Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 18 July 2025

Sec. Psychopathology

Volume 16 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1622626

The sustained effect of 5-week EmotionCore mindfulness training on emotion regulation and emotional intelligence: heterogeneous benefits for depression and anxiety across subgroups

Hui Kou&#x;Hui Kou1†Wei Luo&#x;Wei Luo2†Yiwei WangYiwei Wang3Jia WuJia Wu1Xiaodong LiXiaodong Li1Yi WuYi Wu1Qianguo XiaoQianguo Xiao1Taiyong Bi*Taiyong Bi1*
  • 1Research Center of Humanities and Medicine, Zunyi Medical University, Zunyi, China
  • 2The Institute of Ethnology and Anthropology, Chinese Academy of Social Sciences, Beijing, China
  • 3Art School, Changsha Normal College for Preschool Education, Changsha, China

Background: The present study investigated the sustained effects of a 5-week EmotionCore mindfulness training program on emotion regulation, emotional intelligence (EI), trait mindfulness, depression, and anxiety among 120 undergraduates, while exploring the mediating mechanisms of training effect and heterogeneous effects across subgroups.

Methods: Participants were randomly assigned to a mindfulness group (n=60) or a waitlist control group (n=60), with assessments conducted at baseline (T1), post-training (T2), and one-month follow-up (T3).

Results: Results demonstrated that mindfulness training significantly enhanced adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies (ACERSs), EI, and trait mindfulness at both T2 and T3. Both cross-section and longitudinal mediation models revealed that improvements in ACERSs and EI fully and sequentially mediated the relationship between mindfulness training and trait mindfulness enhancement. Latent Profile Analysis revealed that the high-risk group (high baseline anxiety/depression) exhibited greater reductions in anxiety and depression at T2 and T3 compared to the low-risk group.

Conclusion: These findings suggest EmotionCore mindfulness training fosters trait mindfulness through improvements in ACERSs and EI, and its targeted efficacy is specifically notable within high-risk populations.

Introduction

Mindfulness is a state characterized by intentionally directing attention to present-moment experiences - both internal and external - with a curious, open, receptive, and non-judgmental attitude (1, 2). Mindfulness is not about fighting or controlling thoughts, but rather cultivating an intentional and non-judgmental awareness of the present moment—liberating oneself from the entanglement of ruminating over past experiences and the relentless anxieties of the future (2). Trait/dispositional mindfulness refers to an individual’s characteristic tendency to maintain awareness of the present moment in a nonreactive and nonjudgmental manner (3). Previous studies revealed that mindfulness training is associated with increased trait mindfulness (47). Recently, mindfulness training has been applied in various settings, including clinical psychology, education, and organizational contexts, yielding promising results. Mindfulness training enhances meta-awareness by consciously observing and monitoring thoughts, emotions, and behaviors, empowering individuals to disrupt maladaptively automatic cognition and reaction patterns (810). A large number of studies have demonstrated that mindfulness training can improve mental health and well-being (1117), and reduce emotional problems such as anxiety and depression (12, 1523). In the present study, we developed a program of EmotionCore mindfulness training, to specifically enhance emotional perception and regulation. First, the efficacy of this specialized intervention in improving trait mindfulness was verified. Then, we examined its effect on alleviating anxiety and depression, while evaluating the long-term sustainability of these training benefits, as well as the precise mechanisms underlying these training effects. Existing evidence supports that mindfulness-based interventions often exhibit protective effects over long periods, such as 1-month follow-up (24, 25), 1.5-month follow-up (26), 4-month follow-up (27), 6-month follow-up (28, 29), 12-month follow-up (29), 2.5-year follow-up (30), and even 4-year follow-up (31). Meta-analyses revealed that the effect of mindfulness training may be retained for a period of one to twelve months (32, 33). In the present study, we evaluated the training effect after a one-month break. This period was selected to balance the assessment of immediate post-intervention outcomes with the evaluation of early sustainability, while simultaneously reducing participant attrition rates. We hypothesized that the effect of mindfulness training may retain at the one-month follow-up.

Theories propose that mindfulness training is an effective way to achieve positive psychological outcomes through decentering, disrupting automatic maladaptive cognitive patterns that may be closely associated with emotion regulation. For example, according to the mindful coping model, mindfulness fosters a “decentered” metacognitive state that enhances cognitive flexibility, thereby disrupting maladaptively automatic reactivity and activating adaptive responses (34, 35). Meanwhile, the mindfulness-to-meaning theory further posits that mindfulness can neutralize initial cognitive appraisals for stressors by disrupting the automatic activation of habitual cognition patterns to liberate attention from rumination on stressors, while fostering more flexible and adaptive cognitive processes that encourage positive psychological outcomes (36). Furthermore, the RICH model of mindfulness explicitly delineates four core mechanisms, that is relaxation, insight, contact, and harmony, through which mindfulness exerts its beneficial effects on a number of lower-level intermediate factors, including emotion regulation like rumination and reappraisal, etc., ultimately enhancing psychological well-being (37). Previous studies have demonstrated that emotion regulation (e.g., the ability to manage negative emotions and rumination) plays mediating roles in the relationship between mindfulness and mental health (3841), in the training effects on internalizing symptoms and perceived stress (42), and in the training effects on psychological well-being (43). Among emotion regulation, cognitive emotion regulation (CER) involves the use of a range of conscious cognitive processes that can regulate emotional response, which can be categorized as either adaptive (e.g., acceptance, refocus on planning, positive refocusing, positive reappraisal, and putting into perspective) or nonadaptive (e.g., self-blame, blaming others, rumination or focus on thought, and catastrophizing) strategies (44). Mindfulness is associated with the utilization of cognitive emotion regulation strategies (CERSs). Previous studies revealed that mindfulness was negatively correlated with nonadaptive CERSs as well as positively correlated with adaptive CERSs (45, 46), and both adaptive and nonadaptive CERSs played mediated roles in the relationship between mindfulness and perceived stress (47, 48). More direct evidence revealed that mindfulness training could improve the utilization of adaptive CERSs while reducing the utilization of nonadaptive CERSs (49), such as enhancing cognitive reappraisal and reducing rumination (5052). Thus, we examined the effect of EmotionCore mindfulness training on the utilization of CERSs in the present study. Crucially, we incorporate a longitudinal design with multiple follow-up assessments conducted immediately and 1-month later to examine the stability and persistence of training effects. By employing mediated model analyses, we further investigated how CERSs enhancement mediated the training outcomes.

Emotion regulation is theoretically associated with Emotional Intelligence (EI), with both concepts sharing deep conceptual overlaps in their psychological constructs. EI refers to the ability to perceive and express emotions, integrate emotions into thoughts, understand and utilize emotions for reasoning, as well as regulate one’s own and others’ emotions (53, 54). The Four-Branch Model divides EI into four components: 1) Perceiving emotions, involving recognizing one’s own and other’s emotions, and expressing related needs; 2) Utilizing emotions to facilitate thinking, which refers to the ability to consciously utilize emotions to enhance cognitive processes and problem-solving; 3) Understanding emotions, which encompasses the capacity to understand and deduce the reasons behind emotional shifts within oneself or others and the significance conveyed by emotions; 4) Managing emotions, which refers to the capability to actively regulate their own or others’ emotions (55, 56). Within this framework, emotion regulation seems to emerge as a pivotal component of EI, emphasizing the effective and flexible application of emotion regulation strategies. Empirical studies revealed that EI could positively predict CERSs like reappraisal (5759), and correlated with or predicted reduced emotion regulation difficulties (60, 61). Trait EI facilitated adaptive emotion regulation strategy selection (62). It could positively predict reappraisal (63), and negatively predict the frequency of use of typical dysfunctional emotion regulation strategies (64). Direct evidence revealed that EI training significantly influenced CER (65) and positive reappraisal (66). However, some researchers found that CERS could predict trait EI (67). Collectively, both emotion regulation and EI are involved in the multidimensional process of emotion management, and the flexible utilization and selection of emotion regulation strategies may substantially affect one’s capacity to effectively manage emotions across various contexts. Additionally, mindfulness has been found to be closely associated with EI (6870); and mindfulness training could increase the level of EI (7173). Therefore, it’s plausible to hypothesize that the EmotionCore mindfulness training first improved the utilization of emotion regulation strategies, subsequently enhancing EI.

