ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pediatr., 09 July 2025

Sec. Children and Health

Volume 13 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fped.2025.1562630

Sleep problems from age two to five years and neurological outcome in children born extremely preterm: a cross-sectional study

  • 1Department of Global Public Health and Primary Care, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 2Department of Health Registry Research and Development, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Bergen, Norway
  • 3Department of Psychosocial Science, Faculty of Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 4Department of Clinical Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 5Department of Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway
  • 6Norwegian Competence Center for Sleep Disorders, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway

Introduction: Premature birth is associated with a higher risk of sleep problems and neurodevelopmental disabilities (NDD). We examined relationships between sleep problems and cognitive, motor, and sensory functions in a national cohort of five-year-old children born extremely preterm (EPT) with the purpose of identifying possible means of improving developmental outcomes.

Methods: This study was part of a national cohort study of all children born extremely preterm, defined here as gestational age less than 28 completed weeks, or birth weight below 1,000 g, born in Norway in 1999 and 2000. Parents completed a structured retrospective questionnaire at age five to assess sleep problems from ages two to five years. We assessed cognitive function using the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised (WPPSI-R), evaluated motor function with the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (M-ABC) and classified cerebral palsy (CP) according to the Gross Motor Function Classification System (GMFCS). NDD was graded from no NDD (no disabilities) to NDD 2 and 3 (moderate and severe disability).

Results: Of 372 eligible children, 253 (68%) participated. Parents reported that 28.5% had general sleep problems from ages two to five years. Prevalences of specific problems were 21.7% for nighttime awakenings, 17.8% for difficulty falling asleep, 5.9% for early morning awakening and 1.6% for late morning awakening. Children with Full scale IQ < 85 were at increased risk of general sleep problems (adjusted odds ratio - aOR 1.8), as well as nighttime awakenings (aOR 2.8), and early morning awakenings (OR 2.9), but not for difficulty falling asleep compared to those with higher IQ levels.

EPT children with moderate to severe NDD (NDD 2 and 3) showed a higher prevalence of general sleep problems [adjusted odds ratio (aOR) 3.9], nighttime awakenings (aOR 4.8), and early morning awakenings (OR 7.9) compared to those with no NDD (NDD 0).

Conclusion: General and specific sleep problems were associated with low cognitive function and moderate to severe NDD. Our findings underscore the importance of addressing sleep within a comprehensive care framework for EPT children and highlight the need for addressing and target interventions for sleep problems.

Introduction

Preterm birth can disrupt normal brain development and cause a syndrome described as “encephalopathy of prematurity” (1). The outcome may be various degrees of neurodevelopmental disabilities (NDD), i.e., cognitive, behavioral, motor, and sensory disorders, of which cognitive difficulties are the most common sequelae (2). The risk of NDD increases with decreasing gestational age (GA) at birth (35). Emerging evidence also suggests that prematurity is associated with an increased risk of sleep problems during childhood (68), including the crucial preschool years (9, 10). Additionally, it may lead to disrupted circadian rhythm, for instance causing early morning awakening (1113) during childhood.

In addition, sleep itself plays a pivotal role in brain maturation during early childhood (14), and disrupted sleep may adversely affect various developmental outcomes, including cognitive function (15). Children with various neurological disorders, including cerebral palsy (16) and low cognitive function (1719), often experience sleep problems, but to what extent sleep disorders contribute to the severity of NDD is uncertain and may vary.

Preterm-born children may be more susceptible to the negative impact of disturbed sleep on cognitive function than term-born children (20). Prematurity affects both sleep architecture (21) and neurodevelopment (3), with research showing that specific electroencephalographic (EEG) sleep measures correlate with lower neurodevelopmental performance in extremely preterm children at age 2 (22) and in healthy very preterm children at ages 12–24 months (23).

However, the intricate nature of these relationships remains poorly understood (24), and there is limited research on the connection between neurological sequelae and sleep problems in children born preterm. Studies have suggested that reduced sleep efficiency (20), reduced sleep quality (25), and increased variation in sleep patterns (26, 27) are associated with impaired cognitive function in prematurely born children. However, these studies addressed other sleep parameters and more mature premature infants than those in our study.

In a Norwegian national follow-up study of children born extremely preterm (EPT), we have previously found that the prevalence of sleep problems at 11 years of age increased from 20% among children with no disability (NDD 0) to 67% among children with severe disability (NDD 3) (28). The current study is based on the same cohort and explores the prevalence of general and specific sleep problems in EPT children aged two to five years and their association with cognitive and motor difficulties and severity of NDD.

Understanding the prevalence and implications of sleep problems in young EPT children is crucial, as early interventions could correlate with long-term cognitive and developmental challenges. Therefore, this study seeks to answer the following research question: What is the prevalence of general and specific sleep problems in EPT children aged two to five years, and how are these issues related to cognitive and motor difficulties, as well as the severity of neurodevelopmental disabilities? We hypothesize that EPT children will exhibit a high prevalence of sleep problems, and that these sleep problems will be significantly associated with greater severity of neurological outcome by age five.

