- 1Department of Mental Health Education, Chongqing Medical and Pharmaceutical College, Chongqing, China
- 2Shanhu Experimental Primary School, Chongqing, China
- 3Department of Neurology, Second Affiliated Hospital of Army Medical University (Xinqiao Hospital), Chongqing, China
- 4Department of Medical Psychology, Army Medical University, Chongqing, China
Objective: This study examined the interactive effects of parental burnout and parenting styles (authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive) on childhood social anxiety using response surface analysis (RSA).
Methods: This cross-sectional study was conducted between November and December 2024 in primary schools in Chongqing, China. Participants (parents and their children) were required to complete online questionnaires, including the Parental Burnout Assessment (PBA), the Parenting Styles and Dimensions Questionnaire (PSDQ), and the Social Anxiety Scale for Children (SASC). RSA and polynomial regression models examined the non-linear interactions between parental burnout, parenting styles, and childhood social anxiety.
Results: A total of 724 datasets were included in the study. The findings indicated that significant congruence effects emerged for permissive parenting and parental burnout, and an inverted U-shaped curvature appeared along the line of incongruence with childhood social anxiety (curvature, a4 = −0.70, p = 0.009; slope a3 = −0.57, p = 0.272). Moreover, the curvature (a2 = −0.28, p = 0.089) and slope (a1 = 0.81, p = 0.068) were measured along the line of congruence, indicating that the line of congruence section curve is linearly rising.
Conclusion: This study identifies a significant interactive effect between parental burnout and permissive parenting style on childhood social anxiety, highlighting the need for two-pronged interventions. Future research should investigate the longitudinal causal pathways between parental burnout–parenting style congruence and child social anxiety across diverse cultural contexts.
1 Introduction
From childhood through adolescence, individuals exhibit heightened sensitivity to social feedback, including peer evaluations and interpersonal performance, reflecting the natural progression of cognitive and social competencies. Although transient social apprehension during this developmental stage is normative, persistent escalation of anxiety beyond adaptive coping capacities may precipitate social anxiety in susceptible children (1). This prevalent pediatric psychological disorder is characterized by intense fear during social interactions or performance contexts, accompanied by excessive concerns regarding negative evaluation. It manifests through maladaptive avoidance behaviors (e.g., public speaking difficulties, peer engagement avoidance, or inhibited friendship initiation) (2, 3) and physiological symptoms such as crying, blushing, panic reactions, or emotional outbursts (4). Epidemiological data from Chavira et al. (5) indicated that approximately 19% of children exceed clinical thresholds for social anxiety. Critically, this condition extends beyond transient distress to inflict long-term mental health impairments, heightening risks for social withdrawal, academic dysfunction, comorbid depression, diminished self-esteem, and suicidal ideation in their future development (6, 7). Moreover, affected children reported more frequent and severe somatic complaints such as gastrointestinal disturbances, cardiovascular symptoms, and fatigue compared with peers (8, 9). To develop effective interventions for children with social anxiety and their parents, clinicians and researchers need to identify determinants of pediatric social anxiety, particularly modifiable environmental factors.
Bronfenbrenner's ecological systems theory (10) posits that early psychological development is shaped by multi-layered environmental systems, with the family environment constituting a pivotal influence. This framework emphasizes familial contexts as both primary arenas for early social experiences and critical buffers against external stressors. Consequently, familial factors represent key intervention targets for pediatric social anxiety. Research suggested parenting styles modulate children's social cognitive schema and social anxiety susceptibility through three core mechanisms: emotional responsiveness, rule-setting efficacy, and behavioral monitoring (11, 12). Baumrind's classification (13) delineates three parenting archetypes: authoritative (emotional support with autonomy-promoting guidance), authoritarian (behavioral compliance with limited emotional engagement), and permissive (high acceptance without requisite behavioral guidance). Recent studies demonstrated differential associations between these styles and pediatric social anxiety. Pinquart (14) revealed heightened permissive tendencies among parents of anxious children, suggesting that ambiguous rule structures may exacerbate symptoms. Lei et al. (15) elucidated stronger associations between negative parenting practices and social anxiety in Asian vs. Western contexts. Conversely, authoritarian, authoritative, and overprotective parenting styles are associated with reduced social anxiety (16, 17).
In addition, parental burnout, defined as “emotional exhaustion, reduced parenting motivation, and emotional distance resulting from chronic parenting stress” (18), has garnered empirical attention. In the Chinese cultural context, 35% of parents reported chronic burnout stemming from occupation, parenting, and child education demands (19, 20). Previous research claimed that children of burned-out parents exhibit heightened susceptibility to social anxiety (21, 22). Exhausted parents often demonstrate diminished capacity to provide appropriate attention and feedback, while experiencing increased familial conflicts that impair children's interpersonal skill development (23, 24). Chronic exposure to such low-warmth home environments may foster persistent insecurity, disrupting social skill acquisition through heightened attention bias toward threatening social cues. This aligns with findings that anxious children exhibited cognitive biases when processing social information, disproportionately attending to negative cues (e.g., critical facial expressions) and overinterpreting threats in ambiguous social situations (25).
