Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Sociol., 12 March 2024
Sec. Sociological Theory
Volume 9 - 2024 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fsoc.2024.1194597

Is society caught up in a Death Spiral? Modeling societal demise and its reversal

  • 1Department of Organisation and Personnel Management, Rotterdam School of Management, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Epidemiology and Population Health, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
  • 4Department of Biomedical Data Science, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
  • 5Department of Statistics, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
  • 6Meta-Research Innovation Center at Stanford, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States
  • 7Department of History of Philosophy, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands

Just like an army of ants caught in an ant mill, individuals, groups and even whole societies are sometimes caught up in a Death Spiral, a vicious cycle of self-reinforcing dysfunctional behavior characterized by continuous flawed decision making, myopic single-minded focus on one (set of) solution(s), denial, distrust, micromanagement, dogmatic thinking and learned helplessness. We propose the term Death Spiral Effect to describe this difficult-to-break downward spiral of societal decline. Specifically, in the current theory-building review we aim to: (a) more clearly define and describe the Death Spiral Effect; (b) model the downward spiral of societal decline as well as an upward spiral; (c) describe how and why individuals, groups and even society at large might be caught up in a Death Spiral; and (d) offer a positive way forward in terms of evidence-based solutions to escape the Death Spiral Effect. Management theory hints on the occurrence of this phenomenon and offers turn-around leadership as solution. On a societal level strengthening of democracy may be important. Prior research indicates that historically, two key factors trigger this type of societal decline: rising inequalities creating an upper layer of elites and a lower layer of masses; and dwindling (access to) resources. Historical key markers of societal decline are a steep increase in inequalities, government overreach, over-integration (interdependencies in networks) and a rapidly decreasing trust in institutions and resulting collapse of legitimacy. Important issues that we aim to shed light on are the behavioral underpinnings of decline, as well as the question if and how societal decline can be reversed. We explore the extension of these theories from the company/organization level to the society level, and make use of insights from both micro-, meso-, and macro-level theories (e.g., Complex Adaptive Systems and collapsology, the study of the risks of collapse of industrial civilization) to explain this process of societal demise. Our review furthermore draws on theories such as Social Safety Theory, Conservation of Resources Theory, and management theories that describe the decline and fall of groups, companies and societies, as well as offer ways to reverse this trend.

1 Introduction

Ants rely on each other for survival and often hunt for prey together. They use pheromones to locate each other and they follow the ones in front of them. This usually works quite well, although sometimes the ants get locked in what is called an “ant mill” or “Death Spiral.” This can happen when a subset of ants gets separated from the main foraging group and begin following each other. They start forming a continuously rotating circle, and the ants caught up in this Death Spiral often die from exhaustion. It has even been observed that dead ants are being pushed out of the circle, while the ants maintain their rounds. This “ant mill” or “circular milling paradox” seems to be the evolutionary price that army ants pay for an otherwise successful strategy of collective foraging (cf. Delsuc, 2003). The pathological, dysfunctional behavior is the other side of the coin of otherwise functional behavior. Rosabeth Moss Kanter, who spent years of studying declining organizations, concluded that a process similar to a Death Spiral may be happening to failing companies (Kanter, 2003). After years of success, these companies have trouble managing processes when the tide turns and problems occur. Instead of looking for solutions with an open mind, companies often get caught up in a Death Spiral, making decisions that seem rational, such as downsizing and centralized decision making (cf. Charan et al., 2002; Lamberg et al., 2017). Often these decisions worsen the situation instead of making it better, and self-destructive habits include denial, complacency and cost-inefficiency (Sheth, 2007). Sheth (2007) argues that denial of the new reality and internal turf wars, i.e., territorial impulse, are two dangerous self-destructive habits that can further send a company into decline. Companies are reluctant to admit they are in trouble and instead blame circumstances outside their control (Lorange and Nelson, 1985; Charan et al., 2002). Management research has also shown that long before the crisis within a company becomes apparent, the signs are there, but often go unnoticed or are ignored (Lorange and Nelson, 1985; Fitzgerald, 2005). Having to address these problems down the line, often leads to taking drastic steps and overreaction that may further fuel decline (Lorange and Nelson, 1985; Hafsi and Baba, 2022).

Using the metaphor of a corporate heart attack, Fitzgerald discerns a hidden, subtle and overt phase of decline (Fitzgerald, 2005). In the hidden phase, denial or willful blindness often prohibits management from taking (the right) actions. Against their better judgment, they hope if they ignore it, the market will not notice. In that phase, on average a third of a company’s competitive value is lost. If a new market challenge presents itself, the company is often unable to face the challenge. In the subtle phase, the decline becomes more obvious for those who are observant and know where and how to look and how to interpret what they see. By the end of this phase, often a full two-thirds of the company’s competitive value is lost. Unfortunately, many companies only start to admit and address the problem in the overt phase. By that time, the problems are so big and ingrained, that addressing them has become extremely difficult. While many managers do watch the company’s financials, they often fail to address other metrics such as market-share trends, customer turnover and staff satisfaction. Often these drivers are the earliest predictors of corporate performance. Important blockers of performance are distrust, bureaucracy and low performance expectations, while drivers are decisiveness, accountability and acknowledgement of work. It is key to identify and quantify early warning signals, e.g., an excess of staff, especially managers, a decrease in lower-level workers, tolerance of incompetence, and lack of clear goals (Lorange and Nelson, 1985). Reversing organizational decline starts with the realization and recognition that the organization is in decline. These danger signals should then be aligned with a concrete plan to change. A dialog between top-down and bottom-up is needed (Lorange and Nelson, 1985). If the company is able to take those steps, follow-up monitoring is needed to make sure the changes that are proposed and made are effective (Lorange and Nelson, 1985). While in the early phases underreaction may be the problem, in later phases, the danger comes from overreaction (cf. Lai and Sudarsanam, 1997; Hafsi and Baba, 2022).

We believe that similar processes may happen at the societal level. Recent examples of societal systemic shocks are 9/11, the 2008 global financial crisis and the COVID-19 crisis (Centeno et al., 2022). On a societal level, researchers studying policy success and failure have started to investigate the role of policy under- and over-reactions (Maor, 2012, 2020). Policy overreactions are “policies that impose objective and/or perceived social costs without producing offsetting objective and/or perceived benefits.” (Maor, 2012; p. 235). For instance, preemptive overreaction is a form of policy that will often rely on persuasion by presenting “facts” in a certain way, manufacturing a perceived threat, and using messages to swing the public mood (Maor, 2012). An example is the cull of all pigs in Egypt during the swine flu crisis in 2009, even though zero cases had been reported (Maor, 2012). An important explanation is that in such cases groupthink may play a role. Groupthink, the forced conformity to group values and ethics, has symptoms such as collective rationalization, belief in inherent morality, stereotyped views of outgroups, pressure on dissenters, and self-appointed mind guards (Janis, 1972, 1982a,b; Janis et al., 1978). Preemptive overreaction shows that one is taking forceful and decisive action against a perceived threat, that may never materialize, and motives could be political and/or monetary gain (Maor, 2012).

While the period before the COVID-19 crisis may have been characterized by relative policy underreaction to complex social problems, also referred to as “wicked problems,” such as hunger and poverty (Head, 2018; Head B.W. 2022), the current times may be characterized by overreaction to certain problems. The COVID-19 crisis seemed to be characterized by groupthink and escalation of commitment to one course of action, at the expense of other possible solutions (Joffe, 2021; Schippers and Rus, 2021). Initial low-quality decision-making was followed by decisions that made things worse (Joffe, 2021; Schippers and Rus, 2021). The sheer scale and severe disruption caused by these policies has increased inequalities (Schippers, 2020; Schippers et al., 2022), an important marker of societal decline (Motesharrei et al., 2014).

A meta-theory explaining such disruptive events is Complex Adaptive Systems Theory, a theory that suggests that developments in systems of many constituents are often non-linear and systems, such as societies, show unexpected and self-organizing behavior (Lansing, 2003; Holovatch et al., 2017). Moreover, “mechanisms like tipping points, feedback loops, contagions, cascades, synchronous failures, and cycles that can be responsible for systemic collapse are fundamental characteristics of any complex adaptive system, and can therefore serve as a useful common denominator from which to examine collapses” (Centeno et al., 2022; p. 71). In today’s society, our continued survival increasingly depends on tightly coupled complex and fragile systems (e.g., supply chains), over which no one has responsibility (Centeno et al., 2022). The potential for contemporary collapse makes it more compelling than ever to learn from past collapse for insight (Centeno et al., 2022) and to study global patterns of behavior. The normative basis for our paper relates to ‘the greater good’: What is deemed good for the thriving of humanity is seen as ‘good’ or ‘functional’ while what is seen as ‘bad’ or ‘dysfunctional’ is behavior or decisions that harm the thriving of humanity.

2 Downward spiral

In the current narrative and theory-building review we coin the term Death Spiral Effect to describe this type of overreaction and the resulting cascading effects in policies affecting the general public. We review the current literature and extend complex adaptive systems theory to construct our theoretical model of collapse and reversal of societal decline. We view the Death Spiral Effect as a specific form of complex (mal)adaptive behavior that accelerates decline and makes it hard to reverse decline. Making use of the ant mill metaphor, we theorize that a Death Spiral Effect emerges where a society gets caught up in a dysfunctional behavioral mode. Making use of this metaphor, we aim to aid theory building around this construct (Shepherd and Suddaby, 2016). We describe the elements of this vicious downward cycle, such as rising inequalities, dysfunctional behavior of both elite and masses, and rise of authoritarianism. We examine how the behavioral underpinnings of the resulting environment can lead to escalation through war, famine, and pandemics. While there is a rich literature on early warning signs and markers of societal decline, the underlying mechanisms have received much less attention and explanations often miss the depth that the psychological, sociological and management theories may offer. We draw on theories such as collapsology (the transdisciplinary study of industrial civilization risk of collapse), Complex Adaptive Systems Theory, Social Safety Theory (that focuses on friendly social bonds development and maintenance), Conservation of Resources Theory (that focuses on obtaining and maintaining resources), and general management theories that describe the decline of groups. We also use Social Dominance Theory to explain how and why the resulting inequalities are hard to reverse (Pratto, 1999; Pratto et al., 2006). Our work is also related to X-risk studies, a field of study that looks at existential risks for humanity (Torres, 2018; Moynihan, 2020): We recognize the importance of cascading risks, and hint at how catastrophic (X)-risks could potentially combine to jeopardize human survival (Torres, 2018; Baum et al., 2019; Moynihan, 2020; Undheim and Zimmer, 2023). We use a ‘Big Picture’ approach to these problems (Campbell et al., 2023). We then depict a possible upward spiral, dissecting what elements are needed to reverse the Death Spiral and build a society where people can thrive and prosper. In doing so, we contribute to theory building around the psychological and sociological drivers of societal decline (Swedberg, 2016). Our aim is to contribute to knowledge about societal decline and flourishing in order to enhance mankind’s chances of flourishing.

2.1 Crisis and crisis handling

Several authors have noted that societal decline has similar phases to organizational decline in companies, including early warning signs (Tainter, 1989; Downey et al., 2016; Scheffer, 2016; Jones, 2021; Demarest and Victor, 2022). Compared to decline in organizations, however, the scale at which this happens is bigger, the social consequences are more complex, and the decline may often be a more long-term process. The average lifespan of a company in the Standard and Poor’s 500 index in 2020 was 21.4 years (Clark et al., 2021) while some historical empires have lasted many decades or centuries (Taagepera, 1979). Another difference between organizations and society is that the outcome of decline can often not be buffered by society, such as would be the case in company decline. Also the hard outcomes (which may include war, famine and widespread disease) can be extremely hard to reverse (Downey et al., 2016). These three, war, famine and pandemics, we call the “Triangle of Death,” an expression coined by former Green Beret and combat correspondent Michael Yon (Yon and Peterson, 2022). However, Demarest and Victor (2022), p. 788 note that: “Even today the greatest challenge to knowledge coming from collapse studies–relevant not just for policy-makers and managers, but for the citizens of the entire society–is that no one really deeply believes that total collapse is possible”.

The process of societal decline is complex and may include social-ecological traps, or a mismatch between the responses of people and the social and ecological conditions they face, e.g., depletion of natural resources (Boonstra and De Boer, 2013; Boonstra et al., 2016). For the current review, we feel that the handling of the COVID-19 crisis may have been an example of overreaction making use of non-pharmaceutical interventions that accelerated existing societal problems, such as inequalities (Schippers, 2020; Schippers et al., 2022). Most countries opted for very similar solutions, with forced lockdowns and aggressive restrictions. Countries that chose a different course of action were highly criticized (Tegnell, 2021). Many countries eventually faced excess mortality rates that were highly unequal across groups, exacerbating preexisting inequalities (Alsan et al., 2021; Schippers et al., 2022). Over-reaction was fueled by (unreliable) metrics (Schippers and Rus, 2021; Ioannidis et al., 2022) and groupthink, resulting in irrational or dysfunctional decision making (Joffe, 2021; Hafsi and Baba, 2022). Furthermore, emotions during crises tend to run high, escalating the risk of harmful overreaction both by policy makers and the general public (Sunstein and Zeckhauser, 2010). Governments may suffer from an action bias, a tendency to take action whether it is needed or not, including excessive actions (Patt and Zeckhauser, 2000) despite information that the policies may do more harm than good (for reviews see Joffe and Redman, 2021, Schippers and Rus, 2021; Schippers et al., 2022). Unnecessary crisis response as a form of policy overreaction may sometimes occur as a way to shape voters perceptions of a decisive and active government (Maor, 2020). Excessive action and exercise of control over societal structures, e.g., public health, may enhance centralization of power and decision-making, and authoritarianism (Berberoglu, 2020; Desmet, 2022; Schippers et al., 2022; Simandan et al., 2023). When governments make use of mass media to spread negative information, a self-reinforcing cycle of nocebo effects, “mass hysteria” and policy errors can ensue (Bagus et al., 2021). This effect is exacerbated when the information comes from authoritative sources, the media are politicized, social networks make the information omnipresent (Bagus et al., 2021), and dissenting voices are silenced (Schippers et al., 2022; Shir-Raz et al., 2022). This may lead to a vicious cycle of ineffective dealing with crises, low-quality decision-making and dysfunctional behavior, intensifying the current crises and leading to new ones, and eventually societal decline and even collapse.