Materials and methods

Population

This study was part of a national cohort study of all children born EPT, defined here as GA less than 28 completed weeks, or birth weight (BW) below 1,000 g, born in Norway in 1999 and 2000 (29, 30). We defined children as born small for gestational age (SGA) if the BW was below the 10th percentile for GA and sex for Norwegian children (31). The children were prospectively followed from birth. At five years of age, the parents completed questionnaires addressing the child's sleep behavior, physical and mental health, and social circumstances. These questionnaires were sent to them by mail around the same time as the children were examined at the 19 participating pediatric departments in accordance with the research protocol (32).

Sleep questions

The parents were asked if the child had experienced sleep problems after the age of two years. The response options were: “no sleep problems during these years”, “previous sleep problems but not within the last year” and “still experiencing sleep problems”. A child who either had “previous sleep problems but not within the last year” or “still experiencing sleep problems” was defined as having “general sleep problems between the ages of two and five”. For these children, the parents were asked to specify the nature of the problems by ticking the applicable options: “difficulty falling asleep at night”, “waking up during the night”, “waking up unusually early”, “waking up unusually late” or “other kinds of sleep problems”.

Clinical assessments

Experienced pediatricians conducted general clinical and neurological examinations. Visual and auditory functions were assessed during the clinical examination or were based on records from prior assessments at public healthcare clinics.

Physiotherapists assessed motor functions using the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (M-ABC) test (33). The M-ABC test evaluates motor function in children through eight tasks, which address hand motor skills, ball skills, and balance. Scoring for each task ranges from 0 to 5, where higher scores denote poorer motor performance. A cumulative score above the 95th percentile of age-specific reference values is indicative of a motor skill problem according to the test manual (33).

The M-ABC is categorized into different age bands; our study employed age band 1, which covers four to six year-old children (33).

Motor skills of children with cerebral palsy (CP) were categorized according to the Gross Motor Function Classification System (GMFCS) (34). The GMFCS is a five-level classification: Class 1, means that the child can move freely around in home, in school with more; Class 2, unable to run or jump; Class 3, dependent on devices for walking; and Classes 4 and 5, non-ambulatory CP (34).

Psychologists evaluated cognitive abilities using the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised (WPPSI-R) (35). WPPSI-R is tailored for children between two years and 11 months and seven years and three months. It comprises 12 subtests, which are divided into two categories: six assess verbal IQ, and six performance IQ. A Full Scale Intelligence Quotient (FIQ) is derived from these subscales (35). The reference mean value for the FIQ scores is 100, and the standard deviation (SD) is 15. A borderline FIQ is defined as 1–2 SDs below the mean, i.e., 70–84.

Based on these assessments, each child was assigned a Neurodevelopmental Disability (NDD) score according to the classifications defined in the Extremely Preterm Infants (surfactant C) cure (EPIcure) study, i.e., no, minor, moderate or severe disability (32, 36).

No NDD (No identified disability) was defined as normal vision and hearing, no cerebral palsy, an M-ABC score at or below the 95th percentile, and a FIQ of 85 or higher. NDD 1 (minor disability) included one or more of the following: squint/refractive error, mild hearing loss, cerebral palsy class 1, M-ABC score above the 95th percentile, or FIQ between 1 and 2 SDs below the reference mean value (i.e., 70–84). NDD 2 (moderate disability) included severe visual impairment, need of hearing aid, cerebral palsy class 2–3, or FIQ between 2 and 3 SDs below the reference mean value (i.e., 55–70). NDD 3 (severe disability) included legal blindness, complete deafness, cerebral palsy class 4–5, or FIQ more than 3 SDs below the reference mean value of 100 (i.e., <55).

Statistical analyses

To evaluate sample representativeness, we compared mean GA, mean BW, proportion of SGA children, and prevalence of periventricular haemorrhage grade three or four between participants and non-participants. We used t-tests for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables for comparisons between participants and non-participants, as well as for comparing the prevalence of various sleep problems among EPT children with and without CP, and among EPT children with and without SGA status at birth. The significance level was set at an α-level of 0.05.

We used logistic regression analyses to assess the risks, expressed as odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI), of the various sleep problems according to M-ABC score (dichotomized as normal and abnormal, defined as a score at or above the 95th percentile or lower), FIQ score (dichotomized as normal and score below 85), and NDD groups both unadjusted and after adjusting for SGA vs. non-SGA birth and maternal education (dichotomized as at least a three-year college education or less).

We adjusted for the following parameters: low socioeconomic status and intrauterine growth restriction (born SGA), as both are associated with preterm birth (37), sleep problems (38, 39), and a child's FIQ (40, 41). Notably, the relationship between SGA and a child's FIQ primarily applies to children born prematurely (41). Our previous study also identified SGA as a risk factor for sleep problems in children born prematurely (42). We also performed the same regression analyses with M-ABC and FIQ scores as continuous variables.

Ethics

The study was approved by the Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics (REC numbers: 2009/2271, 2017/1174) and the Norwegian Data Protection Authority. Informed written consent was obtained from the parents.

