Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Robot. AI, 03 July 2023
Sec. Human-Robot Interaction
Volume 10 - 2023 | https://doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2023.1125308

Influence of collaborative customer service by service robots and clerks in bakery stores

  • 1AI Lab, CyberAgent Inc, Tokyo, Japan
  • 2Graduate School of Engineering Science, Osaka University, Osaka, Japan

In recent years, various service robots have been deployed in stores as recommendation systems. Previous studies have sought to increase the influence of these robots by enhancing their social acceptance and trust. However, when such service robots recommend a product to customers in real environments, the effect on the customers is influenced not only by the robot itself, but also by the social influence of the surrounding people such as store clerks. Therefore, leveraging the social influence of the clerks may increase the influence of the robots on the customers. Hence, we compared the influence of robots with and without collaborative customer service between the robots and clerks in two bakery stores. The experimental results showed that collaborative customer service increased the purchase rate of the recommended bread and improved the impressions of the robot and store experience of the customers. Because the results also showed that the workload required for the clerks to collaborate with the robot was not high, this study suggests that all stores with service robots may demonstrate high effectiveness in introducing collaborative customer service.

1 Introduction

In recent years, service robots have been widely used in various scenarios; one such use of a service robot is as a recommendation system for sales promotion. These robots have been experimentally introduced in real commercial facilities and are expected to be a novel approach to product promotion. For example, the service robots recommend products at shopping malls (Song et al., 2021), convenience stores (Kamei et al., 2010), department stores (Watanabe et al., 2015), and bakeries (Song et al., 2022), and experiments have demonstrated an increase in the corresponding product sales. The implementation of recommendation techniques using service robots has been tested in a limited number of situations; however, they are expected to be used widely if their utility is proven in a large number of cases.

When service robots recommend products, two main elements are important to strengthen the influence of the robots: social acceptance and trustworthiness of the robot (Herse et al., 2018b; Ghazali et al., 2020). First, in order to recommend products to customers, the robot needs to initiate a conversation with them. However, service robots in a real environment are often ignored by customers even when they directly approach the customers and talk to them (Lee et al., 2012; Tanaka et al., 2016). An interactive recommendation cannot be achieved if a conversation between the robot and customer is not initiated; consequently, the advantage of using a dialogue system, such as an interactive robot, diminishes. Therefore, we must improve the social acceptance of the robots to achieve interactive recommendations. Next, to find information and products that customers actually prefer, and assist the customers in making purchase decisions, it is essential to build trust between the customers and all recommendation systems including but not limited to robots (Pu et al., 2012). In particular, when using a robot recommendation system, the previous study has reported that a high level of trust in a robot facilitates acceptance of recommendations from the robot (Rau et al., 2009). Improving the level of trust in the robot’s suggestions helps in establishing a more effective recommendation system.

Hence, previous studies have focused on investigating various methods to improve the social acceptance and trustworthiness of the robot itself as a recommendation system. Meanwhile, in this study, we assume that these service robots will not be introduced in an environment where only robots serve the customers but in stores that are already operated by human staff. In this case, the social acceptance and trustworthiness of the robot may be influenced not only by the robot itself but also by other people around the robot, including the working staff. Such influences from the surroundings are called social influences, and are considered not only in human–robot interaction (HRI) (Malhotra and Galletta, 1999) but also in human–human interaction (HHI) (Cialdini and Goldstein, 2004). Although the topic of service operations involving robots and human staff has been discussed (Xiao and Kumar, 2019; Belanche et al., 2020), it is still unclear what benefits can be gained by the influence of the staff through real-world trials.

Accordingly, we assumed that the social acceptance and trustworthiness of robots, which strengthen their influence on the customers, are affected by the surrounding people in a real environment. Therefore, the aim of this study is to improve the social acceptance and trustworthiness of robots as recommendation systems by leveraging social influences and to improve the influence on customers through the improved social acceptance and trustworthiness. In particular, we create an atmosphere in which robots have already been accepted by collaborative customer service (CCS) with store clerks. This is a simple method, but CCS may be able to improve the social acceptance and trustworthiness of the robot from the perspective of the customer. Consequently, the improved influence of the robot owing to high social acceptance and trust can promote sales. In addition, we consider that collaboration with clerks can be adopted universally and it improves the persuasive recommendation by robots proposed in previous studies. Therefore, in this study, we introduced two service robots in two bakery stores. A field experiment was conducted to verify how the recommendation effect of the robot was influenced by CCS between the robot and store clerks.

The remainder of this paper is organized as follows. The related works are described in Section 2. The experimental methodology and results are presented in Sections 3 and 4, respectively. In Section 5, a discussion based on the results and the potential for future research is presented. Finally, Section 6 presents our conclusions.

2 Related works

In order to utilize robots as recommendation systems in the real-world, the social acceptance and trustworthiness of robots are important (Herse et al., 2018b; Ghazali et al., 2020). This is because robots are often ignored by customers in the real-world (Lee et al., 2012; Tanaka et al., 2016) and improving the trustworthiness of robots enhances persuasiveness (Rau et al., 2009). On the other hand, many studies aiming at developing robot recommendation systems directly verify the persuasiveness of robots, which means that the users’ decisions can be influenced by robots, rather than the social acceptance or trustworthiness of robots. Therefore, we mainly present the literature on the persuasiveness of robots which is especially related to the trustworthiness.

Many studies have focused on nonverbal behavior to improve the persuasiveness of robots. In particular, it has been found that a robot’s gaze and gesture expressions improve its persuasiveness (Ham et al., 2015). These nonverbal cues influence its persuasiveness more than simple voice-based communication (Chidambaram et al., 2012). Although simple voice-based communication has limited influence on the persuasiveness, the effects of various types of persuasive verbal communications have been verified. For example, because expertise improves the persuasiveness in HHI (Cialdini, 2001), robots can utilize expert knowledge and rhetorical communication (Andrist et al., 2013). In addition, the persuasiveness of a robot is enhanced through emotional verbalization (Bertacchini et al., 2017; Saunderson and Nejat, 2022) and by utilizing the user’s estimated emotions and social media information (Bertacchini et al., 2017).

The aforementioned studies have focused on improving the persuasiveness of a single robot, but a few studies have attempted the same with multiple robots. In general, people tend to agree with the opinions of others in their surroundings, which is called normative conformity. This concept was experimentally verified in HHI (Asch, 1956). Similarly, people also tend to agree with the opinions of multiple robots (Salomons et al., 2017). The persuasiveness of robots can be improved by the balance theory too (Heider, 1958). In a group with two robots and one human, it has been shown that adjusting the relationship between the two robots to balance the group state increases the persuasiveness of the robot toward the human (Kadowaki et al., 2008). In addition, another study using multiple robots showed that sequential persuasion, in which multiple robots talk repeatedly to customers, enhances persuasiveness, and the level of persuasiveness increases with an increase in the number of robots (Tae et al., 2021). Thus, methods to improve the persuasiveness of robots have been verified from various perspectives.

