Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Pediatr., 23 February 2015
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic New treatment perspectives in autism spectrum disorders View all 17 articles

Autism as a disorder of biological and behavioral rhythms: toward new therapeutic perspectives

\r\n      Sylvie Tordjman,*Sylvie Tordjman1,2*Katherine S. DavlantisKatherine S. Davlantis3Nicolas GeorgieffNicolas Georgieff4Marie-Maude GeoffrayMarie-Maude Geoffray4Mario SperanzaMario Speranza5George M. AndersonGeorge M. Anderson6Jean XavierJean Xavier7Michel Botbol,Michel Botbol1,8Ccile OriolCécile Oriol2Eric Bellissant,Eric Bellissant9,10Julie Vernay-LeconteJulie Vernay-Leconte11Claire Fougerou,Claire Fougerou9,10Anne Hespel,Anne Hespel9,10Aude Tavenard,Aude Tavenard9,10David CohenDavid Cohen7Solenn Kermarrec,Solenn Kermarrec1,2Nathalie CoulonNathalie Coulon1Olivier BonnotOlivier Bonnot1Geraldine Dawson\r\n   Geraldine Dawson3
  • 1Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes, CNRS, UMR 8158, Paris, France
  • 2Pôle Hospitalo-Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent (PHUPEA), Centre Hospitalier Guillaume Régnier, Université de Rennes 1, Rennes, France
  • 3Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, NC, USA
  • 4Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Centre Hospitalier Le Vinatier, Lyon, France
  • 5Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Centre Hospitalier de Versailles, Université de Versailles Saint-Quentin-en-Yvelines, Le Chensay, France
  • 6Child Study Center, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
  • 7Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Assistance Publique – Hôpitaux de Paris, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière, CNRS FRE 2987, University Pierre and Marie Curie, Paris, France
  • 8Service Hospitalo-Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent, CHU de Brest, Université de Bretagne Occidentale, Brest, France
  • 9Clinical Investigation Center, INSERM CIC 0203, University Hospital, Rennes 1 University, Rennes, France
  • 10Department of Clinical Pharmacology, University Hospital, Rennes 1 University, Rennes, France
  • 11Pôle Hospitalo-Universitaire de Psychiatrie Adulte (PHUPA), Centre Hospitalier Guillaume Régnier, Université de Rennes 1, Rennes, France

There is a growing interest in the role of biological and behavioral rhythms in typical and atypical development. Recent studies in cognitive and developmental psychology have highlighted the importance of rhythmicity and synchrony of motor, emotional, and interpersonal rhythms in early development of social communication. The synchronization of rhythms allows tuning and adaptation to the external environment. The role of melatonin in the ontogenetic establishment of circadian rhythms and the synchronization of the circadian clocks network suggests that this hormone might be also involved in the synchrony of motor, emotional, and interpersonal rhythms. Autism provides a challenging model of physiological and behavioral rhythm disturbances and their possible effects on the development of social communication impairments and repetitive behaviors and interests. This article situates autism as a disorder of biological and behavioral rhythms and reviews the recent literature on the role of rhythmicity and synchrony of rhythms in child development. Finally, the hypothesis is developed that an integrated approach focusing on biological, motor, emotional, and interpersonal rhythms may open interesting therapeutic perspectives for children with autism. More specifically, promising avenues are discussed for potential therapeutic benefits in autism spectrum disorder of melatonin combined with developmental behavioral interventions that emphasize synchrony, such as the Early Start Denver Model.

Introduction

Endogenous physiological variations involved in biological rhythms reflect adaptation to the environment. Thus, the sleep–wake rhythm associated with biological circadian rhythms can be viewed as an adaptation to the day–night cycle. Circadian rhythms allow temporal organization of biological functions in relation to environmental changes (1). The periodicity of activities applies to all biological, physiological, and psychological functions; recently, the science of biological rhythms, chronobiology, has emerged with its own theory, science, and education (2).

Furthermore, recent studies in the field of cognitive and developmental psychology have highlighted the importance of rhythmicity and synchrony of motor, emotional, and relational rhythms in early development of social communication. Given the major role of the sleep hormone melatonin in the ontogenetic establishment of diurnal rhythms, the synchronization of peripheral oscillators (also termed clocks) and the regulation of human circadian rhythms (1), melatonin might be involved in the synchrony of motor, emotional, and relational rhythms.

Indeed, relationships might exist, based on the hypothesis of ergodicity (3), between cellular communication networks involving a cellular synchrony (synchronization of cellular oscillations by melatonin) and early social communication development involving a synchrony of motor, emotional, and interpersonal rhythms. Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) – a developmental disorder characterized by social communication impairments associated with repetitive interests and behaviors – provides an interesting and challenging model of abnormal melatonin production in early developmental disorders and its possible relationship with autistic behavioral impairments.

