Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Environ. Sci., 06 May 2015
Sec. Interdisciplinary Climate Studies

Effects of alternative framing on the publics perceived importance of environmental conservation

  • 1Program in Science Learning, Rutgers University, The State University of New Jersey, New Brunswick, NJ, USA
  • 2Department of Human Ecology, Rutgers University, The State University of New Jersey, New Brunswick, NJ, USA

Effective communication of science to the general public is important for numerous reasons, including support for policy, funding, informed public decision making, among others. Prior research has found that scientists participating in public policy and public communication must frame their communication efforts in order to connect with audiences. A frame is the mechanism that individuals use to understand and interpret the world around them. Framing can encourage specific interpretations and reference points for a particular issue or event; especially when meaning is negotiated between the media and public audiences. In this study, we looked at the effect of framing within an environmental conservation context. To do this we had survey respondents rank common issues, among them being environmental conservation, from most important to least important for the government to address. We framed environmental conservation using three synonymous terms (environmental security, ecosystem services, and environmental quality) to assess whether there was an effect on rankings dependent on how we framed environmental conservation. We also investigated the effect of individuals' personality characteristics (identity frame) on those environmental conservation rankings. We found that individuals who self-identified as environmentalist were positively associated with ranking highly (most important) environmental conservation when it was framed as either environmental quality or ecosystem services, but not when it was framed as environmental security. Conversely, those individuals who did not rank themselves highly as self-identified environmentalists were positively associated with environmental conservation when it was framed as environmental security. This research suggests that framing audience specific messages can engender audience support in hot-button issues such as environmental conservation and climate change.

Introduction

Communicating science to the public is cited (Bauer, 2008; Nisbet and Scheufele, 2009; Besley and Tanner, 2011) as an important, but potentially difficult, task for many scientists. Without accurate and persuasive conveyance to the public, science can often have little broader value (McNutt, 2013). In important issues such as global climate change, the scientific consensus does not translate to broader public. For example, a review of thousands of refereed scientific publication reports a 97% consensus among scientists of anthropogenic-induced climate change (Cook et al., 2013). Yet, among the American public, only 39% identify as “concerned believers” in climate change (Saad, 2014b) and just 57% of the American public believe human activities are to blame for climate change (Saad, 2014a). Another example area that has recently been a highly disputed public controversy is public health and in particular, the choice to vaccinate. False information published in the late 1990's (Wakefield et al., 1998) concerning an autism-vaccine link has had long lasting impacts on public health communication continuing to this day (Flaherty, 2011). In a recent study on vaccine safety, half of the respondents reported concern for adverse effects and 11.5% refused a recommended vaccine (Freed et al., 2010). These examples make it is clear the importance for the general public to receive and use the correct scientific information given the ubiquitous choices they make about scientific issues, and take subsequent action on a daily basis.

Framing can promote certain interpretations, evaluations, and solutions by emphasizing particular facets of an event or issue (Entman, 2004). Frame theory, initially defined by Goffman (1974), is the mechanism in which people interpret what is going on around them. Frames also help to simplify complex issues by placing greater weight on some considerations and arguments rather than others, showing why an issue might be a problem, who or what might be responsible, and what should be done. Additionally, frames can provide common reference points and meaning between experts, the media, and the public (Groffman et al., 2010). A powerful example of framing was offered by scientist EO Wilson (Wilson, 2006), who partnered with evangelical Christian leaders, in discussing environmental stewardship in terms of morality and ethics. In reframing, they are engaging an audience that might not attend to climate change issues because of the scientific foundation (Groffman et al., 2010). In the context of this paper, we define framing as certain written word constructs that may change or influence interpretation of information.

While the success of public uptake and interaction with scientific information can be based on a number of different variables such as: public trust (Haerlin and Parr, 1999), personal interaction (Kempe et al., 2011; Silvertown et al., 2011), attitudes (Riddiough et al., 1981), and awareness (Littledyke, 2008); how scientists frame science to public in their communication is equally as important. Academics and professionals have long acknowledged framing as a powerful tool and asset when addressing issues within the sciences and to the public at large (Levin et al., 1998; Wiederhold, 2011). Research on framing effects have found that scientists participating in public policy and public communication must frame their communication efforts to connect with audiences (Nisbet et al., 2003; Nisbet and Huge, 2006; Nisbet and Mooney, 2009).

