Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 14 April 2021
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

Association of Birth Order With Mental Health Problems, Self-Esteem, Resilience, and Happiness Among Children: Results From A-CHILD Study

  • 1Department of Global Health Promotion, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Tokyo, Japan
  • 2Department of Health and Welfare Services, National Institute of Public Health, Saitama, Japan

Objective: This study aimed to investigate the association of birth order with mental health problems, self-esteem, resilience, and happiness among children aged 9–10 years.

Methods: This was a cross-sectional study using data from the Adachi Child Health Impact of Living Difficulty (A-CHILD) study, which was a population-based study of children in fourth grade in public schools in Adachi City, Tokyo, Japan in 2018 (N = 3,744). Parent-rated Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) and self-rated resilience, happiness, and self-esteem score were used to assess child mental health. Multiple regression analysis for continuous outcomes and logistic regression for dichotomous outcomes were used to examine the association of birth order with mental health problems, resilience, happiness, and self-esteem. The analysis was controlled for child sex, mother's age, mother's education, caregiver's depressive symptoms, household income, and living with grandparents.

Results: Last-borns showed the lowest total difficulties score in the SDQ, while only children showed the highest (p < 0.001). The highest prosocial behaviors score was found among last-borns (p < 0.001). Resilience score was also highest among last-borns, followed by first-borns, middle-borns, and only children. The lowest happiness score was found among middle-borns. Self-esteem score did not differ by sibling types. These associations were similar in the adjusted model and dichotomized outcomes model.

Conclusions: Differential impacts of birth order on child mental health, for both positive and negative sides, were found. Further research is warranted to elucidate the mechanism of the association between birth order and the development of behavior problems and the positive aspects such as resilience, happiness, and self-esteem among children.

Introduction

There is an increasing number of mental health problems among adolescents, with 10–20% of them estimated to suffer from these problems in the world (1). In addition, half of the cases of lifetime mental health problems begin by the age of 14 (2). Japan is not exception; for example, the number of suicide among children and adolescents has gradually increased over a decade (3), ranked second in Asia, followed by Korea (4). To reveal this situation, it is crucial to understand risk and protective factors for prevention of mental illness in adolescence and adulthood.

Previous research has shown that later-born children aged 7–12 years in the US have been found to have a higher level of depression and anxiety (5). A population-based study in the UK has shown that later-born adolescents are more likely to have suicide attempts and psychiatric problems (6). Similarly, prior register-based studies have reported that later-born adults aged 16 years are at a higher risk of suicidal behavior in Norway and Sweden (7, 8). These findings from Western countries have indicated later-born have an increased risk of mental health problems. There are several considerations underling the mechanisms of the associations. These include later-born children being more likely to have limited interactions with parents, less parental attention, and diluted available home resources (9, 10). These circumstances have adverse effects on mental health throughout sensitive development periods in childhood, which might last across the life course. To date, however, few epidemiological studies have examined whether birth order is associated with mental health problems among children, particularly, pre-adolescent children. Further, most of the previous studies were conducted in Western countries. There is a lack of empirical studies to investigate the associations in non-Western countries, including Japan.

Positive mental health refers to a state of subjective well-being and functioning well (11). Resilience (12) and self-esteem (13) can be considered important aspects of positive mental health (1416). Further, happiness is known to be one of the indicators based on an individual's hedonistic view or emotional well-being, which suggests positive affectivity and satisfaction with life (17). Previous studies in the US have indicated that first-born adolescents are more likely to have higher self-esteem than other siblings and only children (18), whereas middle-born adolescent males are found to have lower self-esteem (19). Moreover, other research has reported that last-born and only children are happier than first-and middle-born among US young children (20). However, it is still unknown whether these findings in the US enable to be generalized to any other population with different sociocultural characteristics, such as Japan. Indeed, the association of birth order with positive aspects in non-Western countries remains unknown due to the lack of research. Furthermore, limited population-based studies focusing on children have been conducted to examine the association.

Japan has unique sociocultural characteristics affecting child-rearing. Japan had the i.e., system prescribed a patriarchal and primogeniture system in place, which was abolished in 1947. Nonetheless, the i.e., system still influences family relationships to some extent (21, 22). In the familial contexts, Japanese parents may assign a perceived ideal role to their children within a family according to the birth order; in particular, the parents require the older siblings to behave more maturely and responsibly, including taking care of younger siblings (23). Second, Japan has a specific concept of parent-child relationships, known as amae (24). Amae refers to the feeling of dependence on others and the presumption on others' acceptance and indulgence (24, 25), which is somewhat similar to the secure base of attachment in terms of needs for closeness and security (22, 26, 27). Amae has influenced Japanese values, which makes a difference in child-rearing practice, compared to other countries. For example, Japanese parents tend to indulge their children and have closer physical proximity to them (23, 28), whereas parents in Western countries are likely to encourage their children to be independent and behave autonomously from an early age (2931). Furthermore, according to the i.e., system and amae, Japanese parents may modify their child-rearing practice according to birth order (32, 33). Indeed, first-borns are more likely to be subjected to stricter upbringing, whereas last-born is to be well-taken care of by their parents and older siblings (3234). Given the differences in parenting practice between siblings, the unique sociocultural characteristics may influence the association of birth order with child development and mental health. A prior study among pre-school children in Japan reported that birth order was associated with developing children's self-reliance (34). To date, however, there is still uncertainty whether birth order has effects on mental health among Japanese pre-adolescent, say, aged 9–10 years old.

Over the years, family structures have changed in developed countries, including Japan. For example, birth rates have decreased (35). That is, more children have fewer siblings or none. Thus, data reflecting current family contexts is needed to assess the association between birth order and mental health among children. However, few empirical studies have investigated the association in recent years. This study aimed to examine the associations of birth order with mental health problems, and happiness, self-esteem, and resilience among children aged 9–10 years in Japan, using a population-based dataset collected in 2018.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We used data from the Adachi Child Health Impact of Living Difficulty (A-CHILD) study, conducted in all 69 public elementary schools in collaboration with city hall and educational committee in Adachi City, Tokyo, Japan (36). This survey was a longitudinal complete-sample survey started in 2015 as a first-wave for all first-grade children in the schools. In 2018 as a third-wave, questionnaires were distributed to all children aged 9–10 years (fourth grade) attending the schools (n = 5,311). Children responded to the self-reported questionnaire at school and took the questionnaire home so that their caregivers answered it. The questionnaire completed by the caregivers was submitted to the school anonymously. A total of 4,290 participants were eligible (response rate = 80.8%). We excluded children who lived with a single parent or not living with parents (n = 546, 12.7% of respondents). Finally, a sample of, 3,744 children and caregivers were used for analysis (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of participants.

