Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Sustain. Food Syst., 11 February 2021
Sec. Nutrition and Sustainable Diets
Volume 5 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fsufs.2021.594650

Public Attitudes and Willingness to Pay for Cultured Meat: A Cross-Sectional Experimental Study

Bella Nichole Kantor1 Jonathan Kantor2,3,4,5*
  • 1Harvard Extension School, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, United States
  • 2Center for Global Health, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, PA, United States
  • 3Center for Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, PA, United States
  • 4Department of Dermatology, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, PA, United States
  • 5Florida Center for Dermatology, P.A., St. Augustine, FL, United States

Previous research has suggested that the general public is divided over their willingness to consider consuming cultured meat (CM) products. As commercial backing for cultured meat startups increases and the public interest in the US, Europe, and developing countries expands, formally evaluating attitudes to these products will become increasingly important. Willingness to pay (WTP) may provide insight into the level of acceptability of CM products, highlight latent societal preferences, and suggest commercial opportunities. To date, no studies have evaluated the societal WTP for CM products. A cross-sectional internet-based survey was distributed to 300 respondents in the US general population using a survey panel design. The mean age was 30 (range: 18–76), and 47% of respondents were male. We presented respondents with a series of scenarios relating to CM products, framing CM burgers as environmentally friendly vs. as a better alternative to traditional burgers. Prior research has highlighted taste and price as areas of societal concern, but no studies have rigorously evaluated the intersection between these considerations. When CM products were framed as being equivalent in taste to conventional meat, and where their environmental benefits were stressed, respondents were willing to pay significantly more than for a traditional burger ($2.11 vs. $1.00). This WTP jumped to $2.66 when framing the CM burger as the best burger, reflecting a 266% premium that consumers were willing to pay for an appropriately framed CM burger. Framing CM burgers as a better alternative to traditional burgers, rather than focusing on their environmental impact, similarly led to the highest desirability ratings. These preferences were also reflected in a contingent valuation discrete choice experiment examining preferences for paying $1 for a traditional burger vs. $2 for a CM burger. These findings support our hypothesis regarding the existence of what we term the gold-standard bias, a cognitive bias that systematically favors a product or service framed as the best available choice over and above its marginal benefit, and has significant and broad implications for feasibility, pricing, and marketing, suggesting the need for further research in this area.

Introduction

Cultured meat (CM) has been the subject of increased interest and scrutiny over the past several years, as several commercial startups have worked aggressively to move the product from the laboratory to the factory and consumer (Post, 2012). Aside from the intense media interest sparked by CM development (Goodwin and Shoulders, 2013; Laestadius and Caldwell, 2015), the need for alternative pathways for meat production have been spurred by a complex range of market forces, including increasing global demand for meat—a function of both the rising middle class in emerging markets such as China and increased social pressures for meat consumption as a marker of wealth (conspicuous meat consumption)—as well as concerns related to the environmental impact of traditional beef production in terms of greenhouse gas emissions, land use, and water use that may possibly be ameliorated by CM (Tuomisto and Teixeira de Mattos, 2011; Mattick et al., 2015a,b). Moreover, the COVID-19 pandemic has served to further inflame concerns regarding the safety and robustness of the traditional beef marketplace and supply chain, and therefore a scalable alternative source of meat supply may be of significant interest.

There is a burgeoning literature on public attitudes to CM consumption and production; a recent systematic review highlighted 14 studies evaluating consumer attitudes using a range of techniques (Bryant and Barnett, 2018), and an update to this review included 26 additional studies (Bryant and Barnett, 2020). Few studies, however, have attempted to tease out the importance of framing CM products in developing public acceptance, despite the acknowledgment that how CM is described may have a marked impact on public responses (Bryant and Barnett, 2018), though one recent study examined whether providing information on CM impacted taste preferences (Rolland et al., 2020) and others have addressed the importance of positive framing (Bryant and Barnett, 2020). This is a key lacuna, as there is a wide behavioral economics literature that highlights the ways in which framing can affect acceptability, particularly for novel technologies. While studies consistently show that consumers are not interested in framing CM as high-technology and scientifically advanced (Bryant and Dillard, 2019), this has not yet translated into a broad shift in presentation from either the media or CM producers.