Considering the power and influence of frames in communication, capitalizing on framing as a common practice for science communication is relatively unseen. Indeed, Nisbet and Mooney (2009) detailed the influence and effectiveness of frames: from motivation; influencing behavior; garner support for issues; finding common ground; and defining issues.

Additionally, identity frames can impact decision-making and interpretations of information. Identity frames can classically be defined as a cognitive framework or scheme of the characteristics belonging to individuals, or categories of individuals, we identify within and from our social experiences (Abrams and Hogg, 1987; Guichard, 2001). For example, in a study on the effect of identity on European Union officials, researchers found that personal identity associated with home-country affiliation affected member political actions and beliefs while participating in inter-governmental policy development (Egeberg, 1999). These personal identity frames can have important implications for decision making by the public on particular divisive or emotionally charged issues such as environmental protections and climate change.

In this paper we seek to address the issue of framing in science communication from an environmental context. Particularly, we focus on the effect of framing environmental conservation in terms of perceived importance by the public. We also seek to investigate the potential connection between individuals' identity frames of common conservation terms within an environmental context.

Methods

To assess the effect of framing of environmental issues on individuals' perceived importance of these issues, we generated items on various aspects of environmental issues and identity as a part of a broader survey on local greenspaces, environmental beliefs, environmental knowledge, views of the nature of science, and personality factors. These 30 survey items were vetted through focus groups for internal consistency and validity. Environmental knowledge survey items were taken from The National Environmental Education and Training Foundation (NEETF) and Roper Starch Worldwide (1999). This metric is widely known and quite robust to allow us to compare our results to other studies. Survey items for this paper were composed of five-point Likert-scale and dichotomous choice (see Appendix—Supplementary Material, for survey items).

To assess framing in an environmental communication context we generated a set of issue ranking items. Participants were asked to rank 10 issues from 1 to 10 of importance for the American public to address or solve, where a ranking of 1 was most important and a ranking of 10 was least important. Issues could not share rankings of importance, thus individuals' had to prioritize some issues over others. Seven of the 10 issues participants had to rank were: curing cancer; improving quality of US education; decreasing crime and drug use; reducing health care costs; decreasing poverty; growing the US economy; and reducing the budget deficit. Three of the issues were terms that are used synonymously in popular and refereed environmental literature: improving environmental quality; improving environmental security; and improving ecosystem services. Of these 10 issues, we were interested in the potential differential ranking of environmental quality, environmental security, and environmental services. These three terms, in the context of improving or garnering support for conservation of the natural environment, have the potential to confer differing levels of importance to the public. Thus, framing, is this context, could be impacting scientific communication and public decision-making in a way we do not yet know.

Individuals living in six townships near a public university in New Jersey were mailed paper surveys with pre-stamped return envelopes. Those individuals were chosen by random sampling the online white pages listing for the six focal townships. Seventy-five surveys were sent to each township, totaling to 450 surveys sent but given that our method relied on dated addresses, we were not surprised to find that a number of surveys were returned unopened. This meant our effort was 380 surveys making our response rate 19.5%. There were 74 completed returns fairly evenly distributed across the townships. All surveys were kept anonymous and no identifying information was asked of participants.

All statistical analyses were performed in Minitab 17 Statistical Software (2010) package. A Principal Components Analysis (PCA) using a correlation matrix was used to inspect associations among Likert self-report being an environmentalist, having environmental knowledge and rank of terminology (i.e., environmental security, environmental, quality, and environmental protection). The purpose of our study was to look for emerging associations and not to test hypotheses. Therefore, for this research note, we chose to use PCA to inspect for preliminary associations only.

Results

Of the three terms, environmental quality was ranked the most important by participants with an average ranking of 4.730 out of a possible 10 points. Environmental security was ranked second with an average ranking of 6.290 out of a possible 10 points. Environmental services was ranked as the least important with an average ranking of 7.435 out of a possible 10 points.

We constrained our PCA to three components given that we had only five variables. These three components explained 77% of the variance in our dataset. To inspect for associations, we used loadings over ±0.300 as a measure of weight of the construct on that component (see Table 1). For PC 1 (principal component), we found that labeling one's self as an environmentalist is positively associated with ranking highly environmental quality and ecosystem services. This was not the case, however, for environmental security. Environmental security and environmental knowledge were associated along PC 2 with the other variables not loading highly. On PC3, self-reported environmentalist and ecosystem services were associated and negatively associated with environmental security.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Principal component analysis (PCA) for the first three components.