Explanatory Variable

Birth Order

The caregivers provided information regarding the children's birth order. Based on previous birth order studies (37, 38), each child was classified into four categories based on individual's birth order: only child (no siblings), first-born (having only younger siblings), middle-born (having both older and younger siblings), and last-born (having only older siblings).

Outcome Variables

Mental Health

Child mental health problems were assessed by the caregivers using the Japanese version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) (39). The reliability and validity of the SDQ in Japanese children were documented (40). The SDQ includes five items, emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactive/inattention, peer relationship problems, and prosocial behaviors. The total difficulties score was calculated by the sum of the four items except for prosocial behavior, which was used as a continuous outcome. Each item has cut-off scores for normal, borderline, and clinical range; 0–12, 13–15, and 16–40 for total difficulties score; 0–3, 4, and 5–10 for emotional symptoms score; 0–3, 4, and 5–10 for conduct problems score; 0–5, 6, and 7–10 for hyperactivity/inattention score; 0–3, 4, and 5–10 for peer problems score; and 6–10, 5, and 0–4 for prosocial behavior score (41). The score of each item was dichotomized to use as a binary variable (normal and borderline range = 0 and clinical range = 1) for statistical analysis.

Self-Esteem

Children were asked about their self-esteem using the subscales from the Japanese version of the Children's Perceived Competence Scale (CPCS) (42), based on the Perceived Competence Scale for Children (43). The subscales consist of 10 items, for example, “are you satisfied with the way you are now?” or “do you think you have few good points?”. The Cronbach's alpha for the scale was 0.84 in this study. Each item was rated from 1 (no), 2 (rather no), 3 (rather yes), and 4 (yes). Each score was summed and used as a continuous outcome. A higher total score indicated a higher level of self-esteem. The community-based survey reported that the percentage of low self-esteem regarding eight questions related to self-esteem was averagely 7.2% among children aged 10–11years and 8.3% among those aged 13–14 years (44). Further, according to a national survey of Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, the percentage of low self-esteem regarding one question related to self-esteem was 6.4% among primary school children and 8.4% among junior high school children on average over 6 years from 2014 to 2019 (45). These results suggest the percentage of children with low self-esteem could be 10% or less. In this study, the percentage of the score < the 10th percentile was 8.4%. Referring to these findings, we defined the score < the 10th percentile as low self-esteem because the 10th percentile cut-off level could incorporate children with low self-esteem.

Resilience

The Children's Resilient Coping Scale (CRCS) was used to assess child resilience (46). This scale consists of eight items, for example, “speaks positively about their future,” “able to get ready for school, study,” and “do his/her chores without directions,” or “able to give up on things they want or do things that they do not like to do for better future outcomes.” The scale has high internal consistency (46). The caregivers rated their child resilience from zero (never) to four (very frequently). The Cronbach's alpha for the scale was 0.84 in this study. Each score of the eight items was summed, ranging from 0 to 32, and used as a continuous outcome. A higher total score indicated higher resilience. The distribution was left-skewed (more children rating higher scores ≥ in the 10th percentile). Then, the total score of resilience was divided into two categories below the 10th percentile as low resilience to capture a very low level of resilience.

Happiness

Children were asked about their happiness by the question, “how happy do you think you are?” which was rated from 0 (not happy) to 10 (very happy) and used as a continuous outcome. Due to the left-skewed distribution, more children rated higher scores ≥ in the 10th percentile. Indeed, the proportion < the 10th percentile was 5.6%. Further, a community-based survey conducted in Tokyo in 2016 showed that 5.1% of adolescents perceived low happiness (44), which was similar to our proportion of the score < the 10th percentile. Referring to the findings, we defined the score < the 10th percentile as low happiness.

Other Variables

We selected potential confounding variables based on previous research investigating birth order and mental health problems (68, 47). The following variables were included in this study: child's sex (boys or girls), household income (<3.0, 3.0 ≤ 6.0, 6.0 ≤ 10.0, 10.0+ million JPY and unknown; 110 JPY≈ 1 USD), mother's age (Seventy-eight missing data were imputed with the mean of the age) and education attainment (high school graduate or less, some college, college or university graduate, others/unknown), parental history of psychiatric disorders (yes or no), respondent's psychological distress, living with grandparents, and children's school absence due to illness and trauma. The respondent's psychological distress was assessed using the Japanese version of the Kessler 6 (K6), which consisted of six items with a 5-point Likert scale (48). The sum of each score was calculated (range: 0–24); a score of 5–12 was defined as moderate psychological distress, and a score of ≥13 was defined as severe. As for living with grandparents, prior studies have indicated that grandparents' support is related to women's fertility decisions (49, 50). Also, grandparents raising are found to affect grandchildren's mental health (5153). Thus, living with grandparents was included. Also, previous research has shown the association of birth order with PTSD (54) and illness such as metabolic problems (55), allergic symptoms (56), and respiratory diseases (57), which are known to relate to children's mental health (5860). According to these findings, we included children's school absence due to illness and trauma in this analysis. Regarding parental age and education, the mother's age and education attainment correlated with the father's ones (r = 0.40 and 0.65, respectively). Further, in Japan, mothers have more child care time than fathers and play the primary role in child development in daily life (61). Accordingly, we did not take father's age and education attainment into consideration.

