Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 06 March 2020
Sec. Occupational Health and Safety

Subjective Age and Job Satisfaction: A Moderated Mediation Model of Job Burnout and Chronological Age

\nMuhammad Khalid AnserMuhammad Khalid Anser1Moazzam AliMoazzam Ali2Farooq AnwarFarooq Anwar3Muhammad Usman
Muhammad Usman4*
  • 1School of Public Administration, Xi'an University of Architecture and Technology, Xi'an, China
  • 2Department of Management Sciences, University of Okara, Okara, Pakistan
  • 3Lahore Business School, The University of Lahore, Lahore, Pakistan
  • 4Department of Management Sciences, COMSATS University Islamabad, Lahore, Pakistan

Corresponding to the growing calls for theory-driven research on the age-job satisfaction association, the present study investigated direct and indirect (via job burnout) relationships between subjective age (felt age) and job satisfaction. The study also examined the moderating role of chronological age on both direct and indirect (via job burnout) relationships between subjective age and job satisfaction. Survey data were collected in three waves (2 months apart) from 355 employees in 62 firms operating in various service and manufacturing industry sectors in Pakistan. Data were analyzed using structural equation modeling, PROCESS macro for SPSS, and bootstrapping technique. The results showed subjective age was negatively related to job satisfaction, both directly (β = −0.19, p < 0.001) and indirectly, via job burnout (β = −0.09, bootstrap 95% confidence interval limits did not overlap with zero; lower limit = −0.15, upper limit = −0.04). Interestingly, the interaction term (relative subjective age × chronological age) had a significant negative effect on the direct negative association between subjective age and job satisfaction (B = −0.12, p < 0.05) and a significant positive effect on the direct positive relationship between subjective age and job burnout (B = 0.14, p < 0.01), showing that chronological age moderated the direct relationships of subjective age with job satisfaction and job burnout, respectively. Importantly, the results showed that chronological age moderated the indirect association (via job burnout) between subjective age and job satisfaction [bootstrap estimate = −0.025, bias-corrected confidence interval (−0.06, −0.002)]. The present study contributed to the literature on the age-job satisfaction association by suggesting subjective age as an alternative vantage point to look at this link between age and job satisfaction. The findings carry useful practical implications that can help managers counter age stereotyping, employees' feelings of job burnout, and a low level of employees' job satisfaction.

Introduction

Employee job satisfaction has received much attention from researchers and practitioners because of its constructive influence on employees' productivity and performance (1, 2). A plethora of studies have revealed that job satisfaction reduces absenteeism, employee turnover, and counterproductive behaviors (1, 3) and enhances employees' motivation, trust, and citizenship behaviors (4, 5). Therefore, the focus of management on job satisfaction is a vital aspect of managing human resources and employees' behaviors and performance outcomes (6, 7).

Age is one of the widely researched predictors of job satisfaction (812). Despite valuable contributions to both theory and practice, the literature on the age and job satisfaction relationship contains several ambiguities and unaddressed questions. A key concern relevant to our study is the way prior studies have treated age in the age-job satisfaction relationship. Past research has made the simplest choice of the age—chronological age (actual age in years)—to showcase different types of relationships with job satisfaction, such as linear positive and negative, U-shaped, and curvilinear relationships (3, 4, 9, 11). We argue that the chronological age offers an important but a restrictive view of the age-job satisfaction relationship. People with the same chronological age can have different subjective age—subjective views about their age (1315). The assumption that individuals sharing the same chronological age groups will exhibit similar attitudes and behaviors is flawed (13, 15, 16). Rather, people with the same age can behave and act differently depending on their subjective age, which can be equal to, and lower or higher than, their chronological age (13, 17, 18). Indeed, several studies have revealed that the explanatory power of subjective age is stronger than the chronological age [e.g., (13, 15, 16, 1921)]. Moreover, there are growing calls for theory-driven research on the association of age with employees' work-related outcomes (22, 23). Thus, we understand that subjective age at work (subjective age henceforth) may have greater relevance in explaining the variations in employees' attitudes, behaviors, and performance outcomes (22, 23) and can have important implications for managers concerned about employees' work-related outcomes. However, despite important theoretical and practical implications, as noted in previous studies (15, 24) there is a surprising scarcity of studies on the subjective age-job satisfaction relationship.

To fill in these research gaps, we draw on the socioemotional selectivity theory (SST) (25) and the conservation of resources (COR) theory (26, 27) to theorize a negative association of subjective age with job satisfaction. Furthermore, given the scarcity of studies on the subjective age-job satisfaction connection, little is known about the mechanisms underlying the subjective age-job satisfaction link, leaving it unknown why subjective age is negatively related to job satisfaction. To address this research void, we propose that job burnout—an employee's perception that his/her personal resources, such as physical and emotional energies are gradually diminishing (28)—mediates the subjective age-job satisfaction association. We focus on job burnout, as it carries a significant cost for organizations because of its destructive effects on employees' work-related outcomes (29, 30). Job burnout deteriorates employees' mental, physical, and behavioral health (31, 32). Job burnout also results in reduced job satisfaction, turnover, increased absenteeism, and lowered commitment (29, 33).

Finally, previous studies (16, 34, 35) suggest that the consequences of subjective age vary across different age groups. For instance, Fang and Galambos (35) and Bergland et al. (34) suggest that older subjective age bias among middle-aged and old-aged individuals is indicative of low levels of mental and physical energy and life satisfaction, while the older subjective age bias among young people might reflect their maturity, a sense of inclusion, and life satisfaction (16, 24, 34). Drawing on these arguments, we argue that the relationships of subjective age with job burnout and job satisfaction are not straightforward; rather, these relationships may be contingent on employees' chronological age. However, to date, not a single study has hypothesized and empirically shown that chronological age acts as a boundary condition of the subjective age-job satisfaction association and the subjective age-job burnout linkage. The present study addresses these gaps by proposing and revealing that chronological age moderates both direct and indirect subjective age-job satisfaction associations and the subjective age-job burnout association. The proposed model is presented in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The proposed model.