According to Holden (2005), p. 651, a complex adaptive system is “a collection of individual agents with freedom to act in ways that are not always totally predictable and whose actions are interconnected. Examples include a colony of termites, the financial market, and a surgical team”. An important element is emergence, the idea that complex global patterns can emerge from local interactions (Lansing, 2003). In the current paper, following authors such as Buckley (1998) and Lansing (2003), we view society as a complex adaptive system, with nested systems such as organizations and governments as part of the larger system. We suggest that the adaptive system is entering an unstable time, in the form of a series of crises. We extend Complex Systems Theory, by adding the Death Spiral as an element, where the system becomes so unstable, that actors within the network start to hold on to repetitive maladaptive (decision-making) behavior (Schwarzer, 2022); making the same decision over and over again, leading to unwanted results such as war, famine and pandemics (Triangle of Death) and ultimately the demise of society. In doing so, we explain the underlying mechanisms of societal decline and outline possibilities for reversal.

Below we will first define and describe the process of a Death Spiral, and the similarities and differences between a Death Spiral and other concepts such as group think and mass formation. We will do this in the context of the more meta-theory of complex adaptive systems. Second, we will describe the elements of a societal downward (death) spiral, e.g., low-quality decision-making, rise of authoritarianism, and dysfunctional behavior of both the elite and masses (see Figures 1, 2). We will do so in the context of historical as well as current examples. Third, we describe the possibilities for an upward spiral, e.g., presence of a high-quality turn-around leadership, restoration of trust, and development of turnaround strategy.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Death Spiral Effect: downward spiral of societies and/or groups in decline.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Death Spiral model of societies in decline.

2.2 Death Spiral considerations

When people and groups encounter difficulties or trauma (or sometimes for no apparent reason), they can start to make decisions that do not ensure survival, but seem self-destructive at best (cf. Balcombe and De Leo, 2022). They may make decisions to cope with the situations, but these can be characterized as mal-adaptive, non-adaptive, or semi-adaptive (Marien, 2009). Attempts to escape a downward spiral sometimes make it worse, by using counterproductive coping mechanisms (e.g., Freyhofer et al., 2021). The dysfunctional behavior continues if the spiral is not broken, and decline may follow from increasingly fragmented political institutions (cf. Kreml, 1994). In terms of Complex Adaptive Systems Theory, the system is then entering an unstable time, in this case a crisis (Centeno et al., 2022). When the system gets a blow, for instance from financial decline, depletion of resources, or other turns of fortune (Motesharrei et al., 2014), groups or societies may feel compelled to take action without considering carefully whether their decision-making process is valid (cf. Schippers et al., 2014). The threat-rigidity effect predicts a restriction in information processing and constriction of control under conditions of threat (Staw et al., 1981). The whole system becomes unstable and dysfunctional behavior sets in (Mohrman and Mohrman, 1983). The environment becomes generally stressful and threatening, eliciting more and more self-protective and rigid behaviors, that further threatens stability and group survival (Staw et al., 1981).

Finally, individuals and groups may tend to go around their lives in “circles” repeating the same mistakes, seemingly trapped in one behavioral mode. In organizations, similar Death Spiral pathologies can set in when changes in the environment do not invoke adaptation, but secrecy, blame, avoidance as well as passivity and learned helplessness (Kanter, 2003). In the general management literature, dysfunctional behavior is often described as a form of antisocial behavior, intended to bring harm (e.g., Giacalone and Greenberg, 1997; Van Fleet and Griffin, 2006). In the current paper, dysfunctional behavior is seen as counterproductive or ineffective behavior, that may have outlived its usefulness, and does not have the intended effect and may even have (unintended) harmful outcomes (Robinson, 2008). In companies, dysfunctional or counterproductive work behavior undermines efficiency and can range from social loafing (putting less effort when working as part of a group than when working alone), conflict and withdrawal to theft, fraud, bullying and even murder (Robinson, 2008). The more “civilized” forms of dysfunctional behavior, such as social loafing and withdrawal, are most prevalent (Robinson, 2008), and these can become much more common in organizations and societies that are in a downward spiral, and undermine individual autonomy. People feeling powerless in organizations exercising excess power are often triggered to perform counterproductive work behaviors (Lawrence and Robinson, 2007). During the COVID-19 crisis, withdrawal effects became more widespread and the crisis sparked changes in attitudes toward work as well as changing work behaviors inside organizations (Newman et al., 2022). For many workers, stress levels increased, and work performance declined (e.g., Vaziri et al., 2020; Kumar et al., 2021).

At the organizational level, decline often sparks dislike and distrust among managers, who then start to avoid one another, hide information and deflect blame (Kanter, 2003). People within the organization do not act in concert anymore and the dwindling success rate of their actions make them feel helpless (Kanter, 2003). In such situations, managers often resort to micromanagement: trying to control the actions of workers at a frustrating level of detail to steer them back to productivity. The pushback from workers will be to misbehave as a form of organizational resistance (Lawrence and Robinson, 2007), self-reinforcing cycles of micromanagement and counterproductive work behaviors (cf. Jensen and Raver, 2012; Cannon, 2022). A toxic work or societal culture may emerge and persist for some time, with fear as an overriding principle (Cannon, 2022). In society at large, the dangers of a “toxic discourse” around pending disasters (Buell, 1998; Horfrichter, 2000) may have paved the way for drastic measures taken to avoid such disasters (Schippers, 2020). However, some measures taken to prevent these hypothetical or expected future disasters have caused damage, leading to a steep increase in poverty and inequalities (Schippers et al., 2022). Besides many layoffs, many people reflected on their working life and subsequently decided to quit their job. The resulting “Great Resignation” seemed to be a world-wide phenomenon (Jiskrova, 2022; Sull et al., 2022; Del Rio Chanona et al., 2023). In the United States, monthly resignation rates were higher than in the previous 20 years (Statistics, 2021; Jiskrova, 2022). Note that many workers changed jobs and did not withdraw from the work force altogether (“Great Reshuffle”; Sull et al., 2022). However, at the beginning of 2021, more than 40% of workers were thinking of quitting, and a toxic work culture was mentioned as an important reason (Sull et al., 2022). At the same time decline in organizations was often triggered by the COVID-19 crisis and non-pharmaceutical interventions implemented to reduce viral spread, such as closing of restaurants and “non -essential” shops (Brodeur et al., 2021). As early as April 2020 in the United States, the number of active business owners decreased by 22% within just 3 months (Fairlie, 2020; Brodeur et al., 2021). Taken together with other effects such as rising inequalities, increase in immigration, changed labor market, damaged mental health and well-being, this is arguably a big shock to societal cohesion (Silveira et al., 2022), increasing state fragility and decreasing state legitimacy (Seyoum, 2020).

In both society at large, as well as in many companies, toxic cultures can ensue during crises (cf. Meidav, 2021). In such cultures, behavior that management or governments would like to see is rewarded, while many (mal)practices go unchecked, leaving room for fraud and corruption (cf. Kerr, 1975; Meidav, 2021; Breevaart et al., 2022). Indicative of such a toxic culture are: (lowered) level of helpfulness of people, (in)formality and (blind) enforcement of rules, underground avoidance of rules, feeling that things could be better but also feeling unable to change them, moaning “around the water cooler,” loss of morale, lack of initiative, top-down decision making, “double speak,” and lack of cohesion (Cannon, 2022). People are generally willing to do the right thing but find many roadblocks when they try (Myers, 2008). Moreover, historical research has shown that people fall back on “overlearned” comfort behavior, and biases become entrained again. For instance, a fallback on preference for ingroups ensures that during crises diversity efforts in companies are reduced and inequalities rise (Meidav, 2021). During organizational change, employee misconduct increases (Ethics and Compliance Initiative, 2020; Meidav, 2021) including even antisocial behavior (Belschak et al., 2018).

2.3 Death Spiral Effect: definition and key characteristics

Based on the above considerations, here we formally define the Death Spiral Effect as: A vicious cycle of self-reinforcing dysfunctional behavior, characterized by continuous flawed decision making, myopic single-minded focus on one (set of) solution(s), resource loss, denial, distrust, micromanagement, dogmatic thinking and learned helplessness. The Death Spiral is often initiated by an external or internal event (e.g., crisis) causing a trauma or emotional response. In the case of man-made crises, a positive feedback loop of perverse incentives may cause a stable society to spiral into disorder (Centeno et al., 2022). The Death Spiral Effect sets in when a cascade of events is difficult to stop once set in motion (cf. Centeno et al., 2022). On a societal level this spiral results in increasing gap between elite and masses, rising authoritarianism and massive resource loss. For instance, it has been noted that ancient civilizations on the brink of collapse used scarce resource for megalomaniac projects, such as huge temples, in a desperate attempt to legitimize declining institutions, but ultimately staying on the course toward disintegration (Demarest and Victor, 2022).

A Death Spiral is characterized by: (1) initial denial of the problem; (2) continuously and repeated flawed decision-making, often trying to fix the problem with the same ineffective solution over and over again; (3) increasing secrecy and denial, blame and scorn, avoidance and turf-protection, passivity and helplessness; (4) worsening of the situation, and a continuous (series of) crises following, further triggering a “survival mode” and tunnel vision, and (5) the felt or observed inability to escape or snap out of the ineffective cycle of decision-making. Other characteristics that emerge when the Death Spiral becomes apparent are: (1) a negative and distrustful atmosphere; (2) micromanagement: individuals, management or government trying to increase the number of (strict) rules and a focus on the adherence to those rules at the expense of effective problem-solving; and (3) censorship of opinions and knowledge outside the official narrative. These elements may be present to variable degrees concurrently and may reinforce each other. As the downward cycle continues, and resources loss escalates, the desperation principle may set in: a defensive mode in which people or groups aggressively and often irrationally try to hold on to the little resources that are left (Hobfoll et al., 2018), instead of thinking on how to snap out of the situation altogether.

In Figure 2, we discern four phases of societal development and demise: In phase I society is developing and growing. During this phase groups are working together without large problems. Social intuitions are founded and strengthened. In phase II, a functional society is relatively stable although, social inequalities are already increasing in this phase. In phase III, a dysfunctional society, the seeds of discontent sown during phase II have now matured: social inequalities are becoming more extreme, resulting in an increasing number of societal problems and an uptake in societal dysfunctionalities. Governments often react via centralization of power, and a rise in authoritarianism instead of involving the general public in solving societal problems. In phase IV, we see a declining or collapsing society. If the problems of decline, that started in Phase II and III, are not addressed then society will decline and may eventually collapse. Collapse is characterized by the Triangle of Death: war, famine and pandemics (Karabushenko et al., 2021; cf. Kuecker, 2007; Yon and Peterson, 2022; see also Figure 1).

2.4 Differences from other concepts

While we define the Death Spiral Effect as a specific form of collapse within an adaptive complex system, the concept of a Death Spiral is an umbrella concept that has some overlap with but also distinct features from some other concepts, such as group think, mass formation, Abilene paradox, and group polarization. In Table 1 we list those concepts and give an overview of similarities and differences versus the Death Spiral Effect. All those concepts deal with forms of dysfunctional decision-making. However, the main difference is a combination of the repetitiveness of the dysfunctional decision-making process, and the stubborn and prolonged effect of the subsequent series of decision-making (See Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Death Spiral Effect compared to other related concepts.

The Death Spiral Effect differs from groupthink, group polarization and the Abilene paradox in that groupthink, group polarization and the Abilene paradox are often related to a more finite series of decisions around one topic or outcome (e.g., the invasion of the Pig Bay) and focuses more on group harmony and agreement (Janis, 1972, 1982a,b; Harvey, 1974). Thus, while groupthink, group polarization and the Abilene paradox may often be part of a Death Spiral, a Death Spiral is more long-lasting, pervading, and pathological dysfunctional behavior and affects many aspects of a person’s life, a team, a company or even the whole society. At a certain moment, similar to groupthink, self-appointed mind guards appear, but the scale is much bigger. The Death Spiral Effect takes groupthink a step further, it can lead to the collapse of a full society.

Mass formation has also been offered as an explanation for what is happening in society (Desmet, 2022; Schippers et al., 2022). This theory sees the people in society as a swarm, that will move in one direction, following a single narrative. The mass formation concept does not have a going around in circles’ element, that the Death Spiral has. The swarm-like element in this theory states that people do attend to others’ behavior and copy that behavior (Bak-Coleman et al., 2021; Desmet, 2022). Swarm-dynamics are also studied within complex adaptive systems research (Huepe et al., 2011). While mass formation can be part of the Death Spiral Effect, and irrational group behavior is an element of this effect, the Death Spiral gives a broader explanation of what happens if (groups of) people get stuck in this cycle.

The dysfunctional behavior shown in a Death Spiral also includes micromanagement, a leadership style that stifles creativity and innovation (Allcorn, 2022) and has been pointed out to be a danger in terms of human freedom and an open society (Esfeld, 2022; see Table 1). “Tit for tat” is a concept from game theory, that is somewhat similar to the Death Spiral Effect in that parties get stuck in a behavioral mode, reaching suboptimal results for the involved parties, while it would be possible for the parties to change their behavior (and thereby get better results). The key difference is that the scope of the Death Spiral Effect is much broader, with more far-reaching implications and ripple effects.

2.5 Societies in decline: Death Spirals throughout history

Scientists have offered a variety of explanations for the collapse of civilizations through ancient and modern history (Tainter, 1989; Spengler, 1991; Bunce et al., 2009; Scheffer, 2016). Oftentimes, a combination of factors may play a role in societal decline (Jones, 2021). Nevertheless, recurrent patterns operate (Jones, 2021). Oftentimes, markers of decline are clear, and the decline may have set in long before the collapse (Scheffer, 2016). The study of societal collapse, collapsology, is traditionally studied by historians, anthropologists and political scientists. Also, experts in cliodynamics and complex systems have joined this field, although experts within management and psychology to date could potentially have much to offer in terms of behavioral explanations. Similar to the initial phase of decline in companies, societies tend to act too late, they resist change until smooth adjustments have become impossible (Scheffer, 2016). The “sunk cost effect,” based on escalation of commitment prevents people from leaving and abandoning their property, ways of living and beliefs, even when the need to do so becomes apparent (Janssen et al., 2003; Scheffer, 2016). Also, oftentimes elites may have a vested interest in maintaining the status quo (cf. Pratto et al., 2006; Wilkinson and Pickett R. G., 2009).