This article proposes a central role of rhythmicity and synchrony of rhythms in typical child development and offers a new integrative approach, which considers autism as a disorder of biological and behavioral rhythms. In this perspective, promising avenues will be discussed in this article for potential therapeutic benefits in ASD of melatonin and developmental behavioral interventions that emphasize rhythms and synchronization, such as the Early Start Denver Model (ESDM).

Physiological and Behavioral Rhythm Disturbances in Autism

Autism as a Disorder of Biological Rhythms

Alterations in circadian sleep–wake rhythm have frequently been reported in autism (4, 5). More specifically, reduced total sleep and longer sleep latency as well as nocturnal and early morning awakenings are often observed in individuals with ASD (612). Furthermore, prior studies on melatonin in autism have all reported abnormalities in melatonin secretion (see Table 1). In addition, abnormalities in cortisol circadian rhythm have also been reported in autism [for a review, see Tordjman et al. (13)]. In particular, significantly higher frequency of absence of circadian variation in melatonin and cortisol levels was observed in individuals with autism compared to typically developing controls. Golombek et al. (14) described the effects of circadian desynchronization that can enhance susceptibility to certain disorders (metabolic, immune, cognitive, and somatic disorders including cancer). It is noteworthy that congenitally blind children with consequently abnormal melatonin secretion and synchronization [the production of pineal-derived melatonin depends on the light acting through the retinohypothalamic tract (15)] very frequently display autism [up to 42% (16)], whereas hearing impaired children, including hearing loss, show autism less frequently [up to 10% (17)]. More specifically, abnormally low daytime and nighttime melatonin secretion was associated with an absence of melatonin circadian variation in some individuals with autism (18, 19), which in turn, given the role of synchronizer of melatonin, also has consequences on the circadian rhythms network, including the cortisol circadian rhythm (13, 20).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Studies of melatonin levels in individuals with autism.

This blunted circadian rhythmicity with no or little variability might be related to the difficulties in adapting to changes typically observed in individuals with autism. Thus, children with autism who are confronted with physiological continuity due to absent circadian rhythms may have difficulties adapting to changes in either their external or their internal environment (26). Indeed, as previously underlined, the circadian clocks network, synchronized by melatonin and involving an internal system of continuity/discontinuity, allows adaptation to environmental changes.

Similarly, blunted circadian rhythmicity may explain the difficulty observed in many children with autism in adapting to changes in rhythms of their external and internal environment. Thus, rapid rhythms of sensory stimuli in their external environment (e.g., repeated visual stimuli provided by a stroboscopic light) can provoke epileptic seizure in some individuals with autism [approximately one-third of children with autism have epilepsy (27)]. Interestingly, EEG changes tended to be correlated with an abnormal rhythm of melatonin in young adults with autism (22). Furthermore, some parents have reported that their daughters with autism experience epileptic seizures toward the 14th day of their menstrual cycle, which is when luteinizing hormone (LH) levels peak (Tordjman, personal communication), suggesting quite tentatively that individuals with autism may have difficulties adapting to periodic hormonal changes in their internal environment. We can state the following hypothesis: a change in rhythm associated with excessive environmental stimuli might strongly increase arousal and lead to physiological stress which, for some individuals with autism, can disturb the rhythmic activity of a particular brain area, leading it to fall out of sync with the rest of the brain and causing its population of neurons to fire (depolarization and occurrence of an epileptic seizure). This underlines the importance of stable physiological rhythms.

Finally, significant relationships have been found between lower nocturnal melatonin excretion and increased severity of autistic social communication impairments, especially for verbal communication and social imitative play (18, 23). These findings are in agreement with studies suggesting an association between reduced melatonin production and language impairment (22, 28). Along the same line, the systemic administration in the animal model of Zebra Finch of a melatonin-1B receptor antagonist at the beginning of the night shortens the song and motif length and affects the song syllable lengths produced the next day (29). Reduced melatonin activity might create timing problems in biological clocks with physiological and psychological effects that might be, according to Boucher’s model of autism (30) and Wimpory’s theory (31), involved in autistic impairments, notably in autistic social communication impairments. It is noteworthy that deficiency in oxytocin [oxytocin is considered a bonding hormone (32, 33)] has also been reported in autism (34, 35) and bonding is involved in the development of very early social interaction in infants. Interestingly, the release of oxytocin by the posterior pituitary gland follows a robust circadian rhythm in mammals. Further studies are needed to better understand the underlying mechanisms of oxytocin anomalies in autism and to explore, in particular, possible oxytocin rhythm disturbances in ASD. The importance of the synchrony of rhythms in the development of social interaction and communication is detailed in the next section.