Some studies have utilized the persuasiveness of these robots in recommendation systems. In general, in the context of recommendation, the trustworthiness has been shown to shape source credibility and improve persuasion (Haiman, 1949; O’Keefe, 1990). Therefore, in robots as well, several studies have focused on the trustworthiness and persuasiveness of robots as recommendation systems. The basic persuasiveness of robots has been investigated in terms of their embodiment (Herse et al., 2018b) and proactive recommendation ability (Peng et al., 2019). Many recommendation systems have been proposed to estimate the user’s state during the interaction and recommend products according to the estimated state (Carolis et al., 2017; Woiceshyn et al., 2017; Herse et al., 2018b; Alslaity and Tran, 2019). For example, the user’s state such as attitude, emotion, and product preferences was estimated from the voice, posture, and facial expressions, and products were recommended based on these assessments in a dress shopping scenario (Carolis et al., 2017). The long-term adaptability of personalized recommendation systems by estimating user preferences through multiple interactions has been improved (Woiceshyn et al., 2017). Thus, the aim of previous studies was to develop a more persuasive recommendation system by personalizing the system using various methods (e.g., (Alslaity and Tran, 2019)).

The studies introduced in the previous paragraph verified the effectiveness of the robot recommendation system through laboratory experiments. Whereas some studies have demonstrated the practicality of recommendation systems as a new sales promotion method by verifying their effectiveness in real environments. As an example, the successful sale of toothbrushes by a teleoperated robot was demonstrated in a commercial facility (Song et al., 2021), and a recommendation robot in a bakery improved the store sales (Song et al., 2022). Three autonomous robots introduced in a convenience store promoted the sale of the recommended products (Kamei et al., 2010). It has also been shown that the longitudinal recommendation effect increases by adding confidence and word–of–mouth to the recommendation statement (Okafuji et al., 2021). Further, an android robot realized approximately twice as much product sales as the average human salesperson (Watanabe et al., 2015). In addition, robot systems that provide users with coupons and tastings that indirectly influence sales promotions have also been proposed (Shiomi et al., 2013; Tonkin et al., 2017; Okafuji et al., 2022). These studies examined the robot’s size (Shiomi et al., 2013), types of robot behavior (Okafuji et al., 2022), and aggressiveness of the robot’s talk (Tonkin et al., 2017). Studies on robot recommendation systems have also compared the performance of humans and virtual agents (Shiomi et al., 2013; Tonkin et al., 2017; Herse et al., 2018a; Okafuji et al., 2022; Sakai et al., 2022), and some studies have shown that the performance of the robot system exceeds human performance (Okafuji et al., 2022). However, in these studies, the persuasiveness of robots which is especially related to the trustworthiness is mainly evaluated rather than the social acceptance of robots, even though they are conducted in a real environment.

Studies have been conducted on the various aspects of using robots as recommendation systems. However, although they have directly examined the persuasiveness of robots to influence users’ decisions, few studies have thoroughly investigated the social acceptance and trustworthiness that influence their persuasiveness. In addition, although a few studies have focused on the persuasiveness of multiple robots (Kadowaki et al., 2008; Salomons et al., 2017; Tae et al., 2021), most have examined the effect of recommendation by a single robot. As described in the introduction, we assumed that these service robots will be introduced in stores that are already operated by humans. In this case, the social influence of the surrounding people, including store clerks, may also affect the relationship between the robot and customers. In the hospitality field, the topic of three-way interaction among customers, service robots, and employees has been recently discussed (Xiao and Kumar, 2019; Belanche et al., 2020). However, the actual effect of such interactions has not yet been investigated. Therefore, it is necessary to investigate in detail the influence of CCS when introducing service robots at a brick-and-mortar retail establishment.

3 Methodology

3.1 Overview

The aim of this study is to investigate whether the social acceptance and trustworthiness of robots in the service field are improved and whether the recommendation effect is further improved through CCS between robots and clerks. To verify this, we installed service robots that recommended products at two bakery stores and conducted field experiments. The experiment was conducted for 15 days from April 14th to 28th, 2022, and the robot was operated between 10 AM and 2 PM and 3 PM and 5 PM.

3.2 Robot systems

We developed a teleoperated robot system that enabled an operator to remotely control the robot and communicate through it via a web-based video calling application. The system comprised three main components: a robot controller application, an operator interface, and a server. The social robot used in this system was “Sota,” developed by Vstone Co., Ltd., as shown in Figure 1. Sota is approximately 0.3 m tall, and it has functions such as voice and LED-generated facial expressions that light up the eyes and simulate blinking the mouth in sync with speech. Operators can monitor the video sent by the robot and speak accordingly through the robot to the customers in real time. The operator can control the gaze direction and some gestures of the robot using the interface.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Humanoid robot “Sota”.

3.3 Experimental fields

We conducted experiments simultaneously in two bakeries with similar environments, selected from multiple candidates, to evaluate CCS. These bakeries are located in Osaka and Kyoto, Japan, in residential areas outside the city center. Apart from small organizations such as elementary schools, there are no large universities, hospitals or businesses nearby. They sell approximately 50 types of bread. Figure 2 shows the layout of each store and the experimental setup.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Layouts of two bakery stores and experimental setups. The left and right figures show the layouts of the Control and Collaborative Stores, respectively.

3.4 Robot tasks

One robot was installed in each store. The main tasks of the robot were to recommend specific bread products, converse with customers, and announce events such as the availability of freshly baked bread. Welcoming customers and promoting products are listed as tasks that retailers expect robots to perform (Niemelä et al., 2017b); these tasks were included in this experiment. In terms of recommendations, the robot provided information about specific products in detail to arouse the interest of the customer. Regarding the products for recommendation, we discussed with the shop owners and selected five from approximately 50 kinds of bread at each shop.

As verbal behavior, the robot was able to provide and answer questions about detailed information about specific bread since using expertise improves persuasiveness (Andrist et al., 2013). In addition, in order to make recommendations according to the user’s state (Carolis et al., 2017), the robot recommended different specific bread in accordance with the position and behavior of the customer. As nonverbal behavior, the robot always gazed at the customer and expressed gestures (Ham et al., 2015). The gesture expressions include pointing to bread, raising hands, waving the body, and so on. These gesture expressions are automatically generated when approximately 50 types of registered words such as greetings are spoken by operators. In other words, the robot’s nonverbal behavior can all be automated based on the operator’s utterances and the customer’s position. On the other hand, the operators can also adjust the robot’s gaze and express specific gestures that do not depend on the operator’s utterances.