Although family-centered interventions for socially anxious children are widely implemented (26–28), their efficacy remains inconsistent. Longitudinal data from Ginsburg et al. (29) indicated that 41% of cases experienced symptom recurrence within 3 years post-intervention, highlighting limitations in current conceptualizations of the parental burnout–parenting style–child social anxiety pathway.
Bronfenbrenner's ecological systems theory (10) suggests synergistic effects between parental factors, implying that burnout and parenting styles may jointly elevate social anxiety risk. However, existing research (14, 15, 19, 20) predominantly examined isolated variables, either parental burnout or permissive parenting, and overlooked their interaction effect on childhood social anxiety. Crucially, no studies have investigated whether congruence between high parental burnout and permissive parenting predicts anxiety severity. Accordingly, we propose that when parental burnout and permissive parenting style co-occur, it critically undermines parental intervention capacity due to “no rules and no support” environments. Consequently, we hypothesize that high parental burnout, coupled with a permissive parenting style, significantly predicts severe pediatric social anxiety.
Our study addressed the gap by utilizing response surface analysis (RSA) to assess how interactions between parental burnout and parenting styles predicted childhood social anxiety, leveraging RSA's capacity to quantify non-linear effects beyond traditional single-factor approaches. This advanced methodology capitalizes on RSA's capacity to quantify non-linear effects beyond traditional single-factor analyses through polynomial regression modeling with quadratic (X2, Y2) and interaction (XY) terms, thereby enabling identification of complex variable relationships (30). Specifically, we utilized RSA to assess how both congruence (similar levels of parental burnout and parenting styles) and incongruence (divergent levels between these factors) influenced childhood social anxiety. By transcending univariate limitations of conventional regression paradigms, this method elucidated synergistic interactions between parental psychological resources (parental burnout) and caregiver behaviors (parenting styles).
2 Materials and methods
2.1 Participants and procedure
This cross-sectional study was conducted between November and December 2024 across three public elementary schools in Chongqing, China. Participants were recruited through classroom teachers, and one parent, either the mother or father, was invited to complete an online questionnaire assessing parental burnout and parenting styles. Meanwhile, their 8–12-year-old children independently completed an online survey measuring social anxiety. To minimize comprehension bias and attentional limitations inherent in this age group, this study adopted the Social Anxiety Scale for Children (SASC) with only 10 items. A valid dataset was defined as a complete response and matched data from one parent and their child. To facilitate matching, the child and parent were required to enter the “mother’s name and last 4 digits of phone number” to match the dataset. The study protocol was approved by the institutional ethics committee, and online informed consent was obtained from all participants.
2.2 Measures
2.2.1 SASC
The SASC, developed by La Greca and Lopez (31), is a psychometric instrument designed to assess child anxiety experiences in social contexts. This 10-item scale comprises two sub-scales: “fear of negative evaluation” (e.g., I worry that others will laugh at me) and “social avoidance and distress” (e.g., I feel nervous when talking to unfamiliar peers). Each item is rated on a 3-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (never) to 2 (always), with higher values indicating more social anxiety symptoms. In this study, the SASC demonstrated reliability and validity in the Chinese pediatric populations (32), with an overall Cronbach's α coefficient of 0.88.
2.2.2 Parental Burnout Assessment
The Parental Burnout Assessment (PBA) is a validated psychometric instrument designed to evaluate burnout symptoms arising from chronic parenting stress (23). This 21-item scale comprises four sub-scales: “exhaustion in one's parental role” (e.g., I'm exhausted by the efforts I have to make to be a good parent), “contrast with previous parental self” (e.g., I no longer know how to be a good parent), “feelings of being fed up with one's parental role” (e.g., I can no longer tolerate my role as a parent), and “emotional distancing from one's children” (e.g., I only do the bare minimum required for my child). Each item is rated on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (daily), with higher total scores indicating greater parental burnout severity. In this study, the PBA demonstrated reliability and validity in Chinese parental populations (33), with an overall Cronbach's α coefficient of 0.94.
2.2.3 Parenting Styles and Dimensions Questionnaire
The Parenting Styles and Dimensions Questionnaire (PSDQ), designed by Robinson et al. (34), is designed to assess different types of parenting behaviors. The 32-item questionnaire comprises three sub-scales: “authoritative parenting” (e.g., I patiently listen to my child's perspectives and collaboratively establish rules with them), “authoritarian parenting” (e.g., I expect my child to comply with my demands unconditionally), and “permissive parenting” (e.g., I rarely intervene in my child's behavior, even when mistakes may occur). Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Sub-scales are computed independently, with higher scores indicating stronger stylistic tendencies. In this study, the PSDQ demonstrated an overall Cronbach's α coefficient of 0.83, and sub-scales’ Cronbach's α coefficients of 0.95, 0.89, and 0.69, respectively.