Willingness to pay, or contingent valuation, is an established methodology to establish a threshold level of societal acceptability (Meyer et al., 2018; Song and Lee, 2018). It has been used for a range of goods and services, from foodstuffs to cosmetic and surgical medical procedures (Botelho et al., 2017; Chen et al., 2019; Kantor, 2021). While it has the potential to elicit public preferences in a granular fashion, this approach has never been applied to CM products.

Given the importance of CM and the gaps in the literature surrounding both framing and WTP, we performed a WTP analysis that presented US consumers with a range of scenarios involving CM in order to elicit their WTP for CM products and explore the associations between preferences and baseline demographic variables.

Methods

After iterative pilot testing, we developed a cross-sectional online survey of the general US population to assess consumer attitudes to CM and determine their WTP. Ethics approval for this study was obtained from Cambridge University, Judge Business School. The survey was prepared on the Qualtrics platform (Qualtrics Corp, Provo, Utah) and distributed to a sample of the US population aged 18 and over through Prolific Academic (Oxford, United Kingdom), a platform for academic survey research (Peer et al., 2017). Prolific Academic maintains a database of >100,000 potential respondents in multiple countries categorized by age, sex, and location, and sends them an invitation to participate in a study; while there is an option to have Prolific Academic stratify respondents by age, sex, and race to achieve a representative national sample, (Kantor and Kantor, 2020a,b), we used a convenience sample of US-based respondents for this study. Given the survey panel design, no actual response rate is available, though Prolific Academic performs internal checks to establish the validity of respondents' responses. Respondents were rewarded with a small payment (<US$1). Subjects provided consent and were allowed to terminate the survey at any time, and all responses were confidential.

Baseline responses to survey questions were recorded, and demographic information was self-reported by respondents. Responses to questions regarding WTP for CM hamburger patties were recorded using a slider scale ranging from $0 to $5 after anchoring subjects to a $1 price point for a standard beef hamburger. The default position of the slider was set at $1. Subjects were also asked regarding the perceived desirability of CM burgers, with a range of 0 (worse than a regular burger) to 100 (better than a regular burger), with 50 indicating equivalence. The slider's default position was set at 50. We also performed a discrete choice experiment (DCE) to determine what proportion of respondents would prefer to pay $2 for a CM patty rather than $1 for a standard patty.

In order to determine whether framing a CM burger as a better alternative to a standard hamburger patty would affect WTP, we presented respondents with two scenarios distributed in random order. In one iteration, CM burgers were framed as environmentally friendly, and in the alternate presentation they were presented as a better alternative to standard burgers.

The environmentally-friendly framing language was presented as follows: “Imagine that a company has developed a way to make hamburgers that taste, look, and smell, exactly like the usual beef hamburgers that you are used to. Instead of coming from cows, these burgers are made in a factory by growing the meat cells from cows. This means that these burgers are just like regular burgers but they have much less environmental impact (no cow grazing, no methane release) and no cows are killed to make them.”

In contrast, the best-tasting framing was presented as follows: “Imagine that a company has created a way to make the best hamburgers in the world by culturing the meat cells from the world-famous Wagyu beef. These burgers can be available to everyone and taste better than anything out there. Instead of coming from cows, these burgers are made in a factory by growing (culturing) the meat cells from cows.”

T-tests and chi-squared tests were used as appropriate for baseline continuous and categorical variables. Subgroup comparisons of non-normally distributed data were performed using the Kruskal Wallis test. Paired t-tests were used to compare responses stratified by scenario presentation. Univariate logistic regression odds ratios of association were assessed between the dependent variable of preference for paying $2 for a CM patty over $1 for a standard patty in the DCE and baseline characteristics. Univariate logistic regression odds ratios of association were also assessed between the dependent variable of willingness to pay at least $1 for a CM burger (equivalent to a traditional burger) and baseline characteristics. Statistical analyses were performed using Stata 13 for Mac (College Station, Texas).