According to the Whole Trait Theory (74), traits are divided into explanatory and descriptive parts, which are causally related. The descriptive side of traits is characterized by density distributions of states. Therefore, fluctuations in states may influence traits. Meanwhile, the explanatory part consists of mechanisms that produce traits, including cognition; and social-cognitive processes can explain density distributions of states (74). Therefore, it is reasonable to assume that the developmental pathway of mindfulness training involves cognitive shifts related to the emotions preceding, thereby consolidating the trait mindfulness. We thus hypothesize that the 5-week EmotionCore mindfulness training initially improves adaptive CERSs and EI, and subsequently enhances trait mindfulness.

Although mindfulness training is shown to enhance emotion regulation and EI, the associations between these enhancements and their roles in the training effect on mindfulness remain unclear. In the present study, we first examined the impact of 5-week EmotionCore mindfulness training on the utilization of CERSs, EI, trait mindfulness, anxiety, and depression. Then, we further examined whether the improvements of CERSs and EI played sequential mediating roles in the relationship between mindfulness training and trait mindfulness enhancement. At last, subgroups with different characteristics were identified to examine the targeted effects of EmotionCore mindfulness training through latent profile analysis. The present design advances our knowledge of mindfulness training in three aspects. First, the self-designed “EmotionCore mindfulness training” which targets emotional perception and regulation differs from traditional mindfulness training which often emphasizes generalized attention cultivation, providing a more targeted intervention protocol for emotion-related problems. Second, we adopt a longitudinal mediating model to infer the causal mechanisms underlying mindfulness training, elucidating the hierarchical change process in mindfulness-based interventions. Third, we applied the latent profile model to a group which is previously considered homogeneous, and revealed the heterogeneity in training sensitivity, providing empirical support for targeted interventions.

Methods

Participants

Power analysis indicated that a total sample size of 102 participants was needed to detect a significant difference in the training effect between groups (α=0.05, effect size=0.5, power= 0.8). A total of 120 undergraduates were recruited via online advertisements and randomly assigned to either the mindfulness group, which underwent an intensive 5-week mindfulness training program designed for emotional regulation, or the waitlist control group, which participated in two introductory lectures on mindfulness concepts. No significant age differences were observed between the mindfulness group (M=20.27, SD=1.30, ranging from 18 to 24) and the control group (M=19.97, SD=0.88, ranging from 19 to 24) (t(118) =1.48, p=0.142). No significant sex-based differences were observed between the mindfulness group (50 females) and the control group (42 females) (χ²=2.98, df=1, p=0.084).

Inclusion criteria: Undergraduate students aged 18–24, fluent in Chinese, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

Exclusion criteria: Self-reported history of neurological/psychiatric disorders; self-reported anxiety/depressive disorders; current use of psychotropic medications; prior experience with formal mindfulness training or meditation practices.

Ethics approval statement

All procedures conducted in this study involving human participants are approved by the Ethical Committee of Human Research at Zunyi Medical University. Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the experiment. After completing the experiment, control group members were also offered mindfulness training if they volunteered to participate.

Measurements

The Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) It was employed to assess participants’ mindfulness levels, developed by Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer and Toney (75). This 39-item questionnaire comprises five subscales: Observing (e.g., “When I take a shower or bath, I stay alert to the sensations of water on my body”), Describing (e.g., “I can easily put my beliefs, opinions, and expectations into words”), Acting with awareness (e.g., “When I do things, my mind wanders off and I’m easily distracted”), Non-judging (e.g., “I tell myself I shouldn’t be feeling the way I’m feeling”) and Non-reacting (e.g., “When I have distressing thoughts or images, I just notice them and let them go”). Participants rated each item on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1=very rarely true to 5=always true. The Chinese version of the FFMQ demonstrated a Cronbach’s α coefficient of 0.70 in non-clinical populations (76). In the present study, the Cronbach’s α coefficient is 0.820.

The Emotional Intelligence Scale (EIS) It was developed by Schutte, Malouff, Hall, Haggerty, Cooper, Golden and Dornheim (77) to assess individuals’ capacity to perceive, understand, control, and manage emotion. This 33 items scale comprises four subscales: Appraisal of other’s emotion (e.g., “Other people find it easy to confide in me”), Appraisal and Expression of Own Emotion (e.g., “I know when to speak about my personal problems to others”), Regulation of Emotion (e.g., “When my mood changes, I see new possibilities”), and Utilization of Emotion (e.g., “Emotions are one of the things that make my life worth living”). Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1=strongly disagree to 5=strongly agree. The EIS demonstrated a Cronbach’s α of 0.90 and a two-week test-retest reliability coefficient of 0.78 (77). In the present study, the Cronbach’s α coefficient is 0.852.

The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) It was initially developed by Garnefski, Kraaij and Spinhoven (44) and was subsequently translated and revised into Chinese version by Zhu, Luo, Yao, P.Auerbach and JohnR.Z.Abela (78). The questionnaire consists of 36 items and nine subscales. These subscales evaluate a range of strategies employed in cognitive emotion regulation, including self-blame, blaming others, acceptance, refocus on planning, positive refocusing, rumination or focus on thought, positive reappraisal, putting into perspective, and catastrophizing. Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1=strongly disagree to 5=strongly agree, with higher subscale scores reflecting more frequent use of that specific strategy during negative experiences. The adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies (CERSs) include acceptance, putting into perspective, refocusing on planning, positive refocusing, and positive reappraisal subscales. The nonadaptive CERSs consist of self-blame, blaming others, rumination or focus on thought, and catastrophizing subscales. In the Chinese population, the subscale reliability ranged from 0.48 to 0.89 (78). In the present study, the Cronbach’s α coefficient is 0.896.

The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) This 10-item measure was developed by Gross and John (79). It covers two factors: Cognitive Reappraisal (e.g., I control my emotions by changing the way I think about the situation I’m in) and Expressive Suppression (e.g., I control my emotions by not expressing them). Each item is rated on a 7-point Likert scale from 1=strongly disagree to 7=strongly agree. The Cronbach’s α coefficient was 0.79 for Cognitive Reappraisal and 0.73 for Expressive Suppression, and the test-retest reliability across 3 months was 0.69 for both scales (79). In the present study, the Cronbach’s α coefficient is 0.711.

Self-Rating Depression Scale (SDS) (80) This 20-item measurement is designed to assess the frequency of depressive symptoms experienced over the past seven days. Each item is rated on a 4-point Likert scale from 1 =none or a little of the time to 4= most or all of the time. The cumulative score reflects symptom severity. In the present study, the Cronbach’s α coefficient is 0.638.

Self-Rating Anxiety Scale (SAS) (81) This 20-item measurement is used to assess the frequency of anxious symptoms experienced over the past seven days. Each item is rated on a 4-point Likert scale from 1 =none or a little of the time to 4= most or all of the time. The cumulative score reflects anxiety severity. In the present study, the Cronbach’s α coefficient is 0.680.

Procedure

Participants first completed the baseline measurement (T1), filling in all the questionnaires.

Following the baseline assessment, participants were randomly assigned to either the training group, which underwent mindfulness training sessions, or the control group, which participated in two introductory lectures on mindfulness concepts. Participants in the training group engaged systematically in a five-week mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) program. The training was conducted in group-based sessions comprising 30 participants per group, guided by a therapist with three years of expertise in MBSR instruction and sustained meditation practice. The EmotionCore curriculum was designed based on Kabat-Zinn’s foundational MBSR framework (2) and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) (82), and focused on emotional perception and regulation. From MBSR, we adopted foundational practices like mindful eating (to enhance present-moment sensory awareness) and body scans (to cultivate non-judgmental bodily awareness), which form the basis for emotional perception. From MBCT, We integrated emotion-cognition link exercises (e.g., exploring how thoughts shape emotional responses) and non-reactivity training (to reduce automatic emotional reactions), aligning with MBCT’s focus on breaking maladaptive cognitive-emotional cycles. Each week set an emotion-centered theme through a two-hour session, with detailed training content outlined in Table 1. Unlike MBSR (which targets general stress reduction) or MBCT (primarily for depression relapse prevention), EmotionCore is explicitly emotion-centric. Its weekly themes (e.g., “mindfulness and emotional recognition,” “mindfulness and emotion regulation”) systematically train participants to identify emotional components (cognitive appraisal, physiological arousal, behavioral responses; Week 2), understand emotion-cognition dynamics (Week 3), identify the experience and body responses to emotion (Week 4), and apply flexible regulation strategies (Week 5). This structure differentiates EmotionCore by prioritizing emotional perception and adaptive regulation, making it more suitable for populations with subclinical emotional distress (e.g., college students). After the training, participants undertook daily mindfulness exercises at home, and reported their feelings to the trainer. All participants allocated to the mindfulness training group successfully completed the five scheduled training sessions.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The contents of weekly mindfulness training.