Four robot operators, all females in their 20 s, were employed for the tasks. They are all voice actors and skilled performers. All of them had experience operating robots in previous experiments. The operators spoke in a voice by modulating their own voices similar to that of a small child to maintain consistency with the childlike appearance of the robot. This teleoperated approach has been referred to as “Wizard of Oz (WoZ)” (Riek, 2012). Two of the four operators were assigned to each store daily on a random basis to eliminate differences in the operator performance.

The WoZ style was applied in this experiment because if an autonomous robot is used, communication errors might occur, and it will be difficult to evaluate the effectiveness of CCS. In addition, we instructed the operators to not admit to customers that they were human even if they were asked. If customers know that the robot is operated by humans, they would evaluate customer service for the human operator instead of the robot. To perform the experiment correctly, we informed the store clerks at each store that the robot was operated by humans. However, we instructed them not to divulge this information to customers. Therefore, this experimental environment created a scenario wherein the clerks were aware that they worked with a robot-mediated human whereas the customers were given the impression that they were being served by an autonomous robot. These experimental settings have been used in another study (Song et al., 2022).

3.5 Clerk operations

For evaluating CCS, the robot tasks, especially the bread recommendation method, in the two bakeries were the same but the clerks’ operations were different. As a control condition, CCS was not applied in one bakery, and the robot attempted to serve the customers alone. For instance, the robot would say, “Welcome!“, “I recommend the salt bread today because it is the most popular!“, and “Thank you!“. The clerks communicated information to the robot only when freshly baked bread was available; otherwise, they provided customer service as usual, without involving the robot, unless the customer inquired about the robot.

At the other bakery, the robot and clerks provided CCS. Specifically, we generated situations in which the customer was always presented with interactions between the robot and clerks while the customer was visiting the store. For example, the clerk reiterates the statement made by the robot:

• Robot: “I recommend the salt bread today!”

• Clerk: “We recommend the salt bread today!”

The clerk responds to a statement from the robot:

• Robot: “Customers have come!”

• Clerk: “Okay, Thanks! Welcome!”

The clerk chats with the robot:

• Robot: “What is your recommendation?”

• Clerk: “It is the salt bread.”

• Robot: “Hey customers! The salt bread seems to be recommended!”

As in these examples, CCS operation involves two elements: 1. The clerk reiterates the robot’s statement, and 2. The robot communicates not only to the customer but also to the store clerk when welcoming customers or promoting products. We suppose that these interactions convey an atmosphere to the customers that the robot is accepted by the store clerks. On the other hand, in the store without CCS, these two elements were not present, and the robot and the clerk served customers independently. However, when a customer asked about the robot, the clerk showed a positive attitude toward the robot. Thus, attitudes toward the robot did not change in either store.

In addition, such collaboration is considered to strengthen two types of communication: sociality and support giving/getting (Fay, 2011). Informal communication strengthens employee satisfaction (Fay and Kline, 2011) and relationships (Aplarslan and Kilinç, 2015); thus, clerks may easily accept robot-mediated operators. Improving the relationship between the robot and clerks will have a more positive influence on the customer’s impressions of the robot.

Hereafter, the store without CCS is referred to as the Control Store, and the store with CCS is referred to as the Collaborative Store, as shown in Figure 2.

In this experiment, in the Control Store, eight store clerks (all females, three in their teens, four in their 20 s, one in their 30 s, one in their 50 s, 1 with no response) served customers independently of the robots when the robots served customers. In the Collaborative Store, 13 store clerks (12 females and 1 male, six in their teens, three in their 20 s, one in their 30 s, and three in their 40 s) performed CCS with the robot.

3.6 Measurement

In this study, we evaluated the following: purchase rates of the recommended bread and questionnaires presented to customers, clerks, operators, and interaction rates.

First, the sales of the five types of recommended bread were evaluated to verify the influence of the robot. It was difficult to compare the sales between the two stores because the types of bread sold in each store and the recommended breads were different. Therefore, within each store, the influence of the robot was verified by comparing the purchase rates of the recommended bread when the robot was in operation and when it was not in operation. The opening hours of the Control and Collaborative Stores were 7 am to 6 pm and 7 am to 7 pm, respectively. Therefore, the robots operated for 6 hours (10 am–2 pm and 3 pm–5 pm) in each store and recommended specific types of bread, whereas they did not operate for 5 hours at the Control Store and 6 hours at the Collaborative Store each day. When not in operation, the robot remained inside the store, but all movements were stopped. Therefore, the influence of the robot was evaluated by comparing the following equation during the period of its operation and non-operation within each store:

PurchaseRatePR%=NRecNAll×100,(1)

where NRec indicates the units sold for five types of recommended breads and NAll denotes the units sold for all types of bread.

Next, we conducted a survey of the customers and clerks about their impressions of the robot and the store experience. The customer questionnaire was presented at the cash register, and all customers passing through the cash register were prompted to answer the questionnaire. All questionnaires were evaluated using a 1–7 grade Likert scale.

The customer questionnaire was related to the impressions of the robot because the customers did not know that the robot was controlled by human operators. Seven items were listed in the questionnaire: intelligent, useful, easy to talk, enjoyable, friendly, influential, and trustworthy. These are part of the social acceptance model for robots (Heerink et al., 2010). In addition, installing a robot may improve the impressions of the store experience. For example, it has been reported that installing robots in hotels improves their brand experience (Chan and Tung, 2019). Therefore, we also evaluated the impressions of the store experience under the influence of CCS. The following four items were listed in relation to the store experience: 1) “Were you satisfied with this shop?” 2) “Did the robot improve the brand image of the store?” 3) “Did you feel anxious about robots usurping your job?” and 4) “Do you hope to revisit this store?”

Regarding the clerk questionnaire, we asked the participating clerks to answer the questionnaire every day. The clerk questionnaire asked about their impressions of the robot similar to the customer questionnaire. However, the clerk questionnaire may convey the impressions of the human operator, not the robot, because the clerks were aware that the robot was operated by humans. The items listed in the impressions of the robot were the same as those in the customer questionnaire. The four items listed in the impressions of the store experience were 1) “Did the robot improve the impression of the store?” 2) “Did you feel anxious about robots usurping your job?” 3) “Did you feel frustrated about working with the robot?” and 4) “Do you hope to work with the robot again?”

In HHI, humans build rapport through communication, including informal communication (Aplarslan and Kilinç, 2015). However, it is known that humans’ perception of speaking objects is distorted when the appearance of the object is changed, even if they know that the object is operated by humans (Bennett et al., 2017). In addition, teleoperated humanoid robots are recognized as having lower intelligence than humans even when they perform the same task (Kuwamura et al., 2012). Therefore, it is unclear whether the relationships between clerks and robot-mediated operators are established in the same way as in HHI. Hence, the relationship between these variables was investigated using the clerk questionnaire.