2.3 Data analysis
Data analysis was conducted using SPSS 29.0, with RSA implemented via the SPSS RSA plugin (35) following Edwards and Cable's methodological framework (36). Firstly, predictor variables for parental burnout (X) and parenting styles (authoritative, authoritarian, or permissive) scores (Y) were centralized. Then, a polynomial regression model was constructed using the centralized predictor variables (X, Y), their squared terms (X2, Y2), and product terms (XY) as independent variables, with childhood social anxiety (Z) as the dependent variable. This tested the curve relationship and interaction association. Subsequently, the appropriateness of RSA was confirmed by a significant R2 increment and at least one statistically significant quadratic term.
The results were interpreted through response surface features such as response surface stationary points, curvature of cross sections of line of congruence (LOC; X = Y, indicating alignment between parental burnout and parenting style) and line of incongruence (LOIC; X = −Y, indicating the divergent levels between parental burnout and parenting style), and the relationship between the first principal axis (FPA) and a1–a5. A three-dimensional response surface was generated from the polynomial regression output, with parameters for RSA a1–a5 were calculated based on the regression coefficients b0–b5 as follows: a1 = b1 + b2 indicates linear slope along LOC, a2 = b3 + b4 + b5 indicates curvature along LOC, a3 = b1–b2 indicates linear slope along LOIC, a4 = b3–b4 + b5 indicates a curvature along LOIC, a5 = b3–b5 indicates an alignment between FPA and LOC.
Humberg et al. (37) specified four sequential conditions to validate congruence effects. FPA must align with the LOC, the FPA intercept (P10) must not differ significantly from 0 (Condition 1), and the FPA slope (P11) must not differ significantly from 1 (Condition 2). The LOIC (X = −Y) curvature (a4) requires significant negative (Condition 3), and the LOIC slope at the origin (a3) must not differ significantly from 0 (Condition 4). Polynomial regression coefficients quantified variable relationships of b1 (parental burnout X), b2 (parenting type Y), b3 (X2), b5 (Y2), and b4 (X and Y).
Common method variation arising from scale type or answer formats may induce common method bias (CMB), potentially causing bias between data results and actual results (38). To assess CMB in our questionnaire-based study, all items from the three scales underwent exploratory factor analysis (EFA) using Harman's single-factor test with non-rotated principal component analysis. The results of this analysis are reported in the “Results” section.
3 Results
3.1 Demographic characteristics
A total of 1,247 parent questionnaires and 1,049 child questionnaires were collected via the online platform. After excluding 225 unmatched responses and 298 incomplete responses, 724 valid parent–child paired datasets remained for analysis. The demographic characteristics of the participants are presented in Table 1. Among the parent participants, 24.17% had an educational attainment at or below the junior high school level; 21.69% had completed high school or technical secondary school education; 23.2% had junior college education; and 25.69% had an undergraduate education. Regarding marital status, 631 households (87.16%) had married parents, while 57 households (7.87%) had divorced parents. Among the child participants, 49.31% were boys and 50.69% were girls. In addition, 42.68% of the children were the only child in their family. More details are listed in Table 1.
3.2 Common method variance
Generally, if EFA yields two or more factors with eigenvalues >1 and the variance explained by the first factor is <50%, this indicates that there is no severe CMB (39, 40). Our results identified 12 factors with eigenvalues >1, and the variance explained by the first 22.42% is less than the critical threshold of 40%. These findings indicated no significant CMB in this study.
3.3 Descriptive statistics and correlation analysis among variables
Table 2 presents the results of the mean, standard deviation, and correlation between variables. Childhood social anxiety demonstrated significant positive correlations with parental burnout (r = 0.200), authoritarian parenting style (r = 0.202), and permissive parenting style (r = 0.206) and indicated a significant negative correlation with authoritative parenting style (r = −0.165).

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations between parental burnout, parenting styles, and childhood social anxiety.
3.4 The association between parental burnout and authoritative parenting style on child social anxiety
Followed the approach of Fleenor et al. (41), level variance analysis (±0.5 SD threshold) revealed the following case distribution: X > Y (29.83%), X = Y (26.66%), and X < Y (43.51%). This relatively uniform distribution supported RSA implementation. The regression results (Table 3, Figure 1) presented that the R2 of the model was 0.07 (F = 10.74, p < 0.001), indicating a statistically significant model that captured meaningful variance; hierarchical regression was conducted on three second-order terms (X2, Y2, XY), and the increment of R2 was 0.013 (p < 0.05). ΔR2 was significant, and there were statistically significant quadratic terms (p < 0.01), validated by subsequent analysis. The RSA results presented a5 = −0.21 (p = 0.108), indicating that the first main axis matched with LOC, with the curvature (a4 = −0.34, p = 0.058) and slope (a3 = 0.53, p = 0.385. As LOIC did not reach statistical significance, parental burnout and authoritative parenting style matches did not significantly predict childhood social anxiety.