From a psychological point of view, trauma causing a shift in behavioral mode from functional to dysfunctional seems key to understanding the Death Spiral Effect. From a biological point of view, collapse can be viewed as inevitable after a period of large population growth (Downey et al., 2016). As complex systems, common factors may contribute to decline, and these may have ripple or cascading effects (Diamond, 2013). For a long time, the Malthusian catastrophe (the idea that the population growth outgrows the (linear) food supply, causing mass starvation and deaths) was perceived as a major threat (e.g., Diamond, 2013; Ramya et al., 2020). However, within a complex agricultural system, it seems possible to feed a growing world population. Also there seems to be general agreement in the literature that food shortages in past times were not the sole cause of societal collapse, and maybe even more a consequence of societies inability to deal with their problems (Diamond, 2013). Erosion of established systems and resulting lack of loyalty to established political institutions plus an increase in inequalities are all markers of decline (Turchin, 2007; Diamond, 2013; Van Bavel and Scheffer, 2021). In the interconnected globalized ‘system-of-systems’, ‘a failure in one part could lead to disaster across the whole structure’ (Centeno et al., 2022; p. 61). Some see signs that society may be at the brink of collapse (Page, 2005), and that while the scale of disaster can be unprecedented, lessons from the past in terms of complex systems are still relevant today (Centeno et al., 2022). Poor institutional choices result in an inability to solve collective action problems (Page, 2005). It has recently been noted that we live in a great third power shift in modern history, after the first, the rise of the Western world since the 15th century, and the second, near the end of the 19th century, the rise of the United States (Peters et al., 2022). The current power shift is defined by a rise of China, India, Brazil and Russia. An important problem that the US are dealing with is not only the growth of economic inequalities, which are huge, but also political division of society, military overreach and financial crises (Peters et al., 2022). The power elite have positions that enable them to make decisions that have far-reaching consequences for ordinary men and women. They are also often in a position that they can influence politicians an pressure groups (Mills, 2018). At the same time, some authors refer to a netocracy, a global upper-class with a power-base derived from technological advantage and networking skills. The new underclass, or masses, is called consumtariat, whose main activity is consumption, regulated by those in power (Bard and Söderqvist, 2002). Generally, what becomes apparent in the literature is that rising inequalities, which represent basically a growing divide between elite and masses, are an important and potentially reversable marker of societal decline (Moghaddam, 2010; Diamond, 2019).

2.6 Repeated low-quality decision-making

In a society in decline, the rate of decline and possible reversal are codependent on the governmental responses (Toynbee, 1987; Hutton, 2014). In some cases, there will be inaction, if a threat is not perceived as needing urgent action, but equally devastating can be overreaction to a threat (Maor, 2012; Maor, 2020; Hafsi and Baba, 2022). An action bias, a bias favoring action over inaction, often occurs when incentives to take action are bigger than incentives to refrain from action (Patt and Zeckhauser, 2000). After a while of ignoring warning signs, a tendency to react too strongly may take over, and this may also include suboptimal decision-making (Lorange and Nelson, 1985). When a crisis is over, decision makers may often not carefully consider all pros and cons. Taking these kinds of actions is more common than taking preventative, anticipatory actions, such as health advice, action to prevent a health crisis, and actions to prevent an environmental crisis (Patt and Zeckhauser, 2000; Magness and Earle, 2021). In the COVID-19 and accompanying economic crisis for instance, there is evidence of such an action bias (Winsberg et al., 2020; Magness and Earle, 2021, p. 512; Schippers et al., 2022). People often assume that a big problem needs harsh and drastic solutions, while less drastic, but precise solutions, as well as targeted, evidence-based interventions can work better than aggressive solutions (cf. Brown and Detterman, 1987; Wilson, 2011; Walton, 2014). Action bias, along with escalation of commitment and sunk cost fallacy may have played a role in the suboptimal decision-making processes surrounding the COVID-19 crisis (Schippers and Rus, 2021). Combined with the (in hindsight) overestimation made by experts of the expected infection fatality and of the buffering effects of several aggressive measures (Chin et al., 2021; Ioannidis et al., 2022; Pezzullo et al., 2023) led to a disastrous chain of self-perpetuating decision-making (Magness and Earle, 2021; Murphy, 2021). Instead of dialing back, the general political climate and response doubled down on the measures and on defending a narrative in their support, leading to a Death Spiral of low-quality decision making and serious consequences.

2.7 Key marker of societal decline: rising inequalities

In current society, there are some clear signs of societal decline. While dwindling resources are not always apparent in declining societies, a key marker is hierarchical order and an elite with plenty of access to resources while the masses have increasing difficulties to survive (Diamond, 2013; Rushkoff, 2022). Recently, a rather steep increase in inequalities has been observed (for a review see Schippers et al., 2022). This increase is partly caused by wage inequality, which the last 40 years has sharply increased in developing countries (Acemoğlu and Restrepo, 2021). Wage inequality is for a large part caused by automation (Acemoğlu and Restrepo, 2021). While poverty decreased since the 19th century (Sullivan and Hickel, 2023), there are now clear signs that this trend is being reversed. Economic inequality has been found to have a range of effects such as reducing mental and physical health (Wilkinson and Pickett R., 2009; Pickett and Wilkinson, 2015), decreasing trust, cooperation and social cohesion in society (Gustavsson and Jordahl, 2008; Elgar and Aitken, 2010; Van de Werfhorst and Salverda, 2012), heightening violence and social unrest (D’Hombres et al., 2012; Jetten et al., 2021) and increasing support for autocratic leadership (Jetten et al., 2021). Rising inequalities may thus have more far-reaching consequences and destabilizing effects than commonly believed, also via the effect on citizens’ sociopolitical behaviors and decreased social cohesion (Van Bavel, 2016; Jetten et al., 2021). Since the global financial crisis of 2008, this trend toward rising inequalities has become more visible (Jetten et al., 2021). Health within a population gets progressively worse alongside a development of decreased economic equality. Societies with relative equal levels of income commonly also have low levels of stress and high levels of trust, and people in such societies are generally cooperative. In unequal societies distrust rises as the rich fear the poor, they worry to safeguard their wealth, while the poor suffer from stress, status anxiety and bitterness (Wilkinson and Pickett R., 2009; Wilkinson and Pickett R. G., 2009). Health and life expectancy lowers for the poor, unemployed and low-level employees (Smith et al., 1990; Marmot and Shipley, 1996; Neckerman and Torche, 2007; Boehm et al., 2011). Importantly, economic inequality has also been described as a downward spiraling effect of social problems. These include teenage pregnancies, with babies born to such mothers at greater risk of educational failure, juvenile crime and becoming teenage parents themselves, with decreasing health, and increasing imprisonment of those lowest on the social ladder (Wilkinson and Pickett R., 2009). On a grander scale, societies fall apart and societal dysfunction rises when an ever increasing group of have-nots are unable to sustain themselves, let alone earn the money and produce the food to sustain the rich, and the difference between the elite and masses have become too big to bridge (Wilkinson and Pickett R., 2009, Wilkinson and Pickett R. G., 2009).

Note that while most social problems are bigger in unequal countries, suicide and smoking levels are often higher in contemporary relatively equal societies, as aggression and violence is turned inward, and often will be directed at the self, as people tend to blame themselves when things are not great (Wilkinson and Pickett R., 2009). Inequality may be at the root of many problems in societies and more equal societies do better on almost all fronts (Marmot and Shipley, 1996; Wilkinson and Pickett R., 2009; Boehm et al., 2011).

2.8 Historical examples of rising inequalities and societal decline

Prior to the 19th century, most unskilled laborers were able to provide for a family of four (Sullivan and Hickel, 2023). A review on wages and mortality since the 16th century showed that in general extreme poverty was not widespread, with the exception of severe social disruption and dislocation, such as war, famine and institutionalized dispossession. Interestingly, the rise of capitalism initially caused a dramatic decrease of human welfare, in terms of a decline in wages below subsistence level. In several regions, such as Northwest Europe, progress in terms of human welfare only began in the 1880’s, and in other regions as late as the mid-20th century. This period was characterized by anti-colonial and social political movements, and a redistribution of incomes as well public provisioning systems and the welfare state (Sullivan and Hickel, 2023).

Going back even further, during the decline of the Roman Empire, a Death Spiral seems to have been apparent. Even when the end was near, instead of trying to address the problems, there was unrealistic and excessive optimism about the future, and adherence to the past (Grant, 1976). In the earlier periods of the empire, the elites were willing to offer lives and treasure in the service of the common interest, while in the period of decline, the elites became increasing selfish (Turchin, 2007). This went hand in hand with a decline in dearly held values such as thinking for the common good and virtues, enlarged bureaucracies and a rise in inequalities with steep increase in enrichment of the richest 1 percent in Rome, and an impoverishment of the middle classes (Goldsworthy, 2009).

“(…) the richest 1 percent of the Romans during the early Republic was only 10 to 20 times as wealthy as an average Roman citizen. (…) By around A.D. 400, just before the collapse of the empire and when the degree of wealth inequality reached its maximum value, an average Roman noble of senatorial class had property valued in the neighborhood of 20,000 Roman pounds of gold. There was no “middle class” comparable to the small landholders of the third century B.C.; the huge majority of the population was made up of landless peasants working land that belonged to nobles. These peasants had hardly any property at all, but if we estimate it (very generously) at one tenth of a pound of gold, the wealth differential would be 200,000! Inequality grew both as a result of the rich getting richer (late imperial senators were 100 times wealthier than their Republican predecessors) and those of the middling wealth becoming poor, and indeed destitute.” (Turchin, 2007; pp. 160–161).

This rise of inequalities seems an overarching theme in many collapsing empire analyses (Turchin, 2007). The work of Turchin describes a series of nested cycles of periods of relative prosperity and plenty, leading to an increase of population, but also to growing inequalities and dysfunctionality. Inequality affects asabiyya,1 or social cohesion, defined by Turchin as: “the capacity of a social group for concerted collective action” (Turchin, 2007; p. 6). Asabiyya is generally high in times that empires are rising and low when empires are in decline (Turchin, 2007). Similar to the “Universe 25” experiment (described in par. 1.1.10), this in turn leads to a breakdown in collaborative efforts and precedes a period of scarcity. In the next phase, disease, hunger, violence and war then lead to a rapid decline and often collapse of civilization (Turchin, 2007; see Figure 2).

2.9 COVID-19 crisis and rising inequalities

In the context of the COVID-19 crisis, some have stated that this is a great leveler and that “we are all in this together,” however, this is clearly not the case: vulnerable groups have been unevenly negatively impacted (Ali et al., 2020). Inequalities have risen steeply since 2020 (Schippers et al., 2022). While this trend was already visible before the pandemic started (for a review see Neckerman and Torche, 2007), especially billionaire wealth increased dramatically early during the crisis (Schippers et al., 2022; Inequality, 2023). Between March 18, 2020, and October 15, 2021, billionaires’ total wealth increased over 70%, from 2.947 trillion to 5.019 trillion, and the richest five saw an increase in 123 percent. Since then, gains have decreased modestly, because of market losses (Collins, 2022). Corporate profits also spiked as giant corporations used the excuse of crisis-related supply chain bottlenecks to drive up the prices of gasoline, food, and other essentials (Inequality, 2023). While CEO pay increased, general worker pay lagged behind, increasing the CEO-worker pay gap in the United States (Inequality, 2023). To prove this in 2019 average CEO pay was $12,074,288 per annum compared to a median worker yearly pay at the 100 largest low wage employers of $30,416 in the United States; in 2020 yearly average CEO pay was 13,936,558 (a 15.42% increase) for workers it was 30,474 (a meager 0.19% increase; Inequality, 2023).

In effect, global billionaires made 3.9 trillion dollars by the end of 2020, while global workers earnings fell by 3.7 trillion, as millions lost their jobs around the world (International Labour Organization, 2020; Berkhout et al., 2021; p. 12). The lowest-income workers were hit the hardest. In total, it has been estimated that during the crisis, by 2021, 150 million people were driven into extreme poverty (Howton, 2020). With widespread continuing demise, even the rich may start to lose. The crisis has worsened many other aspects of inequality, such as educational, racial, gender, and health inequalities (Byttebier, 2022; for a review see Schippers et al., 2022). Nevertheless, the elite may continue to centralize power and make decisions that are not in the interest of most people (Desmet, 2022). As the “masses” end up being in a downward spiral of dwindling incomes, not being able to pay for essentials, such as food, gas, and medicine, they may experience significant financial barriers and may avoid health care in order to save on costs (Weinick et al., 2005), leading to worsening health status for millions (Schippers et al., 2022). Socio-economic determinants of health are often the result of persistent structural and socio-economical inequalities, exacerbated by the COVID-19 crisis (Ali et al., 2020; Schippers, 2020). The term syndemic describes “a set of closely related and mutually reinforcing health problems that significantly affect the overall health status of a population, against the background of a perpetual pattern of deleterious socio-economic conditions” (Bambra et al., 2020; Byttebier, 2022, p. 1036). Prior pandemic crises such as the Spanish flu and other economic shocks led to an increase in inequalities and unequal health and wealth outcomes (Bambra et al., 2020). Sudden economic shocks, such as the collapse of communism, are related to an increase in morbidity, mental health decline, suicide, increased ill health and deaths from substance use (Bambra et al., 2020). These effects were experienced unequally in poorer regions, and among low-skilled workers, exacerbating health inequalities (Bambra et al., 2020). Interestingly, after the 2008 financial crisis, countries that chose not to cut back on health and social protection budgets, had better outcomes than countries that made austere cuts in those budgets (Stuckler and Basu, 2013; Bambra et al., 2020). In current times, people lower on the social ladder bore the brunt of the negative side effects of the measures, in health, lifestyle changes as well as decrease in income (Schippers et al., 2022), even increasing their vulnerability to viral diseases (Enichen et al., 2022).