Hypotheses Development

Subjective Age and Job Satisfaction

Job satisfaction refers to an employee's positive emotions and attitude toward the job, work environment, and his/her role in the organization (36). Scholars have shown a consensus about the imperative role of job satisfaction in the improvement of organizational practices and performance (3739). Moreover, job satisfaction is one of the significant predictors of employees' vocational happiness, commitment, improved productivity, and citizenship behaviors (7, 38).

The SST posits that in younger years, people often perceive an open-ended future time perspective (FTP), while in older years, they follow a limited FTP (40, 41). In a work context, younger employees prefer to pursue goals that open future career growth opportunities by focusing on the acquisition of new knowledge and skills. In contrast, when employees' chronological age increases, they often demonstrate a low level of interest in professional training and development, the acquisition of new skills, and long-term success (25, 42). Therefore, drawing upon the SST, we argue that employees who report their subjective age higher than their chronological age, they follow a limited FTP (40, 41) and thus, can demonstrate a low level of interest in work activities, such as learning and development, that are linked with the long-term success. In other words, based on the SST, we understand that employees who report subjective age higher than their chronological age may lack important resources, such as passion, energy, and skills.

Furthermore, the COR theory (26) posits that employees endeavor to gain, retain and protect valued resources, such as knowledge, skills, conditions, objects, social support, and energy. The COR theory further suggests that losing or anticipating a loss of valued resources results in burnout at work, a discrepancy between expected and actual performance, and a low level of job satisfaction. Moreover, initial resource loss results in future resource loss. As employees try to invest resources from one domain to the other domains, in which the loss has occurred or the loss is anticipated, the loss or anticipation of the loss in one domain depletes resources in other domains. Thus, building on the COR theory (26), we suggest that employees with older subjective age bias may be deficient in resources (e.g., such as passion, energy, and skills) required to perform their work roles that can lead to a discrepancy between expected and actual performance and a low level of job satisfaction.

Additionally, empirical studies show that youthful subjective age is positively associated with employees' self-efficacy, productivity, performance, and work motivation (23, 43), suggesting that youthful subjective age can be positively related to job satisfaction. Moreover, although empirical studies on the subjective age-satisfaction link in the work context are non-existing, a study in a non-work context (44) showed that youthful subjective age bias is positively related to life satisfaction. Thus, the following hypothesis has been developed.

Hypothesis 1. Subjective age is negatively related to employee job satisfaction.

Job Burnout as a Mediator

To establish job burnout as a mediator of the subjective age-job satisfaction association, we build on the SST (25) and the COR theory (26). The SST suggests that during the lifespan, individuals' time horizon shrinks because of the shifts in their FTP—from an unlimited, an open-ended FTP to a limited FTP—and consequently, they demonstrate a low level of interest passion and energy for the work, professional development, and the acquisition of new skills (42). That is, due to the limited FTP, employees with older subjective age bias can face a shortage of skills and knowledge, given their reduced focus on skill development (42) that, as posited by the COR theory, can deplete their mental and physical energies.

Additionally, the literature on the links between subjective age and health shows that older subjective age bias negatively is negatively related to individuals' health and well-being (45, 46). Thus, the literature suggests that subjective age higher than the chronological age is indicative of poor physical and mental energy (47, 48). Therefore, we infer that middle-aged employees with older subjective age bias (subjective age higher than their chronological age) feel low levels of energy and demonstrate a reduced level of interest in their work roles that if seen through the lens of the COR theory, constitute a loss of important personal resources at work. According to the COR theory, the continual depletion of such resources as physical and emotional energy and professional knowledge can result in job burnout, which is negatively related to several employees' constructive outcomes, including job satisfaction (27). Thus, based on the COR theory, it can be argued that that continual resources' depletion (e.g., a loss of physical and mental energy) or the absence of resources leads to job burnout (26). Job burnout, in turn, negatively affects various work-related outcomes, such as job satisfaction (49, 50). In sum, we understand that job burnout can explain why subjective age is negatively related to job satisfaction.

Hypothesis 2. Job burnout mediates the negative relationship between subjective age and job satisfaction.

Chronological Age as a Moderator

Past research has revealed that perception of subjective age can vary across individuals within the same chronological age group, and that can result in their different perceptions of mental and physical health, subjective well-being, and life satisfaction (15, 16). For instance, middle-aged and older adults with youthful subjective age perceptions reported better physical and emotional health than those with older subjective age bias (34, 51). Indeed, youthful subjective age bias among middle-aged and older adults has been found to have positive associations with mental and physical health, subjective well-being, a sense of inclusion, and life satisfaction (34, 52). Likewise, younger individuals with the same chronological age can have different perceptions of their subjective age and report different perceptions about health and well-being and life satisfaction (15).

Furthermore, the literature on subjective age suggests that older subjective age bias among younger people reflects maturity and confidence that help them gain success and life satisfaction (24, 35, 53). On the contrary, older subjective age bias among middle-aged or older adults is indicative of poor mental and physical health and low levels of energy, passion, and life satisfaction (34). Seen through the lens of the COR theory, poor mental and physical energies among middle-aged employees can negatively affect their work-related attitudes, such as job satisfaction, as well as trigger the feelings of job burnout. Drawing from the above discussion, we understand that as compared to older subjective age bias among younger employees, older subjective age bias among middle-aged employees can have a more profound effect on job burnout and job satisfaction. Thus, the present study postulates the following hypotheses. It is worth noting that following Bellingtier and Neupert (24) and Shinan-Altman and Werner (15), the present study devised a criterion to divide the chronological age of the respondents into two groups—young employees and middle-aged employees.