Discussion

It is clear from this research, the impact of how we communicate scientific concepts extends beyond what was previously thought. Indeed, individuals who self-identified as environmentalists were more likely to associate and respond strongly with words like service and quality, whereas those individuals who did not identify as environmentalists may find these words equally or less appealing than the word security. This finding can have definite impacts for scientists as they communicate issues within the public forum to various groups of people, particularly those people who are divided on their personal and political values. Schuldt et al. (2011) found that individuals who identified as Republicans were significantly affected by question wording, and subsequently were less likely to endorse global warming than when it was framed as climate change, where as Democrats were equally responsive to both terms.

Successful science communication is not only critical for a supportive, literate, and engaged public, but key to success of future generations (Durant et al., 1989; Ziman, 1991; Cajas, 2001; Slovic et al., 2013). Communicating important scientific concepts to the public at large has proven to be a messy and complicated challenge with varying levels of success. This disconnect between scientific findings and public opinion in the context of climate change clearly demonstrates this. While science communication is a complex issue, frames play an important role. In a recent study from the United Kingdom, researchers found that shifting climate action discussions from a negative frame (possibilities of losses) to a positive frame (possibilities of losses not materializing), participants had stronger environmental behavior intentions despite high uncertainty conditions (Morton et al., 2011). The potential to overcome uncertainty in public discussions of climate change is undeniably important.

The implications of this research suggests that how scientists frame their communications in an environmental context to the public, and particular audiences within the public, can have the potential to influence person decision making and policy. Further, similar findings in previous work on climate change found reframing climate change as a public health issue engendered greater response from participants (Maibach et al., 2010). Additionally, Bain et al. (2012) found that framing climate change action to climate change deniers as increasing consideration for one another or improving technological/economic development, those individuals were more supportive of pro-environmental actions. This re-framing from avoidance of environmental harm to improvement of society motivated individuals traditionally left out of the climate change action conversation.

A simple difference in framing environmental protection and conservation three different ways (security, quality, and services) seemed to significantly affect the importance of addressing the issue for the participants. Additionally, identity frames seem to also play an important role in participant decision-making. Within this work we see distinct separation in the rankings of importance between those individuals who self-identify as environmentalists and those who do not. Individuals who most identify as environmentalist seem to respond more positively to environmental conservation in terms of protecting quality for its own sake. Whereas those individuals who least identified as environmentalists seem to respond more positively to protecting the environment when it was framed as a matter of national security. The impact of these identity frames in participant responses, and the associated implications on decision-making, highlights the additional complexity when developing successful communication strategies. This suggests awareness of key audience personality characteristics could help tailor messages about important scientific issues to make those audiences more receptive, as supported by various studies work (Wilson, 2006; Groffman et al., 2010).

Further work is needed to investigate framing phenomenon in environmental communication on a broader scale. Concurrently, particular predictive linkages between personality and demographic characteristics should be investigated further. Numerous studies have highlighted the connection between personality characteristics and political leaning (e.g., Lewis and Bates, 2011; Verhulst et al., 2012). These links between political affiliations and personal and policy decisions have been well-documented, which have broad implications for issues such as climate change. A recent study on predictive characteristics of climate change deniers found that, while controlling for political leaning, race, and gender, being a conservative white male was significantly linked to indicators of climate change denial (McCright and Dunlap, 2011).

Additionally, work from personality psychology shows that personality types of Ph.D climate change scientists and that of the general public in the U.S. differ greatly in core components. Using the Myers-Brigg Type Indicator personality test, Weiler et al. (2012) found that climate scientists were significantly more likely to prefer sensing over intuition while taking in new information around them, and judging over perceiving while dealing with their surroundings than the general public. Thus, these demographic characteristics that have the potential for predictive power in audience decision-making can greatly help scientists frame communication efforts appropriately.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

All human subjects work was approved by the Rutgers University Internal Review Board Approval # 13-390.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fenvs.2015.00036/abstract