Statistical Analysis

First, we compared the characteristics among the four groups: only child, first-borns, middle-borns, and last-borns. Second, we treated the outcomes as continuous variables; that is, we calculated the mean of the scores of the SDQ, self-esteem, happiness, and resilience, and compared by analysis of variance (ANOVA) with post-hoc pairwise comparison using Bonferroni's correction. Third, multiple linear regression analyses were used to examine the association between birth order and mental health problems using each SDQ subscales score, the total difficulties score, the self-esteem score, the happiness score, and the resilience score. The only child was treated as a reference group category. In the adjusted model, covariates (children's sex, mother's age, mother's education, parental history of psychiatric disorders, respondent's psychological distress, household income, living with grandparents, and children's school absence due to illness and trauma) were included. Further, to investigate the association between birth order and the clinical or severe range of the outcomes, multiple logistic regression analyses were performed. These covariates were added in the adjusted model, and we confirmed that multicollinearity is unlikely (all variance inflation factor <2.0). Missing data were substituted by dummy variables. A p-value <0.05 was considered as the level of statistical significance. We used STATA version 15.0 (StataCorp., College Station, TX, USA) for all analyses and followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology Statement (STROBE) guidelines.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee at the National Center for Child Health and Development (Study ID: 1147) and Tokyo Medical and Dental University (Study ID: M2016-284).

Results

Demographics

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the participants. There were 1,278 first-borns (34.1%), 466 middle-borns (12.5%), 1,356 last-borns (36.2%), and 644 only children (17.2%). Almost half of the children were boys (n = 1,908, 51.0%). With regard to mother's age and educational attainment, most of the mothers were 40–44 years old and graduated some college. As for household income, the largest group was between 6 and 10 million JPY. As for caregiver's mental health, the mean scores of K6 showed no difference across all groups. Regardless of the birth order, more than 90% of children lived without grandparents. Approximately 7% of caregivers reported to have psychiatric disease history, and 34% of children had experienced to be absent from school due to illness or trauma.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics.

Outcome Variables by Birth Order

Table 2 presents the mean scores of the SDQ, self-esteem, happiness, and resilience for birth order. Last-borns showed lower score on total difficulties, conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention (p < 0.001, <0.001, and <0.002, respectively) and higher score on prosocial behavior and resilience, compared to other groups (p < 0.001 and <0.001). First-borns showed a higher score of conduct problems than other groups (p < 0.001). The happiness score of middle-borns was the lowest in all groups (p < 0.002). There were no differences in the self-esteem scores among the four groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of the ANOVA between birth order and mental health problems self-esteem, resilience and happiness.

Further, the results of the post-hoc pairwise comparison showed p-value for the association between categories of birth order (Figures 2, 3). As for the SDQ, a significant difference was observed in the total difficulties score between only children and middle-borns, and only children and last-borns; hyperactivity/inattention between only children and last-borns: peer relationship problems between only children and each categories; prosocial behavior between only children and last-borns. As for the self-esteem score, there were no difference between only children and each categories. Moreover, a significant difference was observed in the resilience score between only children and first-borns, and only children and last-borns; the happiness score between only children and middle-borns.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Association of birth order and the SDQ by ANOVA with the post-hoc pairwise comparison. ***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Association of birth order and Self-esteem, Resilience, and Happiness by ANOVA with the post-hoc pairwise comparison. ***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.

Regression Analyses

Table 3 shows multivariate linear regression and logistic regression analysis examining the association of birth order with scores of the SDQ, self-esteem, happiness, and resilience. As for linear regression analysis, the score of total difficulties and peer relationship problems in last-borns, middle-borns, and first-borns were significantly lower than only children as the reference group in the adjusted model. Table 3 also presents the significant association between last-borns and lower score of hyperactivity/inattention and higher score of prosocial behavior (β = −0.06, p < 0.01; β = 0.07, p < 0.01), middle-borns and lower score of emotional symptoms and happiness (β = −0.06, p < 0.01; β = −0.08, p < 0.001), and first-borns and higher score of conduct problems (β = 0.05, p = 0.03). The resilience score is significantly higher in last-borns (β = 0.08, p < 0.001) and first-borns (β = 0.05, p = 0.03). The self-esteem score was not associated with birth order.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Associations between birth order and mental health problems, self-esteem, resilience, and happiness in linear and logistic regression.

As for multivariate logistic regression analysis, using the clinical range of the SDQ and 10th percentile cut-off score of the self-esteem, happiness, and resilience, last-borns, middle-borns, and first-borns had decreased odds ratio (OR) of peer relation problems score after adjusting for other covariates (OR = 0.64, 95% Cl 0.46–0.88; OR = 0.53, 95% CI 0.35–0.81; OR = 0.50, 95% CI 0.36–0.68, respectively). After the adjustment, last-borns were still associated with lower total difficulties score (OR = 0.67, 95% Cl 0.50–0.86). Further, middle-borns were significantly associated with unhappiness (OR = 1.84, 95% Cl 1.10–3.09). There was no association between the self-esteem score and birth order.

Comparing the results of the two models, the directions of the association between birth order and each mental health variable in the linear regression model were mostly consistent with the logistic regression model (Normal and Borderline range vs. Clinical range in the SDQ; Normal vs. Low self-esteem, Low resilience, Unhappiness). When compared the linear regression with logistic regression models by birth order, first-borns showed a significant increase in the risk of peer relationship problems in both models and total difficulties in only the linear regression model. Middle-borns showed significant results of a decreased risk of total difficulties, emotional symptoms, and peer relationship problems and an increased risk of unhappiness in both models. Further, last-borns showed a significant decrease in the risk of total difficulties in both models. No statistical significance in hyperactivity/inattention, prosocial behavior, and resilience among last-borns was found in the logistic regression model.

Discussion

In this study, we found the association between birth order and mental health among children aged 9–10 years. Our results showed that last-borns were less likely to have mental health problems and more likely to have prosocial behaviors and resilience. Middle-borns were found to show the lowest level of happiness, and first-borns were associated with conduct problems. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to examine the association of birth order with mental health among pre-adolescent children on both positive and negative sides.