Hypothesis 3. Employees' chronological age moderates the negative relationship between subjective age and job satisfaction, such that the relationship is stronger for middle-aged employees than for younger employees.

Hypothesis 4. Employees' chronological age moderates the positive relationship between subjective age and job burnout, such that the relationship is stronger for middle-aged employees than for younger employees.

Moreover, as we have alluded above (hypothesis 4), the positive subjective age-job burnout link is stronger for middle-aged employees than for younger employees, suggesting that the mediating effect of job burnout on the negative subjective age-job satisfaction association is contingent on employees' chronological age. Therefore, from a statistical perspective, this study presents a case of moderated mediation (54). More specifically, the present work predicts that the indirect negative relationship (via job burnout) between subjective age and job satisfaction can be stronger for middle-aged employees than for younger employees. Thus, the following hypothesis was developed.

Hypothesis 5. Employees' chronological age moderates the indirect negative relationship (via job burnout) between subjective age and job satisfaction, such that the relationship is stronger for middle-aged employees than for younger employees.

Methods

Data Collection and Analysis

We collected survey data from 355 employees in 62 Pakistani firms belonging to various service and manufacturing sectors. Employees' perceptions of subjective age at work can be different across industries, organizations, and professions. Therefore, following Hirschi (55) and Kreiner (56), we collected data from a heterogeneous sample to capture maximum variance in subjective age. Initially, 70 firms were randomly selected from the total firms listed on the Pakistan Stock Exchange. We managed access to 62 firms through personal and professional references. We collected data in three rounds separated by a 2 months lag. In the first round, data about work experience, education, chronological age, subjective age, and gender were collected. In the second round, we collected data about job burnout. Finally, in the third round, we collected data about job satisfaction. The survey questionnaire was translated into Urdu (the national language of Pakistan) using the Brislin's (57) back-translation method. The translated version of the questionnaire was pilot-tested with six academicians and 20 potential respondents.

A total of 600 employees were randomly selected from the employees of those companies that offered us access. Of these 600 randomly chosen employees, we were able to contact 576 employees, who were provided an information sheet containing information about the purpose of our research and the confidentiality promise. We received written consent for participation in data collection from 552 employees. In the first round, 390 respondents returned the completed questionnaires, while 372 and 368 employees returned the completed survey in the second and third rounds, respectively. After screening the data for negligence, we received 355 matched (59.16% net response rate) useable responses. The responses from the three rounds of data collection were matched using a unique code. The unique code was developed by asking the respondents to write the last two letters of their first name, the last two digits of their city code, the month of their birth, the last two digits of their mobile phone number, and the last two letters of their organization's name. These letters and digits were combined to generate the code. This information was asked in all the data collection rounds. In terms of gender, the sample included 52.4% males and 47.6% females. Respondents' average chronological age was 43 years. As far as education is concerned, 32.3% of the respondents had completed 12 years of schooling, 48% had undergraduate degrees, and 19.7% held master's degrees. We analyzed data using structural equation modeling (SEM) in AMOS 24.0, bootstrapping method, and [51] PROCESS macro for SPSS.

Data were collected in 2018. To minimize the common method bias, we followed Podsakoff et al. (58) recommendation and used a time-lagged design. A 2-months lag has been used in a number of prior publications (5961). Moreover, we also used Harman's single-factor test (62) to diagnose the common method bias. For this purpose, we constrained all the items of the two latent constructs—job satisfaction and job burnout—into one factor, which resulted in explaining 47.64% of the total variance. Since the variance explained by a single factor did not exceed the cut-off value (50%), the common method was not a problem (62).

Variables and Measures

Chronological and Subjective Age

To measure chronological age, the respondents were asked to write their actual age measured in years such as 25, 26, and 27 years. To operationalize subjective age, we followed (23, 47) and used the relative subjective age as a measure of subjective age. For this purpose, we asked employees how old they feel at work independent of his/her actual (chronological) age and then subtracted the chronological age from the subjective age (23). A positive value indicated that a respondent felt older compared to his/her chronological age, while a negative value indicated that a respondent felt younger compared to his/her chronological age. Moreover, employees with chronological age ≤44 years were considered as young, while employees with age >44 years were categorized as middle-aged. As the retirement age in Pakistan is 60 years, the present study did not take into the older age (age > 60 years) individuals.

Job Satisfaction

To measure job satisfaction, Macdonald and Maclntyre's (63) ten-item scale was used. A five-point Likert scale anchored on 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) was used to gauge the responses. Sample items were “I receive recognition for a job well-done” and “I feel close to the people at work.”

Job Burnout

To measure job burnout, we used a seven-item scale (the Copenhagen burnout inventory to measure work-related burnout) developed and validated by Kristensen et al. (28). “Do you feel worn out at the end of the day” was a sample item. The responses were measured on a five-point Likert scale, anchored on 1(never) to 5 (always).

Control Variables

Work experience and gender can affect job burnout (64, 65) and job satisfaction (8, 12) and thus, can confound the results. Moreover, employees' perceptions of job burnout and job satisfaction can vary across industries. Therefore, in the present study, we controlled for gender and industry type.

Results

Non-independence of the Data

As 355 respondents belonged to 62 organizations, we calculated the values of the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) for the outcome variable (job satisfaction) and the mediator (job burnout). ICC1 = 0.01 (ns) for job satisfaction and ICC1 = 0.00 (ns) for job burnout suggested that non-independence was not a problem in our data (66).

Means and Correlations

The means, standard deviations, and inter-construct correlations are presented in Table 1. It is important to note that the mean value of relative subjective age was 2.96 years, suggesting that, on average, the respondents felt that their subjective age was higher than their chronological age.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Means and correlations.