Table 3. Response surface analysis parameters for parental burnout–authoritative parenting interaction predicting child social anxiety.

Figure 1. Response surface analysis of parental burnout–authoritative parenting interaction on childhood social anxiety.
3.5 The association between parental burnout and authoritarian parenting style on child social anxiety
A 0.5 standard deviation threshold was applied for horizontal difference analysis, revealing a balanced case distribution of X > Y (27.35%), X = Y (43.09%), and X < Y (29.56%), supporting the suitability of RSA. As presented in Table 4 and Figure 2, hierarchical regression was conducted on three second-order terms (X2, Y2, XY), and the increment of R2 was 0.01 (p = 0.062). ΔR2 was not significant, but there were statistically significant quadratic terms (p < 0.01). The RSA results presented a5 = −0.29 (p = 0.063), demonstrating that the first principal axis matches the LOC, with the curvature (a4 = −0.15, p = 0.59) and slope (a4 = −0.37, p = 0.485) along the LOIC were not significant. This pattern violates the congruence assumptions for RSA, and thus, parental burnout–authoritarian parenting style congruence does not significantly predict childhood social anxiety.

Table 4. Response surface analysis parameters for parental burnout–authoritarian parenting interaction predicting child social anxiety.

Figure 2. Response surface analysis of parental burnout–authoritarian parenting interaction on childhood social anxiety.
3.6 RSA of parental burnout–permissive parenting style on child social anxiety
A threshold of 0.5 standard deviations was adopted for horizontal difference analysis, yielding the following case distribution: X > Y (27.90%), X = Y (38.95%), and X < Y (33.15%), confirming balanced representation for RSA. The results presented in Table 5 demonstrate first principal axis matches with LOC (a5 = −0.09, p = 0.526), significant LOIC curvature (a4 = −0.70, p = 0.009), and non-significant LOIC slope (a3 = −0.57, p = 0.272). The significant negative LOIC curvature indicated an inverted U-shaped cross section with its vertex at the origin (0,0). Along the LOC, neither curvature (a2 = −0.28, p = 0.089) nor slope (a1 = 0.81, p = 0.068) reached statistical significance, indicating that the LOC section curve was linearly rising. Collectively, these results satisfied strict congruence assumptions, with the response surface ridges line exhibiting an upward trajectory (Figure 3). Specifically, higher congruence between elevated parental burnout levels and permissive parenting styles predicted increased levels of social anxiety in children.

Table 5. Response surface analysis parameters for parental burnout–permissive parenting interaction predicting child social anxiety.

Figure 3. Response surface analysis of parental burnout–permissive parenting interaction on childhood social anxiety.
4 Discussion
This study investigated how congruence between parental burnout and parenting styles (authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive) related to childhood social anxiety. RSA revealed that concurrent high levels of parental burnout and permissive parenting predicted significantly elevated childhood social anxiety. Conversely, congruence of authoritative or authoritarian parenting styles with parental burnout demonstrated no statistically significant association with childhood social anxiety.
According to RSA, the high level of parental burnout and permissive parenting congruence manifested a significant inverted U-shaped curvature along the LOIC. Such a pattern indicated that combined elevations in parental burnout and permissive parenting style predicted increased childhood social anxiety.
These findings support our hypothesis and align with existing literature demonstrating that permissive parenting style negatively correlates with children's socioemotional competence (42, 43), while parental stress and dysfunctional parent–child interaction mediate childhood social anxiety development (44, 45). Bandura's social learning theory (46) provides a theoretical framework for these results, positing that children acquire behavioral patterns through observation and imitation of caregivers' emotional and behavioral expressions. This observational learning process is particularly salient during childhood, facilitating the acquisition of social skills and emotional regulation strategies while shaping children's understanding of social dynamics (47). Notably, when parents experience a high level of parental burnout, they exhibit disengagement through emotionally detached or minimally communicative parenting. Under conditions of high parental burnout, caregivers frequently exhibit disengagement through emotionally detached or minimally communicative parenting. Children may subsequently internalize these withdrawal behaviors (e.g., reduced parental responsiveness) and negative affective states (e.g., irritability and anxiety), adopting similar avoidance strategies during peer conflicts. For instance, ambiguous social cues such as peer laughter may be misinterpreted as an unknown threatening stimulus by a child, potentially triggering emotional breakdown such as running away or yelling (48). Thereby, these maladaptive social learning processes amplify children's vulnerability to social anxiety.