Immediately following the training session, participants underwent post-training assessments (T2) same as the baseline measurements. These identical measurements were repeated one month post-training (T3) to track longitudinal outcomes.

Once all measurements were concluded, participants in the control group were also offered the opportunity to participate in mindfulness training if they wished.

Statistical analysis

First, we examined the differences in scale scores between groups at T1 with a MANOVA, to test if there were any group biases at the baseline level. Next, independent-sample t-tests were conducted on the improvement of measures (i.e., T2-T1 or T3-T1), with the group serving as the independent variable.

Second, cross-sectional mediation models were constructed based on previous studies (8385), with mindfulness training (0=control group, 1=mindfulness group) as the predictive variables, the improvements of adaptive CERSs and EI at T2/T3 as the mediators, and the improvements of trait mindfulness at T2/T3 as outcome variables. Furthermore, longitudinal mediation effects could be revealed in longitudinal designs. Multiple longitudinal mediation models have been proposed for mediation analysis, including the cross-lagged panel mediation models (86), the latent growth mediation models (87, 88), and the latent change score mediation models (89). The cross-lagged panel model can be used with at least three waves of measurement to achieve a fully longitudinal mediation model. This methodological approach defines mediation as a longitudinal process considering embedded causal sequences: a hypothesized predictor is measured prior to the hypothesized mediator, and the hypothesized mediator is measured prior to the hypothesized outcome variable (86). Therefore, a cross-lagged mediation model was based on the latent change scores, with mindfulness training (0=control group, 1=mindfulness group) serving as the independent variable, the improvements of adaptive CERSs and EI at T2 as mediators, and the improvement of FFMQ at T3 as the dependent variable. The improvements were all transformed into standardized scores before entering the models. The PROCESS macro (Model 6) for SPSS provided by Preacher and Hayes (84) was used to test the mediating effects.

Finally, Latent Profile Analysis (LPA) was conducted by Mplus 8.3 using maximum likelihood estimation. LPA is a categorical latent variable approach designed to identify individuals from a heterogeneous population into homogenous subgroups, within a subgroup who often exhibit shared observable characteristics, by analyzing shared patterns in their responses to a defined set of observed variables (90, 91). In the present study, LPA aimed to identify heterogeneity in intervention response, i.e., differences in training effects among participants with varying baseline characteristics. Since the waitlist group received no training (only lectures), LPA in this group could not reflect the specific effects of the intervention. Therefore, LPA was conducted only within the mindfulness group. Eight continuous variables were included: FFMQ, EI, ERQ-CR (Cognitive Reappraisal), ERQ-ES (Expressive Suppression), SDS, SAS, adaptive CERSs, and nonadaptive CERSs at T1. To avoid local maxima, 500 random start values with 50 final stage optimizations were implemented to ensure proper convergence. The optimal class was determined through a comprehensive evaluation of fit indices including the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), Sample-Size Adjusted BIC (aBIC), Lo-Mendell-Rubin adjusted likelihood ratio test (LMR-LRT), and bootstrap likelihood ratio test (BLRT), with Entropy values assessed for classification quality.

Results

Baseline level

We conducted a comprehensive MANOVA to examine scores across multiple psychological measures: FFMQ, EI, adaptive CERSs, nonadaptive CERSs, ERQ-CR, ERQ-ES, SAS, and SDS. As illustrated in Supplementary Table S1, the analysis revealed no significant differences between the mindfulness group and the control group (Fs < 2.45, ps > 0.120), indicating that the two groups were well-matched at the baseline level.

Post-training and follow-up

The scores of all measurements at T2 and T3 were displayed in Supplementary Table S2.

We first calculated the improvements in measurements by subtracting the scores at T1 from the scores at T2 or T3 (Table 2). Independent-sample t-tests on the improvements of measurements revealed that the mindfulness group demonstrated greater improvements in trait mindfulness (T2: t(118)=-2.72, p=0.008; T3: t(118)=-2.08, p=0.039), EI (T2: t(118)=-2.22, p=0.029; T3: t(118)=-2.53, p=0.013), and adaptive CERSs (T2: t(118)=-3.47, p=0.001; T3: t(118)=-3.07, p=0.003) at both T2 and T3. The mindfulness group also demonstrated greater improvements in anxiety (t(118)=2.10, p=0.038) and expressive suppression (t(118)=2.39, p=0.019), but only at T2. However, there weren’t any significant improvements in depression, cognitive reappraisal, and nonadaptive CERSs at both T2 and T3 (all ps>0.05). These results indicate sustained improvements in trait mindfulness, EI, and adaptive CERSs.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Between-group differences in the improvements of all measurements at T2 and T3: descriptive statistics and results of independent-sample t-test (N=120).

Mediation models

As there are only sustained training effects on FFMQ, EI and adaptive CERSs, the following analyses are based on these measurements.

Bivariate correlation analysis was conducted on the improvements of measurements at T2 and T3, as shown in Table 3. Generally, there are positive correlations among all the measurements. Therefore, we further test the mediation roles of adaptive CERSs and EI on the training effect of trait mindfulness by mediation effect analysis.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Bivariate correlations between the improvements of measurements (N=120).

First, cross-sectional mediation models were constructed, with mindfulness training (0=control group, 1=mindfulness group) serving as the independent variable, the improvements of adaptive CERSs and EI at T2 or T3 as mediators, and the improvement of FFMQ at T2 or T3 as the dependent variable (Figure 1). The modeling results at T2 and T3 by the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Model 6) showed that, there were significant total effects (bootstrap test with 2000 samples, T2: β=0.48, t=2.72, p=0.008; T3: β=0.38, t=2.08, p=0.039), but the direct effects were nonsignificant (T2: β=0.29, t=1.65, p=0.101; T3: β=0.15, t=0.93, p=0.355). Importantly, the sequential mediation effects were significant (T2: bootstrap95%CI [0.01, 0.13]; T3: bootstrap95%CI [0.04, 0.26]), indicating that enhancements in adaptive CERSs and EI could fully and sequentially mediate the impact of mindfulness training on trait mindfulness at both T2 and T3. Moreover, the separate mediating effects of the improvements of adaptive CERSs (T2: bootstrap95%CI [-0.05, 0.19]; T3: bootstrap95%CI [-0.09, 0.11]) and EI (T2: bootstrap95%CI [-0.04, 0.18]; T3: bootstrap95%CI [-0.07, 0.21]) were nonsignificant.

Figure 1
Two path diagrams labeled A and B illustrate the effects of mindfulness training. Diagram A shows paths from mindfulness training to changes in ACERS, EI, and TM between T2 and T1, with significant paths including 0.61 to ACERS, 0.31 from ACERS to EI, and 0.29 from EI to TM. Diagram B shows paths for T3 to T1 with significant paths including 0.54 to ACERS, 0.53 from ACERS to EI, and 0.47 from EI to TM. Dashed lines indicate non-significant paths.

Figure 1. The sequential mediation effects of enhancements in adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies (ACERSs) and emotional intelligence (EI) in the relationship between mindfulness training and trait mindfulness (TM) at both T2 (A) and T3 (B) (Δ: Improvement; ***, p<0.001; **p<0.01).

Then, cross-lagged mediation models were constructed, with mindfulness training (0=control group, 1=mindfulness group) serving as the independent variable, the improvements of adaptive CERSs and EI at T2 as mediators, and the improvement of FFMQ at T3 as the dependent variable (Figure 2). The modeling results by the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Model 6) showed that, there were significant total effects (bootstrap test with 2000 samples, β=0.38, t=2.08, p=0.039), but the direct effects were nonsignificant (β=0.24, t=1.31, p=0.192). Importantly, the sequential mediation effect was still significant (bootstrap95%CI [0.004, 0.12]), indicating that enhancements in adaptive CERSs and EI at T2 could fully and sequentially mediate the impact of mindfulness training on trait mindfulness at T3. Meanwhile, the separate mediating effects of the improvements of both adaptive CERSs (bootstrap95%CI [-0.10, 0.16]) and EI (bootstrap95%CI [-0.04, 0.16]) were nonsignificant.

Figure 2
Flowchart showing relationships between mindfulness training and various outcomes. Mindfulness training influences ΔACERSs_T2-T1 (0.61***), which affects ΔEI_T2-T1 (0.31***). ΔEI_T2-T1 impacts ΔTM_T3-T1 (0.24*). Mindfulness training also has direct influences on ΔTM_T3-T1 (0.24) and ΔEI_T2-T1 (0.21), with other non-significant connections. Solid and dashed lines indicate significant and non-significant paths, respectively.