Measurement Model

Our measurement model was assessed using a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). This model included job satisfaction, job burnout, and relative subjective age. The results demonstrated that the model had a good fit with the data, as chi-square (χ2) = 384.77, degrees of freedom (df) = 133, χ2/df = 2.89 <3, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.07 <0.08, incremental fit index (IFI) = Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) = comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.96 > 0.90 (67).

The values of Cronbach alpha (Table 2) for both the variables > 0.70 show that the level of internal consistency for both the variables (job satisfaction and job burnout) was satisfactory. The average variance extracted >0.50 suggested a satisfactory level of convergent validity (67). To assess the discriminant validity, we used the Fornel-Lacker criterion. It is evident from Table 2 that the square roots of average variance extracted of both reflective constructs—job burnout and job satisfaction—were greater than their corresponding inter-construct correlations. It is also evident from Table 2 that average shared variance < maximum shared variance < average variance extracted. Therefore, the measurement scales of job burnout and job satisfaction achieved satisfactory levels of discriminant validity.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Discriminant validity, convergent validity, and internal consistency.

Evaluation of the Structural Model and Mediation Results

First, we tested the direct relationship between relative subjective age and job satisfaction in the structural model (1). Direct (without mediator) negative association between relative subjective age and job satisfaction was found to be significant (β = −0.19, p < 0.001). The model (1) demonstrated a good with the data, as χ2 = 188.69, df = 70, χ2/df = 2.70 < 3, RMSEA = 0.07 < 0.08, and CFI = TLI = IFI = 0.97 > 0.90 (67). Thus, the results supported hypothesis 1.

Then in the structural model (2), the mediator, job burnout was included in the relative subjective age-job satisfaction association. This model (2) also demonstrated a good fit with the data – χ2 = 432.10, df = 178, χ2/df = 2.43 < 3, RMSEA = 0.07 < 0.08, and CFI = TLI = IFI = 0.96 > 0.90 (67). The results of the structural model (2) suggested that job burnout can mediate the relative subjective age-job satisfaction relationship. The structural model (2) is depicted in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Structural model (2): Relationship between relative subjective age and job satisfaction via job burnout.

Finally, the significance of the mediating role of job burnout was tested using bootstrapping in AMOS 24.0 (a sample of size 2,000 was specified at a 95% confidence interval). Table 3 depicts the results.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Direct and indirect effects and 95% confidence intervals for the model (2).

It is evident from Table 3 that after including job burnout as the mediator, the direct negative association of relative subjective age with job satisfaction became insignificant (β = −0.12, bootstrap 95% confidence interval limits overlapped with zero; lower limit = −0.22, upper limit = 0.01), while the relative subjective age-job satisfaction relationship via job burnout was significant (β = −0.09, β = −0.09, bootstrap 95% confidence interval limits did not overlap with zero; lower limit = −0.15, upper limit = −0.04). Moreover, as evident from Figure 2, the effect of control—gender, work experience, and industry type—on both job burnout and job satisfaction was insignificant. Thus, job burnout fully mediated the negative association between relative subjective age and job satisfaction. Thus, the results supported hypothesis 2.

Moderation Results

The moderating effects of chronological age on the direct negative association of subjective age with job satisfaction (hypothesis 3), the positive relationship between subjective age and job burnout (hypothesis 4), and the indirect (via job burnout) negative subjective age-job satisfaction association (hypothesis 5) were examined using Hayes's PROCESS (model 8). The results presented in Table 4 demonstrated that the interaction term (relative subjective age × chronological age) had a significant negative effect on the direct negative association between subjective age and job satisfaction (B = −0.12, p < 0.05), showing that chronological age moderated the direct negative association of relative subjective age with job satisfaction. The interactions plotted at different values (chronological age ≤44 and chronological age > 44) of the moderator are shown in Figure 3. The strength of the negative subjective age-job satisfaction association was assessed using a simple slope test. The simple slope test showed that the negative subjective age-job satisfaction relationship was strong (B = −0.11, p < 0.01) for the respondents with chronological age >44 than for those with chronological age ≤44 (B = 0.01, ns). Thus, hypothesis 3 was supported.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Moderated mediation analysis: Chronological age moderates the direct and indirect relationship between relative SA and job satisfaction (PROCESS model 8, 95% CI).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Chronological age as a moderator of the relationship between relative subjective age and job satisfaction.

Furthermore, the results presented in Table 4 depict that the interaction term (relative subjective age × chronological age) had a significant positive effect on the direct positive relationship between subjective age and job burnout (B = 0.14, p < 0.01), showing that chronological age moderated the direct positive association between relative subjective age and job burnout. The interactions plotted at different values (chronological age ≤44 years and chronological age > 44 years) of the moderator are shown in Figure 4. The strength of the positive association between relative subjective age and job burnout was assessed using the simple slope test. The simple slope test showed that the positive association between relative subjective age and job burnout was strong (B = 0.24, p < 0.001) for the respondents with chronological age > 44 years than for those with chronological age ≤44 years (B = 0.10, p < 0.05). Thus, the results supported hypothesis 4.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Chronological age as a moderator of the relationship between relative subjective age and job burnout.

Finally, the results (Table 4) revealed that chronological age moderated the indirect association (via job burnout) between subjective age and job satisfaction [bootstrap estimate = −0.025, bias-corrected confidence interval (−0.06, −0.002)]. The results presented in Table 4 show that for employees with age >44 years, the indirect negative relationship between subjective age and job satisfaction was significant, while the indirect negative relationship between subjective age and job satisfaction was non-significant for employees with age ≤44 years. Thus, hypothesis 5 was also supported.