Finally, we administered a daily questionnaire to operators of their workload, motivation, and sense of belonging. It is known that informal communication makes people develop a sense of belonging, even during telework, which increases their commitment to the work (Fay, 2011). Therefore, we evaluated whether similar results could be obtained in this experimental environment, even though the operator questionnaire was not directly related to the purpose of this study. The items in the operator questionnaire were based on NASA-TLX (Hart and Staveland, 1988), and included the following: “Did you maintain high motivation throughout today’s work” and “Did you feel a sense of belonging to the store during today’s work?”

4 Results

We analyzed the PRs of the five types of recommended breads, the impressions of the robot and store experience from the customers and clerks, and work performance of the operators when the robot was placed in the two bakeries for 15 days. However, the experiment was canceled on 21st April at the Control Store because of poor physical condition of one of the operators. Thus, the Control Store was evaluated for 14 days. The customer questionnaires at the Control and Collaborative Stores received 216 and 98 responses, respectively (average: 15.4 and 6.5, respectively, per day). The numbers of responses to the clerk questionnaire at the Control and Collaborative Stores were 32 and 43, respectively (average: 2.3 and 2.9, respectively, per day).

Demographic information of customers in the two stores is listed in Table 1 and Table 2. During the experiment period, we annotated customer information from 10 am to 5 pm. The demographic information between the two stores (305 and 543 customers in the Control and Collaborative Stores, respectively) shows no large difference using the chi-squared test (Gender: χ2 (1) = 0.03, p = 0.98, V = 0.01, Age: χ2 (1) = 1.78, p = 0.41, V = 0.05, Group: χ2 (1) = 0.00, p = 1.00, V = 0.00). We used Cramer’s V as the effect size in the chi-squared test.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Demographic information on observed customer age and gender.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Demographic information on customer group.

4.1 Purchase rate of recommended bread

Figure 3 shows the PRs at both bakeries, as represented by Eq. 1, divided into periods when the robot was in operation and when it was not in operation. In the Control Store, the average PR when the robot was not in operation was 15.5% (264/1707), whereas the average PR when the robot was in operation was 14.5% (345/2373). This means that the PR was reduced when the robot was in operation. However, in the Collaborative Store, the average PR when the robot was not in operation was 7.1% (305/4305), whereas the average PR when the robot was in operation was 8.6% (400/4637). This means that the PR increased when the robot was in operation. These results were verified within each store by using the chi-squared test. The results for the Control Store revealed no significant differences in the PRs with and without the robot recommendation: (χ2 (1) = 0.67, p = 0.71, V = 0.01). However, the results in the Collaborative Store revealed significant differences in the PR with and without the robot recommendation: (χ2 (1) = 7.30, p = 0.02, V = 0.03).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Purchase rate (PR) at each store.

4.2 Customer questionnaire

Figure 4 shows the results of the customers’ impressions of the robot obtained from 216 to 98 customer questionnaires in the Control and Collaborative Stores, respectively. The value of 4 is neutral because of the use of the 1–7 grade Likert scale. Thus, the results indicate positive impressions of the robot in both stores, except for the item “Friendly” in the Control Store. In addition, the robot in the Collaborative Store elicited more positive impressions from the customers in all aspects than the robot in the Control Store. These results were nearly identical trends across age and gender. These results were verified using the Mann–Whitney U test. The results revealed significant differences in all items between the stores (Intelligent: U = 7530, p < 0.01, Useful: U = 8312, p < 0.01, Easy to talk: U = 7903.5, p < 0.01, Enjoyable: U = 8337, p < 0.01, Friendly: U = 7458, p < 0.01, Influential: U = 8385.5, p < 0.01, and Trustworthy: U = 8262.5, p < 0.01).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Impressions of the robot by customers. * means p <0.05.

Figure 5 shows the results of the customers’ impressions of the store experience. The results show that the values for the Collaborative Store are greater for all items than those for the Control Store. These results were nearly identical trends across age and gender. The results revealed significant differences between the two stores (Satisfaction: U = 7894, p < 0.01, Brand image: U = 6786.5, p < 0.01, Hope to revisit: U = 8206.5, p < 0.01), with the exception of Anxiety (U = 10,140, p = 0.53).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Impressions of the store experience by customers. * means p <0.05.

4.3 Clerk questionnaire

Figure 6 shows the clerks’ impressions of the robot-mediated operators calculated by 32 and 43 clerk questionnaires in the Control and Collaborative Stores, respectively. The results show positive impressions of the robot-mediated operators in both stores, except for the item “Friendly” in the Control Store. In addition, the robot-mediated operators in the Collaborative Store received better impressions from the clerks in all aspects than the robot-mediated operators in the Control Store. These trends are the same as those of the results for the customers. These results were verified using the Mann–Whitney U test, which revealed significant differences in all items between both stores (Intelligent: U = 407, p < 0.01, Useful: U = 489, p = 0.03, Easy to talk: U = 428, p < 0.01, Enjoyable: U = 395, p < 0.01, Friendly: U = 157.5, p < 0.01, Influential: U = 467, p = 0.01, and Trustworthy: U = 415.5, p < 0.01).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. Impressions of the robot by clerks. * means p <0.05.

Figure 7 shows the results of the clerks’ impressions of the store experience. The results for the Collaborative Store were greater for “Brand image” and “Hope to work with robot” than the corresponding values for the Control Store, and the opposite results were observed for “Anxiety” and “Frustration.” The results of both stores revealed significant differences in “Brand image” (U = 426.5, p < 0.01) and no significant differences in other items (Anxiety: U = 617.5, p = 0.30, Frustration: U = 616, p = 0.42, and Hope to work with robot: U = 567.5, p = 0.18).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 7. Impressions of the store experience by clerks. * means p <0.05.

4.4 Operator questionnaire

Figure 8 shows the results of the questionnaire related to the operators’ workload using NASA-TLX. The items “Mental demand,” “Physical demand,” “Temporal demand,” “Performance,” “Effort,” and “Frustration” are independent subscales, and “Overall weighted workload’ indicates the operator’s overall workload calculated in subscales. These results were verified using the Mann–Whitney U test, and the results revealed no significant differences in all items between both stores (Mental demand: U = 83, p = 0.50, Physical demand: U = 67, p = 0.13, Temporal demand: U = 76, p = 0.32, Performance: U = 95.5, p = 0.93, Effort: U = 64, p = 0.12, Frustration: U = 75, p = 0.30, and Overall weighted workload: U = 83, p = 0.50).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 8. Workload results of the operators using NASA-TLX.

Figure 9 shows the results of the operators’ motivation and sense of belonging. Both results show that the values for the Collaborative Store were greater than those for the Control Store. The results revealed significant differences in both items between the stores (Motivation: U = 34, p < 0.01 and Sense of belonging: U = 42.5, p < 0.01).