Results

Study population

There were no significant differences in mean GA, mean BW, proportion of SGA children, or prevalence of neonatal periventricular hemorrhage grade three or four between participants (n = 253) and non-participants (n = 119) (data not shown).

Sleep and NDD data were obtained for 253 of the 372 (68%) EPT children who were alive at five years of age (Figure 1). Parent and child characteristics are described in Table 1. FIQ scores were available 244 (96%) and M-ABC scores for 230 (91%) of the children. The mean FIQ score was 93.2 (range 48–130) for all, and 94.2 (range 53–130, n = 229) when children with CP were excluded. Mean M-ABC score was 9.8 (range 0–38) for all, and 9.6 (range 0–38, n = 224) when children with CP were excluded. Of the 253 children, 117 (46%) had no NDD (NDD 0), 100 (40%) had minor (NDD 1), 23 (9%) moderate (NDD 2), and 13 (5%) severe disabilities (NDD 3).

Figure 1
Flowchart illustrating the process and outcomes for children with gestational age under twenty-eight weeks or birth weight under one thousand grams. Out of 638 children, 153 were stillborn, 21 died in the delivery room, and 2 refused participation. Of the 464 admitted to the NICU, 86 died there. From the 376 discharged from the NICU, 4 died post-discharge. Finally, data on neurodevelopmental disabilities and sleep were collected for 253 out of 372 eligible participants at five years of age.

Figure 1. The recruitment of participants in a follow-up study of a national cohort of children born extremely preterma in Norway, 1999–2000. aGestational age <28 weeks or birth weight <1,000 g. BW, birthweight, GA, gestational age, NICU, neonatal intensive care unit.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the 253 children born extremely preterma in Norway, 1999–2000, with neurodevelopmental disability (NDD) score and parent-reported sleep data at five years of age.

Sleep problems

General sleep problems were reported for 28.5% of the children. Nighttime awakenings were the most common problem (21.7%) followed by difficulty falling asleep (17.8%) (Table 2). Only four children were reported to wake up unusually late. Therefore, this sleep variable was not included in further analyses. Additionally, 6.7% (n = 17) were reported to have other sleep problems, which were not included in further analyses (Table 2). The prevalence of the various sleep problems did not differ between sexes (data not shown).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Prevalence of parent-reported sleep problems at two to five years in children born extremely preterma in Norway, 1999–2000. (n = 253).

Sleep problems in relation to CP and SGA

Children with CP (n = 22) had a higher prevalence of general sleep problems (31.8% vs. 28.1%, p = 0.72), nighttime awakenings (31.8% vs. 20.8%, p = 0.23), and other sleep problems (9.1% vs. 6.5%, p = 0.64) compared to children without CP; however, these differences were not statistically significant. They also showed a non-significant lower prevalence of difficulty falling asleep (13.6% vs. 18.2%, p = 0.59) and waking up unusually early (4.5% vs. 6.1%, p = 0.77).

The prevalence of sleep problems was higher in SGA children (n = 47) compared to non-SGA children (n = 206), but only differences in general sleep problems (40.4% vs. 25.7%, p = 0.044) and nighttime awakenings (38.3% vs. 18.0%, p = 0.002) were statistically significant.

Sleep problems and FIQ score

Children with FIQ of less than 85 had a significantly higher risk of general sleep problems, nighttime awakenings, and early morning awakenings compared to children with an FIQ of 85 or above. The odds ratios for nighttime awakenings and early morning awakenings remained significant after adjusting for maternal education and SGA vs. non-SGA (Table 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Parent-reported sleep problems at two to five years in children born extremely preterma in Norway, 1999 and 2000, according to intelligence quotient (IQ) at five years. (n = 244).

There was also an increased risk of general sleep problems [OR 0.98, confidence interval (CI) 95% (0.96, 0.99)], nighttime awakenings [OR 0.98, CI 95% (0.96, 0.99)], and early morning awakenings [OR 0.96, CI 95% (0.93, 0.99)] associated with lower FIQ when included as a continuous variable in the regression model (data not shown).

Sleep problems and M-ABC score

The prevalence of any sleep problem did not differ between children with M-ABC score >the 95th and those with lower scores (Table 4). Similarly, M-ABC score was not associated with sleep problems when included as a continuous variable in the regression model (data not shown).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Parent-reported sleep problems at two to five years in children born extremely preterma in Norway, 1999−2000, according to M-ABCc test score at five years. (n = 230).

Sleep problems and NDD

The prevalence of all four types of sleep problems tended to increase with increasing severity of NDD, but the OR was only statistically significant when children in NDD 2 and 3 were compared to children without NDD (NDD 0) and only for general sleep problems, night awakenings and early morning awakenings. The OR was significant both in the unadjusted and adjusted regression analyses (Table 5).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Parent-reported sleep problems at two to five years in children born extremely preterma in Norway in 1999 and 2000, according to the degree of neurodevelopmental disabilitiesb (NDD) at five years. (n = 253).

Discussion

In this national cohort, EPT children with moderate and severe NDD had a four to five-fold increased risk of having general sleep problems, nighttime awakenings, and early morning awakenings as compared to children without NDD at age two to five years. Low or borderline cognitive function as a separate variable was associated with a two to three-fold increased risk of the same sleep outcomes, while low motor function alone was not associated with sleep problems.