Discussion

The present work, first, theorized and examined the relationship between subjective age and job satisfaction and, then, tested the mediating role of job burnout in this subjective age-job satisfaction relationship. Additionally, the study hypothesized that chronological age moderates both direct and indirect relationships between subjective age and job satisfaction. Time-lagged survey data collected from 355 employees in 62 service and manufacturing firms operating in Pakistan and analyzed using SEM and Hayes' PROCESS macro for SPSS supported the hypothesized relationships.

The results showed a significant negative relationship between subjective age and job satisfaction. Our findings suggest that employees with older subjective age bias may lack important resources, such as passion, energy, and skills that lead to a low level of job satisfaction. Our findings concord with previous empirical studies on subjective age in the work context that suggest that subjective is negatively related to employees' work-related outcomes (23, 43, 52), and life satisfaction (44).

Our study revealed that job burnout mediates the negative relationship between subjective age and job satisfaction, indicating that older subjective age bias reflects limited FTP and results in employees' personal resources' depletion, and continual resources' depletion can invoke employees' feelings of job burnout. Thus, our findings also extend support to existing studies [e.g., (47, 48)] that posit that subjective age predicts the depletion of individuals' resources, such as physical and mental health and energy, which symptomizes as anxiety, helplessness, depression and decreased self-esteem feelings (34, 51). Seen in this way, our findings concord with the COR theory (26), which suggests that employees' lack of personal resources, such as emotional and physical energy, can result in job burnout and negatively influences their work-related positive outcomes, such as job satisfaction and job performance.

Finally, the present study showed that as compared to older subjective age bias among younger employees, older subjective age bias among middle-aged employees has a more profound effect on job burnout and job satisfaction. Given the importance of subjective age for theory and practice and the lack of research on the subjective age-job satisfaction relationship, the present study is timely and relevant. Importantly, the present work is based on a diverse sample that belonged to 62 firms operating in different manufacturing and service sectors. Data from the diverse sample is one of the key highlights of the present study, as such a diverse sample allowed us to capture maximum variance and also enhanced the generalizability (55).

Theoretical Contributions

This study makes several contributions to theory by theorizing the subjective age-job satisfaction relationship. Although there is abundant literature on the age-job satisfaction connection (8, 12, 68), the analysis of the literature revealed that the existing literature did not pay attention to the notion of subjective age in the age-job satisfaction link. We attempted to go some way to fill in this void and, importantly, we provided empirical support to the literature [e.g., (20, 21)] that suggests that subjective age can better explain the variations and complexities in employees' work-related outcomes and capture the diversity of the dimensions of the age-job satisfaction relationship. As a key contribution to the literature on subjective age (15, 24), the study established job burnout as a mediator of the subjective age-job satisfaction association. Furthermore, by explaining these relationships using the SST and the COR perspectives, the work at hand responded to the calls for theory-driven research on age and employees' work-related outcomes (22, 23) and advanced the scope of the SST and the COR theory.

Finally, by establishing chronological age as a boundary condition of the subjective age-job satisfaction link, the present study not only explored the complexities involved in this subjective age-job satisfaction connection and enhanced our understanding of when subjective age (24, 51) can have a more pronounced effect on job burnout and job satisfaction but also contributed to the job burnout literature (42, 45, 46) by extending the nomological networks of its antecedents and outcomes. In essence, our study not only theorized and empirically showed that subjective age is negatively related to job satisfaction but also brought to the fore the complex nature of this relationship by establishing job burnout as an important underlying mechanism of the subjective age-job satisfaction association and foregrounding chronological age as a boundary condition of both direct and indirect associations between subjective age and job satisfaction.

Practical Implications

Subjective age higher than the chronological age can have important practical implications for job satisfaction that can influence employees' productivity and performance (43, 52, 69, 70). Our findings suggest that by broadening the measurement and meaning of age, managers can understand an alternative explanation of age and its consequences for employees' work-related outcomes. Consequently, managers can develop policies and strategies to triumph over the factors that can increase employees' subjective age, job burnout, and a low level of job satisfaction. For instance, past research suggests that a sensed lack of control over one's work can lead to older age subjective bias (15, 16, 20). Managers can empower employees to counter such feelings and reduce their older subjective bias.

Moreover, although the aim of this study was not to foreground the role of subjective age in countering age stereotyping, following (71, 72), we suggest that understanding employees' subjective age can help managers counter the age stereotyping at the workplace. The focus on subjective age also provides some critical insights into the aging population and workforce (73) because of the likely difference between chronological age and subjective age and the implications of this difference for employees' behaviors, preferences, and actions. Thus, we suggest that the concepts of age and aging in organizations need to be revisited to make these concepts more relevant and meaningful.

Limitations and Future Research

This study is not without limitations. Reliance on self-reported measures presents a key limitation and should be taken care of when explaining and making sense of the findings of this study. Moreover, although the findings of present work are based on time-lagged data, our research design restricts drawing causal inferences. Future studies can use longitudinal and experimental designs to establish causality. Likewise, our findings reflect that employees' perceptions of subjective age and, thus, do not inform us about managers' perceptions of subjective age and its implications for both managers' and employees' work-related outcomes. Future studies can collect data from managers to understand the managerial perspective of subjective age to offer further theoretical and practical insights.

Additionally, subjective age negatively affects psychological well-being and mental and physical health (15, 16) that, in turn, can negatively affect job satisfaction, suggesting that factors, such as psychological well-being and mental and physical health, can mediate the subjective age-job satisfaction link. Likewise, different individual-level differences, such as employees' perception of meaningful work can moderate the subjective age-job satisfaction relationship, as employees who perceive that their work is valuable and contributes to greater good motivations (61) can be motivated to enhance new skills and knowledge. Thus, they may not perceive the deficiency of resources to perform their work roles and feel less burned out despite older subjective age bias. Given the paucity of research on the mediating and moderating variables of the subjective age-job satisfaction link, it is important to examine the intervening factors and the boundary conditions of the subjective age-job satisfaction association to foreground the intricate nature of this relationship.