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 9. Motivation and sense of belonging of operators. * means p <0.05.

4.5 Interaction rate

Figure 10 shows the rate of customers who interacted with the robot for each store. The results show that the Collaborative Store has a higher interaction rate than the Control Store on all days, and the average results for the Interaction Rate between the two stores revealed significant differences (χ2 (1) = 59.96, p < 0.01, V = 0.18). The regression line at each store are Rate(control) = 0.2× day + 16.9, Rate(collaborative) = 0.6× day + 31.4, respectively. The slopes of the regression lines are positive for both stores.

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 10. Interaction Rate. The dotted line is the regression line of the interaction rate for each store.

5 Discussion

5.1 Effect of collaborative customer service on customers

The results revealed no significant difference in the PR in the Control Store but showed a difference in the PR in the Collaborative Store when comparing the situations where the robot was in operation and not in operation. These results indicate that the use of the robot as a recommendation system had a greater influence on the customers in the Collaborative Store. The robots in both the stores were operated by the same operators and used the same strategy to recommend various types of bread. The Collaborative Store, where the clerks collaborated with the robot, aimed to create an atmosphere in which a relationship between the clerks and the robot was already established. In other words, the results suggest that the persuasiveness of the robot was influenced by the social influence of the clerks, and this influence affected the sales of the recommended types of bread.

Even though the manner of interaction of the robot with the customers was the same in both stores, customers’ impressions of the robot improved for all items in the Collaborative Store. These items include the factors of social acceptance (Heerink et al., 2010) and trustworthiness in the robot. In addition to the results of the questionnaire, the high Interaction Rate of customers also indicates that the social acceptance of the robots in the Collaborative Store was high. In other words, as explained in the introduction, two important factors (social acceptance and trustworthiness) were high for the robot in the actual retail store. The result of high social acceptance implies that the robot was less likely to be ignored by customers, and the information provided by the robot was trusted, which implies that the purchase rate of the products recommended by the robot was high. Therefore, the effectiveness of the robot’s recommendations was strengthened because the customers accepted and trusted the robot owing to social influence, which is consistent with the previous study (Rau et al., 2009). This is also similar to the phenomenon of social influence described in HHI (Cialdini and Goldstein, 2004). The results of this study imply that social influence of the clerks exists even in the use of service robots in the retail industry.

A comparison of the PR between the first and last halves of the experimental period showed the longitudinal influence of the robots. In the Control Store in the first 8 days, the PR when the robot was in operation decreased by approximately 20.3% when compared with the PR when the robot was not in operation. In contrast, the PR when the robot was in operation in the last 7 days increased by 8.8% when compared with the PR when the robot was not in operation. In the Collaborative Store, the PR increased by 7.5% in the first half of the period, whereas it increased by 42.2% in the last half of the period owing to the influence of the robot’s recommendations.

Interestingly, in both stores, the influence of the robot was stronger during the last half of the period. In other words, it is possible that the greater the frequency of contact with the robot, the greater its influence, which is known as the mere exposure effect (Zajonc, 1968). The results of the Interaction Rate, which is related to the customer behavior change, show that the percentage of customers who interacted with the robot increased in the last half of the experiment in both stores. However, this is an indirect result to verify the long-term influence of the robot, and it is still unclear whether the impressions of the robot was strictly improved in the longitudinal duration. This is because we could not collect the questionnaire from customers who saw the robot more than once. These results suggest that CCS between the clerks and the robot may have accelerated the establishment of the relationship between the robot and customers. If a long-term experiment can be conducted to fully establish a robot–customer relationship, the influence of the robot between the Control and Collaborative Stores could be comparable.

The results also show that CCS improves the customers’ impressions of the store experience. In terms of brand image, this result is consistent with the results of a previous study (Chan and Tung, 2019). In contrast, one possible reason why the results for “Anxiety” did not show a significant difference between both stores is that the robot did not perform skillful tasks that made the customer feel anxious. In this study, the robot’s tasks were designed to include welcoming customers and promoting products, as proposed in a previous study (Niemelä et al., 2017b), that is, they were not essential tasks (such as baking bread, displaying the bread, and handling the checkout) for the clerks working at the bakeries. We believe that because these support tasks do not give the impression of replacing human labor, they did not cause a difference in “Anxiety” between the two stores.

Further, the impressions of the robot and store experience were positive for many items, not only in the Collaborative Store but also in the Control Store. This implies that the introduction of service robots creates a positive impression on the customers, which is consistent with the results of previous studies (Niemelä et al., 2017a; Song and Kim, 2022). Therefore, the introduction of robots is beneficial in aspects other than sales, regardless of CCS.

5.2 Influence of collaborative customer service on clerks and operators

The impression formed by clerks of the robot can be interpreted as an evaluation of the human operator rather than the robot. In this situation, the impressions of the robot-mediated operators in the Collaborative Store showed better results for all items than that in the Control Store. The increased communication, including informal communication, between the robot-mediated operators and clerks, had a more positive effect on the robot. This is consistent with the results reported in previous studies on HHI (Fay and Kline, 2011; Aplarslan and Kilinç, 2015). In addition, human perception of a speaking object is distorted even if it is known that the object is operated by another human (Kuwamura et al., 2012; Bennett et al., 2017). However, we found that communicating with a robot-mediated human could produce the same positive impressions as in the normal HHI situation.

Regarding the workload of the operations, the Collaborative Store required collaboration between the clerks and the robot operators, which could have increased their workload. However, at both stores, the clerk questionnaire showed no significant difference for the item “Frustration”; the operator questionnaire also showed no significant difference in the overall weighted workload of NASA-TLX. This implies that the application of CCS in the Collaborative Store did not have a significant effect on the workload, which suggests that CCS may not increase the workload of clerks but can increase the influence of the robot.

In addition, the evaluation of operators showed that CCS improved their motivation and sense of belonging. These results for robot-mediated telework are similar to those of a previous study on conventional telework (Fay, 2011).

5.3 Open questions for collaboration between humans and autonomous service robots

In this study, the experimental design used a robot operated by human operators, and clerks were aware of this setting. Therefore, it was easy to build a relationship between the clerks and robots, as we focused on evaluating CCS without the communication errors caused by autonomous robots. In addition, we expect that these service robots will be autonomous robots in the future, whereupon it is unclear whether they can build a similar relationship with the clerks (Lu et al., 2019; Qiu et al., 2020). This is likely to become a major issue as service robots become more widespread in the future.

In studies on industrial robots, human–robot collaboration has been actively examined (Ajoudani et al., 2018), as they have been prevalent before the introduction of service robots. One topic that is often discussed in human–robot collaboration is the relationship between robots and workers. Because industrial robots have replaced human labor in factories, it has been pointed out that in some cases, employees do not welcome the introduction of robots (Welfare et al., 2019). Therefore, robots must be assigned to activities that do not diminish the preferences and satisfaction of humans. It has been shown that employees prefer to collaborate with robots in such environments (Ferreira et al., 2014; Welfare et al., 2019). The same issue is expected to occur with service robots in the future (McClure, 2018).