Our findings of last-borns having a lower risk of mental health problems are partially in line with previous research in the UK (47). They identified that later-born children were less likely to have mental health problems than children with no older siblings. Our study focused on the definite birth order position, whereas the UK study assessed the number of siblings, regardless of the order. These findings suggest that the presence of older siblings may play a role in mitigating the risk of mental health problems among children; in turn, last-borns may be in the most advantageous position to receive the benefits by nature. There are several possible explanations for the association. First, interactions with older siblings provide contexts to develop social and emotional competencies, which are known as protective factors for mental health and peer relationship problems (6264). In daily life, children with siblings usually spend more time with siblings than parents (65). Interactions with siblings promote understanding of others' emotions, thoughts, and intentions (66), which foster their development of social competence (67). Besides, plays and conflicts with siblings develop emotional regulation (68, 69) and problem-solving skills (70, 71). Interactions through teaching, sharing, and cooperation facilitate prosocial behavior (7274). Given the interactive opportunities, last-borns may have more chances to develop social and emotional competencies from early childhood than other siblings and only one child.

Second, older siblings may play a complementary role in caregiving. When parents are unavailable, siblings can become candidates for attachment relationships and also take on the role of a secure base for younger siblings (75). These relationships provide a sense of security and comfort to younger siblings in insecure family situations (76, 77). Furthermore, last-borns may be more likely to receive more emotional support from older siblings (78). Thus, older siblings can be a source of security for last-borns, which may contribute to the prevention of mental health problems.

Third, parental differential treatment may affect the association of birth order with mental health problems. Parental differential treatment, such as favoritism and unfavorable comparison, can cause not only sibling jealousy (79) but also behavior problems (80), emotional issues (81), and lower self-esteem (82). In Japan, the i.e., system and the practice of amae may strengthen parental differential treatment. It may affect mental health more negatively among older siblings. On the contrary, last-borns may be less likely to be affected, which may lead to a decreased risk of their mental health problems. However, the association between these unique characteristics and mental health problems among Japanese children is still unclear. Hence, further research is needed to confirm the association.

However, previous studies have reported that last-born adolescents and adults have an increased risk of mental illness in the US, the UK, Norway, and Sweden (58), which is inconsistent with our findings. There are two possible explanations for the inconsistency. The one is that the positive effects of the last position on mental health in childhood may vary in adolescence and adulthood. In adolescence, children spend more time with peers and less with family and have more opportunities to face stressful events and peer relationship problems (8385). The benefits of the last position within a family may be attenuated in different life stages. The other one is that sociocultural characteristics in each country may contribute to the inconsistency. Japanese sociocultural characteristics, that is amae and i.e., system, might account for the inconsistent findings between Japan and other countries. Further longitudinal studies are needed to reveal the mechanism of the inconsistency, comparing with other countries.

Our study also demonstrated that last-borns showed a higher resilience score. Previous studies have reported that the development of resilience is associated with social competence (86), problem-solving skills (87) and self-regulation skills (88), and familial factors such as support availabilities (89, 90). Given the presence of older siblings, last-borns may be more likely to develop these skills through interactions with the siblings and receive emotional support from the siblings, which may contribute to promoting the development of resilience from early childhood.

Furthermore, our findings showed that first-borns were associated with conduct problems, which is partially consistent with a prior study in the UK reporting older siblings having an increased risk of the problems (47). This association may be related to several possible explanations. First, first-borns are required to adopt to siblinghood after the birth of siblings (91). After subsequent sibling birth, first-borns may experience less parental attention and interactions, described as “dethronement” by Adler's theory (66). The behavior problems of first-borns may be considered as a stress response to the change of the home environment (47). Second, parents might change their child-rearing style between the first-born and the subsequent children, which may induce jealousy toward younger siblings (92). That is, parental differential treatment may cause first-borns to suffer from emotional dysregulation, impulsiveness, and vulnerability to frustration (82, 93). In addition, as discussed previously, Japanese sociocultural characteristics may promote the differential treatment. Third, parents may require the first-borns to be a role model for younger siblings. Notably, in Japan, parents tend to expect first-borns to be the heir (32). Consequently, the first-borns may feel more responsibilities and emotional pressure (94). Hence, first-borns may be more likely to feel more emotional distress than younger siblings and only one child and, in turn, have an increased risk of conduct problems.

Moreover, we found that middle-borns showed the lowest level of happiness, compared to only children. Prior research demonstrated that parents rated that last-borns and only children aged 3–9 years were happier than first-borns and middle-borns in the US (20). Our study is the first to report that middle-born children rated themselves as the most unhappy. Kidwell suggested that the middle-borns have a lack of uniqueness in the family and no inherent reasons to receive parental attention and recognition, compared to first-and last-borns (19). In addition, a prior study has demonstrated that middle-born youth tend to perceive less closeness to parents (95) and express less positive attitudes toward family than first-borns and last-borns and more positive views toward friends (96). These findings suggest that the presence of both older and younger siblings may induce perceived unhappiness among middle-borns, and the state of unhappiness in childhood may affect their values toward family and peers in adolescence and adulthood. However, the effects of unhappiness on later life among middle-borns remained unclear; further longitudinal research is needed to examine the trajectories.

Our study found no differential association of birth order with self-esteem among pre-adolescent children. Previous research has shown that first-borns and only children tend to have higher self-esteem than later-borns (5, 97). Further, another study has reported that middle-borns have significantly lower self-esteem than first-borns and last-borns (19). However, it is noteworthy that these studies were conducted more than three decades ago and focused on adolescents and young adults, which may lead to inconsistency with our findings. Self-esteem is found to decline across adolescence through the experience of stressful life events (98), for example, school transitions (84, 85). In Japan, school transitions generally occur between the ages of 12 and 15 years. For this reason, the participants in our study typically have no experience of school transitions. Thus, the experience of other stressful life events may contribute to the inconsistency. Moreover, Baldwin and Hoffmann indicated that perceived family support has preventive effects on the decline of self-esteem in adolescence (99). Given the negative views toward family among middle-borns (95, 96), they may have lower stability of self-esteem when facing stressful events; in turn, they may have an increased risk of a decline in self-esteem in adolescence, compared to other siblings and only children. Further longitudinal research is required to reveal the association between birth order and self-esteem from childhood to adulthood.