Finally, this study is contextualized in Pakistan a country where collectivist culture prevails (74, 75). Future studies in different contexts, industries and cultures can further enhance our understanding of the subjective age-job satisfaction link. The implications of subjective age for age stereotyping offer an important area of future research. The relationship of subjective age with employees' performance and productivity is also an important theme that needs to be investigated.

Conclusion

Building on the SST and the COR theory, the present study hypothesized and empirically tested the direct and indirect (via job burnout) relationship between subjective age and job satisfaction. Moreover, the work at hand hypothesized and empirically tested chronological age as a moderator of the direct and indirect relationships of subjective age with job satisfaction. By collecting survey data in three waves (2 months apart) from 355 employees in 62 firms operating in various service and manufacturing industry sectors in Pakistan and analyzing data using structural equation modeling, PROCESS macro for SPSS, and bootstrapping technique, the present study showed that subjective age is negatively related to job satisfaction, both directly and indirectly, via job burnout. By doing so, the present work provided important alternative vantage points to explore the age-job satisfaction association and responded to the calls for theory-driven research on the age-job satisfaction connection (22, 23). Finally, by establishing chronological age as a boundary condition of the subjective age-job satisfaction association and the subjective age-job burnout connection, the present study foregrounded the complexities involved in these relationships and enhanced our understanding of when subjective age has a more pronounced effect on job burnout and job satisfaction. Our findings suggest that managers can comprehend negative implications of age for employees' work-related outcomes more effectively by broadening the measurement and meaning of age.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

This study involving human participants was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Department of Management Sciences, COMSATS University Islamabad, Lahore Campus, Lahore, Pakistan. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MA, MKA, MU, and FA: definition of research objectives, models, hypotheses, data analysis plan, principal article writing, article revision and proofreading, and final approval. MA, FA, and MU: the provision of materials (i.e., questionnaires) and data collection. MKA, MA, FA, and MU: data analysis.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Moyes GD, Williams PA, Koch B. The effects of age and gender upon the perceptions of accounting professionals concerning their job satisfaction and work-related attributes. Manag Audit J. (2006) 21:536–61. doi: 10.1108/02686900610667292

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Weikamp JG, Göritz AS. Organizational citizenship behaviour and job satisfaction: the impact of occupational future time perspective. Hum Relat. (2016) 69:2091–115. doi: 10.1177/0018726716633512

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Cooper-Hakim A, Viswesvaran C. The construct of work commitment: testing an integrative framework. Psychol Bull. (2005) 131:241–59. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Lewig KA, Dollard MF. Emotional dissonance, emotional exhaustion and job satisfaction in call centre workers. Eur J Work Organ Psychol. (2003) 12:366–92. doi: 10.1080/13594320344000200

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Ng TW, Feldman DC. Organizational tenure and job performance. J Manag. (2010) 36:1220–50. doi: 10.1177/0149206309359809

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Al-dalahmeh M, Khalaf R, Obeidat B. The effect of employee engagement on organizational performance via the mediating role of job satisfaction: the case of IT employees in Jordanian banking sector. Mod Appl Sci. (2018) 12:17–43. doi: 10.5539/mas.v12n6p17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Fiori M, Bollmann G, Rossier J. Exploring the path through which career adaptability increases job satisfaction and lowers job stress: the role of affect. J Vocat Behav. (2015) 91:113–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2015.08.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Clark A, Oswald A, Warr P. Is job satisfaction U shaped in age? J Occup Organ Psychol. (1996) 69:57–81. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8325.1996.tb00600.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Riza SD, Ganzach Y, Liu Y. Time and job satisfaction: a longitudinal study of the differential roles of age and tenure. J Manag. (2015) 44:2558–79. doi: 10.1177/0149206315624962

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Pagan R. Ageing and disability: job satisfaction differentials across Europe. Soc Sci Med. (2011) 72:206–15. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Rhodes SR. Age-related differences in work attitudes and behavior: a review and conceptual analysis. Psychol Bull. (1983) 93:328–67. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.93.2.328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Sarwar SZ, Mirza E, Ehsan N, Khan K, Hanif H. Determining the impact of age and LOS on job satisfaction: a case study of Pakistan automotive industry. Int J Hum Resour Manag. (2013) 24:415–35. doi: 10.1080/09585192.2012.674960

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Cleveland JN, Shore LM. Self-and supervisory perspectives on age and work attitudes and performance. J Appl Psychol. (1992) 77:469–84. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.77.4.469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Keyes CL, Westerhof GJ. Chronological and subjective age differences in flourishing mental health and major depressive episode. Aging Ment Health. (2012) 16:67–74. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2011.596811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Shinan-Altman S, Werner P. Subjective age and its correlates among middle-aged and older adults. Int J Aging Hum Dev. (2019) 88:3–21. doi: 10.1177/0091415017752941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Terracciano A. How old do you feel? The role of age discrimination and biological aging in subjective age. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0119293. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0119293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Barak B. Age identity: a cross-cultural global approach. Int J Behav Dev. (2009) 33:2–11. doi: 10.1177/0165025408099485

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Kooij D, Van De Voorde K. How changes in subjective general health predict future time perspective, and development and generativity motives over the lifespan. J Occup Organ Psychol. (2011) 84:228–47. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8325.2010.02012.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Boehmer S. Relationships between felt age and perceived disability, satisfaction with recovery, self-efficacy beliefs and coping strategies. J Health Psychol. (2007) 12:895–906. doi: 10.1177/1359105307082453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Kleinspehn-Ammerlahn A, Kotter-Grühn D, Smith J. Self-perceptions of aging: do subjective age and satisfaction with aging change during old age? J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2008) 63:377–85. doi: 10.1093/geronb/63.6.P377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Teuscher U. Subjective age bias: a motivational and information processing approach. Int J Behav Dev. (2009) 33:22–31. doi: 10.1177/0165025408099487