The prevalence of parent-reported general sleep problems was 28.5%, which is higher than the prevalence reported for unselected preschool children in a Norwegian study (19.2%) (43) and in an American study (10.8%) (44).

In the Norwegian study, parent interviews were conducted to assess various sleep problems, leading to the creation of a composite category for “any sleep disorder”, which included insomnia, hypersomnia, nightmare disorder, and sleepwalking (43). The study included all children born in two specific birth years within a geographical area, with the only exclusion criterion being children whose parents lacked sufficient language skills to complete the questionnaire. The American study is similar to ours in how sleep problems were assessed; parents were asked whether their children had a sleep problem, not through a questionnaire as in our study, but as part of an interview (44). This study was based on a national poll of parents of preschool children, selected through a targeted random sample.

Our findings align with previous research (6) and earlier studies in the same cohort (28). However, among the children in our cohort with no NDD, the prevalence of 20.5% was within range of the 19.2% reported for the unselected Norwegian population suggesting that extreme prematurity per se may not be a significant risk factor for sleep problems at two to five years of age. It is also important to note the potential uncertainty arising from the different methods and definitions used to classify sleep problems.

There are few studies on preschoolers born preterm that allow for direct comparison with our current findings. Our study identified a link between reduced cognitive abilities and early morning awakenings in EPT children. We have identified two studies that report similar findings (26, 27). Ando et al. (26) and Schwichtenberg et al. (27) found that increased variation in daily wake time and increased daily variation in sleep/activity cycles are associated with lower cognitive function in 1.5–2-year-old prematurely born children. We speculate that early morning awakenings and variations in wake-up time and sleep/activity cycles may result from the impact of premature birth on brain development, reflecting an underlying pattern that affects both sleep and cognitive development.

We also identified an association between early morning awakenings and moderate to severe NDD (NDD 2 and 3), which is consistent with our previous study of the same cohort at 11 years of age (28). Other studies of preterm adolescents and adults have also reported an increased prevalence of early morning awakenings (11, 12), but they have not examined the association between sleep and neurodevelopmental factors. The tendency of early morning awakenings among children born preterm may result from various mechanisms, including irregular sleep cycles, early chronotype (45, 46), and environmental influences (47). However, our finding of an association between decreasing FIQ and early morning awakenings likely indicates that the problem may be related to underlying neurological disturbances caused by preterm birth.

We found that borderline or low cognitive function was associated with increased nighttime awakenings, which differs from the findings of Ando et al., who reported no such association (26). However, their study differed markedly in that the children were born at higher GA.

Our finding that general sleep problems were associated with borderline or low cognitive function is comparable with two other studies: A study of 6–9-year-old children born at less than 32 weeks' GA where poor nighttime sleep quality was associated with poor verbal working memory (25) and a study of 6–10-year-old children where children born before 32 weeks' GA more often experienced nighttime awakenings than children born at term (20). Opposite to our findings, they found no association between nighttime awakenings and IQ (20), but the studies may not be comparable because their IQ levels were higher than in our children.

Nighttime awakenings in our two to five-year-old children born EPT may indicate developmental disruptions in the brain and altered development of the sleep-wake cycle. It is also possible that nighttime awakenings cause disruptions in sleep and thereby affect brain development, including cognitive abilities and daily cognitive functioning (15).

We suggest that the association between borderline and low IQ and sleep problems in our cohort of EPT children is clinically significant, as it can have profound implications for both immediate and long-term outcomes. While this association does not necessarily imply causality or etiology, it supports the hypothesis of shared root cause (13). It is also clinically significant in that IQ may be an indicator of neurological development in relation to sleep and, furthermore, that IQ remains relatively stable from early childhood to adulthood (48, 49), especially for those with low IQ (49). This may suggest that the sleep pattern of EPT children with borderline or low IQ may be stable over time and potentially add to their cognitive difficulties. However, this raises questions for further research, as sleep problems can potentially exacerbate the link between lower cognitive function and lower achievement (50).

Future research is necessary to further explore the mechanisms linking low cognitive scores and sleep problems in children born preterm. Longitudinal studies that track cognitive development and sleep patterns over time, along with interventions aimed at improving sleep quality and assessing their impact on cognitive outcomes, are essential. Furthermore, incorporating neurophysiological assessments may help reveal underlying brain changes that contribute to these relationships.

In general, addressing sleep issues in children is important, not only for ensuring optimal development (15), but also for their overall health and quality of life (51). Early identification and treatment of sleep problems in preschool children is crucial, as this period is significant for cognitive and social-emotional development (52). For EPT children, the association between sleep problems and neurodevelopmental outcomes identified in our study at this age is important for developing tailored follow-up interventions, which may ultimately support better developmental outcomes and enhance their overall quality of life.

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of this study include a national population-based sample of EPT children, a prospective design, a high participation rate, and the use of validated instruments for assessing cognitive and motor function. The risk of selection bias was low because of extensive knowledge of non-participants.