Moreover, Bandura's social learning theory emphasizes the critical role of feedback mechanisms in observational learning processes (46). Children in high burnout and permissive households may develop heightened social anxiety due to insufficient parental feedback such as diminished rule-setting and guidance behaviors from parents (49). For instance, when children encounter peer conflicts, permissive and burned parents often fail to provide adequate feedback regarding social norms and appropriate responses. This deficiency may lead children to misinterpret routine social interactions as unpredictable threats, triggering avoidant behaviors (e.g., declining social invitations) and somatic anxiety symptoms (e.g., blushing or panic) (31). Consequently, children raised in such environments with ambiguous behavioral boundaries experience impaired threat perception and social cognition. These distortions prevent accurate interpretation of social cues, thereby exacerbating anxiety symptoms (50, 51). The resulting perceptual biases establish a self-reinforcing cycle wherein maladaptive interpretations potentiate social anxiety pathology.
Furthermore, the congruence between authoritative parenting and parental burnout did not significantly predict childhood social anxiety. This finding is similar to the results reported by some researchers, which indicated that authoritative parenting style reduces children's vulnerability to social anxiety (52, 53). Within China's cultural context, this may be attributed to the resilience of authoritative parenting practices, which emphasize collective values and parental authority. In Chinese culture, authoritative parenting characterized by well-defined rules and rational communication typically persists in a positive parenting behavior even during parental burnout (54). Such parents sustain appropriate behavioral boundaries during high burnout periods (e.g., their children cannot use foul language or hit someone; otherwise, they will be grounded. If others attack their children, then their children should report it to them). This clear social guidance aids children in comprehending interpersonal relationships and establishing clear interaction expectations, thereby buffering uncertainty-induced social anxiety (55). Also, unlike authoritarian approaches, authoritative parents avoid excessive control of their children while offering more opportunities for them to acquire social skills (e.g, peer conflict resolution and self-expression). It may reduce children's excessive concerns about others' evaluations, consequently mitigating social anxiety development. Collectively, although parental burnout may reduce authoritative parents' disciplinary engagement, their pre-established behavioral frameworks continue to provide essential social scaffolding and psychological security.
Moreover, the congruence between authoritarian parenting and parental burnout did not significantly predict childhood social anxiety. This finding contrasts with Western and European research indicating that authoritarian and over-controlling parenting styles as significant predictors of childhood social anxiety (56, 57). This discrepancy may reflect culturally distinct mechanisms underlying Chinese children's response to authoritarian parenting. Within collectivist societies emphasizing group harmony, interdependence, and obedience, parental psychological and behavioral control exhibits different functional outcomes than in individualistic cultures. In the Chinese collectivist context, authoritarian practices are more prevalent and appear to be less detrimental to children compared with Western and European individualistic cultures. Gao et al.'s longitudinal study (58) corroborates this, indicating minimal impact of parental psychological control on Chinese early adolescents' social anxiety. Chinese parents regulate their children's negative behavior by employing guilt-induction strategies grounded in Confucian values (parental self-sacrifice narratives such as I work hard for your future) while emphasizing social consequences of misbehavior. These disciplinary approaches frame control as expressions of concern rather than hostility, potentially mitigating threats and anxiety perceived by children. Critically, parental burnout may attenuate authoritarianism's intensity. Exhausted parents typically reduce coercive control efforts (e.g., punitive demands and physical punishments), inadvertently creating behavioral regulation without excessive domination. This burnout-induced moderation likely counterbalances authoritarianism's anxiety-provoking effects, explaining the non-significant association observed in our study.
In addition, parental mental health further compromises family environmental stability, potentially exacerbating childhood social anxiety through indirect pathways. Existing research confirmed significant associations between parental burnout and anxiety (24, 59). This psychopathological burden impairs caregivers' capacity to maintain emotional regulation, thereby destabilizing the family system. Specifically, burnout manifests through unpredictable parental affect (e.g., sudden anger or emotional withdrawal) (60) and diminishes co-parenting efficacy amid persistent familial conflict (61). Such an unstable and conflict family environment erodes children's emotional security, which is defined as their perceived safety within the familial relationships and observation of parental conflict resolution strategies (62). Parents who experienced burnout while practicing permissive parenting may intensify children's emotional insecurity through burnout-induced household chaos, coupled with permissive parenting's failure to provide children with the necessary guidance for fostering emotional security. When children lack foundational emotional security, they frequently misinterpret external social cues as threats, triggering defensive avoidance behaviors characteristic of social anxiety (63). Therefore, social anxiety symptoms may function as self-protective responses in children who live with burnout and permissive parents.
In general, our findings demonstrated that parents who adopt permissive parenting styles and simultaneously experience parental burnout predicted pediatric social anxiety, revealing the synergistic association between parenting styles and parental burnout in driving childhood social anxiety.
4.1 Implications
The study yields two critical implications for family-based interventions targeting childhood social anxiety. First, psychological interventions for parents should implement integrated assessments evaluating concurrent parenting styles and parental burnout levels, with tailored strategies addressing identified risk profiles. For parents exhibiting burnout–permissive parenting style, intervention should prioritize fatigue management through mindfulness and cognitive–behavior therapy, enhance emotional regulation capacities, and develop boundary-setting competencies to mitigate permissive tendencies. Second, a structured parenting training program should establish developmentally appropriate interaction frameworks, including in family interventions, to help permissive parents establish clear daily interaction rules for their children and reduce potential social uncertainties in their interpersonal communication.