Results

Of the 311 total surveys, 6 refused consent and 5 failed to answer any questions after providing consent, yielding a total of 300 usable surveys. The mean (SD) age was 30 (11.0), and respondents were 47% male. Detailed baseline characteristics of the respondents are included in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and baseline characteristics of respondents, overall and by whether the switched preferences in the DCE in favor of CM burgers when presented as the best option.

When framed as environmentally friendly, respondents were willing to pay a mean of $2.10 [95% confidence intervals (CI) 1.97, 2.24] for a CM patty after being anchored to a $1 reference price for a standard beef patty. When framing the CM burger as the best burger, respondents were willing to pay $2.66 (95% CI 2.50, 2.82) for the CM burger after being anchored to a $1 reference price for a standard beef patty (p < 0.0001). The demand curve highlighted the increased WTP for CM products when using the best framing (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Demand-price curve for WTP, by framing.

Desirability scores for CM patties were 61.5 (95% CI 58.9, 64.0) when framed as environmentally friendly and 68.7 (66.0, 71.4) when framed as the best tasting burgers (p < 0.0001).

For the DCE, of those who expressed a preference, 66.7% (95% CI 0.61, 0.72) of respondents stated they would prefer to pay $2 for a CM patty rather than $1 for a standard patty when presented with the environmentally friendly framing. When presented with the better tasting framing, 80.0% (95% CI 0.75, 0.84) of respondents stated they would prefer to pay $2 for a CM patty rather than $1 for a standard patty (p < 0.0001).

On univariate logistic regression analyses, with the outcome of interest switching preferences in favor of CM burgers in the DCE from framing as environmentally friendly to best tasting, men had greater odds of switching in favor of CM burgers when presented with them as the best alternative with an odds ratio of association (OR) of 2.77 (95% CI 1.37, 5.59). Increasing age was associated with a decrease in the odds of switching when framed as the best burger with an OR of 0.96 (0.93, 1.0).

When evaluating the association of WTP at least $1 (that is, WTP as much or more than a traditional burger) and baseline characteristics, stratified by framing, increasing age was associated with a decreased odds of WTP for both the environmental framing (OR 0.94, 95% CI 0.92, 0.97) and best tasting (OR 0.95, 95% CI 0.92, 0.98) framing approaches (Table 2). Men were more likely to be WTP at least $1 when framed as the best alternative (OR 3.50, 95% CI 1.26, 9.68), and those who identified as religious were less likely to be WTP at least $1 when framed as the best tasting option (OR 0.40, 95% CI 0.17, 0.95).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Association of willingness to pay as much or more than a traditional burger and baseline characteristics, stratified by framing.

Discussion

In this first study evaluating societal WTP for CM hamburgers, we found that the majority of respondents were willing to pay more for CM hamburgers than traditional hamburgers; while this willingness was more pronounced when framing CM burgers as the best available option, it persisted even when framing these burgers as environmentally friendly. These findings suggest that there may be a substantial US market for CM burger products and that manufacturers need not anchor to a price point of equivalency with traditional burgers—which is unlikely to be feasible, particularly in the near future (Bryant, 2020).

We also found that men in particular were more likely to respond to framing the CM hamburgers as the best tasting; whether this was a function of the framing alone or because men were less likely to be swayed by the alternative environmental messaging is an area for future research. Larger studies including sub-populations of interest may be warranted as research in this area progresses, though the increased acceptance of CM by men echoes data from prior studies, further supporting the validity of our findings (Wilks and Phillips, 2017; Slade, 2018; Bryant and Dillard, 2019; Gomez-Luciano et al., 2019b; Shaw and Iomaire, 2019; Bryant and Barnett, 2020; Dupont and Fiebelkorn, 2020; Zhang et al., 2020).

One of the strengths of our study was our use of three separate approaches to assess preferences: a direct WTP approach, a desirability question, and a DCE approach. Critically, all three approaches yielded similar data, demonstrating a WTP more for CM hamburgers whether framed as WTP, increased desirability of CM products, or DCE of preference for paying $2 for a CM burger over $1 for a traditional burger. Taken together, this provides robust evidence that CM hamburgers are commercially viable and will be able to command a price premium over standard hamburgers.