Weaknesses include the lack of validated questionnaires assessing children's sleep problems, as well as the absence of a healthy reference group born at term. Furthermore, previous studies have shown that children born preterm are at risk of sleep-related breathing disorders (5355), which are known causes of nighttime awakenings (56). Sleep-related breathing problems have also been shown to impact cognitive abilities (57). Unfortunately, we did not assess such sleep-related breathing problems in this study.

Weaknesses also include the fact that the included extremely premature (EPT) children were born in 1999 and 2000, which may raise concerns about the relevance of the findings for current populations. The survival rate and neonatal major morbidity in our cohort are comparable to today's survivors within the defined gestational age span of 24–27 weeks, both in Norway and in other high-income countries (58). Stensvold et al. report that no significant improvement in survival was found among viable infants in the 2013–2014 cohort compared to our cohort from 1999 to 2000. They found only small changes in the occurrence of major morbidity and no changes in mortality between the two cohorts (58). Data from the Norwegian neonatal quality register indicate that major morbidity during the neonatal period for survivors with gestational age (GA) <28 weeks has remained stable from 2012 to 2023, alongside some changes in survival rates for those with low GA in recent years (59).

However, it is important to be aware that neurodevelopmental outcomes are linked not only to major morbidity or major brain lesions but also to minor brain injuries, such as diffuse white matter abnormalities and other subtle brain injuries (6062). We have limited knowledge about whether the improvements in treatment in recent years have affected the prevalence of these minor injuries. When the findings from our study are to be compared with those from newer studies, this must be taken into consideration. Additionally, the development of cognitive assessment tools has provided more sensitive instruments to capture neurological changes in preterm populations (63, 64).

Our study is a cross-sectional study, and the absence of longitudinal data is also a limitation. It is also important to acknowledge that factors such as genetics and environment are crucial for neurodevelopment (65). One significant environmental factor that has received scrutiny is the increased screen time among children in recent decades, which may negatively impact early neurodevelopment and has been shown to contribute to adverse cognitive outcomes (66), including for vulnerable EPT children (67). Since our cohort was raised in the early 2000s, any changes in evolving environmental factors must be taken into account, as this may limit the comparability of our findings with those of newer studies.

Socioeconomic factors may affect risks of preterm birth (37), lower cognitive abilities (40), and sleep problems (38). Although socioeconomic status encompasses multiple dimensions, we adjusted for maternal educational level in the adjusted analyses. There was only a modest increase in the proportion of individuals with higher education in Norway from year 2000 to 2020 (68) and no severe changes in other relevant factors such as employment rates (69). This social stability underscores our belief that the findings from our cohort are relevant for extremely premature infants born today.

Conclusions

This study shows that various sleep problems are common among EPT children aged two to five years, and that sleep problems increase with borderline and low cognitive function and neurodevelopmental disabilities. Whether these associations have causal relationships or may interact negatively is not settled, but such a possibility cannot be dismissed. We therefore suggest that sleep behavior needs to be addressed in the follow-up of children born preterm.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because according to the approvals granted for this study by The Regional Committee on Medical Research Ethics and The Norwegian Data Inspectorate, the data files are to be stored properly and in line with the Norwegian Law of Privacy Protection. The data file is not made publically available as this might compromise the respondents' privacy, particularly as some of our participating centers are small and the number of extremely preterm births very limited. Moreover, the data file is currently used by other researchers in our group to prepare future research papers. A subset of the data file with anonymized data may be made available to interested researchers upon reasonable request to Thomas Halvorsen (thomas.halvorsen@helse-bergen.no) and providing permission from The Norwegian Data Inspectorate and the other members of our research group. Requests to access the datasets should be directed todGhvbWFzLmhhbHZvcnNlbkBoZWxzZS1iZXJnZW4ubm8=.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Administration for Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics Western Norway (REK Vest). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

KS: Writing – original draft, Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. MH: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. MV: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing, Project administration. IE: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. TM: Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. BB: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This research project has received support from the Research Fund for Sudden Infant Death Syndrome at the Faculty of Medicine, University of Bergen.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Volpe JJ. Brain injury in premature infants: a complex amalgam of destructive and developmental disturbances. Lancet Neurol. (2009) 8(1):110–24. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70294-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Volpe JJ. The encephalopathy of prematurity-brain injury and impaired brain development inextricably intertwined. Semin Pediatr Neurol. (2009) 16(4):167–78. doi: 10.1016/j.spen.2009.09.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Moore T, Hennessy EM, Myles J, Johnson SJ, Draper ES, Costeloe KL, et al. Neurological and developmental outcome in extremely preterm children born in England in 1995 and 2006: the EPICure studies. Br Med J. (2012) 345:e7961. doi: 10.1136/bmj.e7961

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Rees P, Callan C, Chadda KR, Vaal M, Diviney J, Sabti S, et al. Preterm brain injury and neurodevelopmental outcomes: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. (2022) 150(6):e2022054102. doi: 10.1542/peds.2022-057442

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Johnson S, Marlow N. Early and long-term outcome of infants born extremely preterm. Arch Dis Child. (2017) 102(1):97–102. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2015-309581