The dysfunctional situation in most countries worldwide strengthens the incentives for mass migration into Western countries that still offer better prospects, in theory at least. However, this challenge, if mishandled, may lead to importing poverty (Martin, 2009; Murray, 2017) creating an underclass, and further proclivity of an unequal society and possibly a Death Spiral Effect (cf. Gomberg-Muñoz, 2012; Peters and Shin, 2022). Furthermore, there is some evidence that poverty gives rise to higher crime rates (Dong et al., 2020). In the US, even minor crimes are severely punished, and imprisonment of poor people escalates inequalities (Wacquant, 2009; Wilkinson and Pickett, 2009a).

2.10 Dysfunctional behavior of both elites and masses

Prior research has shown that extreme inequalities lead to dysfunctional societies, both in the animal kingdom as well as in human societies (Grusky and Ku, 2008). In the animal kingdom it has been shown to lead to “behavioral sink” or extreme dysfunctional behavior (Anderson and Bushman, 2002). In the Universe 25 behavioral experiment, mice lived in perfect conditions with enough living space, food and water, but when their numbers grew, inequalities rose and the behavior of all mice became dysfunctional and led to the extinction of the colony, long before the maximum number of mice was reached (Calhoun, 1973; Adams and Ramsden, 2011). It has been argued that in that particular experiment, where resources were plenty, the controlling of resources by a small number of mice, as well as excessive (negative) interaction led to the decline of the colony (Ramsden, 2011). Even after the numbers fell to lower than when pathology set in, mice behavior stayed dysfunctional (Ramsden and Adams, 2009). The extent to which these studies have validity for human society is certainly debatable. For obvious ethical reasons, it is for instance not possible to do a study in which a form of extreme hierarchy is tested, and subsequently lifted, but there is general agreement that countries with high inequalities have more social problems (Grusky and Ku, 2008; Van Bavel, 2016).

Historically, the elite that accelerates problematic societal developments and oftentimes is at the start of the Death Spiral Effect, either because of their greed and hunger for power, or just because power corrupts, is also getting anxious as societal decline progresses (Baker, 2022; Rushkoff, 2022). The pressure to perpetuate economic growth comes with repercussions and an inevitable crumbling of financial markets, as for instance happened in 2008 (Rushkoff, 2009). Rushkoff (2009) had hoped there would be a self-correcting mechanism when financial markets collapse, but this apparently was not the case. As the elite notice that things are going wrong, often, instead of using their wealth to make things better, they use their buffer for protecting themselves from the “masses” and for “escapism.” They start looking for ways to escape the pending societal collapse that they helped creating (Rushkoff, 2022). While the masses experience a loss of freedom and prosperity, and may desperately try to hold on to whatever property and resources they still have (desperation principle; Hobfoll et al., 2018), the elite also realizes disaster may strike and they may also get into a survival mode, and may even start to fight each other (cf., Turchin, 2007).

Currently, the optimism of connectivity and the internet and the possibilities for open source democracy seem to have faded (Rushkoff, 2003). Censorship has set in, along with a loss of scientific freedom (Da Silva, 2021; Kaufmann, 2021; Shir-Raz et al., 2022). The scientific debate was stifled during the COVID-19 crisis and dissenting views were censored (Shir-Raz et al., 2022). Suppression tactics resulted in damaging careers of dissenting doctors and scientists regardless of academic or medical status (Shir-Raz et al., 2022). This has led to a loss of trust in science and institutions (Hamilton and Safford, 2021). Worse, when knowledgeable scientists with reasonable arguments and rigorous data are suppressed, this could offer ammunition to conspiracy theorists to claim that orthodox science is non-tolerant and wrong. Especially centralized censorship may increase certainty in radicalized views (Lane et al., 2021). Anyone questioning. science’ and official governmental narratives may be called a conspiracy theorist, as a way of discrediting and delegitimizing critics (Giry and Gürpınar, 2020). It has been argued that conspiracy theories are also a sign of dissatisfaction with governance, society or policies, and some conspiracy theories may turn out to be true (Swami and Furnham, 2014).

Surveillance capitalism, the collection and commodification of personal data by corporations, shifts the power of governments toward large companies (Big Other). These corporations have the power to observe and influence human thinking and decision-making for example via direct advertising. Direct advertising has become far more aggressive (Schwartz and Woloshin, 2019), especially for products of little benefit and high sales (DiStefano et al., 2023). Effective direct advertising can be guided by surveillance capitalism. This can be a tool for business (knowing customers preferences) but also an invasion of privacy (Zuboff, 2015, 2023, see also Yeung, 2018). Worryingly, strategic actors such as large corporations and governments (i.e., the elite) can unevenly influence the media (media bias), possibly leading to an increasingly narrowing of the definition of “facts/true knowledge” versus “fake news/disinformation,” e.g., stating that only governmental or other elite-endorsed narratives represent the. truth’ (cf. Gehlbach and Sonin, 2014). People and scientists that differ in their views from the official narrative, can be censored, marginalized and expelled, even if they are prudent in their publications and wording within the debate (cf. Prasad and Ioannidis, 2022). Some authors have even contended that this combination may bring us on a path toward totalitarianism (Desmet, 2022), and have called for a way to rethink and uphold democracy and democratic principles (Della Porta, 2021; Ioannidis and Schippers, 2022), as well as democratic control of technology (Gould, 1990).

A more positive solution such as a form of direct democracy is often not considered and if it is, people often feel not capable of bringing this about (cf. Rushkoff, 2019). In general, under such conditions, distrust escalates as the elite starts to fear the masses and the masses fear the elite and both tend to show dysfunctional behavior (cf. Widmann, 2022). When in phase 4 (Figure 2) resources dwindle as a result of the continuously downward spiral, the desperation principle may apply. The desperation principle has been formulated within the conservation of resources theory (COR; Hobfoll et al., 2018). In COR theory, people, organizations and societies strive to obtain and hold on to resources they value. Since resource loss is more salient than resource gain, people go to great length to prevent resource loss. However, individual and groups must invest resources in order to prevent resource loss, recover from losses and/or gain resources. When valuable resources are lost, resource gains become more important (Hobfoll et al., 2018). The desperation principle states that “When people’s resources are outstretched or exhausted, they enter a defensive mode to preserve the self which is often defensive, aggressive, and may become irrational” (Hobfoll et al., 2018, p. 106). Resource loss cycles indicate that tress and a cycle of faulty decision-making may lead to less resources to offset resources loss and these loss spirals “gain in momentum as well as magnitude.” At the same time, “resource gain spirals tend to be weak and develop slowly” (Hobfoll et al., 2018, p. 106). This indicates that once this spiral has set in, it is hard to reverse and the Death Spiral Effect may set in.

3 Reversing the downward spiral: upward spiral

3.1 Breaking free: strategies to overcome societal dysfunctional behavior

In general, grand societal challenges such as rising inequalities, social unrest and societal decline affect large portions of the population, are highly significant, but are potentially solvable (Eisenhardt et al., 2016). Lately, management scholars have applied organizational knowledge to a societal context by formulating solutions for such societal challenges using management theories (George et al., 2016), and models have been offered to integrate literature on resilience with crisis management literature (Williams et al., 2017). For instance scholars have offered solutions to alleviate poverty (e.g., Banerjee and Duflo, 2011; Mair et al., 2016) and psychological injury in the context of large conflict and wars (De Rond and Lok, 2016). With respect to decreasing inequalities, especially work by Mair et al. (2016) could be of interest, as they propose scaffolding as a way to decrease inequalities and alleviate poverty. A downward spiral may be reversed by using an adaptive response. This may be important in order to reverse the trend, but also in case of post-collapse recovery (cf. Bunce et al., 2009). Based on the literature cited above, the following steps may be necessarhin

Making sure that people involved are also participating in the in decision-making process is key. As Perret et al. (2020): 1 state: “The fate of states, companies and organizations are shaped by their decisions. It is then surprising that only a minority of individuals are involved in the decision-making process.” This would suggest that a rigorous change in the way societal decisions are made may be an important point to intervene (Lupia and Matsusaka, 2004; Demarest and Victor, 2022). In general, the upward spiral may start with (1) open mindedness with regards to changes necessary to break the downward spiral. These may include (2) reflection on the current and desired situation, as well as (3) the development of a strategy for the restoration of trust, rather than a focus on finding scapegoats (avoidance of a blame game can be reached for instance via a truth and reconciliation commission), and (4) involvement of more society members. This may lead to (5) higher quality decision-making and enhanced autonomy and positive ripple effects within society, and this may in turn lead to (6) decreased social inequalities and (7) a more free and open society where people can thrive and prosper. Bouncing back from adversity may then require both resilience and compassion, as described below.

3.2 Resilience

The concept of resilience, or how individuals, organizations and societies bounce back from adverse events, is informative (Van Der Vegt et al., 2015), and this is also mentioned as an important concept within complex adaptive systems models (e.g., Centeno et al., 2022). Resilience on all levels seem to be dependent on social integration, for instance on how supportive families and communities are, and this is especially apparent in times of crises (Banerjee and Duflo, 2011; Van Der Vegt et al., 2015). Having resilient networks is also important in this respect, and research on how to strengthen networks and communities may be key to societal resilience and rebuilding society after decline has set in (Van Der Vegt et al., 2015). Trust and compassion, as well as effective communication and collaboration within networks may enable not only more effective response to crises and disasters (Shepherd and Williams, 2014), but also reduce suffering caused by societal decline (Williams and Shepherd, 2017). After disasters, such as earthquakes, it has been found that family firms, especially those that involve more members, are best positioned to make use of posttraumatic entrepreneurial opportunities for recovery and growth (Salvato et al., 2020). Recent work in a company context has shown that companies can react to adverse events in diverse ways to post-shock challenges (Shepherd and Williams, 2022). This research highlights the role of post-adversity growth during adversity and gives insight in the different paths to resilience. This research offers insights in how to break a Death Spiral and move toward an upward spiral.

3.3 Compassion

As a downward spiral is often accompanied with a loss of humanness, the reversal of the downward trend will need a restauration of humanness and compassion. Compassion organizing was coined as a term to describe the coordinated organizational response to human suffering inside and outside of the organization (Dutton et al., 2006). Compassion is an innate response to human suffering, and involves recognition of suffering, empathetic concern and behavior that is aimed at alleviating suffering (Dutton et al., 2006). The reversal of a downward trend of societal decline, may be more difficult than posttraumatic growth after (natural) disasters, by its sheer scale. While a disaster may provoke compassionate organizing to alleviate mass suffering (Shepherd and Williams, 2014; Williams and Shepherd, 2017), what can be done for the alleviation of suffering and crisis management in the context of societal decline may be less obvious (cf. Williams et al., 2017). Often, individuals, teams and organizations working to alleviate suffering experience intense emotions that may spur strong involvement of volunteers and companies, and people often refer to this as a “calling” (De Rond and Lok, 2016; Schabram and Maitlis, 2017; Langenbusch, 2020). However, that sensemaking and strong emotion can also lead to faulty decision-making (Cornelissen et al., 2014; Hafsi and Baba, 2022). In the COVID-19 crisis, digital innovations were suggested as a way to alleviate suffering (Majchrzak and Shepherd, 2021). However, we need rigorous studies on which compassion-based interventions may be effective. It is important to help people to regain a sense of purpose in life and increase posttraumatic growth of individuals and groups in society (De Jong et al., 2020; Dekker et al., 2020).

3.4 Turnaround leadership and culture change

Prior research has shown that leadership is key to follower behavior (Cao et al., 2022). Passive and destructive leadership styles, such as abusive, narcissistic and authoritarian, were associated with higher levels of dysfunctional follower behavior, i.e., workplace aggression. Conversely, ethical leadership, change-oriented as well as relational-oriented leadership was negatively associated with workplace aggression. If leaders’ behavior changes, this also affects organizational culture and behavior of followers.

A historical turnaround leader that managed to get a country out of a negative spiral was Nelson Mandela, in South Africa. Instead of installing tribunals, he established the Truth and Reconciliation Commission. This helped to move beyond blame and regain respect for one another. A problem with leaders that step up in turbulent times, is that they are often not recognized and valued in the midst of the turmoil by the masses, and they may also be seen as enemies of the ruling elite. As they try to reverse the downward spiral, they may face hardship, imprisonment, and sometimes even death. As a case in point, Nelson Mandela spent over 27 years in prison.

Turnaround leadership faces the difficult task to break the negative spiral and restore trust and bring back positive energy within the organization (Bibeault, 1981) or society (Gibson, 2006). This is all the more difficult, because such companies often suffer from collective denial, or unwillingness to admit that there is a problem at all. Sometimes the problems become so big, that people act like the problem does not exist (cf. Meyer and Kunreuther, 2017). On a company level, it has been observed that even though individually, people know and may even admit that the company is in trouble, they collude in collective denial, or pluralistic ignorance (Kanter, 2003). Strategies that successful turnaround leaders in companies often employ are promoting dialog, engendering respect, sparking collaboration and inspiring initiative (Kanter, 2003). The challenge is how far the tactics used by a turn-around leader within an organization can be applied on a societal level as well. Without a shared vision, recovery after collapse in the context of adaptive systems is unlikely (Bunce et al., 2009).

3.5 Avoidance of blame game

During the COVID-19 crisis, many people started to suspect that conspiracies were at play, probably due to both the scale of events, as well as the need for explanations (Van Bavel et al., 2020; Douglas, 2021; Pummerer et al., 2021; Ivanova, 2022). While the belief in conspiracy theories has been related to reduced institutional trust, lower support for and adherence to imposed measures (Pummerer et al., 2021), it can also be seen as an ineffective form of coping with the situation (Schippers, 2020). While people may have a need for finding out who or what is to blame for the situation, the dangers of co-occurring collective narcissism (i.e., exaggerated belief in the greatness of the in-group, which is not recognized by others) and conspiracy theories, such as the endorsement of violence and undemocratic governance, have been pointed out (De Zavala et al., 2022). As the relevance and/or truthfulness of conspiracy theories are often hard to check, constructive ways forward are often blocked if these run rampant. When focusing on parties that are to blame for the situation, while some people may feel that revenge can be helpful, blame mostly fulfills a felt need for retribution and only a subset of people seems to find revenge important and even pleasurable (Szymaniak et al., 2023). Punishment of perpetrators is not very effective to prevent or retribute transgressions in terms of law enforcement (Metz, 2022). In the current situation, this may be even more complicated, as a lot of damage may have been done for the “right” reasons, i.e., in the name of public health (Schippers and Rus, 2021; Schippers et al., 2022). It may be hard to disentangle motivations of individual decision makers and decisions were also made in a context of approval of such measures (cf. Ohlin, 2007). A more constructive approach therefore may be in reconciliation (Metz, 2022), reversing the most aggressive and ineffective policies, and learning from mistakes in order to do better in the future (Schippers et al., 2022). However, if pressure for revenge and retribution escalates, decision-makers who made grave mistakes will likely double down on their mistakes in order to avoid punishment. As many of these decision-makers continue to have power in (or on) public health and science, such defensive continued endorsement of false narratives can be devastating for the credibility of both public health and science at large. Moreover, it is imperative that people can easily experience positive emotions instead of enduring stressors (Johnson, 2022). Preventing long-term stress is critical to quality of life and longevity (Johnson, 2022). Mutual empathy may need to be promoted in generating a positive view for the future (Beck et al., 2018; Halamová et al., 2022).