Several prior studies have tangentially addressed WTP considerations; one study in a US population found that 15.8% of respondents would be willing to pay somewhat or much more than farmed meat (Wilks and Phillips, 2017). A Belgian study noted that after providing respondents with some information on CM products, 13.9% stated that they would “surely” pay more, while 43.9% stated that they would “maybe” pay more; after providing respondents with additional information on CM, these numbers climbed to 35.8 and 27.9%, respectively (Wim Verbeke, 2015). A dichotomous willingness to purchase approach has also been used more recently in a multinational sample that included participants in the UK, Spain, Brazil, and the Dominican Republic, where the authors found that perceptions of healthiness may predict positive attitudes to CM (Gomez-Luciano et al., 2019a). The dampened enthusiasm for WTP for CM in these studies may be a function of timing (increased media reporting on CM likely has contributed to increased societal familiarity, which may breed interest and acceptance), as well as population differences between the US, where a laissez faire attitude to the naturalness of the food supply prevails and Europe, where greater concern has been raised regarding a range of food technology issues, from genetically modified organisms to nanotechnology (Grobe and Rissanen, 2012; Gupta et al., 2012; Lucht, 2015; de Lorenzo et al., 2018; Bryant et al., 2019).

Biases and heuristics have an established role in the field of consumer neuroscience, as an appreciation of the existence and importance of such decision-making shortcuts, sometimes leading to surprising outcomes, has increased over the past decades (Kahneman and Tversky, 2000). While known biases and heuristics number in the hundreds, none fully explain the tendency for subjects to adjust their valuation of a choice based solely on its framing as the best alternative, when all other descriptors are held constant. We hypothesized the existence of a discrete bias favoring choices that are framed as the best available or gold standard–the gold standard bias–and that these choices would systematically be valued more highly than their objective marginal benefit would suggest (Kantor, 2021).

Several associations between baseline demographic variables and measures of WTP are worth mentioning. First, increasing age was associated with a decreased odds of WTP as much or more than a traditional burger, regardless of framing. Each year of age was associated with a 5–6% drop in the odds of being WTP at least the equivalent of a traditional burger for a CM burger. This may be a product of a more traditionalist mindset in older consumers, though further investigation into this finding is needed; regardless, the decreasing enthusiasm for CM with age found in our study echoes prior research in this field, again bolstering the validity of our findings (Wilks and Phillips, 2017; Slade, 2018; Bryant and Dillard, 2019; Mancini and Antonioli, 2019; Shaw and Iomaire, 2019; Bryant and Barnett, 2020; Weinrich et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2020).

Second, the potential impact of framing on men was particularly powerful: the odds of switching over in the DCE from preferring a standard burger for $1 to a CM burger for $2 based on framing as the best option rather than the environmentally friendly option was 2.77 for men. Moreover, examining the baseline WTP as much or more for a CM hamburger than a traditional burger, men had 3.5 times the odds of women in unadjusted analyses. This may be the result of the known tendency of men to be more accepting of CM (Wilks and Phillips, 2017; Slade, 2018; Bryant and Dillard, 2019; Gomez-Luciano et al., 2019b; Shaw and Iomaire, 2019; Bryant and Barnett, 2020; Dupont and Fiebelkorn, 2020; Zhang et al., 2020). Further investigation into this phenomenon is warranted, since understanding whether men respond preferentially to gold-standard framing may have important and wide-ranging implications.

Finally, those who self-identify as religious appeared less likely to be willing to pay at least as much for a CM hamburger as a traditional hamburger; when framed as the best available burger, the odds ratio was 0.40, suggesting that those who identify as religious may be less willing to consider CM hamburgers. From a marketing and outreach standpoint, it will be important to carefully address this population in future efforts aimed at encouraging CM acceptance, and further research is warranted.

