, Wang Q, Huang J, Li X, Liu J. Examining the association between boredom proneness and bedtime procrastination among Chinese college students: A sequential mediation model with mobile phone addiction and negative emotions. Psychol Res Behav Manage. (2023) 16:4329–40. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S431615

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: What is it, and what is it good for? Child Dev Perspect. (2007) 1:68–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2007.00016.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Tanner JL, Arnett JJ. The emergence of emerging adulthood: The new life stage between adolescence and young adulthood. In: Routledge handbook of youth and young adulthood. London, UK: Routledge (2016). p. 50–6.

Google Scholar

22. Arnett JJ. Are college students adults? Their conceptions of the transition to adulthood. J adult Dev. (1994) 1:213–24. doi: 10.1007/BF02277582

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Chen L, Wang L, Qiu XH, Yang XX, Qiao ZX, Yang YJ, et al. Depression among Chinese university students: prevalence and socio-demographic correlates. PloS One. (2013) 8:e58379. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058379

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Lu W, Bian Q, Wang W, Wu X, Wang Z, Zhao M. Chinese version of the Perceived Stress Scale-10: A psychometric study in Chinese university students. PloS One. (2017) 12:e0189543. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0189543

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Kosticova M, Dankulincova Veselska Z, Sokolova L, Dobiášová E. Late bedtime from the perspective of adolescents: A qualitative study. Nat Sci Sleep. (2024) 16:1973–85. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S492595

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Zhuo R. Examining bedtime procrastination through the lens of academic stressors among undergraduate students: academic stressors including mediators of mobile phone addiction and active procrastination. Curr Psychol. (2024) 43:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s12144-024-06038-w

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hill VM, Ferguson SA, Vincent GE, Rebar AL. [amp]]lsquo;It’s satisfying but destructive’: A qualitative study on the experience of bedtime procrastination in new career starters. Br J Health Psychol. (2024) 29:185–203. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12694

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Yan N. Taking a paradoxical perspective on bedtime procrastination. Front Psychol. (2023) 14:1096617. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1096617

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Salas-Rodríguez J, Gómez-Jacinto L, Hombrados-Mendieta MI. Life history theory: Evolutionary mechanisms and gender role on risk-taking behaviors in young adults. Pers Individ Dif. (2021) 175:110752. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110752

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Chen B-B, Chang L. Procrastination as a fast life history strategy. Evol Psychol. (2016) 14:1474704916630314. doi: 10.1177/1474704916630314

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Sear R. Do human ‘life history strategies’ exist? Evol Hum Behav. (2020) 41:513–26. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.09.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Stokols D. A Congruence Analysis of Human Stress. Issues Ment Health Nurs. (1985) 7:35–64. doi: 10.3109/01612848509009449

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hou X, Hu J. Depression and bedtime procrastination: Chain mediation of brooding and perceived stress. Heliyon. (2023) 9:e22672. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2023.e22672

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Meng X, Su H, Li C. Effect of self-efficacy on bedtime procrastination among Chinese university students: A moderation and mediation model. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:863523. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.863523

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Picton A. Work-life balance in medical students: self-care in a culture of self-sacrifice. BMC Med Educ. (2021) 21:1–12. doi: 10.1186/s12909-020-02434-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Yusuf J-EW, Saitgalina M, Chapman DW. Work-life balance and well-being of graduate students. In: Work-Life Balance in Higher Education. London, UK and New York, USA: Routledge (2022). p. 63–88.

Google Scholar

37. Sprung JM, Rogers A. Work-life balance as a predictor of college student anxiety and depression. J Am Coll Health. (2021) 69:775–82. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2019.1706540

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Rodman SA, Daughters SB, Lejuez C. Distress tolerance and rational-emotive behavior therapy: A new role for behavioral analogue tasks. J Rational-Emotive Cognitive-Behavior Ther. (2009) 27:97–120. doi: 10.1007/s10942-009-0090-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Uliaszek AA, Rashid T, Williams GE, Gulamani T. Group therapy for university students: A randomized control trial of dialectical behavior therapy and positive psychotherapy. Behav Res Ther. (2016) 77:78–85. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2015.12.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Yousefi S. Effectiveness of reality therapy group training based on choice theory on distress tolerance and self-compassion in mother with mentally disabled children. Int J Educ Cogn Sci. (2022) 3:44–52. doi: 10.22034/injoeas.2022.160689

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. McHugh RK, Reynolds EK, Leyro TM, Otto MW. An examination of the association of distress intolerance and emotion regulation with avoidance. Cogn Ther Res. (2013) 37:363–7. doi: 10.1007/s10608-012-9463-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Larrazabal Carrillo MA. Distress tolerance predicts day-to-day emotion regulation behaviors. (Publication No. 1563899047) [Master's thesis, College of William & Mary]. W&M ScholarWorks. (2019). doi: 10.21220/s2-b6pm-zy79

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Brown RA, Lejuez C, Strong DR, Kahler CW, Zvolensky MJ, Carpenter LL, et al. A prospective examination of distress tolerance and early smoking lapse in adult self-quitters. Nicot Tobacco Res. (2009) 11:493–502. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntp041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Pilatti A, Michelini Y, Bravo AJ, Pautassi RM. The association between distress tolerance and alcohol outcomes via internal drinking motives. Subst Use Misuse. (2022) 57:230–8. doi: 10.1080/10826084.2021.2002900

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Son M, Jeong G-C. Influence of sensation seeking and life satisfaction expectancy on stock addiction tendency: moderating effect of distress tolerance. Behav Sci. (2023) 13:378. doi: 10.3390/bs13050378

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Clen SL, Mennin DS, Fresco DM. Major depressive disorder. In: Distress tolerance: Theory, research, and clinical applications. The Guilford Press, New York, NY, US (2011). p. 149–70.

