Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 22 October 2018
Sec. Cognition

Commentary: Metacognition and Perspective-Taking in Alzheimer's Disease: A Mini-Review

\r\nRosalba Morese,Rosalba Morese1,2Mario StanzianoMario Stanziano3Sara Palermo,
Sara Palermo4,5*
  • 1Faculty of Biomedical Sciences, Institute of Public Health, Università della Svizzera Italiana, Lugano, Switzerland
  • 2Faculty of Communication, Culture and Society, Università della Svizzera Italiana, Lugano, Switzerland
  • 3Neuroradiology Unit, Azienda Ospedaliera Universitaria “Città della Salute e della Scienza di Torino”, Turin, Italy
  • 4Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
  • 5European Innovation Partnership on Active and Healthy Ageing (EIP-AHA), Brussels, Belgium

A Commentary on
Metacognition and Perspective-Taking in Alzheimer's Disease: A Mini-Review

by Bertrand, E., Landeira-Fernandez, J., Daniel, C., and Mograbi, D. C. (2016). Front. Psychol. 7:1812. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.0181

The neurocognitive approach points out how reduced self-awareness is related to cerebral disorders, particularly concerning focal lesions, emotional and motivational determinants, and concurrent neuropsychological impairments (Palermo, 2017). Given its consequences on patients' quality of life, caregivers' burn-out, compliance with treatment, and prognosis, reduced self-awareness has received increasing attention on the part of neuroscientists (Fernández-Calvo et al., 2015). Indeed, a reduction in self-awareness has been found to be associated with a decline in help-seeking behavior and compliance with medical treatment, presumably because of a reduction in motivation (Palermo, 2017).

In their brilliant mini-review, Bertrand and colleagues drew attention to the fact that neurological and neuropsychiatric patients can present severe—and often underestimated—impairments in metacognitive abilities that may result in “a lack of self-awareness of deficits.” Indeed, executive-metacognitive dysfunctions have been formerly associated with loss of self-awareness across major neurocognitive disorders likely due to Alzheimer's Disease (AD) and frontotemporal dementia (Amanzio et al., 2011, 2013, 2016), Acquired Brain Injury (Palermo et al., 2014), Bipolar Disorder (Palermo et al., 2015) and Parkinson's Disease (Amanzio et al., 2014; Palermo et al., 2017a,b).

Focusing on AD, Bertrand et al. (2016) dwelt on the association between metacognition and perspective-taking as a key aspect for improving quality of life in AD patients. These authors specified that “metacognition refers to the monitoring and regulation of cognitive processes and its impairment can lead to a lack of self-awareness of deficits, or anosognosia” (Bertrand et al., 2016, p. 1).

Anosognosia and reduced self-awareness are frequently used as synonyms in the literature and an overlap between them has been highlighted. This is a delicate question because according to the interpretation given to those terms, the methods of detection and interpretation of the phenomenon could change. This topic was recently treated by Mograbi and Morris (2018). In their interesting Discussion Forum, the authors not only emphasize how the term anosognosia continues to be the most used in the neurological field, but propose a glossary of labels that enhances the differences in use and interpretation of the epiphenomena to which they refer.

In our opinion, the notion of “self-awareness” is not entirely concerned with the concept of “anosognosia,” since the former is a complex neuropsychological notion, defined as “the ability to consciously process information about ourselves in a manner that reflects a relatively objective view while maintaining our unique phenomenological or subjective sense of self ” (Prigatano, 1997, p. 301). Indeed, awareness of illness is above all a form of self-knowledge and a higher-order cognitive function covering information about the state of the disease, its functional consequences, the way in which it affects the patient and influences his/her interaction with the environment (Palermo, 2017). In support of this theoretical framework, Stuss et al. (2001) and Stuss and Anderson (2004) argued that there are several types of self-awareness (the so-called “consciousness” in their theoretical framework), organized hierarchically, which should be differentiated.As we have previously outlined, they proposed different elements in a four-level hierarchical framework of awareness, “where the processing in turn operates in a modular manner at any level of the hierarchy, so that damage in one functional domain may result in a different kind of reduced self-awareness” (Palermo et al., 2014, p. 541). These four types of self-awareness have their own neural underpinnings in distinct substrates, with the lower levels related to a selective disturbance of knowledge [anosognosia] and the higher levels related to self-awareness, perspective-taking and Theory of Mind, with an emphasis on the role of the frontal and medial prefrontal brain areas (Palermo, 2017). Indeed, the cognitive processes involved in metacognitive-executive functions and Theory of Mind have been associated with complex ecological contexts that require the recruitment of the anterior cingulate cortex and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Morese et al., 2016). Moreover, (O‘Keeffe et al., 2007). proposed that any disturbance in monitoring, cognitive flexibility and response inhibition has the potential to distort patients' foresight (and thus self-awareness).