Figure 2. The longitudinal mediation effect of enhancements in adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies (ACERSs) and emotional intelligence (EI) at T2 in the relationship between mindfulness training and enhancements in trait mindfulness (TM) at T3 (Δ: Improvement; ***, p<0.001; *p<0.05).

Latent profile analysis for mindfulness group

In the present study, we did not find significant training effects on emotional symptoms, which may be inconsistent with our hypothesis. We conducted Latent Profile Analysis (LPA) using Mplus 8.3 to classify the members in the mindfulness group into subgroups.

Given the exploratory nature of LPA where the optimal number of latent classes remains undetermined a priori, it is necessary to systematically compare and evaluate competing models with progressively increasing class numbers. This evaluation should integrate statistical criteria, theoretical assumptions, sample size, as well as the interpretability and uniqueness of the classes (92, 93). Thus, we calculated the model fitness from 1 class to 6 classes based on the eight variables at baseline. Critical model fit evaluation information for these class solutions is presented in Table 4. The results revealed consistent declines in AIC, aBIC, and LMR-LRT values alongside uniformly significant BLRT p-values (<0.001), collectively failing to conclusively identify an optimal solution. The LMR-LRT for the two-class model, and the BIC and Entropy for the three-class model suggested good model fit. Considering sample distribution, we opted for the two-class solution due to its balanced distribution (Class 1: n=30; Class 2: n=30), contrasting with the three-class model (Class 1: n=26; Class 2: n=27; Class 3: n=7). Overall, the two-class solution effectively categorized participants in the mindfulness group, with the average latent class probabilities for the most likely latent class membership being 0.99 for Class 1 and 0.96 for Class 2.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Model fit evaluation information.

Subsequently, independent-sample t-tests on the baseline measurements were conducted, based on the two-class grouping. As illustrated in Table 5, participants in Class 1 demonstrated significantly higher trait mindfulness and EI, coupled with lower expressive suppression, depression, anxiety, and nonadaptive CERSs. Thus, the first class could be named as the low-risk group, while the second class could be named as the high-risk group.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Differences across the two classes in terms of the eight variables at baseline: descriptive statistics and results of independent-sample t-test (N=60).

Then, independent-sample t-tests were conducted on the improvements of measurements at T2 and T3. As illustrated in Table 6, participants in Class 2 exhibited significant or marginally significant reductions in depression and anxiety at both T2 and T3 relative to Class 1. It indicates mindfulness training’s targeted efficacy specifically within high-risk populations such as anxious and depressive individuals. It should also be noted that there is no significant difference in improvements in most of the emotional regulations between groups, indicating similar sensitivity to mindfulness training on emotional regulation for the two subgroups.

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Differences across the two classes in terms of the improvements of eight measurements at T2 and T3: descriptive statistics and results of independent-sample t-test (N=60).

Discussion

The present study, utilizing a randomized controlled trial design with 120 participants, initially revealed that a 5-week EmotionCore mindfulness training significantly improved adaptive CERSs, EI, and trait mindfulness, with effects lasting at least one month post-training. Furthermore, both cross-section and longitudinal mediation analysis revealed that the improvements in adaptive CERSs and EI completely and sequentially mediated the relationship between mindfulness training and the enhancement of trait mindfulness. Lastly, when implementing LPA, the high-risk group with high levels of anxiety and depression at baseline demonstrated greater reductions in anxiety and depression compared to the low-risk group, suggesting differential intervention effectiveness based on initial symptom severity.

In our study, we revealed long-lasting training effects of the innovative 5-week EmotionCore mindfulness program on multiple measurements, including adaptive CERSs, EI, and trait mindfulness. These findings are consistent with previous studies showing that mindfulness training improved the utilization of adaptive CERSs (49, 94, 95), enhanced EI (7173, 9699), and fortified trait mindfulness (100106). However, no long-lasting improvements in nonadaptive CERSs, expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal were found in the present study. It may be partly inconsistent with previous studies demonstrating that mindfulness training reduces the utilization of nonadaptive CERSs (49, 94, 95) and improves rumination, catastrophizing, cognitive reappraisal, and expressive suppression (5052, 107110). Notably, our program led to enhanced adaptive CERSs. The differential outcomes for cognitive appraisal and adaptive CERSs in the present study may reflect differences in conceptual structures stemming from measurement tool variances. Cognitive reappraisal is defined as a form of cognitive change that involves construing a potentially emotion-eliciting situation in a way that changes its emotional impact (111), while adaptive CERSs refers to thoughts of attaching a positive meaning to the event in terms of personal growth (44). By comparison, adaptive CERSs emphasize optimistic reinterpretation over mere cognitive shifts. It appears that the EmotionCore mindfulness training is more effective in cultivating positive cognitive reframing. The findings reinforce theoretical frameworks positing mindfulness-driven cognitive shifts through decentering mechanisms (3437, 112), which encourages positive psychological outcomes. Nevertheless, it is worth investigating if there are other factors influencing the training effect on cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression.

Interestingly, both cross-section and longitudinal mediation models revealed that adaptive CERSs and EI are the mediating pathways through which EmotionCore mindfulness training exerts its effects on trait mindfulness. It appears that the 5-week EmotionCore mindfulness training initially changes adaptive CERSs, and then enhances EI, indicating the developmental pathway that cognition shift in emotion experiences precede, thereby facilitating the growth of emotional competencies. These findings support the process model of emotion regulation, specifying the sequence of steps involved in emotion generation (113, 114). The first step is situation selection and modification. Selection refers to taking actions that make it more (or less) likely that one will be in an expected situation that will give rise to desirable (or undesirable) emotions, while modification refers to taking actions that directly alter a situation in order to change its emotional impact. The second step is attentional deployment, which refers to directing one’s attention with the goal of influencing one’s emotional response. The third step is cognitive change, which refers to modifying one’s appraisal of a situation in order to alter its emotional impact. The fourth step is response modulation, which refers to directly influencing experiential, behavioral, or physiological components of the emotional response after the emotion is well developed. Previous studies have revealed that EI is mainly correlated with positive reappraisal (115), and positive refocusing (116). Other studies showed that adaptive CERSs positively predicted EI (116) and CERS predicted trait EI (67), consistent with the present study. Therefore, it is plausible that only by learning and flexibly applying emotion regulation strategies can one enhance their emotion regulation ability, a component of EI.

Sequentially, the improvement of EI led to enhanced trait mindfulness. According to the four-component model of mindfulness, four components are proposed to describe the mechanism through which mindfulness training exerts its effects: 1) attention regulation, 2) body awareness, 3) emotion regulation, and 4) change in perspective on the self (117). When emotional reactions are triggered, the executive attention system could detect conflict with mindful state maintenance and physiological aspects of feelings, enabling accurate emotional response identification. Emotion regulation processes then engage to replace habitual reactions, which facilitates response prevention through nonreactivity, leading to extinction/reconsolidation. This allows individuals to experience the transient nature of perceptions/emotions/cognitions rather than habitual reactions. Awareness of this transience alters the perspective on the self, which refers to a change in perspective about the sense of self and an alteration in first-person subjective experience, described as observer perspective. Evidence has exhibited that emotion regulation, body awareness, and change of self appeared to be the most important mechanisms of action through which mindfulness exerts its beneficial effects on mental health (118, 119). Therefore, after mindfulness training improves adaptive CRESs and EI, the perspective on the self is changed, correlated with changes in trait mindfulness. Based on the present and previous findings, we propose that the EmotionCore mindfulness training may initiate its effects on the utilization of emotion regulation strategies through enhanced meta-awareness, subsequently fostering improvements in EI. Crucially, EI enhancement may then reciprocally strengthen trait mindfulness, thereby establishing a positive feedback loop. Further, the full mediation effect seems to indicate that mindful emotion training cannot directly improve trait mindfulness.