4.2 Limitations
First, the cross-sectional design of this study restricts causal inferences regarding bidirectional parent–child influences, necessitating future longitudinal investigations using cross-lagged panel models to elucidate temporal dynamics. Second, the regional sampling was restricted to Chongqing, China, highlighting the need for multi-site replications with expanded demographic representation. Third, variable-centered analyses may obscure holistic parenting profiles, suggesting person-centered approaches (e.g., latent profile analysis) as valuable future directions. Lastly, cultural factors (e.g., collectivist values emphasizing obedience) may attenuate the influences of authoritarian parenting. Future researchers should replicate the study in individualistic contexts to examine its cultural generalization.
5 Conclusion
This study utilized RSA and revealed that congruence between elevated parental burnout and permissive parenting styles significantly predicts childhood social anxiety. These findings substantiate the interaction principles of family ecosystem theory, indicating that more attention should be paid to burnout and a permissive household. Consequently, clinical and policy initiatives should implement multidimensional interventions simultaneously strengthening parental psychological resources, regulating family emotional expression, and modifying maladaptive parenting practices to disrupt anxiety transmission pathways.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Chongqing Medical and Pharmaceutical College, Chongqing, China. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardians/next of kin.
Author contributions
PY: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft. LY: Data curation, Resources, Writing – review & editing. XL: Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. ZF: Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The authors declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank all participating children and their parents for their patience.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Any alternative text (alt text) provided alongside figures in this article has been generated by Frontiers with the support of artificial intelligence, and reasonable efforts have been made to ensure accuracy, including review by the authors wherever possible. If you identify any issues, please contact us.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Halldorsson B, Creswell C. Social anxiety in pre-adolescent children: what do we know about maintenance? Behav Res Ther. (2017) 99:19–36. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.08.013
2. American Psychiatric Association. The American Psychiatric Association Practice Guidelines for the Psychiatric Evaluation of Adults. 3rd ed Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2016). p. 164.
3. Chavira DA, Stein MB. Childhood social anxiety disorder: from understanding to treatment. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin. (2005) 14:797–818. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2005.05.003
4. Khalid-Khan S, Santibanez M-P, McMicken C, Rynn MA. Social anxiety disorder in children and adolescents: epidemiology, diagnosis, and treatment. Paediatr Drugs. (2007) 9:227–37. doi: 10.2165/00148581-200709040-00004
5. Chavira DA, Stein MB, Bailey K, Stein MT. Child anxiety in primary care: prevalent but untreated. Depress Anxiety. (2004) 20:155–64. doi: 10.1002/da.20039
6. Davidson JRT, Hughes DC, George LK, Blazer DG. The boundary of social phobia: exploring the threshold. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1994) 51:975–83. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950120047008
7. Katzelnick DJ, Kobak KA, DeLeire T, Henk HJ, Greist JH, Davidson JRT, et al. Impact of generalized social anxiety disorder in managed care. Am J Psychiatry. (2001) 158:1999–2007. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.12.1999
8. Hofflich SA, Hughes AA, Kendall PC. Somatic complaints and childhood anxiety disorders. Int J Clin Health Psychol. (2006) 6:229–42.
9. Sackl-Pammer P, Özlü-Erkilic Z, Jahn R, Karwautz A, Pollak E, Ohmann S, et al. Somatic complaints in children and adolescents with social anxiety disorder. Neuropsychiatr. (2018) 32:187–95. doi: 10.1007/s40211-018-0288-8