3.6 Civil and intelligent disobedience

Whether in families, groups, organizations or society, if people perceive that a toxic culture is ingrained or becomes apparent, the majority of people have problems addressing this, out of fear of being excluded from the group, or because they do not know how to reverse the downward trend (Packer, 2009; Richardson, 2020). Richardson (2020) describes that with a change in society toward a “new normal,” people in power will demand obedience. Concentration of power and wealth at the top is often accompanied by forcefully compelling obedience to new customs, rules, and behavior. In the early stages people often either downplay the signs of danger and may succumb to coercion, out of fear for the consequences (Richardson, 2020). People who openly resist, or criticize decisions, often face dire consequences. Effective ways of “resisting” listed by Richardson (2020) are a refusal to accept the new goals and tradition imposed, not buying into the belief that this new order is inevitable, and making a conscious choice to be rather “left behind” than to join in. This all the while maintaining civility and commitment to the common good, and adhering to values that are important to a civil society (Richardson, 2020). Constructive deviance and speaking up (as opposed to silence) are an important step in counteracting (organizational) wrongdoing (Starystach and Höly, 2021). Also, civil and intelligent disobedience can be ways to counteract courses of action chosen by leaders and policy makers that may hurt society or companies as a whole may hurt society or companies as a whole (Chaleff, 2015; Hughes and Burnes, 2023). Some even argue that constructive deviance and intelligent disobedience should become socially expected behavior (Ralston, 2010). This is in line with recommendations to prevent groupthink to make sure to appoint a “devil’s advocate” (Janis, 1982b; Janis, 1983; MacDougall and Baum, 1997; Akhmad et al., 2020). Interestingly, group members that strongly identify with the group are more prone to speak out on collective problems (Packer, 2009). Another action that individuals and groups can take is high-quality listening, as an antidote to polarization, which has become a huge issue in society (Itzchakov et al., 2023; Santoro and Markus, 2023). Recent research indicates that genuinely listening in order to try to understand the others’ perspective has been shown to aid depolarization, and can be. a valuable tool for bridging attitudinal and ideological divides’ (Itzchakov et al., 2023; p. 1).

3.7 Collective action

Besides individuals in groups and societies speaking up and voicing concerns, collective action may have additional benefits. While individual control over the social system seems out of reach, collective action can bring about positive outcomes for the group as a whole (Klandermans, 1997). Key predictors of collective action are perceived injustice, efficacy (i.e., sense of control) and identity (i.e., identification with a group; Van Zomeren et al., 2008; Van Zomeren, 2013). People are also more likely to engage in protests if they perceive injustice for the group they identify with (Klandermans, 2002). Injustice and efficacy seem to be stronger predictors for collective action in case of incidental rather than structural disadvantage, while group identification was a strong predictor for collective action for both types of groups (Kraemer, 2021). While structural disadvantages are more harmful, both psychologically and in terms of inequalities, they are less likely to evoke action-oriented emotional response and collective action (Major, 1994; Schmitt and Branscombe, 2002), and are thus harder to change (Sidanius and Pratto, 2000; Jost and Major, 2001; Sidanius et al., 2004). Such differences and structural injustices often become ingrained and disadvantaged groups may even start to see their state as natural and immutable (Major, 1994). It is then seen as a property of a certain group (Kraemer, 2021) and the existing differences between groups are seen as legitimate (Jost and Major, 2001).

Social Dominance Theory seeks to explain how and why societal group-based inequalities exist and persist, even though people would wish for a more equal society (Pratto, 1999; Pratto et al., 2006). In most societies, some groups enjoy material and symbolic resources, such as political power, wealth, and access to housing and food (Pratto et al., 2006). Both privileged as well as underprivileged groups may come to see the status quo as legitimate, and this is often institutionalized. Profit-maximizing financial institutions, internal security organizations and criminal justices systems may enhance hierarchy (Pratto et al., 2006). Conversely, human and civil rights movements and institutions, welfare organizations and religious organizations may reduce hierarchy. However, such organizations often lack funding and often do not really challenge the status quo (Pratto et al., 2006). When collective action is taken against the status quo, it is often seen as illegitimate and are shut down (Pratto et al., 2006), unfortunately repression of social movements is quite common (Loadenthal, 2016). Historically, non-violent collective actions have been more successful then violent ones in (re)instating democracy (Chenoweth and Stephan, 2008; Chenoweth, 2021), and this type of actions have become much more common (Kraemer, 2021, see also Schippers et al., 2022). Thus, breaking the downward spiral is not easy and is often jeopardized by the ruling elite.

3.8 Top priority: decreasing inequalities

As we have argued that a (large) increase in inequalities is an important marker of societal decline, this problem seems to be of utmost importance to address. In Figure 3, we depict how this can be done. In the United Nations Sustainable Development Goals, SDG10 is reducing inequalities (United Nations, 2022). However, the focus of the targets and indicators seems to be more on enhancing inclusion than on explicitly reducing inequalities (Fukuda-Parr, 2019). This is an important omission, as it would be key to address the issue of extreme inequalities and the concentration of wealth at the top (Fukuda-Parr, 2019). While it is clear from our review that rising inequalities and rising authoritarianism can contribute to significant societal decline and high levels of mortality via the Triangle of Death (disease, famine and war), it is not easy to determine where to start in order to reverse this trend. While this seems a large and complex problem, when thinking of possible solutions, it is clear that effectiveness and ease of implementation matter the most. Communities have a responsibility to investigate methods to act on the social, educational, physical, and mental health crisis. Interventions should be rigorously tested with randomized controlled trials for effectiveness and then audited for their implementation success. At the same time, complete equality should not be something to strive for, as this could kill innovation and creativity, rather, an optimal level of income differences is key (Charles-Coll, 2010). Indeed, it seems there is an optimum level of (in)equality, showing an inverted “U” shaped relationship (Charles-Coll, 2010). In situations where ordinary people are involved in shaping the response to crisis, a further widening of disparities seem to have been prevented (Van Bavel and Scheffer, 2021), and hence this seems a promising avenue in going forward.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Upward spiral breaking the Death Spiral: from societal decline to societal flourishing.

The COVID-19 crisis and measures of unprecedented severity and duration are related to many negative side effects and increase inequalities worldwide (Marmot and Allen, 2020); hence stress, health, and trauma for vulnerable populations must be addressed (Whitehead and Torossian, 2020). It may take a long time to recover from the economic fall-out and rise in inequalities (Whitehead and Torossian, 2020). Governments should take individual and societal well-being as a spearhead for decision-making in the upcoming years (Frijters et al., 2020). Hopefully, with effective interventions, the tide can be turned and a positive spiral can be induced (e.g., Schippers and Ziegler, 2023). However, while many ideas and proposals may emerge, implementing them without rigorous trials may add further waste after we have already endorsed too many failed interventions.

4 Final comments

Societal demise and collapse are part of the cyclical nature of civilizations, that have historically seen a rise and fall over time. The Death Spiral Effect entails rising inequalities and rising authoritarianism, creating an elite that controls access to resources more tightly, and making decisions that may set humanity on a path to famine, war and disease. If the trend of decline is not reversed, the aftermath of a possible collapse will become evident, and society may be unable to achieve post-collapse recovery.

In short, our review, synthesizing research from several fields, indicates that next to turnaround leadership and building resilient communities, using compassion, avoiding a blame culture and strengthening of democracy may help. Ideally, governments, companies, all relevant stakeholders as well as individuals should collaborate toward the goals of a healthier and happier future for all.

Author contributions

MS played the primary role in the conception of the manuscript, writing, reviewing, and revising the manuscript. JI contributed to writing the manuscript and editing the manuscript. ML contributed to and partly wrote the section on “Differences from other concepts,” crafted Table 1, contributed to writing, and editing the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Cristina Calin for her help with crafting Tables and Figures, and editing. Vita Bertrand and Hester Kloet are thanked for their help in further editing the paper. Further, the members of the Erasmus Center for Study and Career Success [https://www.erim.eur.nl/erasmus-centre-for-study-and-career-success/] for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^Turchin spells it asabiya.

References

Acemoğlu, D., and Restrepo, P. (2021) Tasks, automation, and the rise in US wage inequality.

Google Scholar

Adams, J., and Ramsden, E. (2011). Rat cities and beehive worlds: density and design in the modern city. Comp. Stud. Soc. Hist. 53, 722–756. doi: 10.1017/s0010417511000399

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Akhmad, M., Chang, S., and Deguchi, H. (2020). Closed-mindedness and insulation in groupthink: their effects and the devil’s advocacy as a preventive measure. J. Comput. Soc. Sci. 4, 455–478. doi: 10.1007/s42001-020-00083-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ali, S., Asaria, M., and Stranges, S. (2020). COVID-19 and inequality: are we all in this together? Can. J. Public Health 111, 415–416. doi: 10.17269/s41997-020-00351-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allcorn, S. (2022). Micromanagement in the workplace. Organ. Soc. Dyn. 22, 83–98. doi: 10.33212/osd.v22n1.2022.83

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alsan, M., Chandra, A., and Simon, K. (2021). The great Unequalizer: initial health effects of COVID-19 in the United States. J. Econ. Perspect. 35, 25–46. doi: 10.1257/jep.35.3.25

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, C. A., and Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 27–51. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arendt, H. (2017). The origins of totalitarianism. Penguin Classics.

Google Scholar

Bagus, P., Peña-Ramos, J. A., and Bayón, A. S. (2021). COVID-19 and the political economy of mass hysteria. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:1376. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18041376

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bak-Coleman, J. B., Alfano, M., Barfuss, W., Bergstrom, C. T., Centeno, M. A., Couzin, I. D., et al. (2021). Stewardship of global collective behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 118:118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2025764118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, N. (2022). These billionaire preppers are planning for the apocalypse. Here’s why. ABC News. Available at: https://www.abc.net.au/news/2022-10-20/tech-billionaires-are-planning-for-the-apocalypse/101546216.

Google Scholar

Balcombe, L., and De Leo, D. (2022). The potential impact of adjunct digital tools and technology to help distressed and suicidal men: an integrative review. Front. Psychol. 12:371. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.796371

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bambra, C., Riordan, R., Ford, J., and Matthews, F. (2020). The COVID-19 pandemic and health inequalities. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 74:214401. doi: 10.1136/jech-2020-214401

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Banerjee, A., and Duflo, E. (2011). Poor economics: a radical rethinking of the way to fight global poverty. Choice Rev. Online 49:49. doi: 10.5860/choice.49-1575

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bard, A., and Söderqvist, J. (2002). Netocracy: the new power elite and life after capitalism. Available at: https://openlibrary.org/books/OL8768843M/Netocracy.

Google Scholar

Baum, S. D., Armstrong, S., Ekenstedt, T., Häggström, O., Hanson, R., Kuhlemann, K., et al. (2019). Long-term trajectories of human civilization. Foresight 21, 53–83. doi: 10.1108/fs-04-2018-0037

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, D. E., Teddy, H. L., Sergey, S., Rica, V., and Thomas, Q. J. (2018). Spiral dynamics in action: Humanity’s master code. New York: John Wiley and Sons.

Google Scholar

Beebe, W. (1921). Edge of the jungle. Available at: http://gutenberg.org/ebooks/25888.

Google Scholar

Belschak, F. D., Hartog, D. N. D., and De Hoogh, A. H. B. (2018). Angels and demons: the effect of ethical leadership on Machiavellian employees’ work behaviors. Front. Psychol. 9:1082. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01082

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berberoglu, B. (2020) The global rise of authoritarianism in the 21st century. New York: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Berkhout, E., Galasso, N., Lawson, M., Morales, P. A. R., Taneja, A., and Pimentel, D. A. V. (2021). The inequality virus: bringing together a world torn apart by coronavirus through a fair, just and sustainable economy.

Google Scholar

Bibeault, D. (1981) Corporate turnaround: how managers turn losers into winners! Available at: https://ci.nii.ac.jp/ncid/BA53369567.

Google Scholar

Boehm, J. K., Peterson, C., Kivimaki, M., and Kubzansky, L. (2011). A prospective study of positive psychological well-being and coronary heart disease. Health Psychol. 30, 259–267. doi: 10.1037/a0023124

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, W. J., Björkvik, E., Haider, L. J., and Masterson, V. (2016). Human responses to social-ecological traps. Sustain. Sci. 11, 877–889. doi: 10.1007/s11625-016-0397-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, W. J., and De Boer, F. W. (2013). The historical dynamics of social-ecological traps. AMBIO J. Hum. Environ. 43, 260–274. doi: 10.1007/s13280-013-0419-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Breevaart, K., Wisse, B., and Schyns, B. (2022). Trapped at work: the barriers model of abusive supervision. Acad. Manag. Perspect. 36, 936–954. doi: 10.5465/amp.2021.0007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brodeur, A., Gray, D., Islam, A., and Bhuiyan, S. (2021). A literature review of the economics of COVID-19. J. Econ. Surv. 35, 1007–1044. doi: 10.1111/joes.12423

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, L. D., and Detterman, L. B. (1987). Small interventions for large problems: reshaping urban leadership networks. J. Appl. Behav. Sci. 23, 151–168. doi: 10.1177/0021886387232002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckley, W. F. (1998). Society -- a complex adaptive system: Essays in social theory. Abingdon: Taylor and Francis.