Importance of Synchrony of Rhythms for Social Communication in Typical Development and Autism Spectrum Disorder

Several studies, based on animal models and human perinatal development, suggest that stable patterns of repeated stimuli in the form of maternal physiological rhythms, involving cross-modal perception such as regular cardiac rhythm, which provides the fetus with auditory and vibratory stimuli, allow the fetus to integrate sensory information facilitating prenatal perceptual learning and develop a coherent representation of his or her internal and external environment (3638). Fluctuations in the physiological rhythms (variants), such as variations in the maternal cardiac rhythm and also variations in hormone levels involved in the circadian rhythms that are already present during the fetus life (the fetus’ circadian rhythms are the mother’s ones), occurring in a background of regular repetition of identical sequences (invariants), may help the fetus to develop the ability to adapt to change in an environment characterized by high regularity. As previously emphasized (38), very early mother–infant relations provide a secure environment based on the repetition of invariants, while at the same time promoting adaptation to change through the presence of variants. It is through the regular repetition of identical sequences of discontinuity, such as circadian rhythms, that a continuum is constructed associated with the development of adaptation to changes.

The development of the very earliest form of communication relies on the sharing of emotions between mother and infant, when, for example, the infant is suckling in his or her mother’s arms, through emotional synchrony that enhances the integration of sensory inputs (39, 40). Concerning memory processes, emotions enable to “fix” events, just as a photographic fixing agent sets images (38). Cortisol (a stress and arousal neurohormone) crosses the placental barrier. The cortisol circadian rhythm, as well as the melatonin circadian rhythm in the fetus and infant after birth, are those of maternal cortisol and melatonin. Indeed, the infant’s circadian cortisol and melatonin rhythms are only established between 2 and 3 months of age in typical development, at the same time that infants begin to have more regular sleep–wake cycles associated with nighttime sleep lasting 6–8 h (41, 42). Interestingly, this period coincides in typical development with the emergence of social smiling by the second month of life (43), the advent of mirror self-recognition at around 3 months of age (44), and increased brain activation to speech occurring between 3 and 4 months of age (45).

At birth, the human immaturity of the cerebral cortex allows initial learning to influence the neural architecture through perceptual-action mapping (46, 47). The infant’s social skills, especially including imitation, shared attention, and empathic understanding, also contribute to the development of learning (46, 48). Social synchrony can be defined as the dynamic and reciprocal adaptation of the temporal structure of behaviors between interactive partners (49). In typically developing children, the quality of social interaction depends on an active dialog between the parent and the infant (50, 51). Numerous studies have been emphasizing the importance of parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing in social communication development (52).

Also, biological markers were associated with relational synchrony. First, oxytocin administration to parent enhances infant physiological and behavioral readiness for social engagement and parallels an oxytocin increase in infants (32). Second, neural correlates were found using hyper-scanning recordings of EEG brain activity and measures of neural synchronization between distant brain regions of interacting individuals through a free imitation task (53). Dumas and colleagues’ study (53) was the first to record dual EEG activity in dyads of subjects during spontaneous non-verbal interaction. Eleven same-sex pairs were scanned. They found that interpersonal rhythmic oscillations were correlated with the emergence of synchronization in the brain’s alpha–mu band between the right centro-parietal regions (an area involved in social interaction) of both participants. Developmental psychologists now study interaction not only as the addition of two behaviors but also as a global phenomenon in which synchrony is considered as social per se. To describe the dialog between two partners engaged in behavioral and affective exchange, developmental psychologists more and more take into consideration rhythm and temporal course of both behavior and affect, regarded as key expressions of adaptation during interaction (49, 52).

Only a few studies have addressed the importance of infant–caregiver synchrony/reciprocity in the development of social communication involving infants who subsequently are diagnosed with autism. It appears appropriate in the field of autism to consider the combined domain of social communication, as the most recent version of the ADOS scale does, as well as the recently released DSM-5 American classification. Methods to investigate this issue include studies using early home videos (54), parental interviews focusing on early abnormalities (55), and prospective assessment of children at risk of ASD (e.g., siblings) (56, 57). Studies have revealed a pervasive developmental course in infants who were later diagnosed with ASD. Thus, the first signs were abnormalities in eye contact, imitation, disengagement, joint attention, orienting to name, and body language. These behaviors are parts of the affective tuning disturbances; the term affective tuning, defined by Stern (58) as the “execution of behaviors expressing the emotional ownership of a shared affective state,” refers to infant–caregiver emotional communication based on rhythmic similarity from the second semester of life onward. Also, these behaviors are important precursors of later-developing symptoms. However, whether these first signs impair early infant–parent interactions and whether they reflect already autistic behavioral impairments in the infant remain to be explored. In two related studies based on home movies of children later diagnosed with autism, Saint-Georges et al. (54) and Cohen et al. (59) showed that motor and emotional asynchrony was present between infants and parents before 12 months of age, and parents perceived weaker initiatives from their children. In addition, parents experienced weaker interactive responsiveness from their children and increasingly tried to compensate this perceived deficit by soliciting behaviors through touching the infant. This was particularly observed after 12 months of age for the fathers of infants who were later diagnosed with autism.