References

Abrams, D., and Hogg, M. A. (1987). Language attitudes frames of reference and social identity: a Scottish dimension. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 6, 201–213. doi: 10.1177/0261927X8763004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bain, P. G., Hornsey, M. J., Bongiorno, R., and Jeffries, C. (2012). Promoting pro-environmental action in climate change deniers. Nat. Clim. Change 2, 600–603. doi: 10.1038/nclimate1532

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, M. W. (2008). Paradigm Change for Science Communication: Commercial Science Needs a Critical Public. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Besley, J. C., and Tanner, A. H. (2011). What science communication scholars think about training scientists to communicate. Sci. Commun. 33, 239–263. doi: 10.1177/1075547010386972

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cajas, F. (2001). The science/technology interaction: implications for science literacy. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 38, 715–729. doi: 10.1002/tea.1028

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, J., Nuccitelli, D., Green, S. A., Richardson, M., Winkler, B., Painting, R., et al. (2013). Quantifying the consensus on anthropogenic global warming in the scientific literature. Environ. Res. Lett. 8:024024. doi: 10.1088/1748-9326/8/2/024024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durant, J. R., Evans, G. A., and Thomas, G. P. (1989). The public understanding of science. Nature 340, 11–14. doi: 10.1038/340011a0

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egeberg, M. (1999). Transcending intergovernmentalism? Identity and role perceptions of national officials in EU decision-making. J. Eur. Public Policy 6, 456–474. doi: 10.1080/135017699343621

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Entman, R. M. (2004). Projections of Power: Framing News, Public Opinion, and US Foreign Policy. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar

Flaherty, D. K. (2011). The vaccine-autism connection: a public health crisis caused by unethical medical practices and fraudulent science. Ann. Pharmacother. 45, 1302–1304. doi: 10.1345/aph.1Q318

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freed, G. L., Clark, S. J., Butchart, A. T., Singer, D. C., and Davis, M. M. (2010). Parental vaccine safety concerns in 2009. Pediatrics 125, 654–659. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-1962

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goffman, E. (1974). Frame Analysis: An Essay on the Organization of Experience. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Groffman, P. M., Stylinski, C., Nisbet, M. C., Duarte, C. M., Jordan, R., Burgin, A., et al. (2010). Restarting the conversation: challenges at the interface between ecology and society. Front. Ecol. Environ. 8, 284–291. doi: 10.1890/090160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guichard, J. (2001). “Adolescents' scholastic fields identity frames and future projects,” in Navigating Through Adolescence: European Perspectives, Chapter 11, ed J.-E. Nurmi (New York, NY: RoutledgeFalmer), 275–302.

Google Scholar

Haerlin, B., and Parr, D. (1999). How to restore public trust in science. Nature 400, 499–499. doi: 10.1038/22867

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kempe, A., Daley, M. F., McCauley, M. M., Crane, L. A., Suh, C. A., Kennedy, A. M., et al. (2011). Prevalence of parental concerns about childhood vaccines: the experience of primary care physicians. Am. J. Prev. Med. 40, 548–555. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2010.12.025

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., and Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: a typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 76, 149–188. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1998.2804

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, G. J., and Bates, T. C. (2011). From left to right: how the personality system allows basic traits to influence politics via characteristic moral adaptations. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 546–558. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02016.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Littledyke, M. (2008). Science education for environmental awareness: approaches to integrating cognitive and affective domains. Environ. Educ. Res. 14, 1–17. doi: 10.1080/13504620701843301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maibach, E. W., Nisbet, M., Baldwin, P., Akerlof, K., and Diao, G. (2010). Reframing climate change as a public health issue: an exploratory study of public relations. BMC Public Health 10:299. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-299

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCright, A. M., and Dunlap, R. E. (2011). Cool dudes: the denial of climate change among conservative white males in the United States. Global Environ. Change 21, 1163–1172. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2011.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNutt, M. (2013). Improving scientific communication. Science 342, 13. doi: 10.1126/science.1246449

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minitab 17 Statistical Software. (2010). [Computer Software]. State College, PA: Minitab Inc.