Therefore, when introducing autonomous service robots in the future, we should pay attention to the relationship between robots and workers. In particular, we should eliminate the fear of workers about the introduction of service robots (McCartney and McCartney, 2020). Involving workers in co-designing the human–robot collaboration can be effective in enabling workers to gain an understanding of service robots and not feel anxious about their jobs (Niemelä et al., 2019). This study designed welcoming customers and promoting specific products as the tasks of the robot, but these are the expectations for robots that were answered by workers through workshops (Niemelä et al., 2017b). Therefore, the actual introduction of service robots for performing these tasks may not be perceived positively by the workers. Thus, several considerations are required for tasks to be assigned to service robots (Shi et al., 2016; Niemelä et al., 2017c).

In addition, although the WoZ robot was used in this study, the results of this study show that the impression of the robot by the clerks can be improved by CCS with the clerks rather than by providing customer service independently. This may have similar implications for autonomous service robots. Talking to the clerk from the robot while providing customer service is one of the elements of CCS presented in this study, and it can be implemented through interaction design on the robot side. In the future, it will be necessary to verify whether autonomous robots can build relationships with store clerks through various interaction designs including those proposed in this study.

5.4 Limitations

This study had some limitations. First, we selected two bakery stores with similar environments from several candidate stores to verify the influence of CCS. However, these two stores did not have strictly identical environments such as different types of bread sold. In addition, we compared the PRs during periods when the robot was in operation and not in operation. Because we had no pre-experiment sales data for recommended bread for each store by time of day we were unable to eliminate the effect of time of day on PRs from our analysis results. In other words, the tendency of the recommended bread purchased by the time of day may vary. Thus, we cannot argue that the results of this study can be strictly attributed to CCS. We also considered conducting A/B testing using a single store. In this case, it would be difficult to verify the effect of CCS because the results would be greatly influenced by the customer’s experience. In fact, this study showed that the influence of the robot improved in the last half of the experiment. Thus, we used two bakery stores to conduct this study to eliminate the factor of customer’s experience. This problem is not limited to this study but applies to many studies with real-world field experiments. We need to discuss the extent to which rigorous comparisons are required in future field experiments.

Second, the long-term influence of the robot was not investigated in this study, as the longitudinal influence of the robot was examined for only 15 days. Previous studies on the social acceptance of robots spanned several months, for example, 6 months (de Graaf et al., 2016). Therefore, 15 experimental days were not sufficient to investigate the long-term effect. The results of this study show that the influence of the robot improved in the last half of the experiment; however, the PR and Interaction results include customers who visited more than once, but we could not clearly verify their impact. This is because when testing in real stores, it is very difficult to identify which customers visited more than once during the experimental period. In addition, it is unclear whether the social acceptance and trustworthiness of the robot also improved because we cannot collect the questionnaire from customers who interacted with the robot more than once. Thus, it is also unclear whether this effect will be maintained over several months. We expect that the long-term relationship between the robot and customers will be maintained if we continue using the WoZ robot. However, using an autonomous robot is likely to reduce the long-term influence of the robot.

Finally, the introduction of autonomous service robots was not considered in this study. As discussed in the previous section, the introduction of autonomous service robots into a store has many potential issues such as the tasks to be assigned to the robots and functions to be considered to build a relationship with the store clerks. Because all stakeholders have different expectations regarding the introduction of service robots (Niemelä et al., 2017b; McCartney and McCartney, 2020), it is important to carefully discuss with these stakeholders and co-design autonomous robots before introducing them into the workplace (Niemelä et al., 2019).

6 Conclusion

The aim of this study was to increase the influence of robots as a recommendation system by leveraging the social influence of clerks. Therefore, we compared the influence of robots with and without collaborative customer service (CCS) with clerks in two bakery stores.

The results showed that CCS increased the purchase rate of the recommended types of bread and improved the impressions of the robot and store experience of the customers. In addition, the impression formed by the clerks of the robot was also improved. Because the results showed that the workload for the collaboration between the robot and clerks was not high, this study suggests that all stores with service robots may show high effectiveness in introducing CCS.

In this study, WoZ robots were deployed in bakery stores to investigate the effects of CCS. However, these robots will operate autonomously in the future. In this case, various potential issues need to be considered, such as the tasks to be assigned to the robots and functions needed to build relationships with customers and workers.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusion of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Research Ethics Committee of Osaka University. We informed all customers of the experiment and the related videotaping through a notification board. The study was conducted on an opt-out basis for unwilling customers who did not wish to be videotaped.

Author contributions

YO, SS, and JB conceptualized the study. SS and JB programmed the software. YO collected and analyzed the data. YO wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

Authors YO, SS, and JB were employed by CyberAgent Inc.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ajoudani, A., Zanchettin, A. M., Ivaldi, S., ffer, A. A. S., Kosuge, K., and Khatib, O. (2018). Progress and prospects of the human—Robot collaboration. Aut. Robot.42, 957–975. doi:10.1007/s10514-017-9677-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alslaity, A., and Tran, T. (2019). “Towards persuasive recommender systems,” in IEEE International Conference on Information and Computer Technologies, Kahului, HI, USA, 14-17 March 2019 (IEEE), 143–148. doi:10.1109/INFOCT.2019.8711416

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrist, S., Spannan, E., and Mutlu, B. (2013). “Rhetorical robots: Making robots more effective speakers using linguistic cues of expertise,” in ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, Tokyo, Japan, 03-06 March 2013 (IEEE), 341–348. doi:10.1109/HRI.2013.6483608

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aplarslan, A. M., and Kilinç, U. K. (2015). The power of informal communication and perceived organizational support on energy at work and extra-role behavior: A survey on teachers. J. Hum. Sci.12, 113–138. doi:10.14687/ijhs.v12i2.3243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychol. Monogr. General Appl.70, 1–70. doi:10.1037/h0093718

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belanche, D., Casaló, L. V., Flavián, C., and Schepers, J. (2020). Service robot implementation: A theoretical framework and research agenda. Serv. Industries J.40, 203–225. doi:10.1080/02642069.2019.1672666

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bennett, M., Williams, T., Thames, D., and Scheutz, M. (2017). “Differences in interaction patterns and perception for teleoperated and autonomous humanoid robots,” in IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems, Vancouver, BC, Canada, 24-28 September 2017 (IEEE), 6589–6594. doi:10.1109/IROS.2017.8206571

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertacchini, F., Bilotta, E., and Pantano, P. (2017). Shopping with a robotic companion. Comput. Hum. Behav.77, 382–395. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carolis, B. D., de Gemmis, M., Lops, P., and Palestra, G. (2017). Recognizing users feedback from non-verbal communicative acts in conversational recommender systems. Pattern Recognit. Lett.99, 87–95. doi:10.1016/j.patrec.2017.06.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, A. P. H., and Tung, V. W. S. (2019). Examining the effects of robotic service on brand experience: The moderating role of hotel segment. J. Travel and Tour. Mark.36, 458–468. doi:10.1080/10548408.2019.1568953

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chidambaram, V., Chiang, Y. H., and Mutlu, B. (2012). “Designing persuasive robots: How robots might persuade people using vocal and nonverbal cues,” in ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, Boston, MA, USA, 05-08 March 2012 (IEEE), 293–300. doi:10.1145/2157689.2157798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cialdini, R. B., and Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annu. Rev. Psychol.55, 591–621. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cialdini, R. B. (2001). Harnessing the science of persuasion. Harv. Bus. Rev.79, 72–79.