This study has several limitations. First, this study did not evaluate the quality of sibling relationships. Poor sibling relationships are found to affect children's mental health negatively (100). Second, we did not differentiate the sibling compositions, such as boy-boy, boy-girl, girl-boy, girl-girl (four types by sex of two siblings), boy-boy-boy, boy-boy-girl, boy-girl-boy, boy-girl-girl, girl-boy-boy, girl-boy-girl, girl-girl-boy, girl-girl-girl (eight types by sex of three siblings), or age difference, say, 1 year difference or 5 years difference, due to limited sample size. Sibling relationships may vary depending on the compositions (92). Sibling compositions might change the magnitude of the effects of birth order on mental health. Third, we could not assess the influence of peer relationships. Peer relationships, such as bullying and victimization, are important factors that affect mental health among school children, and such exposure might vary by sibling positions (101). Thus, the quality of peer relationships might modify the effects of birth order. Fourth, we did not assess families who had experienced perinatal loss through miscarriage, stillbirth, and neonatal death. Prior research reports that the experience influences subsequent pregnancy (102, 103) and parenting behavior (103, 104). Thus, the experience may affect the number of children and parenting styles, which suggests that the effects of birth order among families having the experience may differ from those who have not. Fifth, children's medical issues, such as specific psychiatric disorders and environmental stress, and trauma experience, were not assessed. Also, in this study, children's mental health conditions were not evaluated by more objective measurement tools, such as DSM-5 criteria and the teacher-rated SDQ. Sixth, we did not assess parental personality and parenting styles. These may affect the association between birth order and children's mental health. Finally, this study used cross-sectional data and could not consider past mental health conditions. Further studies are needed to elucidate the effects of birth order on child mental health trajectories, adjusting for parenting style and sibling relationships in a longitudinal design.

Despite these limitations, our findings can help parents identify children at risk for mental health problems in pre-adolescence. Information and guidance about the association of birth order with mental health may allow parents to notice initial symptoms of child mental health problems and manage these problems before adolescence at an earlier stage. In addition, supporting parental rearing practice may also help minimize the adverse effects of birth order on mental health among children. Further, based on birth-order, intervention focusing on both parent-child and sibling relationships may be beneficial to improving mental health among children.

In conclusion, our study found that birth order had differential associations with mental health in both positive and negative aspects among Japanese children aged 9–10. These findings may be helpful to prevent mental health problems depending on birth order in adolescence. Future longitudinal studies are needed to elucidate the mechanisms of the effects of birth order on mental health and the trajectories across the life stages.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Tokyo Medical and Dental University Ethics Committee. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

YF contributed to conception, design, analysis, interpretation, drafted, and critically revised the manuscript. SD and AI contributed to conception, design, data acquisition, analysis, interpretation, and critically revised the manuscript. MO contributed to conception, design, data acquisition, and critically revised the manuscript. TF contributed to conception, design, data acquisition, analysis, interpretation, drafted, and critically revised the manuscript. All authors gave final approval and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding

This study was supported by a Health Labor Sciences Research Grant, Comprehensive Research on Lifestyle Disease from the Japanese Ministry of Health, Labor and Welfare (H27-Jyunkankito-ippan-002), Research of Policy Planning and Evaluation from the Japanese Ministry of Health, Labor and Welfare (H29-Seisaku-Shitei-004), Innovative Research Program on Suicide Countermeasures (IRPSC), and Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS KAKENHI Grant Numbers 16H03276 and 16K21669), St. Luke's Life Science Institute Grants, and the Japan Health Foundation Grants.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are particularly grateful to the staff members and central office of Adachi City Hall for conducting the survey. We would like to thank everyone who participated in the surveys. In particular, we would also like to thank Mayor Yayoi Kondo, Mr. Syuichiro Akiu, and Ms. Yuko Baba of Adachi City Hall, all of whom contributed significantly to completion of this study.

References

1. Merikangas KR, He J-P, Burstein M, Swanson SA, Avenevoli S, Cui L, et al. Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2010) 49:980–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. JAMA Psychiatry. (2005) 62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Ministory of Health Labour and Welfare. White Paper on Suicide Prevention in Japan. (2020). Available online at: https://www.mhlw.go.jp/content/r2h-1-3.pdf (accessed January 29, 2021).

Google Scholar

4. OECD. Health at a Glance 2015: OECD Indicators (2015). Available from: https://doi.org/10.1787/health_glance-2015-en (accessed February 20, 2020).

5. Gates L, Lineberger MR, Crockett J, Hubbard J. Birth order and its relationship to depression, anxiety, and self-concept test scores in children. J Genet Psychol. (1988) 149:29–34. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1988.10532136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Easey KE, Mars B, Pearson R, Heron J, Gunnell D. Association of birth order with adolescent mental health and suicide attempts: a population-based longitudinal study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2019) 28:1079–86. doi: 10.1007/s00787-018-1266-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bjørngaard JH, Bjerkeset O, Vatten L, Janszky I, Gunnell D, Romundstad P. Maternal age at child birth, birth order, and suicide at a young age: a sibling comparison. Am J Epidemiol. (2013) 177:638–44. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwt014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Rostila M, Saarela J, Kawachi I. Birth order and suicide in adulthood: evidence from Swedish population data. Am J Epidemiol. (2014) 179:1450–7. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Downey DB. Number of siblings and intellectual development: the resource dilution explanation. Am Psychol. (2001) 56:497–504. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.6-7.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Price J. Parent-child quality time: does birth order matter? J Hum Resour. (2008) 43:240–65. doi: 10.3368/jhr.43.1.240