Bertrand et al. (2016) suggested that the Default Mode Network may be useful to shed light on the role of perspective-taking in awareness of deficits in AD. They presented this relationship by referring to the Cognitive Awareness Model (CAM), which incorporates a comparator system within the central executive to detect mismatches between a personal database and experience of failures and successes (Agnew and Morris, 1998). When a discrepancy is found, a signal is sent out to the metacognitive awareness system (the fourth level in the model by Stuss et al., 2001), enabling a conscious experience of failure/success. If the executive system does not work properly, the comparator mechanism may not detect mismatches, and subsequently experienced failures may not produce any metacognitive output or conscious awareness, leading to an “executive unawareness” in the CAM (Agnew and Morris, 1998).

This model was applied by Amanzio et al. (2011), who addressed the relationship between executive dysfunctions and reduced self-awareness in AD using fMRI and an event-related specific executive task (a Go/NoGo task adapted from Braver et al., 2001; Palermo et al., 2018). Their findings suggest that lower self-awareness in AD patients is linked to reduced functional recruitment in the medial prefrontal circuit, in particular in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex (ACC) (Amanzio et al., 2011). Furthermore, response-inhibition, self-monitoring, and set-shifting dysfunctions have to be considered prominent features of reduced self-awareness in AD patients (Amanzio et al., 2011, 2013), while apathy and disinhibition would seem to be the first relevant behavior changes in AD patients with reduced self-awareness (Amanzio et al., 2011).

In conclusion, reduced self-awareness is often observed in AD patients who have suffered major damage to the central executive system. This could be due to the fact that the central executive system is a metacognitive facility of interest in controlling the flow of information in tasks requiring initiation, planning, set-shifting, strategy allocation, monitoring and inhibition (Amanzio et al., 2011, 2013). As indicated by Bertrand et al. (2016), the CAM is one of the most fruitful models for understanding the irrefutable liaison between executive system dysfuntion, metacognitive disabilities and the occurrence of reduced self-awareness. The issues raised in this commentary have implications in considering executive-metacognitive disabilities as a neuropsychological marker for reduced self-awareness in AD.

We would like to conclude by drawing attention to the fact that this phenomenon is worthy of clinical investigation. Indeed, distinct major neurocognitive disorders, brain pathologies and mood disturbances exhibit executive-metacognitive abnormalities as part of the anomalies of overlapping neural pathways. In this context, ACC hypofunctionality seems to represent one of the relevant underpinnings of a lack of self-awareness (Amanzio et al., 2011; Palermo et al., 2014, 2015).

Author Contributions

The commentary is based on a concept developed by SP who performed the review and critique processes as PI. All the authors listed have contributed to writing the commentary. All authors have approved the submission of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Agnew, S., and Morris, R. (1998). The heterogeneity of anosognosia for memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: a review of the literature and a proposed model. Aging Ment. Health 2, 9–15. doi: 10.1080/13607869856876