Google Scholar

Buell, L. (1998). Toxic discourse. Crit. Inq. 24, 639–665. doi: 10.1086/448889

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunce, M., Mee, L., Rodwell, L. D., and Gibb, R. (2009). Collapse and recovery in a remote small island–a tale of adaptive cycles or downward spirals? Glob. Environ. Chang. 19, 213–226. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2008.11.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Byttebier, K. (2022). Covid-19 and capitalism, Economic and financial law and policy - shifting insights & values.

Google Scholar

Calhoun, J. B. (1973). Death squared: the explosive growth and demise of a mouse population. Proc. R. Soc. Med. 66, 80–88. doi: 10.1177/00359157730661p202

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, F., Tricco, A. C., Munn, Z., Pollock, D., Saran, A., Sutton, A., et al. (2023). Mapping reviews, scoping reviews, and evidence and gap maps (EGMs): the same but different–the “big picture” review family. Syst. Rev. 12:45. doi: 10.1186/s13643-023-02178-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cannon, J. (2022). Toxic cultures at work: The eight drivers of a toxic culture and a process for change. Abingdon: Taylor and Francis.

Google Scholar

Cao, W., Van der Wal, R. C., and Taris, T. W. (2022). Leadership and workplace aggression: a Meta-analysis. J. Bus. Ethics 186, 347–367. doi: 10.1007/s10551-022-05184-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Centeno, M. Á., Callahan, P., Larcey, P., and Patterson, T. (2022). “Globalization as adaptive complexity: learning from failure” in Perspectives on Public Policy in Societal-Environmental Crises. eds. A. Izdebski, J. Haldon, and P. Filipkowski (Berlin: Springer), 59–74.

Google Scholar

Chaleff, I. (2015). Intelligent disobedience: doing right when what you’re told to do is wrong. Berrett-Koehler Publishers. Available at: https://www.scholartext.com/book/88827646.

Google Scholar

Charan, R., Useem, J., and Harrington, H. (2002). Why companies fai. Fortune 145, 50–54.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Charles-Coll, J. A. (2010). The optimal rate of inequality: a framework for the relationship between income inequality and economic growth. MPRA Paper. Available at: https://ideas.repec.org/p/pra/mprapa/28921.html.

Google Scholar

Chenoweth, E. (2021) Civil resistance: What everyone needs to know. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Chenoweth, E., and Stephan, M. J. (2008). Why civil resistance works: the strategic logic of nonviolent conflict. Int. Secur. 33, 7–44. doi: 10.1162/isec.2008.33.1.7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chin, V. W. L., Ioannidis, J. P. A., Tanner, M. A., and Cripps, S. (2021). Effect estimates of COVID-19 non-pharmaceutical interventions are non-robust and highly model-dependent. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 136, 96–132. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.03.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, A. E., D’Ambrosio, C., and Lepinteur, A. (2021). The fall in income inequality during COVID-19 in four European countries. J. Econ. Inequal. 19, 489–507. doi: 10.1007/s10888-021-09499-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, C. (2022). Updates: Billionaire wealth, U.S. job losses and pandemic profiteers. Available at: https://inequality.org/great-divide/updates-billionaire-pandemic/.

Google Scholar

Cooke, B. (1999). The social-psychological limits of participation: the New Tyranny? London: Zed Books.

Google Scholar

Cornelissen, J., Mantere, S., and Vaara, E. (2014). The contraction of meaning: the combined effect of communication, emotions, and materiality on sensemaking in the Stockwell shooting. J. Manag. Stud. 51, 699–736. doi: 10.1111/joms.12073

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cutler, D. M., and Zeckhauser, R. (1998). Adverse selection in health insurance. Forum Health Econ. Policy 1:1056. doi: 10.2202/1558-9544.1056

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Hombres, B., Weber, A., and Leandro, E. (2012). Literature review on income inequality and the effects on social outcomes. JRC Scientific and Policy Reports.

Google Scholar

Da Silva, J. A. T. (2021). How to shape academic freedom in the digital age? Are the retractions of opinionated papers a prelude to “cancel culture” in academia? Curr. Res. Behav. Sci. 2:100035. doi: 10.1016/j.crbeha.2021.100035

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Jong, E. M., Ziegler, N., and Schippers, M. C. (2020). From shattered goals to meaning in life: life crafting in times of the COVID-19 pandemic. Front. Psychol. 11:708. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.577708

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Rond, M., and Lok, J. (2016). Some things can never be unseen: the role of context in psychological injury at war. Acad. Manag. J. 59, 1965–1993. doi: 10.5465/amj.2015.0681

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Zavala, A. G., Bierwiaczonek, K., and Ciesielski, P. (2022). An interpretation of meta-analytical evidence for the link between collective narcissism and conspiracy theories. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 47:101360. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101360

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dekker, I., de Jong, E. M., Schippers, M. C., de Bruijn-Smolders, M., Alexiou, A., and Giesbers, B. (2020). Optimizing students’ mental health and academic performance: AI-enhanced life crafting. Front. Psychol. 11:1063. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01063

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Rio Chanona, R. M., Hermida-Carrillo, A., Sepahpour-Fard, M., Sun, L., Topinkova, R., and Nedelkoska, L. (2023) Mental health concerns prelude the great resignation: evidence from social media. Research Square (Research Square).

Google Scholar

Della Porta, D. (2021). Progressive social movements, democracy and the pandemic. New York: De Gruyter, pp. 209–226.

Google Scholar

Delsuc, F. (2003). Army ants trapped by their evolutionary history. PLoS Biol. 1:e37:E37. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0000037

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Demarest, A. A., and Victor, B. (2022). Constructing policy to confront collapse: ancient experience and modern risk. Acad. Manag. Perspect. 36, 768–800. doi: 10.5465/amp.2019.0039

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmet, M. (2022). The psychology of totalitarianism. Vermont: Chelsea Green Publishing.

Google Scholar

Diamond, J. (2013). Collapse: How societies choose to fail or survive. United Kingdom: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Diamond, J. (2019). Upheaval: How nations cope with crisis and change. United Kingdom: Penguin.

Google Scholar

DiStefano, M. J., Markell, J. M., Doherty, C. C., Alexander, G. C., and Anderson, G. F. (2023). Association between drug characteristics and manufacturer spending on direct-to-consumer advertising. JAMA J. Am. Med. Assoc. 329, 386–392. doi: 10.1001/jama.2022.23968

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, B., Egger, P., and Guo, Y. (2020). Is poverty the mother of crime? Evidence from homicide rates in China. PLoS One 15:e0233034. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0233034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas, K. M. (2021). COVID-19 conspiracy theories. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 24, 270–275. doi: 10.1177/1368430220982068

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Downey, S. S., Haas, R., and Shennan, S. (2016). European Neolithic societies showed early warning signals of population collapse. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 113, 9751–9756. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602504113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dutton, J. E., Worline, M. C., Frost, P. J., and Lilius, J. (2006). Explaining compassion organizing. Adm. Sci. Q. 51, 59–96. doi: 10.2189/asqu.51.1.59

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenhardt, K. M., Graebner, M. E., and Sonenshein, S. (2016). Grand challenges and inductive methods: rigor without rigor mortis. Acad. Manag. J. 59, 1113–1123. doi: 10.5465/amj.2016.4004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elgar, F. J., and Aitken, N. (2010). Income inequality, trust and homicide in 33 countries. Eur. J. Pub. Health 21, 241–246. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckq068

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Enichen, E., Harvey, C., and Demmig-Adams, B. (2022). COVID-19 spotlights connections between disease and multiple lifestyle factors. Am. J. Lifestyle Med. 17, 231–257. doi: 10.1177/15598276221123005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Esfeld, M. (2022). From the open society to the closed society: reconsidering popper on natural and social science. Futures 137:102920. doi: 10.1016/j.futures.2022.102920

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ethics and Compliance Initiative. (2020). Global business ethics survey: pressure in the workplace: possible risk factors and those at risk. Vienna, VA: Ethics and Compliance Initiative.

Google Scholar

Fairlie, R. W. (2020). The impact of COVID-19 on small business owners: evidence from the first three months after widespread social-distancing restrictions. J. Econ. Manag. Strategy 29, 727–740. doi: 10.1111/jems.12400

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzgerald, T. (2005). Predicting and preempting the corporate heart attack. Corporate Finance Rev. 10, 5–10.

Google Scholar

Freud, S. (1921) Massenpsychologie und Ich-Analyse.

Google Scholar

Freyhofer, S., Ziegler, N., de Jong, E. M., and Schippers, M. C. (2021). Depression and anxiety in times of COVID-19: how coping strategies and loneliness relate to mental health outcomes and academic performance. Front. Psychol. 12:684. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.682684

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frijters, P., Clark, A. E., Krekel, C., and Layard, R. (2020). A happy choice: wellbeing as the goal of government. Behavioural Public Policy 4, 126–165. doi: 10.1017/bpp.2019.39

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fukuda-Parr, S. (2019). Keeping out extreme inequality from theSDGAgenda – the politics of indicators. Global Pol. 10, 61–69. doi: 10.1111/1758-5899.12602

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gehlbach, S., and Sonin, K. (2014). Government control of the media. J. Public Econ. 118, 163–171. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2014.06.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

George, G., Howard-Grenville, J., Joshi, A., and Tihanyi, L. (2016). Understanding and tackling societal grand challenges through management research. Acad. Manag. J. 59, 1880–1895. doi: 10.5465/amj.2016.4007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giacalone, R. A., and Greenberg, J. (1997). Antisocial behavior in organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc.

Google Scholar

Gibson, J. L. (2006). The contributions of truth to reconciliation. J. Confl. Resolut. 50, 409–432. doi: 10.1177/0022002706287115

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giry, J., and Gürpınar, D. (2020). Functions and uses of conspiracy theories in authoritarian regimes. Abingdon: Routledge, pp. 317–329.

Google Scholar

Goldsworthy, A. K. (2009). How Rome fell: Death of a superpower. New Haven: Yale University Press.

Google Scholar

Gomberg-Muñoz, R. (2012). Inequality in a “POSTRACIAL” era. Du Bois Rev. 9, 339–353. doi: 10.1017/s1742058x11000579

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gould, C. C. (1990). Rethinking democracy:Freedom and social co-operation in politics, economy, and society. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Grant, M. (1976). The fall of the Roman empire: A reappraisal. London, UK: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.

Google Scholar

Grusky, D. B., and Ku, M. C. (2008). “Gloom, doom, and inequality” in Social stratification: Class, race, and gender in sociological perspective (Colorado: Westview Press).

Google Scholar

Gustavsson, M., and Jordahl, H. (2008). Inequality and trust in Sweden: some inequalities are more harmful than others. J. Public Econ. 92, 348–365. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2007.06.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hafsi, T., and Baba, S. (2022). Exploring the process of policy overreaction: the COVID-19 lockdown decisions. J. Manag. Inq. 32, 152–173. doi: 10.1177/10564926221082494

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Halamová, J., Greškovičová, K., Baránková, M., Strnádelová, B., and Krizova, K. (2022). There must be a way out: the consensual qualitative analysis of best coping practices during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front. Psychol. 13:48. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.917048

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, L. C., and Safford, T. G. (2021). Elite cues and the rapid decline in trust in science agencies on Covid 19. Soc. Perspec. 64, 988–1011. doi: 10.1177/07311214211022391

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, J. B. (1974). The abilene paradox: the management of agreement. Organ. Dyn. 3, 63–80. doi: 10.1016/0090-2616(74)90005-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Head, B. (2018). Forty years of wicked problems literature: forging closer links to policy studies. Polic. Soc. 38, 180–197. doi: 10.1080/14494035.2018.1488797

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Head, B. (2022). Coping with wicked problems in policy design. 10. Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar Publishing, p. 00018.

Google Scholar

Head, B. W. (2022). Wicked problems in public policy: Understanding and responding to complex challenges. Berlin: Springer Nature.

Google Scholar

Hernández, M. S. (1988). Group formation and ideology. Text and context. Int J Psychoanal 69, 163–170.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hobfoll, S. E., Halbesleben, J., Neveu, J. P., and Westman, M. (2018). Conservation of resources in the organizational context: the reality of resources and their consequences. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psych. Organ. Behav. 5, 103–128. doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032117-104640

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holden, L. M. (2005). Complex adaptive systems: concept analysis. J. Adv. Nurs. 52, 651–657. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2005.03638.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holovatch, Y., Kenna, R., and Thurner, S. (2017). Complex systems: physics beyond physics. Eur. J. Phys. 38:023002. doi: 10.1088/1361-6404/aa5a87

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Horfrichter, R. (2000). Reclaiming the environmental debate: the politics of health in a toxic culture. Available at: http://eprints.cdlib.org/uc/item/7gj2n9j8.pdf.

Google Scholar

Howton, F. Y. (2020). COVID-19 to add as many as 150 million extreme poor by 2021. Available at: https://www.worldbank.org/en/news/press-release/2020/10/07/covid-19-to-add-as-many-as-150-million-extreme-poor-by-2021.

Google Scholar

Huepe, C., Zschaler, G., do, A. L., and Gross, T. (2011). Adaptive-network models of swarm dynamics. New J. Phys. 13:073022. doi: 10.1088/1367-2630/13/7/073022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, M., and Burnes, B. (2023). “Toward intelligent disobedience: academics leading by example” in Organizational change, leadership and ethics. eds. R. T. By, B. Burnes, and M. Hughes (Abingdon: Taylor and Francis).

Google Scholar

Hutton, A. (2014). A belated return for Christ?’: The reception of Arnold J. Toynbee’s A study of Historyin a British context, 1934–1961. Eur. Rev. Hist. 21, 405–424. doi: 10.1080/13507486.2014.915290

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Inequality.org. (2023). Inequality and Covid-19. Available at: https://inequality.org/facts/inequality-and-covid-19/.

Google Scholar

International Labour Organization. (2020). ILO monitor: COVID-19 and the world of work. Available at: https://www.ilo.org/global/topics/coronavirus/impacts-and-responses/WCMS_824092/lang--en/index.htm.

Google Scholar

Ioannidis, J. P. A., Cripps, S., and Tanner, M. A. (2022). Forecasting for COVID-19 has failed. Int. J. Forecast. 38, 423–438. doi: 10.1016/j.ijforecast.2020.08.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ioannidis, J. P. A., and Schippers, M. C. (2022). Saving democracy from the pandemic. Available at: https://www.tabletmag.com/sections/science/articles/saving-democracy-from-pandemic.