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Akkermans J, de Lange AH, van der Heijden BIDT, Jansen PG, Dikkers JS. What about time? Examining chronological and subjective age and their relation to work motivation. Career Dev Int. (2016) 21:419–39. doi: 10.1108/CDI-04-2016-0063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Kunze F, Raes AM, Bruch H. It matters how old you feel: antecedents and performance consequences of average relative subjective age in organizations. J Appl Psychol. (2015) 100:1511–26. doi: 10.1037/a0038909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Bellingtier JA, Neupert SD. Feeling young and in control: daily control beliefs are associated with younger subjective ages. J Gerontol B Psychol. (2019). doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbz015. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Carstensen LL, Isaacowitz DM, Charles ST. Taking time seriously: a theory of socioemotional selectivity. Am Psychol. (1999) 54:165–81. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Hobfoll SE. Conservation of resources: a new attempt at conceptualizing stress. Am Psychol. (1989) 44:513–24. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.3.513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hobfoll SE, Halbesleben J, Neveu JP, Westman M. Conservation of resources in the organizational context: the reality of resources and their consequences. Ann Rev Organ Psychol Organ Behav. (2018) 5:103–28. doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032117-104640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kristensen TS, Borritz M, Villadsen E, Christensen KB. The copenhagen burnout inventory: a new tool for the assessment of burnout. Work Stress. (2005) 19:192–207. doi: 10.1080/02678370500297720

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Halbesleben JR, Buckley MR. Burnout in organizational life. J Manage. (2004) 30:859–79. doi: 10.1016/j.jm.2004.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Ochoa P. Impact of burnout on organizational outcomes, the influence of legal demands: the case of Ecuadorian physicians. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:662. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Cordes CL, Dougherty TW. A review and an integration of research on job burnout. Acad Manag Rev. (1993) 18:621–56. doi: 10.5465/amr.1993.9402210153

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Jackson SE, Maslach C. After effects of job related stress: families as victims. J Organ Behav. (1982) 3:63–77. doi: 10.1002/job.4030030106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Cropanzano R, Rupp DE, Byrne ZS. The relationship of emotional exhaustion to work attitudes, job performance, and organizational citizenship behaviors. J Appl Psychol. (2003) 88:160–9. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.1.160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Bergland A, Nicolaisen M, Thorsen K. Predictors of subjective age in people aged 40–79 years: a five-year follow-up study. the impact of mastery, mental and physical health. Aging Ment Health. (2014) 18:653–61. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2013.869545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Fang S, Galambos NL. Bottom dogs on campus: how subjective age and extrinsic self-esteem relate to affect and stress in first semester of university. J Youth Stud. (2015) 18:537–52. doi: 10.1080/13676261.2014.992308

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Locke EA. The nature and causes of job satisfaction. In: Dunnette MD, editor. Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology. Chicago, IL: Rand McNally (1976). p. 1297–349.

Google Scholar

37. Bogg J, Cooper C. Job satisfaction, mental health, and occupational stress among senior civil servants. Hum Relat. (1995) 48:327–41. doi: 10.1177/001872679504800306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Judge TA, Piccolo RF, Podsakoff NP, Shaw JC, Rich BL. The relationship between pay and job satisfaction: a meta-analysis of the literature. J Vocat Behav. (2010) 77:157–67. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2010.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Wood S, Van Veldhoven M, Croon M, de Menezes LM. Enriched job design, high involvement management and organizational performance: the mediating roles of job satisfaction and well-being. Hum Relat. (2012) 65:419–45. doi: 10.1177/0018726711432476

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Zacher H, Frese M. Remaining time and opportunities at work: relationships between age, work characteristics, and occupational future time perspective. Psychol Aging. (2009) 24:487–93. doi: 10.1037/a0015425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Zacher H, Frese M. Maintaining a focus on opportunities at work: the interplay between age, job complexity, and the use of selection, optimization, and compensation strategies. J Organ Behav. (2011) 32:291–318. doi: 10.1002/job.683

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Dionisi AM, Barling J, Dupré KE. Revisiting the comparative outcomes of workplace aggression and sexual harassment. J Occup Organ Psychol. (2012) 17:398–408. doi: 10.1037/a0029883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Rejeski WJ, Miller ME, Foy C, Messier S, Rapp S. Self-efficacy and the progression of functional limitations and self-reported disability in older adults with knee pain. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2001) 56:S261–5. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.5.S261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Westerhof GJ, Barrett AE. Age identity and subjective well-being: a comparison of the United States and Germany. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2005) 60:S129–36. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.3.S129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Westerhof GJ, Miche M, Brothers AF, Barrett AE, Diehl M, Montepare JM, et al. The influence of subjective aging on health and longevity: a meta-analysis of longitudinal data. Psychol Aging. (2014) 29:793–802. doi: 10.1037/a0038016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Wurm S, Tomasik MJ, Tesch-Römer C. On the importance of a positive view on ageing for physical exercise among middle-aged and older adults: cross-sectional and longitudinal findings. Psychol Health. (2010) 25:25–42. doi: 10.1080/08870440802311314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Caudroit J, Stephan Y, Chalabaev A, Le Scanff C. Subjective age and social-cognitive determinants of physical activity in active older adults. J Aging Phy Act. (2012) 20:484–96. doi: 10.1123/japa.20.4.484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Wurm S, Tesch-Römer C, Tomasik MJ. Longitudinal findings on aging-related cognitions, control beliefs, and health in later life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2007) 62:156–64. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.3.P156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Bobko NA, Barishpolets AT. Work ability, age and its perception, and other related concerns of Ukraine health care workers. Exp Aging Res. (2002) 28:59–71. doi: 10.1080/036107302753365559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Witt LA, Andrews MC, Carlson DS. When conscientiousness isn't enough: Emotional exhaustion and performance among call center customer service representatives. J Manag. (2004) 30:149–60. doi: 10.1016/j.jm.2003.01.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Brannon D, Miller CJ. What's my age again? The influence of subjective age on consumer health-related attitudes. Health Mark Q. (2019) 36:254–70. doi: 10.1080/07359683.2019.1618011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Stephan Y, Caudroit J, Chalabaev A. Subjective health and memory self-efficacy as mediators in the relation between subjective age and life satisfaction among older adults. Aging Ment Health. (2011) 15:428–36. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2010.536138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Galambos NL, Turner PK, Tilton-Weaver LC. Chronological and subjective age in emerging adulthood: the crossover effect. J Adolesc Res. (2005) 20:538–56. doi: 10.1177/0743558405274876