Surprisingly, we did not find long-lasting training effects of the 5-week EmotionCore mindfulness training on anxiety or depression in the first place. This is unusual as numerous previous studies have shown strong evidence that mindfulness training is an effective intervention for emotional disorders (12, 16–23, 15). Therefore, it is quite necessary to account for the negative result of the emotional symptoms. We note that the effectiveness of mindfulness training may be different across different populations. Meta-analyses revealed that among healthy individuals, healthcare professionals benefited most from mindfulness training, followed by general populations and then students (120). Studies showed lower benefits observed in student populations compared to general populations or working adults (121), and the intervention effectiveness of mindfulness training for clinical samples was better than that for healthy individuals in Eastern countries (122). In the present study, the participants are healthy undergraduates who have relatively low depressive and anxious levels, and exhibit relatively lower sensitivity to mindfulness training in previous studies. The floor effect may prevent further reductions in emotional symptoms. The present LPA results revealed that the training efficacy of EmotionCore mindfulness training for both anxiety and depression was moderated by the levels of emotional health at baseline. The high-risk group, characterized by higher levels of anxiety and depression at baseline, exhibited symptom reduction at both T2 and T3 follow-ups compared to their low-risk counterparts. This pattern resonates with the “floor effect” hypothesis in psychotherapeutic interventions (123). These findings highlight the EmotionCore mindfulness training program’s targeted therapeutic potency for the emotional symptoms among high-risk populations and its general efficacy for enhancing emotional regulation.

Limitations

First, the study focused solely on undergraduate populations, which may limit the generalization of its conclusions. Future research should investigate the efficacy of EmotionCore mindfulness training and its mechanisms across diverse groups, including clinical and cross-cultural samples.

Second, key variables such as emotion regulation relied on self-report measures, making them susceptible to response biases (e.g., social desirability) and inadequate for capturing objective behavioral or physiological indicators. For example, heart rate variability is closely associated with emotion regulation (124, 125); amygdala–prefrontal cortex connectivity is closely associated with mindfulness (126129). Subsequent studies should incorporate multimodal assessments such as experimental emotion-regulation paradigms and neuroimaging to further verify the impact of EmotionCore mindfulness training on emotion regulation and its underlying neural mechanisms.

Third, the current research merely assessed the sustained effects of EmotionCore training at a one-month follow-up. In the future, conducting longitudinal studies is imperative to thoroughly explore the sustained efficacy of this training through extended follow-up periods.

Fourth, the latent profile analysis primarily focused on baseline emotional problems, emotion regulation strategies, emotional intelligence, and trait mindfulness, overlooking potential moderators like personality traits and environmental factors. Future research should identify supplementary moderators to enhance personalized approaches for high-risk subgroups while establishing a hierarchical intervention framework that tailors training programs to participants’ distinct psychological profiles.

Fifth, the lack of control over participants’ spontaneous mindfulness practice during the follow-up period introduces a limitation. While we could not restrict such practices (due to ethical considerations), their potential influence on T3 outcomes should be acknowledged. Evidence revealed that the majority of participants receiving an 8-week mindfulness training demonstrated ongoing compliance with the mindfulness practice at 3 years (130). Future research may explore the role of habitual practice in the long-term effects.

Lastly, the waitlist control group was administered two introductory lectures on mindfulness concepts in this study, whereas the training group participated in a 5-week systematic mindfulness intervention. Although preliminary analyses revealed no significant between-group differences in key baseline variables, someone may argue that the difference in improvements between groups may stem from factors other than mindfulness. A meta-analysis study suggested that active (including guided breathing, health education, lifestyle education, math, nutrition classes, reading groups, relaxation, or sitting in silence) and passive (including simple test-retest or waitlist controls) control groups produced statistically indistinguishable results, indicating that mindfulness is a crucial ingredient in producing the training effects (131). Nevertheless, future studies could use an active control group to more rigorously control non-specific effects.

Conclusions

This study provides robust evidence for the efficacy of a 5-week EmotionCore mindfulness training program in immediately and sustainably enhancing adaptive CERSs, EI, and trait mindfulness among undergraduates. Critically, the findings elucidate the full and sequential mediating role of adaptive CERSs and EI in linking mindfulness training to trait mindfulness, supporting a developmental pathway in that cognitive regulatory capacities precede emotional competencies. Notably, the training demonstrated targeted benefits for high-risk individuals with elevated baseline anxiety and depressive symptoms, who exhibited greater reductions in anxiety and depression compared to the low-risk group.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethical Committee of Human Research at Zunyi Medical University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

HK: Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft. WL: Validation, Methodology, Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Investigation, Resources, Funding acquisition. YWa: Writing – original draft, Methodology, Visualization, Formal analysis. JW: Writing – original draft, Formal analysis, Data curation, Visualization, Investigation. XL: Writing – original draft, Investigation, Formal analysis. YWu: Investigation, Writing – original draft. QX: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. TB: Writing – review & editing, Funding acquisition, Resources, Conceptualization, Supervision.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the Guizhou Provincial Science and Technology Projects [Basic Science of Guizhou -(2024) Youth 309, Guizhou platform talents(2021) 1350-046], Zunyi Science and Technology Cooperation (HZ(2024)311), Funding of the Chinese Academy of Social Sciences (2024SYZH005), Peking University Longitudinal Scientific Research Technical Service Project (G252).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1622626/full#supplementary-material.

References

1. Academic Mindfulness Interest Group M and M. Academic Mindfulness Interest Group. Mindfulness-based psychotherapies: A review of conceptual foundations, empirical evidence and practical considerations. Aust New Z J Psychiatry. (2006) 40:285–94. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2006.01794.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Kabat-Zinn J. Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom books of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York, NY: Delta Books (1990).

Google Scholar

3. Carpenter JK, Conroy K, Gomez AF, Curren LC, and Hofmann SG. The relationship between trait mindfulness and affective symptoms: A meta-analysis of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ). Clin Psychol Rev. (2019) 74:101785. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101785

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Quaglia JT, Braun SE, Freeman SP, McDaniel MA, and Brown KW. Meta-analytic evidence for effects of mindfulness training on dimensions of self-reported dispositional mindfulness. psychol Assess. (2016) 28:803–18. doi: 10.1037/pas0000268

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Kiken LG, Garland EL, Bluth K, Palsson OS, and Gaylord SA. From a state to a trait: Trajectories of state mindfulness in meditation during intervention predict changes in trait mindfulness. Pers Individ Dif. (2015) 81:41–6. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Shapiro SL, Brown KW, Thoresen C, and Plante TG. The moderation of Mindfulness-based stress reduction effects by trait mindfulness: Results from a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Psychol. (2011) 67:267–77. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20761

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Sakagami M, Yokono O, Abeywickrama HM, Seki N, Miyasaka M, Uchiyama M, et al. Changes in trait mindfulness after a brief mindfulness training program of self-breathing. Healthcare. (2024) 12:2019. doi: 10.3390/healthcare12202019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Christoff Hadjiilieva K. Mindfulness as a way of reducing automatic constraints on thought. Biol Psychiatry: Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2025) 10:393–401. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2024.11.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kang Y, Gruber J, and Gray JR. Mindfulness and de-automatization. Emotion Rev. (2013) 5:192–201. doi: 10.1177/1754073912451629

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kang Y, Gruber J, and Gray JR. The wiley blackwell handbook of mindfulness. (The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester: John Wiley and Sons, Ltd) (2014). pp. 168–85.

Google Scholar

11. Carsley D, Khoury B, and Heath NL. Effectiveness of mindfulness interventions for mental health in schools: a comprehensive meta-analysis. Mindfulness. (2018) 9:693–707. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0839-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Dunning DL, Griffiths K, Kuyken W, Crane C, Foulkes L, Parker J, et al. Research Review: The effects of mindfulness-based interventions on cognition and mental health in children and adolescents – a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2019) 60:244–58. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12980

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Gu J, Strauss C, Bond R, and Cavanagh K. How do mindfulness-based cognitive therapy and mindfulness-based stress reduction improve mental health and wellbeing? A systematic review and meta-analysis of mediation studies. Clin Psychol Rev. (2015) 37:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Kallapiran K, Koo S, Kirubakaran R, and Hancock K. Review: Effectiveness of mindfulness in improving mental health symptoms of children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Child Adolesc Ment Health. (2015) 20:182–94. doi: 10.1111/camh.12113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Shankland R, Tessier D, Strub L, Gauchet A, and Baeyens C. Improving mental health and well-being through informal mindfulness practices: an intervention study. Appl Psychology: Health Well-Being. (2021) 13:63–83. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12216

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Spijkerman MPJ, Pots WTM, and Bohlmeijer ET. Effectiveness of online mindfulness-based interventions in improving mental health: A review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Clin Psychol Rev. (2016) 45:102–14. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.03.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Zhu JL, Schülke R, Vatansever D, Xi D, Yan J, Zhao H, et al. Mindfulness practice for protecting mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. Trans Psychiatry. (2021) 11:329. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01459-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Fulambarkar N, Seo B, Testerman A, Rees M, Bausback K, Bunge E, et al. Review: Meta-analysis on mindfulness-based interventions for adolescents’ stress, depression, and anxiety in school settings: a cautionary tale. Child Adolesc Ment Health. (2023) 28:307–17. doi: 10.1111/camh.12572