Google Scholar

Isenberg, D. J. (1986). Group polarization: a critical review and meta-analysis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 50, 1141–1151. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.6.1141

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Itzchakov, G., Weinstein, N., Leary, M., Saluk, D., and Amar, M. (2023). Listening to understand: the role of high-quality listening on speakers’ attitude depolarization during disagreements. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2023:366. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000366

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivanova, M. (2022). Religious narratives and religious coping strategies in the context of COVID-19 pandemic. Cult. Relig. Stud. 10, 570–577. doi: 10.17265/2328-2177/2022.10.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Janis, I. L. (1972). Victims of groupthink: A psychological study of foreign-policy decisions and fiascoes. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Google Scholar

Janis, I. L. (1982a). Groupthink: psychological studies of policy decisions and fiascoes, Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. Available at: https://ci.nii.ac.jp/ncid/BA57518206.

Google Scholar

Janis, I. L. (1982b). Victims of groupthink. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Google Scholar

Janis, I. L. (1983). Groupthink: Psychological studies of policy decisions and fiascoes. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Google Scholar

Janis, I. L. (2003). “Groupthink” in A first look at communication theory. ed. E. A. Griffin (New York: McGraw Hill).

Google Scholar

Janis, I. L., Elowitz, L., and Mann, L. (1978). Decision making--a psychological analysis of conflict, choice, and commitment. Mil. Aff. 1:412. doi: 10.2307/1987412

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Janssen, M. A., Kohler, T. A., and Scheffer, M. (2003). Sunk-cost effects and vulnerability to collapse in ancient societies. Curr. Anthropol. 44, 722–728. doi: 10.1086/379261

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, J. M., and Raver, J. L. (2012). When self-management and surveillance collide. Group Org. Manag. 37, 308–346. doi: 10.1177/1059601112445804

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jetten, J., Peters, K., Álvarez, B., Casara, B. G. S., Dare, M., Kirkland, K., et al. (2021). Consequences of economic inequality for the social and political vitality of society: a social identity analysis. Polit. Psychol. 42, 241–266. doi: 10.1111/pops.12800

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiskrova, G. K. (2022). Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on the workforce: from psychological distress to the great resignation. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 76, 525–526. doi: 10.1136/jech-2022-218826

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Joffe, A. R. (2021). COVID-19: rethinking the lockdown groupthink. Front. Public Health 9:778. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.625778

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Joffe, A. R., and Redman, D. (2021). The SARS-COV-2 pandemic in high income countries such as Canada: a better way forward without lockdowns. Front. Public Health 9:904. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.715904

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, S. (2022). In times of adversity: a neuroscience perspective on stress, health, and implications for society post-pandemic. Yale J Biol Med 95, 165–170.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jones, D. (2021). Barbarigenesis and the collapse of complex societies: Rome and after. PLoS One 16:e0254240. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0254240

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jost, J. T., and Major, B. (2001). Emerging perspectives on the psychology of legitimacy. In: J. T. Jost and B. Major The Psychology of Legitimacy: Emerging Perspectives on Ideology, Justice, and Intergroup Relations. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 3–30.

Google Scholar

Kanter, R. M. (2003). Leadership and the psychology of turnarounds. Harv Bus Rev 81, 58–67.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Karabushenko, P. L., Shebzukhova, T. A., and Vartumyan, V. (2021). Waiting for the perfect storm: political elites facing the 2020 challenge. The European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences.

Google Scholar

Kaufmann, E. (2021). Academic freedom in crisis: punishment, political discrimination, and self-censorship. Center for the Study of Partisanship and Ideology, No. 2, pp. 1–195.

Google Scholar

Kerr, S. (1975). On the folly of rewarding a, while hoping for B. Acad. Manag. J. 18, 769–783. doi: 10.2307/255378

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y. (2001). A comparative study of the. Abilene paradox’ and. groupthink. Public Adm. Q. 25, 168–189.

Google Scholar

Klandermans, B. (1997). The social psychology of protest. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Klandermans, B. (2002). How group identification helps to overcome the dilemma of collective action. Am. Behav. Sci. 45, 887–900. doi: 10.1177/0002764202045005009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraemer, K. R. (2021). Strategic nonviolent struggle in the twenty first century. J. Soc. Encounters 5, 51–54.

Google Scholar

Kreml, W. P. (1994). Decline of empire. Adm. Soc. 26, 78–98. doi: 10.1177/009539979402600105

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuecker, G. D. (2007). The perfect storm: catastrophic collapse in the 21st century. Int. J. Environ. Cult. Econ. Soc. Sustain. 3, 1–10. doi: 10.18848/1832-2077/cgp/v03i05/54427

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, P., Kumar, N., Aggarwal, P., and Yeap, J. A. L. (2021). Working in lockdown: the relationship between COVID-19 induced work stressors, job performance, distress, and life satisfaction. Curr. Psychol. 40, 6308–6323. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-01567-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, J., and Sudarsanam, S. (1997). Corporate restructuring in response to performance decline: impact of ownership, governance and lenders. Rev. Finance 1, 197–233. doi: 10.1023/a:1009732309191

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamberg, J., Ojala, J., and Peltoniemi, M. (2017). Thinking about industry decline: a qualitative meta-analysis and future research directions. Bus. Hist. 60, 127–156. doi: 10.1080/00076791.2017.1340943

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, J. E., McCaffree, K., and Shults, F. L. (2021). Is radicalization reinforced by social media censorship?. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.

Google Scholar

Langenbusch, C. (2020). A lot to lose: organizational identity and emotions in institutional contexts.

Google Scholar

Lansing, J. S. (2003). Complex adaptive systems. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 32, 183–204. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.32.061002.093440

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, T. B., and Robinson, S. L. (2007). Ain’t misbehavin: workplace deviance as organizational resistance. J. Manag. 33, 378–394. doi: 10.1177/0149206307300816

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Bon, G. (1895). Psychologie des foules. Paris: Alcan.

Google Scholar

Loadenthal, M. (2016). Activism, terrorism, and social movements: the “Green scare” as monarchical power. Research in Social Movements, Conflicts and Change.

Google Scholar

Lorange, P., and Nelson, R. T. (1985). How to recognize and avoid organizational decline: observations of organizational decline causes of organizational decline early waSignals intervention alternatives safeguarding against decline references. Sloan Manag. Rev. 1:41.

Google Scholar

Lupia, A., and Matsusaka, J. G. (2004). DIRECT DEMOCRACY: new approaches to old questions. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 7, 463–482. doi: 10.1146/annurev.polisci.7.012003.104730

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDougall, C., and Baum, F. (1997). The Devil’s advocate: a strategy to avoid groupthink and stimulate discussion in focus groups. Qual. Health Res. 7, 532–541. doi: 10.1177/104973239700700407

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Magness, P. W., and Earle, P. C. (2021). The origins and political persistence of COVID-19 lockdowns. Indep. Rev. 2021, 503–521.

Google Scholar

Mair, J., Wolf, M., and Seelos, C. (2016). Scaffolding: a process of transforming patterns of inequality in small-scale societies. Acad. Manag. J. 59, 2021–2044. doi: 10.5465/amj.2015.0725

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Majchrzak, A., and Shepherd, D. A. (2021). Can digital innovations help reduce suffering? A crowd-based digital innovation framework of compassion venturing. Inf. Organ. 31:100338. doi: 10.1016/j.infoandorg.2021.100338

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Major, B. (1994). From social inequality to personal entitlement: the role of social comparisons, legitimacy appraisals, and group membership. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1, 293–355. doi: 10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60156-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maor, M. (2012). Policy overreaction. J. Publ. Policy 32, 231–259. doi: 10.1017/s0143814x1200013x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maor, M. (2020). Policy overreaction styles during manufactured crises. Policy Polit. 48, 523–539. doi: 10.1332/030557320x15894563823146

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marien, M. (2009). Futures thinking and macro-systems: illuminating our era of mal-adaptive, non-adaptive, and semi-adaptive systems. World Futures Rev. 1, 5–13. doi: 10.1177/194675670900100203

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marmot, M., and Allen, J. (2020). COVID-19: exposing and amplifying inequalities. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 74:214720. doi: 10.1136/jech-2020-214720

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marmot, M., and Shipley, M. J. (1996). Do socioeconomic differences in mortality persist after retirement? 25 year follow up of civil servants from the first Whitehall study. BMJ 313, 1177–1180. doi: 10.1136/bmj.313.7066.1177

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, P. (2009). Importing poverty?: immigration and the changing face of rural America. Choice Rev. Online 47:47. doi: 10.5860/choice.47-0397

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meidav, N. (2021). Ethical behavior is the antidote to toxic culture. Strateg. HR Rev. 20, 47–50. doi: 10.1108/shr-10-2020-0088

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Metz, T. (2022). A reconciliation theory of state punishment: an alternative to protection and retribution. R. Inst. Philos. Suppl. 91, 119–139. doi: 10.1017/S1358246121000400

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, R. E., and Kunreuther, H. (2017). The ostrich paradox: why we underprepare for disasters. Available at: https://www.amazon.com/Ostrich-Paradox-Why-Underprepare-Disasters/dp/1613630808.

Google Scholar

Mills, C. W. (2018). “The power elite” in Inequality in the 21st century. eds. D. Grusky and J. Hill (New York: Routledge), 80–88.

Google Scholar

Moghaddam, F. M. (2010). The new global insecurity: how terrorism, environmental collapse, economic inequalities, and resource shortages are changing our world. Available at: http://ci.nii.ac.jp/ncid/BB03078504.

Google Scholar

Mohrman, S. A., and Mohrman, A. M. (1983). Employee involvement in declining organizations. Hum. Resour. Manag. 22, 445–465. doi: 10.1002/hrm.3930220410

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Motesharrei, S., Rivas, J., and Kalnay, E. (2014). Human and nature dynamics (HANDY): modeling inequality and use of resources in the collapse or sustainability of societies. Ecol. Econ. 101, 90–102. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2014.02.014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moynihan, T. (2020). Existential risk and human extinction: an intellectual history. Futures 116:102495. doi: 10.1016/j.futures.2019.102495

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, J. (2021). Cascading expert failure. Social Science Research Network.

Google Scholar

Murray, D. (2017). The strange death of Europe: Immigration, identity. Islam. Bloomsbury Publishing.

Google Scholar

Myers, N. (2008). The citizen is willing, but Society Won’t Deliver. Canada: desLibris.

Google Scholar

Neckerman, K. M., and Torche, F. (2007). Inequality: causes and consequences. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 33, 335–357. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.33.040406.131755

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, A., Eva, N., Bindl, U. K., and Stoverink, A. C. (2022). Organizational and vocational behavior in times of crisis: a review of empirical work undertaken during the COVID-19 pandemic and introduction to the special issue. Appl. Psychol. 71, 743–764. doi: 10.1111/apps.12409

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohlin, J. D. (2007). Group think: the law of conspiracy and collective reason. J. Crim. Law Criminol. 98, 147–206.

Google Scholar

Packer, D. J. (2009). Avoiding groupthink. Psychol. Sci. 20, 546–548. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02333.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, S. E. (2005). Are we collapsing? A review of Jared Diamond’s collapse: how societies choose to fail or succeed. J. Econ. Lit. 43, 1049–1062. doi: 10.1257/002205105775362032

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Patt, A., and Zeckhauser, R. (2000). Action Bias and environmental decisions. J. Risk Uncertain. 21, 45–72. doi: 10.1023/a:1026517309871

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perret, C., Hart, E., and Powers, S. T. (2020). From disorganized equality to efficient hierarchy: how group size drives the evolution of hierarchy in human societies. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 287:20200693. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2020.0693

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, M. A., Green, B., and Fuller, S. (2022). China’s rise, the Asian century and the clash of meta-civilizations. Educ. Philos. Theory 55, 674–684. doi: 10.1080/00131857.2022.2032654

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, M. E., and Shin, A. J. (2022). Inequality and immigration policy. Stud. Comp. Int. Dev. 58, 224–251. doi: 10.1007/s12116-022-09372-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pezzullo, A. M., Axfors, C., Contopoulos-Ioannidis, D. G., Apostolatos, A., and Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2023). Age-stratified infection fatality rate of COVID-19 in the non-elderly population. Environ. Res. 216:114655. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2022.114655

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickett, K. E., and Wilkinson, R. G. (2015). Income inequality and health: a causal review. Soc. Sci. Med. 128, 316–326. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.12.031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prasad, V., and Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2022). Constructive and obsessive criticism in science. Eur. J. Clin. Investig. 52:e13839. doi: 10.1111/eci.13839

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratto, F. (1999). The puzzle of continuing group inequality: piecing together psychological, social, and cultural forces in social dominance theory. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1, 191–263. doi: 10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60274-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., and Levin, S. (2006). Social dominance theory and the dynamics of intergroup relations: taking stock and looking forward. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 17, 271–320. doi: 10.1080/10463280601055772

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pummerer, L., Böhm, R., Lilleholt, L., Winter, K., Zettler, I., and Sassenberg, K. (2021). Conspiracy theories and their societal effects during the COVID-19 pandemic. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 13, 49–59. doi: 10.1177/19485506211000217

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ralston, E. (2010). Deviance or norm? Exploring corporate social responsibility. Eur. Bus. Rev. 22, 397–410. doi: 10.1108/09555341011056177

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramsden, E. (2011). From rodent utopia to urban hell: population, pathology, and the crowded rats of NIMH. Isis 102, 659–688. doi: 10.1086/663598

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramsden, E., and Adams, J. (2009). Escaping the laboratory: the rodent experiments of John B. Calhoun & Their Cultural Influence. J. Soc. Hist. 42, 761–797. doi: 10.1353/jsh/42.3.761

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramya, R., Babu, C. C., and Akshay, P. (2020). Population backfire: hindsight to Malthusian catastrophe. Shanlax Int. J. Arts Sci. Hum. 8, 121–126. doi: 10.34293/sijash.v8i1.3248

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, G. P. (2020). Can systems thinking be an antidote to extensive evil? Syst. Res. Behav. Sci. 38, 401–412. doi: 10.1002/sres.2677

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, S. L. (2008). “Dysfunctional workplace behavior” in The SAGE handbook of organizational behavior: Volume I - Micro approaches. eds. J. Barling and C. L. Cooper (London: SAGE Publications Ltd.), 141–159.