10. Bronfenbrenner U. The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press (1979). p. 352.
11. Morris AS, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR. The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc Dev. (2007) 16:361–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
12. Greco LA, Morris TL. Paternal child-rearing style and child social anxiety: investigation of child perceptions and actual father behavior. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2002) 24:259–67. doi: 10.1023/A:1020779000183
13. Baumrind D. Effects of authoritative parental control on child behavior. Child Dev. (1966) 37:887–907. doi: 10.2307/1126611
14. Pinquart M. Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with externalizing problems of children and adolescents: an updated meta-analysis. Dev Psychol. (2017) 53:873–932. doi: 10.1037/dev0000295
15. Lei Y, Wang YY, Wan JM, Patel C, Li H. Association between negative parent-related family interactions and child social anxiety: a cross-cultural systematic review and meta-analysis. J Anxiety Disord. (2023) 99::102771. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2023.102771
16. Abdallah ES, ELzeiny HH, Abdel-Hady RF, El-Sheikh MS. Association between social phobia and parenting styles among secondary school students. Am J Nurs Sci. (2016) 5:96–105. doi: 10.11648/j.ajns.20160503.14
17. Segrin C, Woszidlo A, Givertz M, Montgomery N. Parent and child traits associated with overparenting. J Soc Clin Psychol. (2013) 32:569–95. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2013.32.6.569
18. Mikolajczak M, Gross JJ, Roskam I. Parental burnout: what is it, and why does it matter? Clin Psychol Sci. (2019) 7:1319–29. doi: 10.1177/2167702619858430
19. Ajilchi B, Kargar FR, Ghoreishi MK. Relationship of the parenting styles of overstressed mothers with the anxiety of their children. Procedia Soc Behav Sci. (2013) 82:520–4. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.06.302
20. Song T, Wang W, Chen S, Li Y. Mothers’ parental burnout and adolescents’ social adaptation and security: the mediating role of parenting style. Curr Psychol. (2024) 43:23144–55. doi: 10.1007/s12144-024-06045-x
21. Qiu J, Shen B, Zhao M, Wang Z, Xie B, Xu Y. A nationwide survey of psychological distress among Chinese people in the COVID-19 epidemic: implications and policy recommendations. Gen Psychiatr. (2020) 33::e100213. doi: 10.1136/gpsych-2020-100213
22. Lades LK, Laffan K, Daly M, Delaney L. Daily emotional well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. Br J Health Psychol. (2020) 25:902–11. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12450
23. Roskam I, Brianda ME, Mikolajczak M. A step forward in the conceptualization and measurement of parental burnout: the parental burnout assessment (PBA). Front Psychol. (2018) 9:758. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00758
24. Mikolajczak M, Raes ME, Avalosse H, Roskam I. Exhausted parents: sociodemographic, child-related, parent-related, parenting and family-functioning correlates of parental burnout. J Child Fam Stud. (2018) 27:602–14. doi: 10.1007/s10826-017-0892-4
25. Weissman AS, Chu BC, Reddy LA, Mohlman J. Attention mechanisms in children with anxiety disorders and in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: implications for research and practice. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2012) 41:117–26. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.651993
26. Rahmani N, Teymuri S, Bayazi MH, Rajaei AR. The effectiveness of a family-friendly program on parental meta-worry beliefs and health anxiety and social anxiety disorder in children. Curr Psychol. (2023) 42:11344–54. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02423-x
27. Hunger-Schoppe C, Schweitzer J, Hilzinger R, Krempel L, Deußer L, Sander A, et al. Integrative systemic and family therapy for social anxiety disorder: manual and practice in a pilot randomized controlled trial (SOPHO-CBT/ST). Front Psychol. (2022) 13::867246. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.867246
28. Drake KL, Ginsburg GS. Family factors in the development, treatment, and prevention of childhood anxiety disorders. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (2012) 15:144–62. doi: 10.1007/s10567-011-0109-0
29. Ginsburg GS, Becker-Haimes EM, Keeton C, Kendall PC, Iyengar S, Sakolsky D, et al. Results from the child/adolescent anxiety multimodal extended long-term study (CAMELS): primary anxiety outcomes. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2018) 57:471–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.03.017
30. Shanock LR, Baran BE, Gentry WA, Pattison SC, Heggestad ED. Polynomial regression with response surface analysis: a powerful approach for examining moderation and overcoming limitations of difference scores. J Bus Psychol. (2010) 25:543–54. doi: 10.1007/s10869-010-9183-4
31. La Greca AM, Lopez N. Social anxiety among adolescents: linkages with peer relations and friendships. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (1998) 26:83–94. doi: 10.1023/A:1022684520514
32. Zhao J, Xing X, Wang M. Psychometric properties of the Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale (SCAS) in Mainland Chinese children and adolescents. J Anxiety Disord. (2012) 26:728–36. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.05.006
33. Cheng H, Wang W, Wang S, Li Y, Liu X, Li Y. Validation of a Chinese version of the parental burnout assessment. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:321. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00321
34. Robinson CC, Mandleco B, Olsen SF, Hart CH. Authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting practices: development of a new measure. Psychol Rep. (1995) 77:819–30. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1995.77.3.819
35. Qiu Z. RSA: SPSS macro for response surface analysis. SPSS extension version 4. (2024). Available online at: https://github.com/zongmanqiu/RSA (Accessed May 15, 2025).
36. Edwards JR, Cable DM. The value of value congruence. J Appl Psychol. (2009) 94:654–77. doi: 10.1037/a0014891
37. Humberg S, Nestler S, Back MD. Response surface analysis in personality and social psychology: checklist and clarifications for the case of congruence hypotheses. Soc Psychol Pers Sci. (2019) 10:409–19. doi: 10.1177/1948550618757600
38. Johnson RE, Rosen CC, Djurdjevic E. Assessing the impact of common method variance on higher order multidimensional constructs. J Appl Psychol. (2011) 96:744–61. doi: 10.1037/a0021504
39. Malhotra NK, Kim SS, Patil A. Common method variance in IS research: a comparison of alternative approaches and a reanalysis of past research. Manag Sci. (2006) 52:1865–83. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.1060.0597
40. Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Podsakoff NP. Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annu Rev Psychol. (2012) 63:539–69. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100452
41. Fleenor JW, McCauley CD, Brutus S. Self-other rating agreement and leader effectiveness. Leadersh Q. (1996) 7:487–506. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(96)90003-X