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amanzio, M., D'Agata, F., Palermo, S., Rubino, E., Zucca, M., Galati, A., et al. (2016). Neural correlates of reduced awareness in instrumental activities of daily living in frontotemporal dementia. Exp. Gerontol. 83, 158–164. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2016.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amanzio, M., Palermo, S., Zibetti, M., Leotta, D., Rosato, R., Geminiani, G., et al. (2014). Self-unawareness of levodopa induced dyskinesias in patients with Parkinson's disease. Brain Cogn. 90, 135–141. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amanzio, M., Torta, D. M. E., Sacco, K., Cauda, F., D'agata, F., Duca, S., et al. (2011). Unawareness of deficits in Alzheimer's disease: role of the cingulate cortex. Brain 134, 1061–1076. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amanzio, M., Vase, L., Leotta, D., Miceli, R., Palermo, S., and Geminiani, G. (2013). Impaired awareness of deficits in Alzheimer's disease: the role of everyday executive dysfunction. JINS 19, 63–72. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertrand, E., Landeira-Fernandez, J., and Mograbi, D. C. (2016). Metacognition and perspective-taking in Alzheimer's disease: a mini-review. Front. Psychol. 7:1812. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Gray, J. R., Molfese, D. L., and Snyder, A. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cereb. Cortex 11, 825–836. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.9.825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Calvo, B., Contador, I., Ramos, F., Olazarán, J., Mograbi, D. C., and Morris, R. G. (2015). Effect of unawareness on rehabilitation outcome in a randomised controlled trial of multicomponent intervention for patients with mild Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 25, 448–477. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2014.948461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mograbi, D. C., and Morris, R. G. (2018). Anosognosia. Cortex 103, 385–386. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morese, R., Rabellino, D., Sambataro, F., Perussia, F., Valentini, M. C., Bara, B. G., et al. (2016). Group membership modulates the neural circuitry underlying third party punishment. PLoS ONE 11:e0166357. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O‘Keeffe, F. M., Murray, B., Coen, R. F., Dockree, P. M., Bellgrove, M. A., Garavan, H., et al. (2007). Loss of insight in frontotemporal dementia, corticobasal degeneration and progressive supranuclear palsy. Brain 130, 753–764. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palermo, S. (2017). “Reduced self-awareness across pathologies: involvement of the anterior cingulate cortex,” in Horizons in Neuroscience Research, Vol. 28, eds A. Costa and E. Villalba (Happauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers), 137–176.

Google Scholar

Palermo, S., Cauda, F., Costa, T., Duca, S., Gallino, G., Geminiani, G., et al. (2015). Unawareness of bipolar disorder: the role of the cingulate cortex. Neurocase 21, 438–447. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2014.917682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palermo, S., Leotta, D., Bongioanni, M. R., and Amanzio, M. (2014). Unawareness of deficits in ischemic injury: role of the cingulate cortex. Neurocase 20, 540–555. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2013.826686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palermo, S., Lopiano, L., Zibetti, M., Rosato, R., Leotta, D., and Amanzio, M. (2017a). A novel framework for understanding reduced awareness of dyskinesias in Parkinson's Disease. Park Rel. Dis. 39, 58–63. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2017.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palermo, S., Morese, R., Zibetti, M., Dematteis, F., Sirgiovanni, S., Stanziano, M., et al. (2017b). Impulse control disorder and response-inhibition alterations in Parkinson's disease. a rare case of totally absent functionality of the medial-prefrontal cortex and review of literature. J. Adv. Res. 8, 713–716. doi: 10.1016/j.jare.2017.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palermo, S., Stanziano, M., and Morese, R. (2018). Commentary: anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:171. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prigatano, G. P. (1997). “The problem of impaired self-awareness in neuropsychological rehabilitation,” in Neuropsychological Rehabilitation Fundamentals, Innovations, and Directions, ed J. Leon-Carrion (Florida: GR/St Lucie Press), 301–312.

Google Scholar

Stuss, D., Picton, T., and Alexander, M. (2001). “Consciousness, self-awareness and the frontal lobe,” in The Frontal Lobes and Neuropsychiatric Illness, eds S. Salloway, P. Malloy, and M. Alexander (Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.), 101–109.

PubMed Abstract

Stuss, D. T., and Anderson, V. (2004). The frontal lobes and theory of mind: developmental concepts from adult focal lesion research. Brain Cogn. 55, 69–83. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00271-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: metacognition, self-awareness, cognitive awareness model, anterior cingulate (ACC), Alzheimer's disease

Citation: Morese R, Stanziano M and Palermo S (2018) Commentary: Metacognition and Perspective-Taking in Alzheimer's Disease: A Mini-Review. Front. Psychol. 9:2010. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02010

Received: 23 July 2018; Accepted: 01 October 2018;
Published: 22 October 2018.

Edited by:

Claudia Gianelli, Universität Potsdam, Germany

Reviewed by:

Elodie Bertrand, Pontifical Catholic University of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

Copyright © 2018 Morese, Stanziano and Palermo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sara Palermo, sara.palermo@unito.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.