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Keyes CLM. Subjective well-being in mental health and human development research worldwide: an introduction. Soc Indic Res. (2006) 77:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s11205-005-5550-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Rutter M. Resilience concepts and findings: implications for family therapy. J Fam Ther. (1999) 21:119–44. doi: 10.1111/1467-6427.00108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Mann M, Hosman CMH, Schaalma HP, de Vries NK. Self-esteem in a broad-spectrum approach for mental health promotion. Health Educ Res. (2004) 19:357–72. doi: 10.1093/her/cyg041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Das JK, Salam RA, Lassi ZS, Khan MN, Mahmood W, Patel V, et al. Interventions for adolescent mental health: an overview of systematic reviews. J Adolesc Health. (2016) 59:S49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.06.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Dray J, Bowman J, Campbell E, Freund M, Wolfenden L, Hodder RK, et al. Systematic review of universal resilience-focused interventions targeting child and adolescent mental health in the school setting. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2017) 56:813–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.07.780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Orpana H, Vachon J, Dykxhoorn J, McRae L, Jayaraman G. Monitoring positive mental health and its determinants in Canada: the development of the positive mental health surveillance indicator framework. Health Promot Chronic Dis Prev Can. (2016) 36:1–10. doi: 10.24095/hpcdp.36.1.01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Deci EL, Ryan RM. Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: an introduction. J Happiness Stud. (2006) 9:1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9018-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Falbo T. Relationships between birth category, achievement, and interpersonal orientation. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1981) 41:121–31. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.41.1.121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kidwell JS. The neglected birth order: middleborns. J Marriage Fam. (1982) 44:225–35. doi: 10.2307/351276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Park N, Peterson C. Character strengths and happiness among young children: content analysis of parental descriptions. J Happiness Stud. (2006) 7:323–41. doi: 10.1007/s10902-005-3648-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Sugimoto Y. An Introduction to Japanese Society. 4th ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2014). p. 64–90.

Google Scholar

22. Umemura T, Traphagan JW. Reviewing Japanese concepts of Amae and Ie to deeper understand the relevance of secure-base behavior in the context of Japanese caregiver-child interactions. Integr Psychol Behav Sci. (2015) 49:714–36. doi: 10.1007/s12124-015-9316-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Holloway SD, Behrens KY. Parenting self-efficacy among Japanese mothers: qualitative and quantitative perspectives on its association with childhood memories of family relations. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. (2002) 96:27–43. doi: 10.1002/cd.42

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Doi T. The Anatomy of Dependence. JP: Kodansha International (1973).

Google Scholar

25. Shaffer DR. Developmental Psychology: Childhood and Adolescence. 4th ed. Bermont: Thomson Brooks/Cole Publishing Co. (1995). p. 689.

Google Scholar

26. Rothbaum F, Kakinuma M, Nagaoka R, Azuma H. Attachment and Amae. J Cross Cult Psychol. (2007) 38:465–86. doi: 10.1177/0022022107302315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Rothbaum F, Weisz J, Pott M, Miyake K, Morelli G. Attachment and culture: security in the United States and Japan. Am Psychol. (2000) 55:1093–104. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.10.1093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Behrens KY. A multifaceted view of the concept of Amae: reconsidering the indigenous Japanese concept of relatedness. Hum Dev. (2004) 47:1–27. doi: 10.1159/000075366

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Niiya Y, Ellsworth PC, Yamaguchi S. Amae in Japan and the United States: an exploration of a “culturally unique” emotion. Emotion. (2006) 6:279–95. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Power TG, Kobayashi-Winata H, Kelley ML. Childrearing patterns in Japan and the United States: a cluster analytic study. Int J Behav Dev. (1992) 15:185–205. doi: 10.1177/016502549201500202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Markus HR, Kitayama S. Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol Rev. (1991) 98:224–53. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Shirasa T. Kyoudai Kankei to Sono Kankei Ryouiki no Bunkenshuusei 2 (Japanese). JP: Kawashima shoten (2004).

33. Someya T, Uehara T, Kadowaki M, Tang SW, Takahashi S. Effects of gender difference and birth order on perceived parenting styles, measured by the EMBU scale, in Japanese two-sibling subjects. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2000) 54:77–81. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2000.00640.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Kojima Y. Effects of birth order on self-reliance of Japanese preschoolers during daily routines. Psychology. (2019) 10:1262–8. doi: 10.4236/psych.2019.109081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ministory of Health Labour and Welfare. Vital Statistics of Japan. (2017). Available online at: https://www.mhlw.go.jp/toukei/saikin/hw/jinkou/suikei18/dl/2018gaiyou.pdf (accessed November 1, 2019).

Google Scholar

36. Ochi M, Isumi A, Kato T, Doi S, Fujiwara T. Adachi child health impact of living difficulty (A-CHILD) study: research protocol and profiles of participants. J Epidemiol. (2020) 31:77–89. doi: 10.2188/jea.JE20190177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Carballo JJ, Garcia-Nieto R, Alvarez-Garcia R, Caro-Canizares I, Lopez-Castroman J, Munoz-Lorenzo L, et al. Sibship size, birth order, family structure and childhood mental disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2013) 48:1327–33. doi: 10.1007/s00127-013-0661-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Damian RI, Roberts BW. The associations of birth order with personality and intelligence in a representative sample of U.S. high school students. J Res Pers. (2015) 58:96–105. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2015.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. (1997) 38:581–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Moriwaki A, Kamio Y. Normative data and psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire among Japanese school-aged children. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. (2014) 8:1. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-8-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Matsuishi T, Nagano M, Araki Y, Tanaka Y, Iwasaki M, Yamashita Y, et al. Scale properties of the Japanese version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): a study of infant and school children in community samples. Brain Dev. (2008) 30:410–5. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2007.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Sakurai S. The investigation of self-consciousness in the 5th-and 6th-grade children. Jap J Exp Soc Psyhol. (1992) 32:85–94. doi: 10.2130/jjesp.32.85

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Harter S. The perceived competence scale for children. Child Dev. (1982) 53:87–97. doi: 10.2307/1129640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bureau of Social Welfare and Public Health. Survey of Living Conditions on Chidren in Tokyo. (2017). Available online at: https://www.fukushihoken.metro.tokyo.lg.jp/smph/joho/soshiki/syoushi/syoushi/oshirase/kodomoseikatsujittaityousakekka.html (accessed January 29, 2021).

45. Ministry of Education Culture Sports Science and Technology. Zenkokutekina gakuryoku chousa (Zenkoku gakuryoku gakushuu joukyou chousatou) (2019). Available online at: https://www.nier.go.jp/19chousakekkahoukoku/19summary.pdf (accessed January 30, 2021).