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Trickett J, Hill C, Austin T, Johnson S. The impact of preterm birth on sleep through infancy, childhood, and adolescence and its implications. Children. (2022) 9(5):618. doi: 10.3390/children9050626

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Visser SSM, van Diemen WJM, Kervezee L, van den Hoogen A, Verschuren O, Pillen S, et al. The relationship between preterm birth and sleep in children at school age: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. (2021) 57:101447. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2021.101447

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lyu J, Groeger JA, Barnett AL, Li H, Wang L, Zhang J, et al. Associations between gestational age and childhood sleep: a national retrospective cohort study. BMC Med. (2022) 20(1):253. doi: 10.1186/s12916-022-02443-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Romeo DM, Leo G, Lapenta L, Leone D, Turrini I, Brogna C, et al. Sleep disorders in low-risk preschool very preterm children. Sleep Med. (2019) 63:137–41. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2019.04.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Durankus F, Aladag Ciftdemir N, Vatansever Ozbek U, Duran R, Acunas B. Comparison of sleep problems between term and preterm born preschool children. Sleep Med. (2020) 75:484–90. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2020.09.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Bjorkqvist J, Paavonen J, Andersson S, Pesonen AK, Lahti J, Heinonen K, et al. Advanced sleep-wake rhythm in adults born prematurely: confirmation by actigraphy-based assessment in the Helsinki study of very low birth weight adults. Sleep Med. (2014) 15(9):1101–6. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.04.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hibbs AM, Storfer-Isser A, Rosen C, Ievers-Landis CE, Taveras EM, Redline S. Advanced sleep phase in adolescents born preterm. Behav Sleep Med. (2014) 12(5):412–24. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2013.825838

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Bennet L, Walker DW, Horne RSC. Waking up too early—the consequences of preterm birth on sleep development. J Physiol. (2018) 596(23):5687–708. doi: 10.1113/JP274950

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Mirmiran M, Maas YG, Ariagno RL. Development of fetal and neonatal sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep Med Rev. (2003) 7(4):321–34. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2002.0243

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Liu JH, Ji XP, Pitt S, Wang GH, Rovit E, Lipman T, et al. Childhood sleep: physical, cognitive, and behavioral consequences and implications. World J Pediatr. (2022) 18(2):109–18. doi: 10.1007/s12519-022-00647-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Angriman M, Caravale B, Novelli L, Ferri R, Bruni O. Sleep in children with neurodevelopmental disabilities. Neuropediatrics. (2015) 46(3):199–210. doi: 10.1055/s-0035-1550151

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Esposito M, Carotenuto M. Borderline intellectual functioning and sleep: the role of cyclic alternating pattern. Neurosci Lett. (2010) 485(2):89–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.08.062

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Esposito M, Carotenuto M. Intellectual disabilities and power spectra analysis during sleep: a new perspective on borderline intellectual functioning. J Intellect Disabil Res. (2014) 58(5):421–9. doi: 10.1111/jir.12036

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Harper L, McAnelly S, Walshe I, Ooms A, Tuffrey-Wijne IM. Behavioural sleep problems in children and adults with intellectual disabilities: an integrative literature review. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil. (2023) 36(5):916–28. doi: 10.1111/jar.13116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Hagmann-von Arx P, Perkinson-Gloor N, Brand S, Albert D, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Grob A, et al. In school-age children who were born very preterm, sleep efficiency is associated with cognitive function. Neuropsychobiology. (2014) 70(4):244–52. doi: 10.1159/000369026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Gogou M, Haidopoulou K, Pavlou E. Sleep and prematurity: sleep outcomes in preterm children and influencing factors. World J Pediatr. (2019) 15(3):209–18. doi: 10.1007/s12519-019-00240-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Tokariev A, Roberts JA, Zalesky A, Zhao X, Vanhatalo S, Breakspear M, et al. Large-scale brain modes reorganize between infant sleep states and carry prognostic information for preterms. Nat Commun. (2019) 10(1):2619. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-10467-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Scher MS, Steppe DA, Banks DL. Prediction of lower developmental performances of healthy neonates by neonatal EEG-sleep measures. Pediatr Neurol. (1996) 14(2):137–44. doi: 10.1016/0887-8994(96)00013-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Uchitel J, Vanhatalo S, Austin T. Early development of sleep and brain functional connectivity in term-born and preterm infants. Pediatr Res. (2022) 91(4):771–86. doi: 10.1038/s41390-021-01497-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. McCann M, Bayliss DM, Anderson M, Campbell C, French N, McMichael J, et al. The relationship between sleep problems and working memory in children born very preterm. Child Neuropsychol. (2018) 24(1):124–44. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2016.1235144

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ando A, Ohta H, Yoshimura Y, Nakagawa M, Asaka Y, Nakazawa T, et al. Sleep maturation influences cognitive development of preterm toddlers. Sci Rep. (2021) 11(1):15921. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-95495-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Schwichtenberg AJ, Christ S, Abel E, Poehlmann-Tynan JA. Circadian sleep patterns in toddlers born preterm: longitudinal associations with developmental and health concerns. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2016) 37(5):358–69. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000287