42. Mishra P, Kiran UV. Parenting style and social anxiety among adolescents. Int J Appl Home Sci. (2018) 5:117–23.
43. Akinsola EF, Udoka PA. Parental influence on social anxiety in children and adolescents: its assessment and management using psychodrama. Psychology. (2013) 4:246–53. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.43A037
44. Ollendick TH, Benoit KE. A parent–child interactional model of social anxiety disorder in youth. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (2012) 15:81–91. doi: 10.1007/s10567-011-0108-1
45. Platt R, Williams SR, Ginsburg GS. Stressful life events and child anxiety: examining parent and child mediators. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. (2016) 47:23–34. doi: 10.1007/s10578-015-0540-4
47. Bandura A, Hall P. Albert Bandura and social learning theory. In: MacBlain S, editor. Learning Theories for Early Years. London: SAGE Publications (2018). p. 35–6.
48. Nikolić M, Colonnesi C, de Vente W, Bögels SM. Blushing in early childhood: feeling coy or socially anxious? Emotion. (2016) 16:475–87. doi: 10.1037/emo0000131
49. Roskam I, Raes ME, Mikolajczak M. Exhausted parents: development and preliminary validation of the parental burnout inventory. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:163. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00163
50. Hudson JL, Rapee RM. Parent–child interactions and anxiety disorders: an observational study. Behav Res Ther. (2001) 39:1411–27. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00107-8
51. Banerjee R, Henderson L. Social-cognitive factors in childhood social anxiety: a preliminary investigation. Soc Dev. (2001) 10:558–72. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00180
52. Sandhu GK, Sharma V. Social withdrawal and social anxiety in relation to stylistic parenting dimensions in the Indian cultural context. Res Psychol Behav Sci. (2015) 3:51–9. doi: 10.12691/rpbs-3-3-2
53. Baharshanjani N, Taherifar Z. The relationship between parenting styles and social anxiety symptoms with the mediating role of fear of negative evaluation. J Fam Relat Stud. (2022) 2:22–31. doi: 10.22098/jhrs.2022.11311.1061
54. Chen X, Dong Q, Zhou H. Authoritative and authoritarian parenting practices and social and school performance in Chinese children. Int J Behav Dev. (1997) 21:855–74. doi: 10.1080/016502597384703
55. Smetana JG. Current research on parenting styles, dimensions, and beliefs. Curr Opin Psychol. (2017) 15:19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.012
56. Rana SA, Akhtar S, Tahir MA. Parenting styles and social anxiety among adolescents. New Horiz. (2013) 7:21–34.
57. Garcia KM, Carlton CN, Richey JA. Parenting characteristics among adults with social anxiety and their influence on social anxiety development in children: a brief integrative review. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:614318. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.614318
58. Gao D, Liu J, Xu L, Mesman J, van Geel M. Early adolescent social anxiety: differential associations for fathers’ and mothers’ psychologically controlling and autonomy-supportive parenting. J Youth Adolesc. (2022) 51:1858–71. doi: 10.1007/s10964-022-01636-y
59. Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, Heyward D. Maternal depression and child psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (2011) 14:1–27. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1
60. Brenning K, De Clercq B, Soenens B. The role of mothers’ and fathers’ perfectionistic concerns and emotional dysregulation in the co-occurrence between work-family conflict and parental burnout. J Child Fam Stud. (2024) 33:1158–71. doi: 10.1007/s10826-024-02801-6
61. Zhang Q, Zhao M. Influence of co-parenting on parental burnout: a moderated mediation model. Behav Sci. (2024) 14:159. doi: 10.3390/bs14030159
62. Notko M, Sevón E. Conflicts in family relations, children’s emotions and agency. Child Soc. (2018) 32:61–72. doi: 10.1111/chso.12227
Keywords: parenting style, parental burnout, child social anxiety, response surface analysis, permissiveness
Citation: Yang P, Yang L, Liu X and Feng Z (2025) When exhaustion meets permissiveness: a response surface analysis of parental burnout–parenting style interactions on childhood social anxiety. Front. Pediatr. 13:1640094. doi: 10.3389/fped.2025.1640094
Received: 3 June 2025; Accepted: 19 August 2025;
Published: 10 September 2025.
Edited by:
Peiwei Liu, University of California, Berkeley, United StatesReviewed by:
Marcin Moroń, University of Silesia in Katowice, PolandJiangjie Sun, Hefei University of Technology, China
Copyright: © 2025 Yang, Yang, Liu and Feng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Xuerong Liu, bGl1eHVlcm9uZ0B0bW11LmVkdS5jbg==; Zhengzhi Feng, Znp6QHRtbXUuZWR1LmNu