46. Doi S, Fujiwara T, Ochi M, Isumi A, Kato T. Association of sleep habits with behavior problems and resilience of 6- to 7-year-old children: results from the A-CHILD study. Sleep Med. (2018) 45:62–8. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Lawson DW, Mace R. Siblings and childhood mental health: evidence for a later-born advantage. Soc Sci Med. (2010) 70:2061–9. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Furukawa TA, Kawakami N, Saitoh M, Ono Y, Nakane Y, Nakamura Y, et al. The performance of the Japanese version of the K6 and K10 in the World Mental Health Survey Japan. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. (2008) 17:152–8. doi: 10.1002/mpr.257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Hayslip B Jr., Blumenthal H, Garner A. Social support and grandparent caregiver health: one-year longitudinal findings for grandparents raising their grandchildren. J Gerontol Series B. (2014) 70:804–12. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Tanskanen AO, Jokela M, Danielsbacka M, Rotkirch A. Grandparental effects on fertility vary by lineage in the United Kingdom. Hum Nat (Hawthorne, NY). (2014) 25:269–84. doi: 10.1007/s12110-014-9200-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Dunifon R. The influence of grandparents on the lives of children and adolescents. Child Dev Perspect. (2013) 7:55–60. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Pearson RM, Culpin I, Loret de Mola C, Matijasevich A, Santos IS, Horta BL. Grandmothers' mental health is associated with grandchildren's emotional and behavioral development: a three-generation prospective study in Brazil. BMC Psychiatry. (2019) 19:184. doi: 10.1186/s12888-019-2166-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Sadruddin AFA, Ponguta LA, Zonderman AL, Wiley KS, Grimshaw A, Panter-Brick C. How do grandparents influence child health and development? A systematic review. Soc Sci Med. (2019) 239:112476. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Green B, Griffiths EC. Birth order and post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Health Med. (2014) 19:24–32. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2013.774432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Ayyavoo A, Savage T, Derraik JGB, Hofman PL, Cutfield WS. First-born children have reduced insulin sensitivity and higher daytime blood pressure compared to later-born children. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. (2013) 98:1248–53. doi: 10.1210/jc.2012-3531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Perzanowski MS, Canfield SM, Chew GL, Mellins RB, Hoepner LA, Jacobson JS, et al. Birth order, atopy, and symptoms of allergy and asthma among inner-city children attending Head Start in New York City. Clin Exp Allergy. (2008) 38:968–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2222.2008.02967.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Rudan I, Boschi-Pinto C, Biloglav Z, Mulholland K, Campbell H. Epidemiology and etiology of childhood pneumonia. Bull World Health Organ. (2008) 86:408–16. doi: 10.2471/BLT.07.048769

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Craske MG, Poulton R, Tsao JC, Plotkin D. Paths to panic disorder/agoraphobia: an exploratory analysis from age 3 to 21 in an unselected birth cohort. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2001) 40:556–63. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200105000-00015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Goodwin RD, Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ. Asthma and depressive and anxiety disorders among young persons in the community. Psychol Med. (2004) 34:1465–74. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Teyhan A, Galobardes B, Henderson J. Child allergic symptoms and well-being at school: findings from ALSPAC, a UK cohort study. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0135271. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0135271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Statistics Bureau of Japan. Japan Statistical Yearbook 2018. (2018). Available online at: https://www.stat.go.jp/english/data/nenkan/67nenkan/index.html (accessed January 29, 2021).

62. Slaughter V, Imuta K, Peterson CC, Henry JD. Meta-analysis of theory of mind and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Dev. (2015) 86:1159–74. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. McGee R, Stanton WR. Sources of distress among New Zealand adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (1992) 33:999–1010. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00921.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Stormshak EA, Bellanti CJ, Bierman KL. The quality of sibling relationships and the development of social competence and behavioral control in aggressive children. Dev Psychol. (1996) 32:79–89. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.1.79

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Tucker CJ, McHale SM, Crouter AC. Links between older and younger adolescent siblings' adjustment: the moderating role of shared activities. Int J Behav Dev. (2008) 32:152–60. doi: 10.1177/0165025407087214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. McHale SM, Updegraff KA, Whiteman SD. Sibling relationships and influences in childhood and adolescence. J Marriage Fam. (2012) 74:913–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01011.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Bosacki S, Wilde Astington J. Theory of mind in preadolescence: relations between social understanding and social competence. Soc Dev. (1999) 8:237–55. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00093

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Youngblade LM, Dunn J. Individual differences in young children's pretend play with mother and sibling: links to relationships and understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs. Child Dev. (1995) 66:1472–92. doi: 10.2307/1131658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Kramer L. Learning emotional understanding and emotion regulation through sibling interaction. Early Educ Dev. (2014) 25:160–84. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2014.838824

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Brown JR, Donelan-McCall N, Dunn J. Why talk about mental states? The significance of children's conversations with friends, siblings, and mothers. Child Dev. (1996) 67:836–49. doi: 10.2307/1131864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Howe N, Rinaldi CM, Jennings M, Petrakos H. “No! the lambs can stay out because they got cozies”: constructive and destructive sibling conflict, pretend play, and social understanding. Child Dev. (2002) 73:1460–73. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Dunn J, Munn P. Siblings and the development of prosocial behaviour. Int J Behav Dev. (1986) 9:265–84. doi: 10.1177/016502548600900301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Howe N, Recchia H. Playmates and teachers: reciprocal and complementary interactions between siblings. J Fam Psychol. (2005) 19:497–502. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Howe N, Recchia H. Individual differences in sibling teaching in early and middle childhood. Early Educ Dev. (2009) 20:174–97. doi: 10.1080/10409280802206627

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Samuels HR. The effect of an older sibling on infant locomotor exploration of a new environment. Child Dev. (1980) 51:607–9. doi: 10.2307/1129305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Jenkins J. Sibling Relationships in Disharmonious Homes: Potential Difficulties and Protective Effects. Children's Sibling Relationships: Developmental and Clinical Issues. Hillsdale, MI: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. (1992). p. 125–38.