Google Scholar

de Graaf, M. M. A., Allouch, S. B., and van Dijk, J. A. G. M. (2016). Long-term evaluation of a social robot in real homes. Interact. Stud.17, 1–25. doi:10.1075/is.17.3.08deg

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fay, M. J. (2011). Informal communication of co-workers: A thematic analysis of messages. Qual. Res. Organ. Manag.6, 212–229. doi:10.1108/17465641111188394

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fay, M. J., and Kline, S. L. (2011). Coworker relationships and informal communication in high-intensity telecommuting. J. Appl. Commun. Res.39, 144–163. doi:10.1080/00909882.2011.556136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, P., Doltsinis, S., and Lohse, N. (2014). Symbiotic assembly systems – A new paradigm. Procedia CIRP17, 26–31. doi:10.1016/j.procir.2014.01.066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghazali, A. S., Ham, J., Barakova, E., and Markopoulos, P. (2020). Persuasive robots acceptance model (pram): Roles of social responses within the acceptance model of persuasive robots. Int. J. Soc. Robotics12, 1075–1092. doi:10.1007/s12369-019-00611-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haiman, F. S. (1949). An experimental study of the effects of ethos in public speaking. Speech Monogr.16, 190–202. doi:10.1080/03637754909374974

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ham, J., Cuijpers, R. H., and Cabibihan, J. J. (2015). Combining robotic persuasive strategies: The persuasive power of a storytelling robot that uses gazing and gestures. Int. J. Soc. Robotics7, 479–487. doi:10.1007/s12369-015-0280-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, S. G., and Staveland, L. E. (1988). Development of nasa-tlx (task load index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. Adv. Psychol.52, 139–183. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62386-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heerink, M., Kröse, B., Evers, V., and Wielinga, B. (2010). Assessing acceptance of assistive social agent technology by older adults: The almere model. Int. J. Soc. Robotics2, 361–375. doi:10.1007/s12369-010-0068-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley and Sons. doi:10.1037/10628-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herse, S., Vitale, J., Ebrahimian, D., Tonkin, M., Ojha, S., Sidra, S., et al. (2018a). “Bon appetit! robot persuasion for food recommendation,” in ACM/IEEE international conference on human-robot interaction (Chicago, IL: Association for Computing Machinery), 125–126. doi:10.1145/3173386.3177028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herse, S., Vitale, J., Tonkin, M., Ebrahimian, D., Ojha, S., Johnston, B., et al. (2018b). “Do you trust me, blindly? Factors influencing trust towards a robot recommender system,” in IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, Nanjing, China, 27-31 August 2018 (IEEE), 7–14. doi:10.1109/ROMAN.2018.8525581

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kadowaki, K., Kobayashi, K., and Kitamura, Y. (2008). “Influence of social relationships on multiagent persuasion,” in 7th international joint conference on Autonomous agents and multiagent systems (Estoril: International Foundation for Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems), 1221–1224.

Google Scholar

Kamei, K., Shinozawa, K., Ikeda, T., Utsumi, A., Miyashita, T., and Hagita, N. (2010). “Recommendation from robots in a real-world retail shop,” in International conference on multimodal interfaces and the workshop on machine learning for multimodal interaction (Beijing: Association for Computing Machinery), 1–8. doi:10.1145/1891903.1891929

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuwamura, K., Minato, T., Nishio, S., and Ishiguro, H. (2012). “Personality distortion in communication through teleoperated robots,” in IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, Paris, France, 09-13 September 2012 (IEEE), 49–54. doi:10.1109/ROMAN.2012.6343730

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M. K., Kiesler, S., Forlizzi, J., and Rybski, P. (2012). “Ripple effects of an embedded social agent: A field study of a social robot in the workplace,” in SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems (Austin, TX: Association for Computing Machinery), 695–704. doi:10.1145/2207676.2207776

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, L., Cai, R., and Gursoy, D. (2019). Developing and validating a service robot integration willingness scale. Int. J. Hosp. Manag.80, 36–51. doi:10.1016/j.ijhm.2019.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malhotra, Y., and Galletta, D. F. (1999). “Extending the technology acceptance model to account for social influence: Theoretical bases and empirical validation,” in Annual Hawaii International Conference on Systems Sciences, Maui, HI, USA, 05-08 January 1999 (IEEE), 1–14. doi:10.1109/HICSS.1999.772658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCartney, G., and McCartney, A. (2020). Rise of the machines: Towards a conceptual service-robot research framework for the hospitality and tourism industry. Int. J. Contemp. Hosp. Manag.32, 3835–3851. doi:10.1108/IJCHM-05-2020-0450

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClure, P. K. (2018). you’re fired, says the robot: The rise of automation in the workplace, technophobes, and fears of unemployment. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev.36, 139–156. doi:10.1177/0894439317698637

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemelä, M., Arvola, A., and Aaltonen, I. (2017a). “Monitoring the acceptance of a social service robot in a shopping mall: First results,” in ACM/IEEE international conference on human-robot interaction (Vienna: Association for Computing Machinery).