Many authors have studied imitation in children with autism (imitation of other people’s faces, gestures, or vocal signals) in order to better understand the development of autistic social communication impairments. This specific type of imitation is referred to as “spatial” imitation to highlight the capacity to produce an instantaneous copy of the form of the signal. However, another way to communicate with others is to perform a “temporal” imitation of their behavior (60). This is what humans do through rhythmic finger or foot taping, dancing, singing, and drumming in synchrony with others (61). Xavier et al. (62) highlighted the importance of rhythmicity and synchrony in the development of children’s imitative exchanges with peers. From birth, a child has a predisposition to engage, intersubjectively, with the rhytmic actions and awareness of other persons, and to move in synchrony with them (44, 63). Synchronic imitation is an important preverbal way to communicate among peers (64, 65). This reciprocal experience concerns two children able and motivated to coordinate their behavior with the non-ritualized behavior of the other, in both form and timing and to alternate turns between model and imitator (66). The impression of fluidity in the coordination of movements between partners is underlined by mutual attention, engagement, continuous adaptation, and turn taking (67). This rhythmic process made of ludic spontaneous imitation reveals moments of discontinuity occurring in a background of continuity. Neural bases of this coupling activity are constituted by the neuron mirrors system (68), with the same evidence showing that the neuronal structures involved when a mental state is experienced, are also recruited during the observation of others. Guionnet et al. (69) designed a free imitation paradigm in an fMRI study to examine some neural correlates of social interaction. Their results agree with those previously evidenced (70, 71) concerning the core circuit of imitation, but they found different activations between the situation of imitating and that of being imitated.

A special quality of temporal imitation is the ability to use different motor movements in order to communicate. Thus, simple finger tapping can be synchronized with another’s head nodding or trunk movements, whether entrained by one of the movements or in response to external synchronizing stimuli such as music. Although animals and humans can perceive rhythms and produce rhythmic motor patterns, only humans can adapt their rhythmic movements to external rhythms (72) [with the exception of the cockatoo (73)]. The ability to be rhythmically synchronized with the environment appears important for infant development in the emotional, cognitive, social, and sensorimotor realms (44, 74). It has been demonstrated that the human fetus and newborn already have the capacity to perceive and produce rhythms (75). The ability to produce temporally adapted motor patterns comes later and depends on the specific motor system involved and the relationship between the beat presented and the spontaneously occurring motor tempo of the infant (76, 77). It should be fruitful to longitudinally examine children with autism in terms of ability to adapt their own rhythm to external rhythms. Interestingly, clinical observations suggest that some children with autism are able to respond to an external rhythmic vocalization by a similar rhythmic motor pattern such as hand flapping (Tordjman, personal communication). However, previous studies reported disorganized rhythms, stereotypies, and poor synchrony in most of these children (78), which might be related to the low melatonin levels reported to be associated in autism with the severity of verbal communication and social imitative play impairments (18, 23). Melatonin, as a regulator of physiological rhythms and oscillations, might enhance the capacity of children with ASD to synchronize their movements with movements of others (this synchronization of movements is needed for imitative play) and with external rhythmic auditory stimuli (such as music and/or human voice enhancing their verbal skills). Interestingly, in a study of social smiling in infants (79), there was no difference in frequency of smiling between 2- and 5-month-old infants with and without ASD during infant–caregiver face-to-face interactions. However, whereas typically developing infants showed a significant increase in smiling rate when caregivers were smiling, smiling in infants later diagnosed with ASD was not synchronized with smiling in caregivers and was not contingent upon caregiver behavior (caregiver facial expressions and vocalizations). Furthermore, it is noteworthy that, as previously indicated in this section, the infant’s melatonin circadian rhythm is established between 2 and 3 months of age, and a study (80) reported that eye contact was normal in 2-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism but declined between 2 and 6 months of age, suggesting an additional argument in favor of a possible relationship between the well-replicated nocturnal melatonin deficit in autism (see Table 1) and the development of autistic social communication impairments.

Repetitive Behaviors and Interests

Repetitive behaviors and interests are defined, according to DSM-5 criteria (81), as a repetition of identical sequences of behaviors for motor stereotypies (motor stereotypies involve repetitive maladaptive movements) or thoughts for restrictive patterns of interests (restrictive and repetitive interests involve fixated interests, adherence to routines, or ritualized and rigid thinking patterns). Thus, repetitive behaviors and interests can be viewed as behavioral responses to the need to create discontinuity that is repeated at regular intervals, which could have been fundamentally lacking in the physiological development of children with autism due to the melatonin deficit reported in autism. Our finding (18) observed in a sample of 43 adolescents and young adults with autism (nocturnal excretion of 6-SM was significantly negatively correlated with repetitive use of objects), taken together with improvement of stereotyped behaviors following administration of melatonin in 24 children and adolescents with ASD (82), supports this hypothesis.