Morton, T. A., Rabinovich, A., Marshall, D., and Bretschneider, P. (2011). The future that may (or may not) come: how framing changes responses to uncertainty in climate change communications. Global Environ. Change 21, 103–109. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2010.09.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbet, M. C., Brossard, D., and Kroepsch, A. (2003). Framing science the stem cell controversy in an age of press/politics. Int. J. Press/Polit. 8, 36–70. doi: 10.1177/1081180X02251047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbet, M. C., and Huge, M. (2006). Attention cycles and frames in the plant biotechnology debate managing power and participation through the press/policy connection. Harv. Int. J. Press/Polit. 11, 3–40. doi: 10.1177/1081180X06286701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisbet, M. C., and Mooney, C. (2009). Science and Society: framing science. Science 316, 56. doi: 10.1126/science.1142030

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nisbet, M. C., and Scheufele, D. A. (2009). What's next for science communication? Promising directions and lingering distractions. Am. J. Bot. 96, 1767–1778. doi: 10.3732/ajb.0900041

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riddiough, M. A., Willems, J. S., Sanders, C. R., and Kemp, K. (1981). Factors affecting the use of vaccines: considerations for immunization program planners. Public Health Rep. 96:528.

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | Google Scholar

Saad, L. (2014a). A Steady 57% in U.S. Blame Humans for Global Warming. Gallup Poll. Available online at: http://www.gallup.com/poll/167972/steady-blame-humans-global-warming.aspx

Saad, L. (2014b). One in Four in U.S. are Solidly Skeptical of Global Warming. Gallup Poll. Available online at: http://www.gallup.com/poll/168620/one-four-solidly-skeptical-global-warming.aspx

Schuldt, J. P., Konrath, S. H., and Schwarz, R. (2011). “Global warming” or “climate change”? Whether the planet is warming depends on the question wording. Public Opin. Q. 75, 115–124. doi: 10.1093/poq/nfq073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvertown, J., Cook, L., Cameron, R., Dodd, M., McConway, K., Worthington, J., et al. (2011). Citizen science reveals unexpected continental-scale evolutionary change in a model organism. PLoS ONE. 6:e18927. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018927

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slovic, P., Flynn, J., and Kunreuther, H. (2013). (eds.). Risk Media and Stigma: Understanding Public Challenges to Modern Science and Technology. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

The National Environmental Education and Training Foundation (NEETF) and Roper Starch Worldwide. (1999). Environmental readiness for the 21st century: the eighth annual national report card on environmental attitudes knowledge and behavior. Washington, DC: The National Environmental Education and Training Foundation (NEETF) and Roper Starch Worldwide.

Verhulst, B., Eaves, L. J., and Hatemi, P. K. (2012). Correlation not causation: the relationship between personality traits and political ideologies. Am. J. Pol. Sci. 56, 34–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2011.00568.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wakefield, A. J., Murch, S. H., Anthony, A., Linnell, J., Casson, D. M., Malik, M., et al. (1998). Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia non-specific colitis and pervasive developmental disorder in children. Lancet 351, 637–641. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)11096-0

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiler, C. S., Keller, J. K., and Olex, C. (2012). Personality type differences between Ph. D. climate researchers and the general public: implications for effective communication. Clim. Change 112, 233–242. doi: 10.1007/s10584-011-0205-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiederhold, B. K. (2011). Citizen scientists generate benefits for researchers, educators, society, and themselves. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 14, 703–704. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2011.1534

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, E. O. (2006). The Creation: An Appeal to Save Life on Earth. New York, NY: Norton.

Google Scholar

Ziman, J. (1991). Public understanding of science. Sci. Technol. Hum. Val. 16, 99–105. doi: 10.1177/016224399101600106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: framing, conservation, identity frames, environmental issues, communication

Citation: Sorensen AE, Clark D and Jordan RC (2015) Effects of alternative framing on the publics perceived importance of environmental conservation. Front. Environ. Sci. 3:36. doi: 10.3389/fenvs.2015.00036

Received: 23 January 2015; Accepted: 23 April 2015;
Published: 06 May 2015.

Edited by:

Veerasamy Sejian, Indian Council of Agricultural Research, India

Reviewed by:

Venkata Krishna Jandhyala, The University of Western Ontario, Canada
E. Christien Michael Parsons, George Mason University, USA

Copyright © 2015 Sorensen, Clark and Jordan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Amanda E. Sorensen, Program in Science Learning, Rutgers University, The State University of New Jersey, Waller Hall 108, 59 Lipman Drive, New Brunswick, NJ 08901, USA, amasoren@rutgers.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.