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Stangenes KM, Fevang SK, Grundt J, Donkor HM, Markestad T, Hysing M, et al. Children born extremely preterm had different sleeping habits at 11 years of age and more childhood sleep problems than term-born children. Acta Paediatr. (2017) 106(12):1966–72. doi: 10.1111/apa.13991

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Markestad T, Kaaresen PI, Ronnestad A, Reigstad H, Lossius K, Medbo S, et al. Early death, morbidity, and need of treatment among extremely premature infants. Pediatrics. (2005) 115(5):1289–98. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1482

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Westby Wold SH, Sommerfelt K, Reigstad H, Ronnestad A, Medbo S, Farstad T, et al. Neonatal mortality and morbidity in extremely preterm small for gestational age infants: a population-based study. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. (2009) 94(5):F363–7. doi: 10.1136/adc.2009.157800

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Skjaerven R, Gjessing HK, Bakketeig LS. Birthweight by gestational age in Norway. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand. (2000) 79(6):440–9. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0412.2000.079006440.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Leversen KT, Sommerfelt K, Ronnestad A, Kaaresen PI, Farstad T, Skranes J, et al. Prediction of neurodevelopmental and sensory outcome at 5 years in Norwegian children born extremely preterm. Pediatrics. (2011) 127(3):e630–8. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Henderson S, Sugden D. The Movement Assessment Battery for Children. Swedish ed. Stockholm: Psykologforlaget AB (1996).

Google Scholar

34. Palisano R, Rosenbaum P, Walter S, Russell D, Wood E, Galuppi B. Development and reliability of a system to classify gross motor function in children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol. (1997) 39(4):214–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1997.tb07414.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Weschsler D. Weschsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, Revised. Swedish ed. Stockholm: Psykologforlaget AB (1999).

Google Scholar

36. Marlow N, Wolke D, Bracewell MA, Samara M, Group EPS. Neurologic and developmental disability at six years of age after extremely preterm birth. N Engl J Med. (2005) 352(1):9–19. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa041367

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Koullali B, Oudijk MA, Nijman TA, Mol BW, Pajkrt E. Risk assessment and management to prevent preterm birth. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. (2016) 21(2):80–8. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2016.01.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Bagley EJ, Kelly RJ, Buckhalt JA, El-Sheikh M. What keeps low-SES children from sleeping well: the role of presleep worries and sleep environment. Sleep Med. (2015) 16(4):496–502. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.10.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Yiallourou SR, Wallace EM, Miller SL, Horne RS. Effects of intrauterine growth restriction on sleep and the cardiovascular system: the use of melatonin as a potential therapy? Sleep Med Rev. (2016) 26:64–73. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2015.04.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. von Stumm S, Plomin R. Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence. Intelligence. (2015) 48:30–6. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2014.10.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Vollmer B, Edmonds CJ. School age neurological and cognitive outcomes of fetal growth retardation or small for gestational age birth weight. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne). (2019) 10:186. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2019.00186

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Stangenes KM, Hysing M, Fevang SK, Elgen IB, Halvorsen T, Markestad T, et al. Prenatal and neonatal factors predicting sleep problems in children born extremely preterm or with extremely low birthweight. Front Pediatr. (2018) 6:178. doi: 10.3389/fped.2018.00178

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Steinsbekk S, Wichstrom L. Stability of sleep disorders from preschool to first grade and their bidirectional relationship with psychiatric symptoms. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2015) 36(4):243–51. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000134

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Mindell JA, Meltzer LJ, Carskadon MA, Chervin RD. Developmental aspects of sleep hygiene: findings from the 2004 national sleep foundation. Sleep Med. (2009) 10(7):771–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.07.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Simpkin CT, Jenni OG, Carskadon MA, Wright KP Jr, Akacem LD, Garlo KG, et al. Chronotype is associated with the timing of the circadian clock and sleep in toddlers. J Sleep Res. (2014) 23(4):397–405. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12142

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Randler C, Fassl C, Kalb N. From lark to owl: developmental changes in morningness-eveningness from newborns to early adulthood. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:45874. doi: 10.1038/srep45874

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Lercher P, Dzhambov AM, Persson Waye K. Environmental perceptions, self-regulation, and coping with noise mediate the associations between children’s physical environment and sleep and mental health problems. Environ Res. (2025) 264(Pt 2):120414. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2024.120414

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Yu HH, McCoach DB, Gottfried AW, Gottfried AE. Stability of intelligence from infancy through adolescence: an autoregressive latent variable model. Intelligence. (2018) 69:8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2018.03.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Schneider W, Niklas F, Schmiedeler S. Intellectual development from early childhood to early adulthood: the impact of early IQ differences on stability and change over time. Learn Individ Differ. (2014) 32:156–62. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.02.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Erath SA, Tu KM, Buckhalt JA, El-Sheikh M. Associations between children’s intelligence and academic achievement: the role of sleep. J Sleep Res. (2015) 24(5):510–3. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12281