The autistic deficit in melatonin secretion might lead to physiological rhythm disturbances in autism impairing biological circadian rhythms and even, in certain cases, to an “endless” physiological continuity provoked by the absence of variation in melatonin levels. From this perspective, stereotyped behaviors and interests can be seen as offering to children with autism rhythmic forms providing rhythmicity and discontinuity through the creation of repeated identical patterns. Albert Goldbeter (83), director of the Chronobiology Unit at the Brussels Sciences University, underlines that life is rhythm. It is noteworthy that the word pineal comes from pine cone that is a Mesopotamian symbol representing the source of life and the power of regeneration. In Asia, the pineal gland is considered as the 7th chakra and represents the location of the soul and the highest level of consciousness.

Donald Winnicott (84) emphasized that “the main problem for a typically developing child is to be able to create a continuum out of discontinuity.” According to him, the first optimal container (“holding environment”) for the newborn is the progressive internalization of the rhythmic structures of feeding (rhythmic ebb and flow corresponding to the kinesthetic experience of suckling), providing a sense of continuing existence. We can nevertheless hypothesize, as we did in the previous section and in a prior article (26), that this internalization process starts far earlier, in the womb, through maternal physiological rhythms; conversely, based on clinical observations in autism, children with autism create discontinuity out of continuity. We can also state the hypothesis that children with autism, confronted with an “endless” physiological continuity (or at least with an absence of physiological discontinuities repeated at regular intervals), would develop anxiety and stress due to the lack of secure stable rhythms, and would therefore try to create by themselves rhythmicity in order to control them through stereotyped behaviors. However, the relationships between stereotyped behaviors and stress responses or anxiety need to be clarified given discrepant results reported in some studies (85, 86).

Finally, it should be noted that several studies have also recently opened new behavioral, neurobiological, and pharmacological perspectives on autistic repetitive behaviors, especially on self-injurious behaviors [for a review, see Adler et al. (87), Minshawi et al. (88), and Stigler (89)]. Thus, research on potential therapeutic effects for repetitive behaviors and interests should investigate benefits of melatonin in ASD (see next section) but should not be limited to the administration of melatonin.

Therapeutic Benefits of Melatonin in Autism Spectrum Disorder

Published melatonin treatment studies in autism are presented in Table 2. Many have been most concerned with effects of melatonin on sleep. Reviews, commentaries, and meta analyses of melatonin treatment studies are presented in Table 3. These two tables come from a prior article of our team (26).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Studies on potential therapeutic benefits of melatonin in autism.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Review, meta-analysis, and discussion of therapeutic uses of melatonin in autism.

As previously discussed (26), the treatment studies have a number of limitations. In some studies individuals covering a wide age range were included with pre-pubertal, pubertal, and post-pubertal individuals being studied (102, 103, 105). Reported differences in pineal melatonin secretion according to age and pubertal stage, coupled with the clearly developmental nature of autism, suggests that therapeutic effects of melatonin might vary considerably with age and pubertal status (23). Studies have also been limited by small sample sizes (9093, 95, 96, 98, 102, 104). In larger studies, heterogeneous groups are often examined and have included blind individuals and individuals with various neurological disabilities with concomitant intellectual disability. In such studies, often results seen for the autism subgroup are not separately presented (97, 100, 103107). The specificity and interpretation of the results with respect to autism are often unclear. Additional research is needed to determine whether and which findings might be specific to autism or whether melatonin’s effect might be similar across groups. It can be hypothesized that melatonin’s effects are due to actions on certain behavioral dimensions and that they can be observed across disorders. However, it should be pointed out that in our studies (18, 23), lower melatonin excretion was significantly associated with social communication impairments rather than with sleep problems. Future studies of melatonin in ASD should simultaneously examine melatonin levels, sleep problems, autistic behavioral impairments, and level of functioning so that a more complete picture can emerge.

It is worth noting that just a few of the therapeutic trials of melatonin have assessed effects on autistic behavioral impairments. These include reports of improved communication (105), reduced social withdrawal (82, 99), decreased stereotyped behaviors and rigidity (82, 102), and reduced anxiety (99, 103). Furthermore, at time the improvements noted were not sufficiently detailed. For example, Wright et al. (105) reported significant communication improvement without reporting separate verbal or non-verbal scores. In other instances, investigators did not use validated instruments; thus, Garstang and Wallis (102) reported improvements in rigidity based solely on parents’ and teachers’ comments, while Wasdell et al. (103) reported reductions in anxiety based on caregivers’ comments. All of the published clinical trials of melatonin in autism/ASD have used sleep measures as their main outcome variable and none have studied the dose–response relationship for melatonin. To reiterate, there is a real need for further studies on the therapeutic effects of melatonin that are conducted on large, relatively homogeneous samples and that employ validated behavioral assessments.