Pricing sends a range of signals to purchases; products that are priced lower than expected may signal to consumers that a product may be inferior, while high prices tend to signal the potential for added value (Jiang et al., 2014; Mastrobuoni et al., 2014). Such signaling is particularly important for products that lack an obvious valuation metric, such as services and some food products, such as meat (Utaka, 2008; Becerril Arreola, 2013). High prices may send aspirational value signals above and beyond baseline measures of legitimacy and quality (Askin and Bothner, 2016), although some have questioned in the past whether such signaling is truly effective (Koku, 1995). Indeed, if consumers believe that CM products have a greater market share they may be more likely to be willing to adopt CM products (Slade, 2018). In the US, USDA grades such as Prime and Choice were introduced in part to provide a reproducible quality metric (O'Quinn et al., 2015); in recent years, the proliferation of measures such as grass-fed, free-range, organic, and other labels have introduced additional complexity in the marketplace. This complexity may promote a tendency to elide over potential differences, as consumer behavior and behavioral economics studies have highlighted consumers' inability to reproducibly consider more than a handful of options (Carmon et al., 2003; Mick et al., 2004; Novemsky and Kahneman, 2005). Moreover, since CM products will likely be more expensive and have a smaller supply when they come to market, this itself may help drive a cycle of interest and increased WTP, at least in the short term.

This study has several limitations. As with any survey-based research, the generalizability of our findings may be limited, particularly as the population of respondents may not accurately reflect the general US population. Response and social desirability biases are always an issue with survey-based work, though the latter may be partially mitigated by the anonymous nature of the survey. Overall, 28% of respondents stated that they eat <1 burger per week on average; future research better elucidating the differential preferences of those who do and do not regularly consume burgers (beyond a vegetarian/non-vegetarian classification) may be helpful.

In conclusion, we found that respondents were willing to pay more than double for a CM hamburger patty than they would for a traditional burger patty, regardless of framing. These findings have critical implications as companies begin to scale-up manufacturing capacity and pricing strategies are developed. Future work including developing a demand-price curve for the general US population is warranted, since these findings may have both financial (ideal pricing to maximize profitability) and marketing (framing CM products as a better alternative rather than environmentally friendly) implications.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Cambridge Judge Business School. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JK: study conception. JK and BK: study design, statistical analyses, and writing. JK: oversight. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Askin, N., and Bothner, M. S. (2016). Status-aspirational pricing: the “chivas regal” strategy in U.S. higher education, 2006–2012. Adm. Sci. Q. 61, 217–253. doi: 10.1177/0001839216629671

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becerril Arreola, R. (2013). Modeling consumer preferences for status-signaling brands: branding, pricing, and product-line decisions (Dissertation). University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA. Retrieved from: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/5kj9q2dw

Google Scholar

Botelho, A., Dinis, I., Lourenço-Gomes, L., Moreira, J., Costa Pinto, L., and Simões, O. (2017). The effect of sequential information on consumers' willingness to pay for credence food attributes. Appetite 118, 17–25. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, C., and Barnett, J. (2018). Consumer acceptance of cultured meat: a systematic review. Meat Sci. 143, 8–17. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2018.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, C., and Barnett, J. (2020). Consumer acceptance of cultured meat: an updated review (2018–2020). Appl. Sci. 10:5201. doi: 10.3390/app10155201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, C., and Dillard, C. (2019). The impact of framing on acceptance of cultured meat. Front. Nutr. 6:103. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2019.00103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, C., Szejda, K., Parekh, N., Desphande, V., and Tse, B. (2019). A survey of consumer perceptions of plant-based and clean meat in the USA, India, and China. Front. Sustain. Food Syst. 3:11. doi: 10.3389/fsufs.2019.00011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, C. J. (2020). Culture, meat, and cultured meat. J. Anim. Sci. 98, 1–7. doi: 10.1093/jas/skaa172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmon, Z., Wertenbroch, K., and Zeelenberg, M. (2003). Option attachment: when deliberating makes choosing feel like losing. J. Consum. Res. 30, 15–29. doi: 10.1086/374701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, D., Ishii, M., Nellis, J., Bater, K., Darrach, H., Liao, D., et al. (2019). Assessment of casual observers' willingness to pay for increased attractiveness through rhinoplasty. JAMA Facial Plast. Surg. 21, 27–31. doi: 10.1001/jamafacial.2018.1526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Lorenzo, V., Prather, K. L., Chen, G. Q., O'Day, E., von Kameke, C., Oyarzun, D. A., et al. (2018). The power of synthetic biology for bioproduction, remediation and pollution control: the UN's sustainable development goals will inevitably require the application of molecular biology and biotechnology on a global scale. EMBO Rep. 19:e45658. doi: 10.15252/embr.201745658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dupont, J., and Fiebelkorn, F. (2020). Attitudes and acceptance of young people toward the consumption of insects and cultured meat in Germany. Food Qual. Prefer. 85:103983. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103983