Google Scholar

47. Schmidt NB, Mitchell M, Keough M, Riccardi C. Anxiety and its disorders. In: Distress tolerance: Theory, research, and clinical applications. The Guilford Press, New York, NY, US (2011). p. 105–25.

Google Scholar

48. Jonker LC. Stress in a college workplace and its relationship with certain correlates and predictive variables. Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch (2016). Available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10019.1/98482 (Accessed March 9, 2016).

Google Scholar

49. Ellis A, Knaus WJ. Overcoming Procrastination. New York: Institute for Rational Living (1977).

Google Scholar

50. Kroese FM, De Ridder DTD, Evers C, Adriaanse MA. Bedtime procrastination: Introducing a new area of procrastination. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:611–30. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00611

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Ma X, Zhu L, Guo J. Reliability and validity of the bedtime procrastination scale in Chinese college students. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2021) 29:717–20. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2021.04.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. (1983) 24:385. doi: 10.2307/2136404

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Yang T, Huang H. An epidemiological study on stress among urban residents in social transition period. Chin J Epidemiol. (2003) 24:760–4. doi: 10.3760/cma.j.issn.1674-6554.2007.04.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Figueredo A, Vasquez G, Brumbach B, Schneider S, Sefcek J, Tal I, et al. Consilience and Life History Theory: From genes to brain to reproductive strategy. Dev Rev. (2006) 26:243–75. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2006.02.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Sai X, Zhao Y, Geng Y. Revision of the Mini-K scale in Chinese College students. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2022) 30:1160–4. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2022.05.030

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Simons JS, Gaher RM. The distress tolerance scale: development and validation of a self-report measure. Motiv Emotion. (2005) 29:83–102. doi: 10.1007/s11031-005-7955-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Liu X, Gao J. Psychometric properties of the Chinese revised version of distress tolerance scale. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2023) 31:96–101. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2023.01.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee J-Y, Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J Appl Psychol. (2003) 88:879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Kohr RL, Games PA. Robustness of the analysis of variance, the Welch procedure and a Box procedure to heterogeneous variances. J Experimental Educ. (1974) 43(1):61-9. doi: 10.1080/00220973.1974.10806305

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Tang W, Dai Q, Wang G, Hu T, Xu W. Impact of parental absence on insomnia and nightmares in Chinese left-behind adolescents: A structural equation modeling analysis. Children Youth Serv Rev. (2020) 114:105076. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105076

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, USA: Guilford publications (2017).

Google Scholar

62. Hayes A. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. New York, USA: The Guilford Press (2013). p.7.

Google Scholar

63. Karyotaki E, Cuijpers P, Albor Y, Alonso J, Auerbach RP, Bantjes J, et al. Sources of stress and their associations with mental disorders among college students: results of the world health organization world mental health surveys international college student initiative. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:1759. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01759

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. LeBlanc NJ, Brown M, Henin A. Anxiety disorders in emerging adulthood. In: Clinical handbook of anxiety disorders From theory to practice. Cham, Switzerland: Springer (2020). p. 157–73.

Google Scholar

65. Nauts S, Kamphorst BA, Stut W, De Ridder DT, Anderson JH. The explanations people give for going to bed late: a qualitative study of the varieties of bedtime procrastination. Behav sleep Med. (2019) 17:753–62. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2018.1491850

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Oliveira A, Pereira B, Rosário P, Magalhães P. Bedtime procrastination during the second COVID-19 lockdown in Portugal. Sleep Sci. (2022) 15:105–11. doi: 10.5935/1984-0063.20220031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Li C, Gu J, Li Y, Xia B, Meng X. The effect of perceived stress on binge eating behavior among Chinese university students: a moderated mediation model. Front Psychiatry. (2024) 15:1351116. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1351116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: perceived stress, bedtime procrastination, life history strategy, distress tolerance, Chinese college students

Citation: He Q, Wu H, Meng X and Li C (2025) The relationship between perceived stress and bedtime procrastination among Chinese college students: a moderated mediation model. Front. Psychiatry 16:1547389. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1547389

Received: 18 December 2024; Accepted: 18 March 2025;
Published: 09 April 2025.

Edited by:

Yibo Wu, Peking University, China

Reviewed by:

Jinjin Huang, University of Denver, United States
Novina Sabila Zahra, Gadjah Mada University, Indonesia

Copyright © 2025 He, Wu, Meng and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chunlu Li, bGlndWFuMTk4MEAxNjMuY29t; Xiaolu Meng, bXhsbXljQDEyNi5jb20=

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.