Synchronization of Motor, Emotional, and Relational Rhythms: Therapeutic Benefits of the Early Start Denver Model in Autism Spectrum Disorder

Although there is a growing interest in the role of motor, emotional, and interpersonal rhythms in autism, there have been only a few attempts to focus on behavioral synchronization of social communication during therapeutic and educational intervention in autism. The ESDM, developed by Sally Rogers and Geraldine Dawson (119, 120), is a comprehensive behavioral and developmental early intervention approach designed for delivery to 12- to 60-month-old children. The model uses the knowledge of how typical infants, toddlers, and pre-schoolers develop, in addition to knowledge of the ways in which autism affects early development, in order to facilitate an appropriate developmental trajectory in young children with autism beginning at the earliest ages. The goals of the ESDM are to reduce the severity of disabling autism symptoms in very young children, and to accelerate children’s developmental rates in a multitude of domains, including cognitive, social–emotional, and language domains. In order to do so, there is a strong focus on interpersonal engagement marked by synchrony and reciprocity during teaching, as research demonstrates that these are closely tied to successful developmental outcomes (121124). Within the ESDM, the therapist and child work together to develop and sustain coordinated, synchronous activity routines into which teaching opportunities are embedded. The ESDM reflects an understanding of the importance of synchrony of motor, emotional, and relational rhythms for the early development of young children with autism.

Thus, the ESDM allows one to initiate interaction with the child by synchronizing the therapist’s motor, emotional, and relational rhythms with the child’s rhythms (providing invariants/continuity). For example, if the child uses an object with a rhythmic motor pattern, the therapist will reproduce this behavior with exactly the same rhythmic pattern. The evolution of the child is then facilitated by introducing new and different rhythms (providing variants/discontinuity). In addition, the adjustment of the therapist’s behaviors, movements, level of arousal, and posture (for example, standing up, lying down, or sitting) to those of the child contributes to foundational invariants necessary during the initial phase. Furthermore, the ESDM requires the program to be implemented in different environments (such as home, school, and caregiver settings), which also contributes to the introduction of variants.

The ESDM is both a curriculum and a set of teaching practices. A specific developmental curriculum, administered every 3 months in typical practice, defines the skills to be taught at any given time. In addition, a manual of teaching practices outlines the ways in which these skills are to be taught. Embedded within both the curriculum and the set of teaching practices is a focus on rhythms and synchrony. In other words, when therapists are working within the model, a focus on rhythms permeates both what they teach and how they teach it.

The ESDM curriculum outlines skills to be taught in multiple areas, including cognitive, social–emotional, and language domains. The curriculum focuses heavily upon the teaching of skills that promote engagement with other people in a synchronous, rhythmic way. Many young children with autism enter an ESDM program with weaknesses in these skills, such as imitation, joint attention, orienting to name, and eye contact, and these have broad influences upon the manner in which these children can engage with others. It is well recognized that learning occurs within a social context and that social skills, such as imitation and shared attention, provide a foundation for many aspects of learning, including language, cognitive, and social–emotional abilities. Thus, the ESDM focuses heavily on the development of social engagement and interactional synchrony early on in order to further support learning of a wide range of skills.

One area of focus within the ESDM, which is closely tied to the concept of rhythmicity, is imitation. Young children with autism often exhibit deficits in their imitation skills, including those of imitating others’ facial expressions and movements, gestures, body actions, and actions on objects. From the beginning of a young child’s ESDM program, teaching of imitation is stressed, from basic imitation of actions on objects to more nuanced imitation of sound effects produced in play. Spontaneous and appropriate imitation of others is typically rhythmic and marked by mutual attention, continuous adaptation, and turn taking. The beginning stages of facilitating imitation and social engagement often start with imitating the child, thereby entering into a rhythmic interaction with the child. By imitating the child’s movements and establishing a synchronous interaction, eye contact and mutual engagement in the interaction are promoted (125).

Rhythmicity is also involved in the way teaching occurs within the ESDM upon multiple levels, from the basic structure of each interactive routine to the ways in which adults engage with children during interaction. Social communication is about sharing moments of synchrony with others, and the ESDM supports the emergence of interactional synchrony. The therapist facilitates the occurrence of synchronous moments and strings them together into routines into which the therapist can naturally embed teaching opportunities. The primary vehicle through which all teaching is accomplished in this model is the joint activity routine, which is permeated by moments of rhythmic, synchronous interaction, and enriched with positive affect. Adult and child are attuned to one another, both taking the lead, both following the other’s lead, taking turns, and creating a positive and motivating activity in conjunction with one another.

The joint activity routine, although flexible and naturalistic, adheres to a four-part structure: (1) opening, (2) theme, (3) elaboration, and (4) closing. This structure provides a set of invariants, against which multiple variants can occur at differing levels. In order to begin engagement within a joint activity routine, the adult works hard to find the child’s smile. This can often be accomplished by getting into the child’s own rhythm (imitating a child’s actions is a very powerful tool to facilitate motivated response and interaction) or by finding a rhythm that the child likes. For example, perhaps a child finds a toy drum and begins banging on it with his hand. In the ESDM, the therapist would join the child and would likely take her/his own drum and imitate the child’s actions, joining his rhythm. This would be considered the opening phase of the joint activity routine.