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez-Luciano, C. A., de Aguiar, L. K., Vriesekoop, F., and Urbano, B. (2019a). Consumers' willingness to purchase three alternatives to meat proteins in the United Kingdom, Spain, Brazil and the Dominican Republic. Food Qual. Prefer. 78:103732. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.103732

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez-Luciano, C. A., Vriesekoop, F., and Urbano, B. (2019b). Towards food security of alternative dietray proteins: a comparison between Spain and the Dominican Republic. Amfiteatru Econ. 21, 393–407. doi: 10.24818/EA/2019/51/393

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodwin, J. N., and Shoulders, C. W. (2013). The future of meat: a qualitative analysis of cultured meat media coverage. Meat Sci. 95, 445–450. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2013.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grobe, A., and Rissanen, M. E. (2012). Nanotechnologies in agriculture and food - an overview of different fields of application, risk assessment and public perception. Recent Pat Food Nutr Agric. 4, 176–186. doi: 10.2174/2212798411204030176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gupta, N., Fischer, A. R., van der Lans, I. A., and Frewer, L. J. (2012). Factors influencing societal response of nanotechnology: an expert stakeholder analysis. J Nanopart Res. 14:857. doi: 10.1007/s11051-012-0857-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, B., Ni, J., and Srinivasan, K. (2014). Signaling through pricing by service providers with social preferences. Mark. Sci. 33, 641–654. doi: 10.1287/mksc.2014.0850

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (2000). Choices, Values, and Frames. Translated From Eng by. Cambridge. New York Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Kantor, B. N., and Kantor, J. (2020a). Mental health outcomes and associations during the COVID-19 pandemic: a cross-sectional population-based study in the United States. Front. Psychiatry 11:569083. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.569083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kantor, B. N., and Kantor, J. (2020b). Non-pharmaceutical interventions for pandemic COVID-19: a cross-sectional investigation of US general public beliefs, attitudes, and actions. Front. Med. 7:384. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2020.00384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kantor, J. (2021). Willingness to pay for surgical treatments for basal cell carcinoma: a population-based cross-sectional study. Dermatol. Surg. doi: 10.1097/DSS.0000000000002874

CrossRef Full Text

Koku, P. (1995). Price signaling: does it ever work? J. Consum. Mark. 12:45. doi: 10.1108/07363769510080988

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laestadius, L. I., and Caldwell, M. A. (2015). Is the future of meat palatable? Perceptions of in vitro meat as evidenced by online news comments. Public Health Nutr. 18, 2457–2467. doi: 10.1017/S1368980015000622