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Johannsen M, Nissen ER, Lundorff M, and O’Toole MS. Mediators of acceptance and mindfulness-based therapies for anxiety and depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. (2022) 94:102156. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2022.102156

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Maddock A and Blair C. How do mindfulness-based programmes improve anxiety, depression and psychological distress? A systematic review. Curr Psychol. (2023) 42:10200–22. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02082-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Parsons D, Gardner P, Parry S, and Smart S. Mindfulness-based approaches for managing stress, anxiety and depression for health students in tertiary education: a scoping review. Mindfulness. (2022) 13:1–16. doi: 10.1007/s12671-021-01740-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Song Y and Lindquist R. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on depression, anxiety, stress and mindfulness in Korean nursing students. Nurse Educ Today. (2015) 35:86–90. doi: 10.1016/j.nedt.2014.06.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Sverre KT, Nissen ER, Farver-Vestergaard I, Johannsen M, and Zachariae R. Comparing the efficacy of mindfulness-based therapy and cognitive-behavioral therapy for depression in head-to-head randomized controlled trials: A systematic review and meta-analysis of equivalence. Clin Psychol Rev. (2023) 100:102234. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2022.102234

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Krusche A, Cyhlarova E, and Williams JMG. Mindfulness online: an evaluation of the feasibility of a web-based mindfulness course for stress, anxiety and depression. BMJ Open. (2013) 3:e003498. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-003498

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Ellett L. Mindfulness for paranoid beliefs: evidence from two case studies. Behav Cogn Psychother. (2013) 41:238–42. doi: 10.1017/S1352465812000586

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Hilt LM, Swords CM, and Webb CA. Randomized controlled trial of a mindfulness mobile application for ruminative adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2025) 54:99–112. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2022.2158840

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Roeser RW, Mashburn AJ, Skinner EA, Choles JR, Taylor C, Rickert NP, et al. Mindfulness training improves middle school teachers’ occupational health, well-being, and interactions with students in their most stressful classrooms. J Educ Psychol. (2022) 114:408–25. doi: 10.1037/edu0000675

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Carlson LE, Ursuliak Z, Goodey E, Angen M, and Speca M. The effects of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduction program on mood and symptoms of stress in cancer outpatients: 6-month follow-up. Supportive Care Cancer. (2001) 9:112–23. doi: 10.1007/s005200000206

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Carlson LE, Speca M, Faris P, and Patel KD. One year pre–post intervention follow-up of psychological, immune, endocrine and blood pressure outcomes of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) in breast and prostate cancer outpatients. Brain Behavior Immun. (2007) 21:1038–49. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.04.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Earley MD, Chesney MA, Frye J, Greene PA, Berman B, Kimbrough E, et al. Mindfulness intervention for child abuse survivors: A 2.5-year follow-up. J Clin Psychol. (2014) 70:933–41. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Solhaug I, de Vibe M, Friborg O, Sørlie T, Tyssen R, Bjørndal A, et al. Long-term mental health effects of mindfulness training: a 4-year follow-up study. Mindfulness. (2019) 10:1661–72. doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01100-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hathaisaard C, Wannarit K, and Pattanaseri K. Mindfulness-based interventions reducing and preventing stress and burnout in medical students: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Asian J Psychiatry. (2022) 69:102997. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2021.102997

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fisher V, Li WW, and Malabu U. The effectiveness of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on the mental health, HbA1C, and mindfulness of diabetes patients: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Appl Psychology: Health Well-Being. (2023) 15:1733–49. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12441

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Garland EL. The meaning of mindfulness: A second-order cybernetics of stress, metacognition, and coping. Complementary Health Pract Rev. (2007) 12:15–30. doi: 10.1177/1533210107301740

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Garland E, Gaylord S, and Park J. The role of mindfulness in positive reappraisal. EXPLORE. (2009) 5:37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2008.10.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Garland EL, Farb NA, Goldin PR, and Fredrickson BL. The mindfulness-to-meaning theory: extensions, applications, and challenges at the attention–appraisal–emotion interface. psychol Inq. (2015) 26:377–87. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2015.1092493

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Nyklíček I. Emotion regulation and well-being. Nyklíček I, Vingerhoets A, and Zeelenberg M, editors. New York, NY: Springer New York (2011) p. 101–18.

Google Scholar

38. Coffey KA, Hartman M, and Fredrickson BL. Deconstructing mindfulness and constructing mental health: understanding mindfulness and its mechanisms of action. Mindfulness. (2010) 1:235–53. doi: 10.1007/s12671-010-0033-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Desrosiers A, Vine V, Klemanski DH, and Nolen-Hoeksema S. MINDFULNESS AND EMOTION REGULATION IN DEPRESSION AND ANXIETY: COMMON AND DISTINCT MECHANISMS OF ACTION. Depression Anxiety. (2013) 30:654–61. doi: 10.1002/da.22124

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Freudenthaler L, Turba JD, and Tran US. Emotion regulation mediates the associations of mindfulness on symptoms of depression and anxiety in the general population. Mindfulness. (2017) 8:1339–44. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0709-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Wimmer L, von Stockhausen L, and Bellingrath S. Improving emotion regulation and mood in teacher trainees: Effectiveness of two mindfulness trainings. Brain Behav. (2019) 9:e01390. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1390

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Fung J, Kim JJ, Jin J, Chen G, Bear L, Lau AS, et al. A randomized trial evaluating school-based mindfulness intervention for ethnic minority youth: exploring mediators and moderators of intervention effects. J Abnormal Child Psychol. (2019) 47:1–19. doi: 10.1007/s10802-018-0425-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Scafuto F, Quinto RM, Ghiroldi S, Montecucco NF, Presaghi F, Iani L, et al. The mediation role of emotion regulation strategies on the relationship between mindfulness effects, psychological well-being and distress among youths: findings from a randomized controlled trial. Curr Psychol. (2024) 43:24295–307. doi: 10.1007/s12144-024-06081-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Garnefski N, Kraaij V, and Spinhoven P. Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Pers Individ Dif. (2001) 30:1311–27. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00113-6

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Malik S and Perveen A. Mindfulness and anxiety among university students: Moderating role of cognitive emotion regulation. Curr Psychol. (2023) 42:5621–8. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-01906-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Motevalli S, Salahshour HM, and Bailey RP. The mediating role of cognitive flexibility in the relationship between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and mindfulness in patients with type 2 diabetes. J Affect Disord. (2023) 339:676–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2023.07.043

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Prakash RS, Hussain MA, and Schirda B. The role of emotion regulation and cognitive control in the association between mindfulness disposition and stress. Psychol Aging. (2015) 30:160–71. doi: 10.1037/a0038544

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Farahani S, Rafiepoor A, and Jafari T. The role of mediator of cognitive emotion regulation strategies in the relationship between mindfulness and perceived stress in people with high blood pressure. Int J Appl Behav Sci. (2019) 6:52–9. doi: 10.22037/ijabs.v6i2.26810

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Zhu P, Liu X, Shang X, Chen Y, Chen C, Wu Q, et al. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for quality of life, psychological distress, and cognitive emotion regulation strategies in patients with breast cancer under early chemotherapy—a randomized controlled trial. Holistic Nurs Pract. (2023) 37:131–42. doi: 10.1097/HNP.0000000000000580

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Kaunhoven RJ and Dorjee D. Mindfulness versus cognitive reappraisal: the impact of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on the early and late brain potential markers of emotion regulation. Mindfulness. (2021) 12:2266–80. doi: 10.1007/s12671-021-01692-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Kumar S, Feldman G, and Hayes A. Changes in mindfulness and emotion regulation in an exposure-based cognitive therapy for depression. Cogn Ther Res. (2008) 32:734–44. doi: 10.1007/s10608-008-9190-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Cerna C, García FE, and Téllez A. Brief mindfulness, mental health, and cognitive processes: A randomized controlled trial. Psych J. (2020) 9:359–69. doi: 10.1002/pchj.325

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Salovey P and Mayer JD. Emotional intelligence. Imagination Cogn Pers. (1990) 9:185–211. doi: 10.2190/DUGG-P24E-52WK-6CDG

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Mayer JD, Salovey P, and Caruso D. Handbook of intelligence. Sternberg RJ, editor. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2000) p. 396–420.