Next, the child and adult would develop a theme. This can be considered a continuation of the rhythm introduced during the opening phase – in our example, banging a drum slowly with hands. The adult and child would take turns doing so, each leading and each following. The skillful adult would embed teaching opportunities into these turns – perhaps a focus on eye contact during dyadic engagement, or on imitation of actions on objects, or on giving and taking objects with eye contact. Both partners then play within this rhythm for a while, until one introduces a variant, or elaboration. In the ESDM, elaboration occurs by introducing a change into the joint activity routine, a change in the rhythm which often allows different teaching targets to be practiced. In this example, the child and adult might start to play peek-a-boo behind the drums. A new rhythm would need to be established, and both partners would work together in order to do so. An elaboration of this sort is a rhythmic fluctuation occurring against a background of invariants (e.g., the same interactive partner, the same material, the same setting), which allows the child to learn to adapt to change and teaches the child how to engage with people and materials differently. Finally, this rhythm would likely begin to fade, the teaching value of the activity would start to diminish, and/or the child would begin to lose motivation. The fourth stage of the joint activity routine – the closing – is the last part of the joint activity routine. Adult and child might help one another clean up the materials, and then both partners would begin an entirely new joint activity routine. This could often involve a change in location, activity level, or teaching domain, and an entirely new set of coordinated interactions would be developed.

Within the joint activity routine, synchrony pervades within several different levels and in several different areas. Adult and child are attuned to one another and responsive to one another in terms of sensory input and output, motor actions, and emotions. Coordinated dyadic engagement, well-balanced in terms of leader and follower, pervades a high-quality joint activity routine. It is quite natural to view this ever-present synchrony in terms of a focus on shared nuanced rhythms.

There is also a focus on rhythmicity within some of the broader teaching practices of the ESDM; for example, in the way in which a teaching session is constructed. As mentioned above, teaching is delivered within joint activity routines. For a therapist-delivered session, these routines are strung together within a session to occupy a full 1- or 2-h time period. Within that time period, therapist and child move around to occupy several spaces. In a well-coordinated way, they engage together in a joint activity routine at the table, and then may move onto the floor for the next. They may sit together, then they may stand or walk or run. The child’s level of arousal is carefully monitored, and the session is marked by alternations of quiet thoughtfulness and active play, all coordinated artfully by a skilled adult. All of these changes occur against a stable background, as the session is marked by stability and continuity, with the introduction of variants when appropriate. The ESDM, a comprehensive, behavioral, and developmental early intervention designed for infants and toddlers with autism, is rooted within a sense of and appreciation for rhythmicity and synchrony at multiple levels, ranging from the specific skills that are taught to how those skills are taught, woven together and supported by a broader structure focused on maintaining a well-coordinated, synchronous set of dyadic interactions.

Conclusion: Toward an Integrative Approach Combining the Use of Melatonin with the ESDM

Taken all together, ASD could be seen as a disorder of rhythmicity with, more specifically, impairments in the synchrony of rhythms. Alternatively, such asynchrony might play an important role in a possibly large subgroup of individuals that forms part of the heterogeneous ASD category. In this article, we proposed an integrative approach to study desynchronization in biological and psychological rhythms in ASD and develop an etiopathogenic hypothesis as well as therapeutic perspectives for ASD based on this integrative approach. Indeed, this integrated physiological and psychological approach opens important therapeutic perspectives for ASD based on regulation of physiological rhythms (in particular, through the use of chronobiotics such as melatonin, and also through light exposure, use of regularly scheduled bedtime, wake up, meals, or activities) combined with synchronization of motor, emotional, and relational rhythms through developmental behavioral intervention such as the ESDM. Further studies are required to better ascertain the underlying mechanisms of physiological alterations induced by temporal desynchronization and to better understand the role of biological rhythms and rhythmicity in the development of social communication, repetitive behaviors, and interests or adaptation to changes, and therefore in the development of autism involving impairments in these domains.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Pevet P, Challet E. Melatonin: both master clock output and internal time-giver in the circadian clocks network. J Physiol Paris (2011) 105:170–82. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2011.07.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Botbol M, Cabon P, Kermarrec S, Tordjman S. Biological and psychological rhythms: an integrative approach to rhythm disturbances in autistic disorder. J Physiol Paris (2013) 107(4):298–309. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.03.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Molenaar PCM, Campbell CG. The new person-specific paradigm in psychology. Curr Dir Psychol Sci (2009) 18(2):112–7. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01619.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Glickman G. Circadian rhythms and sleep in children with autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2010) 34(5):755–68. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Kotagal S, Broomall E. Sleep in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Pediatr Neurol (2012) 47(4):242–51. doi:10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2012.05.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Honomichl RD, Goodlin-Jones BL, Burnham M, Gaylor E, Anders T. Sleep patterns of children with pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord (2002) 32:553–61. doi:10.1023/A:1021254914276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Hoshino Y, Wanatabe H, Yashima Y, Kaneko M, Kumashiro H. An investigation on the sleep disturbance of autistic children. Folia Psychiatr Neurol Jpn (1984) 38:45–51.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Johnson CR. Sleep problems in children with mental retardation and autism. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am (1996) 5:673–83.