Google Scholar

77. Teti DM, Ablard KE. Security of attachment and infant-sibling relationships: a laboratory study. Child Dev. (1989) 60:1519–28. doi: 10.2307/1130940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Gass K, Jenkins J, Dunn J. Are sibling relationships protective? A longitudinal study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2007) 48:167–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01699.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Loeser MK, Whiteman SD, McHale SM. Siblings' perceptions of differential treatment, fairness, and jealousy and adolescent adjustment: a moderated indirect effects model. J Child Fam Stud. (2016) 25:2405–14. doi: 10.1007/s10826-016-0429-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Meunier JC, Roskam I, Stievenart M, De Moortele GV, Browne DT, Wade M. Parental differential treatment, child's externalizing behavior and sibling relationships: bridging links with child's perception of favoritism and personality, and parents' self-efficacy. J Soc Pers Relat. (2012) 29:612–38. doi: 10.1177/0265407512443419

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Shanahan L, McHale SM, Crouter AC, Osgood DW. Linkages between parents' differential treatment, youth depressive symptoms, and sibling relationships. J Marriage Fam. (2008) 70:480–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2008.00495.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Feinberg ME, Neiderhiser JM, Simmens S, Reiss D, Hetherington EM. Sibling comparison of differential parental treatment in adolescence: gender, self-esteem, and emotionality as mediators of the parenting-adjustment association. Child Dev. (2000) 71:1611–28. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Brown BB, Larson J. Peer relationships in adolescence. In: Lerner RM, Steinberg L, editors. Handbook of Adolescent Psychology: Contextual Influences on Adolescent Development, Vol 2, 3rd edn. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc. (2009). p. 74–103.

Google Scholar

84. Cole DA, Maxwell SE, Martin JM, Peeke LG, Seroczynski AD, Tram JM, et al. The development of multiple domains of child and adolescent self-concept: a cohort sequential longitudinal design. Child Dev. (2001) 72:1723–46. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Wigfield A, Eccles JS, Mac Iver D, Reuman DA, Midgley C. Transitions during early adolescence: changes in children's domain-specific self-perceptions and general self-esteem across the transition to junior high school. Dev Psychol. (1991) 27:552–65. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.4.552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. Personality, adrenal steroid hormones, and resilience in maltreated children: a multilevel perspective. Dev Psychopathol. (2007) 19:787–809. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Masten AS, Barnes AJ. Resilience in children: developmental perspectives. Children. (2018) 5:98. doi: 10.3390/children5070098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Zolkoski SM, Bullock LM. Resilience in children and youth: a review. Child Youth Serv Rev. (2012) 34:2295–303. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2012.08.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Dirks MA, Persram R, Recchia HE, Howe N. Sibling relationships as sources of risk and resilience in the development and maintenance of internalizing and externalizing problems during childhood and adolescence. Clin Psychol Rev. (2015) 42:145–55. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Benzies K, Mychasiuk R. Fostering family resiliency: a review of the key protective factors. Child Fam Soc Work. (2009) 14:103–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2206.2008.00586.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Volling BL. The transition to siblinghood: a developmental ecological systems perspective and directions for future research. J Fam Psychol. (2005) 19:542–9. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Whiteman SD, McHale SM, Soli A. Theoretical perspectives on sibling relationships. J Fam Theory Rev. (2011) 3:124–39. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-2589.2011.00087.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Volling BL. Family transitions following the birth of a sibling: an empirical review of changes in the firstborn's adjustment. Psychol Bull. (2012) 138:497–528. doi: 10.1037/a0026921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Baskett LM. Sibling status effects: adult expectations. Dev Psychol. (1985) 21:441–5. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.21.3.441

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Salmon CA, Daly M. Birth order and familial sentiment: middleborns are different. Evol Hum Behav. (1998) 19:299–312. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00022-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Salmon C. Birth order and relationships. Hum Nat. (2003) 14:73–88. doi: 10.1007/s12110-003-1017-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Schwab MR, Lundgren DC. Birth order, perceived appraisals by significant others, and self-esteem. Psychol Rep. (1978) 43:443–54. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1978.43.2.443

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Robins RW, Trzesniewski KH. Self-esteem development across the lifespan. Curr Direct Psychol Sci. (2005) 14:158–62. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00353.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Baldwin SA, Hoffmann JP. The dynamics of self-esteem: a growth-curve analysis. J Youth Adolesc. (2002) 31:101–13. doi: 10.1023/A:1014065825598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Buist KL, Vermande M. Sibling relationship patterns and their associations with child competence and problem behavior. J Fam Psychol. (2014) 28:529–37. doi: 10.1037/a0036990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Menesini E, Camodeca M, Nocentini A. Bullying among siblings: the role of personality and relational variables. Br J Dev Psychol. (2010) 28:921–39. doi: 10.1348/026151009X479402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Geller PA, Kerns D, Klier CM. Anxiety following miscarriage and the subsequent pregnancy: a review of the literature and future directions. J Psychosom Res. (2004) 56:35–45. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00042-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Lamb EH. The impact of previous perinatal loss on subsequent pregnancy and parenting. J Perinat Educ. (2002) 11:33–40. doi: 10.1891/1058-1243.11.2.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Coleman PK, Reardon DC, Cougle J. The quality of the caregiving environment and child developmental outcomes associated with maternal history of abortion using the NLSY data. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2002) 43:743–57. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: birth order, mental health, resilience, happiness, self-esteem

Citation: Fukuya Y, Fujiwara T, Isumi A, Doi S and Ochi M (2021) Association of Birth Order With Mental Health Problems, Self-Esteem, Resilience, and Happiness Among Children: Results From A-CHILD Study. Front. Psychiatry 12:638088. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.638088

Received: 05 December 2020; Accepted: 17 March 2021;
Published: 14 April 2021.

Edited by:

Michaela Pascoe, Victoria University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Isabelle Esther Bauer, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, United States
Omneya Ibrahim, Suez Canal University, Egypt

Copyright © 2021 Fukuya, Fujiwara, Isumi, Doi and Ochi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Takeo Fujiwara, fujiwara.hlth@tmd.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.