Google Scholar

55. Mayer JD and Salovey P. Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Educational implications. Salovey P and Sluyter D, editors. New York, NY, US: Basic Books (1997) p. 3–31.

Google Scholar

56. Mayer JD, Salovey P, and Caruso DR. Emotional intelligence: theory, findings, and implications. psychol Inq. (2004) 15:197–215. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1503_02

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Quintana-Orts C, Mérida-López S, Rey L, Neto F, and Extremera N. Untangling the emotional intelligence-suicidal ideation connection: the role of cognitive emotion regulation strategies in adolescents. J Clin Med. (2020) 9:3116. doi: 10.3390/jcm9103116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Thomas C and Zolkoski S. Preventing stress among undergraduate learners: the importance of emotional intelligence, resilience, and emotion regulation. Front Educ. (2020) 5. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2020.00094

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Sha J, Tang T, Shu H, He K, and Shen S. Emotional intelligence, emotional regulation strategies, and subjective well-being among university teachers: A moderated mediation analysis. Front Psychol. (2022) 12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.811260

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Bodrogi B, Bereczkei T, and Deak A. Be aware, make it clear, and take the Lead: emotion regulation difficulties and emotional intelligence as moderators of cognitive reappraisal. Curr Psychol. (2022) 41:6795–807. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-01182-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Zhou Y, Qu W, and Ge Y. The role of trait emotional intelligence in driving anger: The mediating effect of emotion regulation. Transportation Res Part F: Traffic Psychol Behav. (2022) 88:281–90. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2022.05.024

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Mikolajczak M, Nelis D, Hansenne M, and Quoidbach J. If you can regulate sadness, you can probably regulate shame: Associations between trait emotional intelligence, emotion regulation and coping efficiency across discrete emotions. Pers Individ Dif. (2008) 44:1356–68. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.12.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Gao Z and Yang Y. The predictive effect of trait emotional intelligence on emotion regulation strategies: The mediating role of negative emotion intensity. System. (2023) 112:102958. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2022.102958

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Zanella F, Monachesi B, and Grecucci A. Resting-state BOLD temporal variability in sensorimotor and salience networks underlies trait emotional intelligence and explains differences in emotion regulation strategies. Sci Rep. (2022) 12:15163. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-19477-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Ashori M and Jalil-Abkenar SS. Emotional intelligence: Quality of life and cognitive emotion regulation of deaf and hard-of-hearing adolescents. Deafness Educ Int. (2021) 23:84–102. doi: 10.1080/14643154.2020.1766754

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Delhom I, Satorres E, and Meléndez JC. Emotional intelligence intervention in older adults to improve adaptation and reduce negative mood. Int Psychogeriatrics. (2022) 34:79–89. doi: 10.1017/S1041610220003579

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Beyazit U, Yurdakul Y, and Ayhan AB. The mediating role of trait emotional intelligence in the relationship between parental neglect and cognitive emotion regulation strategies. BMC Psychol. (2024) 12:314. doi: 10.1186/s40359-024-01817-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Miao C, Humphrey RH, and Qian S. The relationship between emotional intelligence and trait mindfulness: A meta-analytic review. Pers Individ Dif. (2018) 135:101–7. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.051

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Teal C, Downey LA, Lomas JE, Ford TC, Bunnett ER, Stough C, et al. The role of dispositional mindfulness and emotional intelligence in adolescent males. Mindfulness. (2019) 10:159–67. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-0962-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Huang J, Xie L, Guo R, Wang J, Lin J, Sun Z, et al. Abnormal brain activity patterns during spatial working memory task in patients with end-stage renal disease on maintenance hemodialysis: a fMRI study. Brain Imaging Behav. (2021) 15:1898–911. doi: 10.1007/s11682-020-00383-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Ajilchi B, Amini HR, Ardakani ZP, Zadeh MM, and Kisely S. Applying mindfulness training to enhance the mental toughness and emotional intelligence of amateur basketball players. Australas Psychiatry. (2019) 27:291–6. doi: 10.1177/1039856219828119

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Nadler R, Carswell JJ, and Minda JP. Online mindfulness training increases well-being, trait emotional intelligence, and workplace competency ratings: A randomized waitlist-controlled trial. Front Psychol. (2020) 11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00255

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Kou H, Bi T, Chen S, Li X, He Y, Xie Q, et al. The impact of mindfulness training on supportive communication, emotional intelligence, and human caring among nursing students. Perspect Psychiatr Care. (2022) 58:2552–61. doi: 10.1111/ppc.13093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Fleeson W and Jayawickreme E. Whole trait theory. J Res Pers. (2015) 56:82–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2014.10.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, and Toney L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment. (2006) 13:27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Deng Y-Q, Liu X-H, Rodriguez MA, and Xia C-Y. The five facet mindfulness questionnaire: psychometric properties of the chinese version. Mindfulness. (2011) 2:123–8. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0050-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Schutte NS, Malouff JM, Hall LE, Haggerty DJ, Cooper JT, Golden CJ, et al. Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Pers Individ Dif. (1998) 25:167–77. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00001-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Zhu X, Luo F, Yao S, Auerbach RP, and Abela JRZ. Reliability and validity of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire-chinese version. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2007), 121–4. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2007.02.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Gross JJ and John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2003) 85:348–62. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Zung WW. A self-rating depression scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1965) 12:63–70. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1965.01720310065008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Zung WW. A rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics: J Consultation Liaison Psychiatry. (1971) 12:371–9. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(71)71479-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Segal ZV, Teasdale JD, Williams JM, and Gemar MC. The mindfulness-based cognitive therapy adherence scale: inter-rater reliability, adherence to protocol and treatment distinctiveness. Clin Psychol Psychother. (2002) 9:131–8. doi: 10.1002/cpp.320

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Xu W, Wang Y, and Liu X. Effectiveness of 8-week mindfulness training improving negative emotions. Chin Ment Health J. (2015) 29:497–502. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2015.07.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Preacher KJ and Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav Res Methods Instruments Comput. (2004) 36:717–31. doi: 10.3758/BF03206553

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Baron RM and Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1986) 51:1173–82. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Cole DA and Maxwell SE. Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. J Abnorm Psychol. (2003) 112:558–77. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Cheong J, Mackinnon DP, and Khoo ST. Investigation of mediational processes using parallel process latent growth curve modeling. Struct Equation Modeling: A Multidiscip J. (2003) 10:238–62. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM1002_5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

88. von Soest T and Hagtvet KA. Mediation analysis in a latent growth curve modeling framework. Struct Equation Modeling: A Multidiscip J. (2011) 18:289–314. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2011.557344

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Selig JP and Preacher KJ. Mediation models for longitudinal data in developmental research. Res Hum Dev. (2009) 6:144–64. doi: 10.1080/15427600902911247

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Spurk D, Hirschi A, Wang M, Valero D, and Kauffeld S. Latent profile analysis: A review and “how to” guide of its application within vocational behavior research. J Vocational Behav. (2020) 120:103445. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2020.103445

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Williams GA and Kibowski F. Handbook of methodological approaches to community-based research: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press (2016) p. 143–51.

Google Scholar

92. Grunschel C, Patrzek J, and Fries S. Exploring different types of academic delayers: A latent profile analysis. Learn Individ Dif. (2013) 23:225–33. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.09.014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Pastor DA, Barron KE, Miller BJ, and Davis SL. A latent profile analysis of college students’ achievement goal orientation. Contemp Educ Psychol. (2007) 32:8–47. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.10.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Esmaeili A, Masoomeh K, Ensiyeh N, and Miri MR. Effectiveness of mindfulness-based cognitive group therapy on cognitive emotion regulation of patients under treatment with methadone. J Subst Use. (2018) 23:58–62. doi: 10.1080/14659891.2017.1348553

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

95. kakhki ZB, Mashhadi A, Yazdi SAA, and Saleh S. The effect of mindful parenting training on parent–child interactions, parenting stress, and cognitive emotion regulation in mothers of preschool children. J Child Family Stud. (2022) 31:3113–24. doi: 10.1007/s10826-022-02420-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Charoensukmongkol P. Benefits of mindfulness meditation on emotional intelligence, general self-efficacy, and perceived stress: evidence from Thailand. J Spirituality Ment Health. (2014) 16:171–92. doi: 10.1080/19349637.2014.925364

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Ksiksou J, Maskour L, El Batri B, and Alaoui MS. The effect of a mindfulness training program on perceived stress and emotional intelligence among nursing students in Morocco: An experimental pilot study. Acta Neuropsychologica. (2022) 20:371–83. doi: 10.5604/01.3001.0016.0894