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucht, J. M. (2015). Public acceptance of plant biotechnology and GM crops. Viruses 7, 4254–4281. doi: 10.3390/v7082819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mancini, M. C., and Antonioli, F. (2019). Exploring consumers' attitude towards cultured meat in Italy. Meat Sci. 150, 101–110. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2018.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mastrobuoni, G., Peracchi, F., and Tetenov, A. (2014). Price as a signal of product quality: some experimental evidence*. J. Wine Econ. 9, 135–152. doi: 10.1017/jwe.2014.17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattick, C. S., Landis, A. E., and Allenby, B. R. (2015a). A case for systemic environmental analysis of cultured meat. J. Integr. Agric. 14:249. doi: 10.1016/S2095-3119(14)60885-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattick, C. S., Landis, A. E., Allenby, B. R., and Genovese, N. J. (2015b). Anticipatory life cycle analysis of in vitro biomass cultivation for cultured meat production in the United States. Environ. Sci. Technol. 49, 11941–11949. doi: 10.1021/acs.est.5b01614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, J., Shankar, V., and Berry, L. L. (2018). Pricing hybrid bundles by understanding the drivers of willingness to pay. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 46:497. doi: 10.1007/s11747-017-0546-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mick, D., Broniarczyk, S., and Haidt, J. (2004). Choose, choose, choose, choose, choose, choose, choose: emerging and prospective research on the deleterious effects of living in consumer hyperchoice. J. Bus. Ethics 52, 207–211. doi: 10.1023/B:BUSI.0000035906.74034.d4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novemsky, N., and Kahneman, D. (2005). How do intentions affect loss aversion? JMR 42, 139–140. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.42.2.139.62295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Quinn, T. G., Brooks, J. C., and Miller, M. F. (2015). Consumer assessment of beef tenderloin steaks from various USDA quality grades at 3 degrees of doneness. J. Food Sci. 80, S444–S449. doi: 10.1111/1750-3841.12775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peer, E., Brandimarte, L., Samat, S., and Acquisti, A. (2017). Beyond the turk: alternative platforms for crowdsourcing behavioral research. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 70, 153–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Post, M. J. (2012). Cultured meat from stem cells: challenges and prospects. Meat Sci. 92:297. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2012.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolland, N. C. M., Markus, C. R., and Post, M. J. (2020). The effect of information content on acceptance of cultured meat in a tasting context. PLoS ONE 15:e0231176. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, E., and Iomaire, M. M. C. (2019). A comparative analysis of the attitudes of rural and urban consumers towards cultured meat. Br. Food J. 121, 1782–1800. doi: 10.1108/BFJ-07-2018-0433

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slade, P. (2018). If you build it, will they eat it? Consumer preferences for plant-based and cultured meat burgers. Appetite 125, 428–437. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.02.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, H. J., and Lee, E.-K. (2018). Evaluation of willingness to pay per quality-adjusted life year for a cure: a contingent valuation method using a scenario-based survey. Medicine 97:e12453. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000012453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuomisto, H. L., and Teixeira de Mattos, M. J. (2011). Environmental impacts of cultured meat production. Environ. Sci. Technol. 45:6117. doi: 10.1021/es200130u

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Utaka, A. (2008). Pricing strategy, quality signaling, and entry deterrence. Int. J. Ind. Organ. 26, 878–888. doi: 10.1016/j.ijindorg.2007.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinrich, R., Strack, M., and Neugebauer, F. (2020). Consumer acceptance of cultured meat in Germany. Meat Sci. 162:107924. doi: 10.1016/j.meatsci.2019.107924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilks, M., and Phillips, C. J. C. (2017). Attitudes to in vitro meat: a survey of potential consumers in the United States. PLoS ONE 12:e0171904. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0171904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wim Verbeke, P. S. E. J. V. L. (2015). Challenges and prospects for consumer acceptance of cultured meat. J. Integr. Agric. 14, 285–294. doi: 10.1016/S2095-3119(14)60884-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, M., Li, L., and Bai, J. (2020). Consumer acceptance of cultured meat in urban areas of three cities in China. Food Control 118:107390. doi: 10.1016/j.foodcont.2020.107390

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: willingness to pay, cultured meat-based proteins, biases and heuristics, preferences, gold standard bias

Citation: Kantor BN and Kantor J (2021) Public Attitudes and Willingness to Pay for Cultured Meat: A Cross-Sectional Experimental Study. Front. Sustain. Food Syst. 5:594650. doi: 10.3389/fsufs.2021.594650

Received: 14 September 2020; Accepted: 15 January 2021;
Published: 11 February 2021.

Edited by:

Bruno Sobral, Colorado State University, United States

Reviewed by:

Anna Saba, Council for Agricultural and Economics Research (CREA), Italy
Mark J. Post, Maastricht University, Netherlands
Christopher John Bryant, University of Bath, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2021 Kantor and Kantor. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jonathan Kantor, jonkantor@gmail.com

Download