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Blackwell CK, Hartstein LE, Elliott AJ, Forrest CB, Ganiban J, Hunt KJ, et al. Better sleep, better life? How sleep quality influences children’s life satisfaction. Qual Life Res. (2020) 29(9):2465–74. doi: 10.1007/s11136-020-02491-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Fernandez E. Early childhood: dimensions and contexts of development and well-being. In: Ben-Arieh A, Casas F, Frønes I, Korbin J, editors. Handbook of Child Well-Being. Dordrecht: Springer (2014). p. 1629–47. doi: 10.1007/978-90-481-9063-8_65

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Huang YS, Paiva T, Hsu JF, Kuo MC, Guilleminault C. Sleep and breathing in premature infants at 6 months post-natal age. BMC Pediatr. (2014) 14:303. doi: 10.1186/s12887-014-0303-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Rosen CL, Larkin EK, Kirchner HL, Emancipator JL, Bivins SF, Surovec SA, et al. Prevalence and risk factors for sleep-disordered breathing in 8- to 11-year-old children: association with race and prematurity. J Pediatr. (2003) 142(4):383–9. doi: 10.1067/mpd.2003.28

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Marcus CL, Meltzer LJ, Roberts RS, Traylor J, Dix J, D'Ilario J, et al. Long-term effects of caffeine therapy for apnea of prematurity on sleep at school age. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. (2014) 190(7):791–9. doi: 10.1164/rccm.201406-1092OC

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Sinha D, Guilleminault C. Sleep disordered breathing in children. Indian J Med Res. (2010) 131:311–20.20308756

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

57. Trosman I, Trosman SJ. Cognitive and behavioral consequences of sleep disordered breathing in children. Med Sci. (2017) 5(4):30. doi: 10.3390/medsci5040030

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Stensvold HJ, Klingenberg C, Stoen R, Moster D, Braekke K, Guthe HJ, et al. Neonatal morbidity and 1-year survival of extremely preterm infants. Pediatrics. (2017) 139(3):e20161821. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1821

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Tusor N, Benders MJ, Counsell SJ, Nongena P, Ederies MA, Falconer S, et al. Punctate white matter lesions associated with altered brain development and adverse motor outcome in preterm infants. Sci Rep. (2017) 7(1):13250. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13753-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Ramenghi LA, Fumagalli M, Righini A, Bassi L, Groppo M, Parazzini C, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging assessment of brain maturation in preterm neonates with punctate white matter lesions. Neuroradiology. (2007) 49(2):161–7. doi: 10.1007/s00234-006-0176-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Allin MP, Kontis D, Walshe M, Wyatt J, Barker GJ, Kanaan RA, et al. White matter and cognition in adults who were born preterm. PLoS One. (2011) 6(10):e24525. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024525

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Kezer F, Arik RS. An examination and comparison of the revisions of the wechsler intelligence scale for children. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences. (2012) 46:2104–10. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.05.436

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kamphaus RW, Winsor AP, Rowe EW, Sangwon K. A history of intelligence test interpretation. In: Flanagan DP, Harrison PL, editors. Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues. 3rd ed. New York: Guilford (2012). p. 56–70.

Google Scholar

65. Tucker-Drob EM, Briley DA, Harden KP. Genetic and environmental influences on cognition across development and context. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2013) 22(5):349–55. doi: 10.1177/0963721413485087

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough S. Association between screen time and children’s performance on a developmental screening test. JAMA Pediatr. (2019) 173(3):244–50. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5056

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Vohr BR, McGowan EC, Bann C, Das A, Higgins R, Hintz S, et al. Association of high screen-time use with school-age cognitive, executive function, and behavior outcomes in extremely preterm children. JAMA Pediatr. (2021) 175(10):1025–34. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.2041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Statistics on Educational attainment of the population: Statistics Norway; [updated 11 June 2024]. Available online at: https://www.ssb.no/en/utdanning/utdanningsniva/statistikk/befolkningens-utdanningsniva (Accessed June 06, 2025).

Google Scholar

69. Statistics on Employment, register-based Statistics Norway [updated 26 February 2025]. Available online at: https://www.ssb.no/en/arbeid-og-lonn/sysselsetting/statistikk/sysselsetting-registerbasert. (Accessed June 06, 2025).

Google Scholar

Keywords: sleep, sleep problems, extremely preterm birth, neurodevelopment, cognition, minor motor problems

Citation: Stangenes KM, Hysing M, Vollsæter M, Elgen IB, Markestad T and Bjorvatn B (2025) Sleep problems from age two to five years and neurological outcome in children born extremely preterm: a cross-sectional study. Front. Pediatr. 13:1562630. doi: 10.3389/fped.2025.1562630

Received: 17 January 2025; Accepted: 18 June 2025;
Published: 9 July 2025.

Edited by:

Sara Uccella, University of Genoa, Italy

Reviewed by:

Jayne Spiller, University of Leicester, United Kingdom
Deborah Preiti, Giannina Gaslini Institute (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright: © 2025 Stangenes, Hysing, Vollsæter, Elgen, Markestad and Bjorvatn. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kristine Marie Stangenes, a3Jpc3RpbmUuc3RhbmdlbmVzQGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.