Google Scholar

Rushkoff, D. (2003). Open source democracy: how online communication is changing offline politics. Available at: https://ci.nii.ac.jp/ncid/BA90919634.

Google Scholar

Rushkoff, D. (2009). LET IT DIE: Rushkoff on the economy. Available at: https://arthurmag.com/2009/03/16/let-it-die-rushkoff-on-the-economy/.

Google Scholar

Rushkoff, D. (2019). Team Human. Washington, DC: National Geographic Books.

Google Scholar

Rushkoff, D. (2022). Survival of the richest: escape fantasies of the tech billionaires. Melbourne, Australia: Scribe Publications.

Google Scholar

Salvato, C., Sargiacomo, M., Amore, M. D., and Minichilli, A. (2020). Natural disasters as a source of entrepreneurial opportunity: family business resilience after an earthquake. Strateg. Entrep. J. 14, 594–615. doi: 10.1002/sej.1368

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Santoro, E., and Markus, H. R. (2023). Listening to bridge societal divides. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2023:101696. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2023.101696

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schabram, K., and Maitlis, S. (2017). Negotiating the challenges of a calling: emotion and enacted sensemaking in animal shelter work. Acad. Manag. J. 60, 584–609. doi: 10.5465/amj.2013.0665

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheffer, M. (2016). Anticipating societal collapse; hints from the stone age. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 113, 10733–10735. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1612728113

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schippers, M. C. (2020). For the greater good? The devastating ripple effects of the Covid-19 crisis. Front. Psychol. 11:740. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.577740

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schippers, M. C., Edmondson, A. C., and West, M. (2014). Team reflexivity as an antidote to team information-processing failures. Small Group Res. 45, 731–769. doi: 10.1177/1046496414553473

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schippers, M. C., Ioannidis, J. P. A., and Joffe, A. R. (2022). Aggressive measures, rising inequalities, and mass formation during the COVID-19 crisis: an overview and proposed way forward. Front. Public Health 10:965. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.950965

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schippers, M. C., and Rus, D. (2021). Optimizing decision-making processes in times of COVID-19: using reflexivity to counteract information-processing failures. Front. Psychol. 12:525. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.650525

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schippers, M. C., and Ziegler, N. (2023). Shaping ideal futures: writing a letter to the future. Social Science Research Network.

Google Scholar

Schmidt, A. (2016). Groupthink. Encyclopedia Britannica. Available at: https://www.britannica.com/science/groupthink.

Google Scholar

Schmitt, M., and Branscombe, N. R. (2002). The meaning and consequences of perceived discrimination in disadvantaged and privileged social groups. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 12, 167–199. doi: 10.1080/14792772143000058

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, L. M., and Woloshin, S. (2019). Medical Marketing in the United States, 1997-2016. JAMA J. Am. Med. Assoc. 321, 80–96. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.19320

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarzer, N. (2022) The math of body, soul, and the universe. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Google Scholar

Seyoum, B. (2020). State fragility and human development: a study with special emphasis on social cohesion. Int. J. Soc. Econ. 48, 39–61. doi: 10.1108/ijse-07-2020-0445

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepherd, D. A., and Suddaby, R. (2016). Theory building; a review and integration. J. Manag. 43, 59–86. doi: 10.1177/0149206316647102

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepherd, D. A., and Williams, T. A. (2014). Local venturing as compassion organizing in the aftermath of a natural disaster: the role of localness and community in reducing suffering. J. Manag. Stud. 51, 952–994. doi: 10.1111/joms.12084

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepherd, D. A., and Williams, T. A. (2022). Different response paths to organizational resilience. Small Bus. Econ. 61, 23–58. doi: 10.1007/s11187-022-00689-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheth, J. N. (2007). The self-destructive habits of good companies: and how to break them (paperback). London, UK: Pearson Education.

Google Scholar

Shir-Raz, Y., Elisha, E., Martin, B., Ronel, N., and Guetzkow, J. (2022). Censorship and suppression of COVID-19 heterodoxy: tactics and counter-tactics. Minerva 61, 1–27. doi: 10.1007/s11024-022-09479-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sidanius, J., and Pratto, F. (2000). Social dominance: an intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression. Choice Rev. Online 37:37. doi: 10.5860/choice.37-3413

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sidanius, J., Pratto, F., van Laar, C., and Levin, S. (2004). Social dominance theory: its agenda and method. Polit. Psychol. 25, 845–880. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00401.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Silveira, S., Hecht, M., Matthaeus, H., Adli, M., Voelkle, M. C., and Singer, T. (2022). Coping with the COVID-19 pandemic: perceived changes in psychological vulnerability, resilience and social cohesion before, during and after lockdown. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:3290. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19063290

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simandan, D., Rinner, C., and Capurri, V. (2023). The academic left, human geography, and the rise of authoritarianism during the COVID-19 pandemic. Geogr. Ann. Ser. B Human Geogr. 2023, 1–21. doi: 10.1080/04353684.2023.2168560

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, G. D., Shipley, M. J., and Rose, G. (1990). Magnitude and causes of socioeconomic differentials in mortality: further evidence from the Whitehall study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 44, 265–270. doi: 10.1136/jech.44.4.265

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spengler, O. (1991). The decline of the West. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Starystach, S., and Höly, K. (2021). Silence and deviance in organizations. The Silence of Organizations. How Organizations Cover up Wrongdoings, pp. 69–84.

Google Scholar

Statistics. (2021). JOLTS home: U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. Available at: https://www.bls.gov/jlt/.

Google Scholar

Staw, B. M., Sandelands, L. E., and Dutton, J. E. (1981). Threat rigidity effects in organizational behavior: a multilevel analysis. Adm. Sci. Q. 26:501. doi: 10.2307/2392337

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoner, J. A. F. (1961). A comparison of individual and group decisions involving risk. Available at: https://dspace.mit.edu/handle/1721.1/11330.

Google Scholar

Stuckler, D., and Basu, S. (2013). The body economic: Why austerity kills. New York: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Sull, D., Sull, C., and Zweig, B. (2022) Toxic culture is driving the great resignation. Available at: https://sloanreview.mit.edu/article/toxic-culture-is-driving-the-great-resignation/.

Google Scholar

Sullivan, D., and Hickel, J. (2023). Capitalism and extreme poverty: a global analysis of real wages, human height, and mortality since the long 16th century. World Dev. 161:106026. doi: 10.1016/j.worlddev.2022.106026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sunstein, C., and Zeckhauser, R. (2010). “Dreadful possibilities, neglected probabilities” in The irrational economist: Making decisions in a dangerous world. eds. E. Michel-Kerjan and P. Slovic (New York: PublicAffairs), 116–123.

Google Scholar

Swami, V., and Furnham, A. (2014). Political paranoia and conspiracy theories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 218–236.

Google Scholar

Swedberg, R. (2016). Before theory comes theorizing or how to make social science more interesting. Br. J. Sociol. 67, 5–22. doi: 10.1111/1468-4446.12184

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Szymaniak, K., Harmon-Jones, S. K., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2023). Further examinations of attitudes toward discrete emotions, with a focus on attitudes toward anger. Motiv. Emot. 47, 476–493. doi: 10.1007/s11031-022-09998-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Taagepera, R. (1979). Size and duration of empires: growth-decline curves, 600 B.C. to 600 a.D. on JSTOR. Available at: https://www.jstor.org/stable/1170959.

Google Scholar

Tainter, J. A. (1989). The collapse of complex societies. Choice Rev. Online 26:2760. doi: 10.5860/choice.26-2760

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tegnell, A. (2021). The Swedish public health response to COVID-19. APMIS 129, 320–323. doi: 10.1111/apm.13112

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tinbergen, N. (1946). Inleiding tot de diersociologie. [introduction to animal sociology]. Gorinchem, Noorduijn and son.

Google Scholar

Torres, P. (2018). Who would destroy the world? Omnicidal agents and related phenomena. Aggress. Violent Behav. 39, 129–138. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2018.02.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Toynbee, A. J. (1987). A study of history: volume I: abridgement of volumes I-VI. Abridgement by D.C. Somervell. Oxford: Oxford Paperbacks.

Google Scholar

Turchin, P. (2007). War and peace and war: The rise and fall of empires. United Kingdom: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Undheim, T., and Zimmer, D. (2023). Intersections, reinforcements, cascades: proceedings of the 2023 Stanford existential risks conference. The Stanford Existential Risks Initiative.

Google Scholar

United Nations. (2022) Goal 10 | Department of Economic and Social Affairs. Available at: https://sdgs.un.org/goals/goal10.

Google Scholar

Van Bavel, B. J. P. (2016). The invisible hand? How market economies have emerged and declined since AD 500. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Van Bavel, J. J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., et al. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 460–471. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Bavel, B., and Scheffer, M. (2021). Historical effects of shocks on inequality: the great leveler revisited. Hum. Soc. Sci. Commun. 8:4. doi: 10.1057/s41599-021-00763-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van De Werfhorst, H. G., and Salverda, W. (2012). Consequences of economic inequality: introduction to a special issue. Res. Soc. Stratif. Mobil. 30, 377–387. doi: 10.1016/j.rssm.2012.08.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Der Vegt, G. S., Essens, P., Wahlström, M., and George, G. (2015). Managing risk and resilience. Acad. Manag. J. 58, 971–980. doi: 10.5465/amj.2015.4004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Fleet, D. D., and Griffin, R. W. (2006). Dysfunctional organization culture. J. Manag. Psychol. 21, 698–708. doi: 10.1108/02683940610713244

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Zomeren, M. (2013). Four core social-psychological motivations to undertake collective action. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 7, 378–388. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12031

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Zomeren, M., Postmes, T., and Spears, R. (2008). Toward an integrative social identity model of collective action: a quantitative research synthesis of three socio-psychological perspectives. Psychol. Bull. 134, 504–535. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.504

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaziri, H., Casper, W. J., Wayne, J. H., and Matthews, R. A. (2020). Changes to the work–family interface during the COVID-19 pandemic: examining predictors and implications using latent transition analysis. J. Appl. Psychol. 105, 1073–1087. doi: 10.1037/apl0000819

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wacquant, L. (2009). Prisons of poverty. Available at: http://loicwacquant.net/assets/Books/TOC/PoP_TOC.pdf.

Google Scholar

Walton, G. M. (2014). The new science of wise psychological interventions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 73–82. doi: 10.1177/0963721413512856

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinick, R. M., Byron, S. C., and Bierman, A. S. (2005). Who can’t pay for health care? J. Gen. Intern. Med. 20, 504–509. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2005.0087.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehead, B. R., and Torossian, E. (2020). Older adults’ experience of the COVID-19 pandemic: a mixed-methods analysis of stresses and joys. The Gerontologist 61, 36–47. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnaa126

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Whyte, W. H. (2012). GroupThink, (Fortune 1952). Fortune, 22 July. Available at: https://fortune.com/2012/07/22/groupthink-fortune-1952/.

Google Scholar

Widmann, T. (2022). Fear, Hope, and COVID-19: emotional elite rhetoric and its impact on the public during the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic. Polit. Psychol. 43, 827–850. doi: 10.1111/pops.12831

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkinson, R., and Pickett, K. (2009). The Spirit level. Why more equal societies almost always do better. Soc. Justice Res 25, 102–107. doi: 10.1007/s11211-012-0148-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkinson, R. G., and Pickett, K. E. (2009). Income inequality and social dysfunction. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 35, 493–511. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-115926

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, T. A., Gruber, D. A., Sutcliffe, K. M., Shepherd, D. A., and Zhao, E. Y. (2017). Organizational response to adversity: fusing crisis management and resilience research streams. Acad. Manag. Ann. 11, 733–769. doi: 10.5465/annals.2015.0134

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, T. A., and Shepherd, D. A. (2017). To the rescue!? Brokering a rapid, scaled and customized compassionate response to suffering after disaster. J. Manag. Stud. 55, 910–942. doi: 10.1111/joms.12291

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, T. (2011) Redirect: The surprising new science of psychological change. United Kingdom: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Winsberg, E., Brennan, J., and Surprenant, C. W. (2020). How government leaders violated their epistemic duties during the SARS-COV-2 crisis. Kennedy Inst. Ethics J. 30, 215–242. doi: 10.1353/ken.2020.0013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, K. (2018). Five fears about mass predictive personalization in an age of surveillance capitalism. Int. Data Privacy Law 8, 258–269. doi: 10.1093/idpl/ipy020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yon, M., and Peterson, J. B. (2022). Cometh the horsemen: pandemic, famine, war | Michael Yon | EP 274. Available at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=R7gAEkzIgvw.

Google Scholar

Zhang, S. X., Wang, Y., Rauch, A., and Wei, F. (2020). Unprecedented disruption of lives and work: health, distress and life satisfaction of working adults in China one month into the COVID-19 outbreak. Psychiatry Res. 288:112958. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112958

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuboff, S. (2015). Big other: Surveillance capitalism and the prospects of an information civilization. J. Inform. Technol. 30, 75–89. doi: 10.1057/jit.2015.5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuboff, S. (2023). “The age of surveillance capitalism” in Social Theory Re-Wired. eds. W. Longhofer and D. Winchester (New York: Routledge), Available at: https://www.taylorfrancis.com/chapters/edit/10.4324/9781003320609-27/age-surveillance-capitalism-shoshana-zuboff

Google Scholar

Keywords: Death Spiral Effect, complex adaptive systems, societal collapse, income inequalities, dysfunctional behavior, elite and masses, turnaround leadership, strengthening of democracy

Citation: Schippers MC, Ioannidis JPA and Luijks MWJ (2024) Is society caught up in a Death Spiral? Modeling societal demise and its reversal. Front. Sociol. 9:1194597. doi: 10.3389/fsoc.2024.1194597

Received: 03 May 2023; Accepted: 19 February 2024;
Published: 12 March 2024.

Edited by:

Todd L. Matthews, Sacred Heart University, United States

Reviewed by:

Annette Freyberg-Inan, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands
Laurens Buijs, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands

Copyright © 2024 Schippers, Ioannidis and Luijks. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michaéla C. Schippers, mschippers@rsm.nl

Download