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Yuan Y. Mindfulness training on the resilience of adolescents under the COVID-19 epidemic: A latent growth curve analysis. Pers Individ Dif. (2021) 172:110560. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110560

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Zhou A, Yuan Y, and Kang M. Mindfulness intervention on adolescents’ Emotional intelligence and psychological capital during the COVID-19 pandemic: A randomized controlled trial. Int J Ment Health Promotion. (2022) 24:665–77. doi: 10.32604/ijmhp.2022.019623

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Fabbro A, Fabbro F, Capurso V, D’Antoni F, and Crescentini C. Effects of mindfulness training on school teachers’ self-reported personality traits as well as stress and burnout levels. Perceptual Motor Skills. (2020) 127:515–32. doi: 10.1177/0031512520908708

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Liu X, Xu W, Wang Y, Williams JMG, Geng Y, Zhang Q, et al. Can inner peace be improved by mindfulness training: A randomized controlled trial. Stress Health. (2015) 31:245–54. doi: 10.1002/smi.2551

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Heeren A, Deplus S, Peschard V, Nef F, Kotsou I, Dierickx C, et al. Does change in self-reported mindfulness mediate the clinical benefits of mindfulness training? A controlled study using the french translation of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire. Mindfulness. (2015) 6:553–9. doi: 10.1007/s12671-014-0287-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Visted E, Vøllestad J, Nielsen MB, and Nielsen GH. The impact of group-based mindfulness training on self-reported mindfulness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mindfulness. (2015) 6:501–22. doi: 10.1007/s12671-014-0283-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Soler J, Cebolla A, Feliu-Soler A, Demarzo MMP, Pascual JC, Baños R, et al. Relationship between meditative practice and self-reported mindfulness: the MINDSENS composite index. PloS One. (2014) 9:e86622. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0086622

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Li L and Li M. Effects of mindfulness training on psychological capital, depression, and procrastination of the youth demographic. Iran J Public Health. (2020) 49:1692–700. doi: 10.18502/ijph.v49i9.4086

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Wang Y, Tian J, and Yang Q. On mindfulness training for promoting mental toughness of female college students in endurance exercise. Evidence-Based Complementary Altern Med. (2021) 2021:5596111. doi: 10.1155/2021/5596111

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Atta MHR, El-Ashry AM, and Abd El-Gawad Mousa M. The effects of mindfulness-based techniques on self-rumination, cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression among patients with major depression: a nursing perspective. J Res Nurs. (2024) 29:366–85. doi: 10.1177/17449871241252011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Turner JA, Anderson ML, Balderson BH, Cook AJ, Sherman KJ, Cherkin DC, et al. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and cognitive behavioral therapy for chronic low back pain: similar effects on mindfulness, catastrophizing, self-efficacy, and acceptance in a randomized controlled trial. Pain. (2016) 157:2434–44. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000635

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Garland EL, Gaylord SA, Palsson O, Faurot K, Mann JD, Whitehead WE, et al. Therapeutic mechanisms of a mindfulness-based treatment for IBS: effects on visceral sensitivity, catastrophizing, and affective processing of pain sensations. J Behav Med. (2012) 35:591–602. doi: 10.1007/s10865-011-9391-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Kearson A, Samawi L, Burrowes S, Keaser M, Zhang J, Campbell C, et al. (312) the effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on pain catastrophizing in episodic migraine. J Pain. (2019) 20:S52. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2019.01.469

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Lazarus RS and Alfert E. Short-circuiting of threat by experimentally altering cognitive appraisal. J Abnormal Soc Psychol. (1964) 69:195–205. doi: 10.1037/h0044635

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Shapiro SL, Carlson LE, Astin JA, and Freedman B. Mechanisms of mindfulness. J Clin Psychol. (2006) 62:373–86. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20237

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol. (1998) 2:271–99. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: current status and future prospects. psychol Inq. (2015) 26:1–26. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Extremera N, Sánchez-Álvarez N, and Rey L. Pathways between ability emotional intelligence and subjective well-being: bridging links through cognitive emotion regulation strategies. Sustainability. (2020) 12:2111. doi: 10.3390/su12052111

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Thomas EA, Hamrick LA, Owens GP, and Tekie YT. Posttraumatic growth among undergraduates: Contributions from adaptive cognitive emotion regulation and emotional intelligence. Traumatology. (2020) 26:68–73. doi: 10.1037/trm0000203

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Hölzel BK, Lazar S, Gard T, Schuman-Olivier Z, Vago DR, Ott U, et al. How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspect psychol Sci. (2011) 6:537–59. doi: 10.1177/1745691611419671

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Tran US, Cebolla A, Glück TM, Soler J, Garcia-Campayo J, von Moy T, et al. The serenity of the meditating mind: A cross-cultural psychometric study on a two-factor higher order structure of mindfulness, its effects, and mechanisms related to mental health among experienced meditators. PloS One. (2014) 9:e110192. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110192

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Burzler MA, Voracek M, Hos M, and Tran US. Mechanisms of mindfulness in the general population. Mindfulness. (2019) 10:469–80. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-0988-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Khoury B, Sharma M, Rush SE, and Fournier C. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for healthy individuals: A meta-analysis. J Psychosomatic Res. (2015) 78:519–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.03.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Querstret D, Morison L, Dickinson S, Cropley M, and John M. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for psychological health and well-being in nonclinical samples: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Stress Manage. (2020) 27:394–411. doi: 10.1037/str0000165

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Ren Z, Zhang Y, and Jiang G. Effectiveness of mindfulness meditation in intervention for anxiety: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica Sin. (2018) 50:283–305. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00283

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Harkness E, Macdonald W, Valderas JM, Coventry P, Gask L, Bower P, et al. Identifying psychosocial interventions that improve both physical and mental health in patients with diabetes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Diabetes Care. (2010) 33:926–30. doi: 10.2337/dc09-1519

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Mather M and Thayer JF. How heart rate variability affects emotion regulation brain networks. Curr Opin Behav Sci. (2018) 19:98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.12.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Appelhans BM and Luecken LJ. Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Rev Gen Psychol. (2006) 10:229–40. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.10.3.229

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Doll A, Hölzel BK, Bratec SM, Boucard CC, Xie X, Wohlschläger AM, et al. Mindful attention to breath regulates emotions via increased amygdala–prefrontal cortex connectivity. NeuroImage. (2016) 134:305–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Hölzel BK, Hoge EA, Greve DN, Gard T, Creswell JD, Brown KW, et al. Neural mechanisms of symptom improvements in generalized anxiety disorder following mindfulness training. NeuroImage Clin. (2013) 2:448–58. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.03.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Melis M, Schroyen G, Pollefeyt J, Raes F, Smeets A, Sunaert S, et al. The impact of mindfulness-based interventions on brain functional connectivity: A systematic review. Mindfulness. (2022) 13:1857–75. doi: 10.1007/s12671-022-01919-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Gotink RA, Meijboom R, Vernooij MW, Smits M, and Hunink MGM. 8-week Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction induces brain changes similar to traditional long-term meditation practice – A systematic review. Brain Cogn. (2016) 108:32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.07.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Miller JJ, Fletcher K, and Kabat-Zinn J. Three-year follow-up and clinical implications of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduction intervention in the treatment of anxiety disorders. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. (1995) 17:192–200. doi: 10.1016/0163-8343(95)00025-M

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Verhaeghen P. Mindfulness as attention training: meta-analyses on the links between attention performance and mindfulness interventions, long-term meditation practice, and trait mindfulness. Mindfulness. (2021) 12:564–81. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01532-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: EmotionCore mindfulness training, emotion regulation, emotional intelligence, mediation analysis, latent profile analysis

Citation: Kou H, Luo W, Wang Y, Wu J, Li X, Wu Y, Xiao Q and Bi T (2025) The sustained effect of 5-week EmotionCore mindfulness training on emotion regulation and emotional intelligence: heterogeneous benefits for depression and anxiety across subgroups. Front. Psychiatry 16:1622626. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2025.1622626

Received: 04 May 2025; Accepted: 29 June 2025;
Published: 18 July 2025.

Edited by:

Qinghua He, Southwest University, China

Reviewed by:

Yang Wang, University of Maryland, United States
Alfonso-José Moreno-Gómez, Escuni, Spain

Copyright © 2025 Kou, Luo, Wang, Wu, Li, Wu, Xiao and Bi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Taiyong Bi, Yml0YWl5b25nQDEyNi5jb20=

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.