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression Based Approach. New York, NY: The Guilford Press (2013).

Google Scholar

55. Hirschi A. Callings and work engagement: moderated mediation model of work meaningfulness, occupational identity, and occupational self-efficacy. J Counsel Psychol. (2012) 59:479–85. doi: 10.1037/a0028949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Kreiner GE. Consequences of work-home segmentation or integration: a person environment fit perspective. J Organ Behav. (2006) 27:485–507. doi: 10.1002/job.386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Brislin RW. Translation and content analysis of oral and written materials. Methodology. (1980) 389–444.

Google Scholar

58. Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee JY, Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J Applied Psychol. (2003) 88:879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Abdullah MI, Dechun H, Ali M, Usman M. Ethical leadership and knowledge hiding: a moderated mediation model of relational social capital, and instrumental thinking. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:2403. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Khan MAS, Jianguo D, Ali M, Saleem S, Usman M. Interrelationships between ethical leadership, green psychological climate, and organizational environmental citizenship behavior: the moderating role of gender. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:1977. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Usman M, Javed U, Shoukat A, Bashir NA. Does meaningful work reduce cyberloafing? Important roles of affective commitment and leader-member exchange. Behav Inform Technol. (2019). doi: 10.1080/0144929X.2019.1683607. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. (1999) 6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Macdonald S, Maclntyre P. The generic job satisfaction scale: scale development and its correlates. Empl Assist Q. (1997) 13:1–16. doi: 10.1300/J022v13n02_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Vassos M, Nankervis K, Skerry T, Lante K. Can the job demand-control-(support) model predict disability support worker burnout and work engagement? J Intellect Dev Dis. (2019) 44:139–49. doi: 10.3109/13668250.2017.1310818

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Lin TC, Lin HS, Cheng SF, Wu LM, Ou-Yang MC. Work stress, occupational burnout and depression levels: a clinical study of paediatric intensive care unit nurses in Taiwan. J Clin Nurs. (2016) 25:1120–30. doi: 10.1111/jocn.13119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Bliese PD. Within-group agreement, non-independence, and reliability: implications for data aggregation and analysis. In: Klein KJ, Kozlowski SWJ, editors. Multilevel Theory, Research, and Methods in Organizations: Foundations, Extensions, and New Directions. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass (2000). p. 349–81.

Google Scholar

67. Hair JF, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE. Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th Edn. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall (2010).

Google Scholar

68. Luthans F, Thomas LT. The relationship between age and job satisfaction: curvilinear results from an empirical study–a research note. Pers Rev. (1989) 18:23–6. doi: 10.1108/00483488910133350

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Stephan Y, Chalabaev A, Kotter-Grühn D, Jaconelli A. Feeling younger, being stronger: an experimental study of subjective age and physical functioning among older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2012) 68:1–7. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Montepare JM. Subjective age: toward a guiding lifespan framework. Int J Behav Dev. (2009) 33:42–6. doi: 10.1177/0165025408095551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Eibach RP, Mock SE, Courtney EA. Having a senior moment: Induced aging phenomenology, subjective age, and susceptibility to ageist stereotypes. J Exp Soc Psychol. (2010) 46:643–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Kunze F, Boehm SA, Bruch H. Age diversity, age discrimination climate and performance consequences—a cross organizational study. J Organ Behav. (2011) 32:264–90. doi: 10.1002/job.698

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Kooij DT, de Lange AH, Jansen PG, Dikkers JS. Beyond chronological age. examining perceived future time and subjective health as age-related mediators in relation to work-related motivations and well-being. Work Stress. (2013) 27:88–105. doi: 10.1080/02678373.2013.769328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Khan MAS, Jianguo D, Usman M, Ahmad MI. Moderated mediation model of interrelations between workplace romance, wellbeing, and employee performance. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:2158. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Khan MAS, Du Jianguo AAH, Mushtaq TU, Usman M. Affective commitment foci as parallel mediators of the relationship between workplace romance and employee job performance: a cross-cultural comparison of the people's republic of china and Pakistan. Psychol Res Behav Manag. (2018) 11:267–78. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S168542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: subjective age, chronological age, job satisfaction, job burnout, moderated-mediation

Citation: Anser MK, Ali M, Anwar F and Usman M (2020) Subjective Age and Job Satisfaction: A Moderated Mediation Model of Job Burnout and Chronological Age. Front. Public Health 8:62. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.00062

Received: 28 October 2019; Accepted: 19 February 2020;
Published: 06 March 2020.

Edited by:

Daniel P. Bailey, Brunel University London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Muhammad Aamir Shafique Khan, Jiangsu University, China
Aide Aracely Maldonado-Macías, Universidad Autónoma de Ciudad Juárez, Mexico

Copyright © 2020 Anser, Ali, Anwar and Usman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Muhammad Usman, hmusman10@yahoo.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.