Google Scholar

Niemelä, M., Heikkilä, P., and Lammi, H. (2017b). “A social service robot in a shopping mall: Expectations of the management, retailers and consumers,” in ACM/IEEE international conference on human-robot interaction (Vienna: Association for Computing Machinery), 227–228. doi:10.1145/3029798.3038301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemelä, M., Heikkilä, P., Lammi, H., and Oksman, V. (2017c). “Shopping mall robots — Opportunities and constraints from the retailer and manager perspective,” in International conference on social Robotics (Tsukuba: Spinger), 485–494. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-70022-9_48

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemelä, M., Heikkilä, P., Lammi, H., and Oksman, V. (2019). A social robot in a shopping mall: Studies on acceptance and stakeholder expectations. Switzerland: Springer Nature. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-17107-0_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okafuji, Y., Baba, J., Nakanishi, J., Amada, J., Yoshikawa, Y., and Ishiguro, H. (2021). “Persuasion strategies for social robot to keep humans accepting daily different recommendations,” in IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems, Prague, Czech Republic, 27 September 2021 - 01 October 2021 (IEEE), 1950–1957. doi:10.1109/IROS51168.2021.9636772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okafuji, Y., Ozaki, Y., Baba, J., Nakanishi, J., Ogawa, K., Yoshikawa, Y., et al. (2022). Behavioral assessment of a humanoid robot when attracting pedestrians in a mall. Int. J. Soc. Robotics14, 1731–1747. doi:10.1007/s12369-022-00907-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Keefe, D. J. (1990). Persuasion: Theory and research. Newbury Park: CA: Sage Publications. doi:10.1177/002194369202900107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, Z., Kwon, Y., Lu, J., Wu, Z., and Ma, X. (2019). Design and evaluation of service robot’s proactivity in decision-making support process. CHI Conf. Hum. Factors Comput. Syst.98, 1–13. doi:10.1145/3290605.3300328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pu, P., Chen, L., and Hu, R. (2012). Evaluating recommender systems from the user’s perspective: Survey of the state of the art. User Model. User-Adapted Interact.22, 317–355. doi:10.1007/s11257-011-9115-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qiu, H., Li, M., Shu, B., and Bai, B. (2020). Enhancing hospitality experience with service robots: The mediating role of rapport building. J. Hosp. Mark. Manag.29, 247–268. doi:10.1080/19368623.2019.1645073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rau, P. P., Li, Y., and Li, D. (2009). Effects of communication style and culture on ability to accept recommendations from robots. Comput. Hum. Behav.25, 587–595. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riek, L. D. (2012). Wizard of oz studies in hri: A systematic review and new reporting guidelines. J. Human-Robot Interact.1, 119–136. doi:10.5898/JHRI.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakai, K., Nakamura, Y., Yoshikawa, Y., and Ishiguro, H. (2022). Effect of robot embodiment on satisfaction with recommendations in shopping malls. IEEE Robotics Automation Lett.7, 366–372. doi:10.1109/LRA.2021.3128233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salomons, N., Sebo, S. S., Qin, M., and Scassellati, B. (2017). A minority of one against a majority of robots: Robots cause normative and informational conformity. ACM Trans. Human-Robot Interact.10 (15), 1–22. doi:10.1145/3442627

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunderson, S., and Nejat, G. (2022). Investigating strategies for robot persuasion in social human–robot interaction. IEEE Trans. Cybern.52, 641–653. doi:10.1109/TCYB.2020.2987463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, C., Satake, S., Kanda, T., and Ishiguro, H. (2016). “How would store managers employ social robots?,” in ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, Christchurch, New Zealand, 07-10 March 2016 (IEEE), 519–520. doi:10.1109/HRI.2016.7451835

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiomi, M., Shinozawa, K., Nakagawa, Y., Miyashita, T., Sakamoto, T., Terakubo, T., et al. (2013). Recommendation effects of a social robot for advertisement-use context in a shopping mall. Int. J. Soc. Robotics5, 251–262. doi:10.1007/s12369-013-0180-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, C. S., and Kim, Y. K. (2022). The role of the human-robot interaction in consumers’ acceptance of humanoid retail service robots. J. Bus. Res.146, 489–503. doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2022.03.087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, S., Baba, J., Nakanishi, J., Yoshikawa, Y., and Ishiguro, H. (2022). Service robots in a bakery shop: A field study. arXiv:2208.09260. doi:10.48550/arXiv.2208.09260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, S., Baba, J., Nakanishi, J., Yoshikawa, Y., and Ishiguro, H. (2021). Teleoperated robot sells toothbrush in a shopping mall: A field study. Ext. Abstr. 2021 CHI Conf. Hum. Factors Comput. Syst.221, 1–6. doi:10.1145/3411763.3451754

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tae, M. I., Ogawa, K., Yoshikawa, Y., and Ishiguro, H. (2021). Using multiple robots to increase suggestion persuasiveness in public space. Appl. Sci.11, 6080. doi:10.3390/app11136080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, K., Yamashita, N., Nakanishi, H., and Ishiguro, H. (2016). “Teleoperated or autonomous?: How to produce a robot operator’s pseudo presence in hri,” in ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human Robot Interaction, Christchurch, New Zealand, 07-10 March 2016 (IEEE), 133–140. doi:10.1109/HRI.2016.7451744

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tonkin, M., Vitale, J., Ojha, S., Williams, M. A., Fuller, P., Judge, W., et al. (2017). “Would you like to sample? Robot engagement in a shopping centre,” in IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, Lisbon, Portugal, 28 August 2017 - 01 September 2017 (IEEE), 42–49. doi:10.1109/ROMAN.2017.8172278

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watanabe, M., Ogawa, K., and Ishiguro, H. (2015). Can androids be salespeople in the real world?Annu. ACM Conf. Ext. Abstr. Hum. Factors Comput. Syst.5, 251–262. doi:10.1145/2702613.2702967

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Welfare, K. S., Hallowell, M. R., Shah, J. A., and Riek, L. D. (2019). “Consider the human work experience when integrating robotics in the workplace,” in ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, Daegu, Korea (South), 11-14 March 2019 (IEEE), 75–84. doi:10.1109/HRI.2019.8673139

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woiceshyn, L., Wang, Y., Nejat, G., and Benhabib, B. (2017). “Personalized clothing recommendation by a social robot,” in International Symposium on Robotics and Intelligent Sensors, Ottawa, ON, Canada, 05-07 October 2017 (IEEE), 179–185. doi:10.1109/IRIS.2017.8250118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, L., and Kumar, V. (2019). Robotics for customer service: A useful complement or an ultimate substitute?J. Serv. Res.24, 9–29. doi:10.1177/1094670519878881

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J. Personality Soc. Psychol.9, 1–27. doi:10.1037/h0025848

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social service robot, customer service, recommendation system, social influence, field trial

Citation: Okafuji Y, Song S, Baba J, Yoshikawa Y and Ishiguro H (2023) Influence of collaborative customer service by service robots and clerks in bakery stores. Front. Robot. AI 10:1125308. doi: 10.3389/frobt.2023.1125308

Received: 16 December 2022; Accepted: 19 June 2023;
Published: 03 July 2023.

Edited by:

Ker-Jiun Wang, University of Pittsburgh, United States

Reviewed by:

Francesco Del Duchetto, University of Lincoln, United Kingdom
Laura Fiorini, University of Florence, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Okafuji, Song, Baba, Yoshikawa and Ishiguro. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yuki Okafuji, okafuji_yuki_xd@cyberagent.co.jp

Download