Google Scholar

9. Patzold LM, Richdale AL, Tonge BJ. An investigation into sleep characteristics of children with autism and Asperger’s disorder. J Paediatr Child Health (1998) 34:528–33. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1754.1998.00291.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Richdale AL, Prior MR. The sleep-wake rhythm in children with autism. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (1995) 4:175–86. doi:10.1007/BF01980456

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Richdale AL. Sleep problems in autism: prevalence, cause, and intervention. Dev Med Child Neurol (1999) 41:60–6. doi:10.1017/S0012162299000122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Schreck KA, Mullick JA. Parental reports of sleep problems in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord (2000) 30:127–35. doi:10.1023/A:1005407622050

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Tordjman S, Anderson GM, Kermarrec S, Olivier Bonnot O, Geoffray MM, Brailly-Tabard S, et al. Altered circadian patterns of salivary cortisol in low-functioning children and adolescents with autism. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2014) 50:227–45. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Golombek DA, Casiraghi LP, Agostino PV, Paladino N, Duhart JM, Plano SA, et al. The times they’re a-changing: effects of circadian desynchronization on physiology and disease. J Physiol Paris (2013) 107(4):310–22. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.03.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Revell VL, Skene DJ. Light-induced melatonin suppression in humans with polychromatic and monochromatic light. Chronobiol Int (2007) 24:1125–35. doi:10.1080/07420520701800652

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Brown R, Hobson RP, Lee A, Stevenson J. Are there “autistic-like” features in congenitally blind children? J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1997) 38(6):693–703. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01696.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Donaldson AI, Heavner KS, Zwolan TA. Measuring progress in children with autism spectrum disorder who have cochlear implants. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg (2004) 130:666–71. doi:10.1001/archotol.130.5.666

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Tordjman S, Anderson GM, Bellissant E, Botbol M, Charbuy H, Camus F, et al. Day and nighttime excretion of 6-sulphatoxymelatonin in adolescents and young adults with autistic disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2012) 37:1990–7. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.04.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kulman G, Lissoni P, Rovelli F, Roselli MG, Brivio F, Sequeri P. Evidence of pineal endocrine hypofunction in autistic children. Neuro Endocrinol Lett (2000) 20:31–4.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Tordjman S, Anderson GM, McBride PA, Hertzig M, Snow M, Hall L, et al. Plasma beta-endorphin, adrenocorticotropin hormone and cortisol in autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1997) 38(6):705–16. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01697.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Ritvo ER, Ritvo R, Yuliwer A, Brothers A, Freeman BJ, Plotkin S. Elevated daytime melatonin concentration in autism. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (1993) 2:75–8. doi:10.1007/BF02098862

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Nir I, Meir D, Zilber N, Knobler H, Hadjez J, Lerner Y. Brief report: circadian melatonin, thyroid-stimulating hormone, prolactin, and cortisol levels in serum of young adults with autism. J Autism Dev Disord (1995) 25:641–54. doi:10.1007/BF02178193

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Tordjman S, Anderson GM, Pichard N, Charbuy H, Touitou Y. Nocturnal excretion of 6-sulphatoxymelatonin in children and adolescents with autistic disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2005) 57:134–8. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Melke J, Goubran Botros H, Chaste P, Betancur C, Nygren G, Anckarssater H, et al. Abnormal melatonin synthesis in autism spectrum disorder. Mol Psychiatry (2008) 13(1):90–8. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4002016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Mulder EJ, Anderson GM, Kemperman RFJ, Oosterloo-Duinkerken A, Minderaa RB, Kema IP. Urinary excretion of 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid, serotonin and 6-sulphatoxymelatonin in normoserotonemic and hyperserotonemic autistic individuals. Neuropsychobiology (2010) 61:27–32. doi:10.1159/000258640

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Tordjman S, Najjar I, Bellissant E, Anderson GM, Barburoth M, Cohen D, et al. Advances in the research of melatonin in autism spectrum disorders: literature review and new perspectives. Int J Mol Sci (2013) 14(10):20508–42. doi:10.3390/ijms141020508

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Amiet C, Gourfinkel-An I, Bouzamondo A, Tordjman S, Baulas M, Lechat P, et al. Epilepsy in autism is associated with intellectual disability and gender: evidence from a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry (2008) 64(7):577–82. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.030

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hu VW, Sarachana T, Kim KS, Nguyen A, Kulkarni S, Steinberg ME, et al. Gene expression profiling differentiates autism case-controls and phenotypic variants of autism spectrum disorders: evidence for circadian rhythm dysfunction in severe autism. Autism Res (2009) 2:78–97. doi:10.1002/aur.73

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Jansen R, Metzdorf R, Van der Roest M, Fusani L, ter Maat A, Gahr M. Melatonin affects the temporal organization of the song of the zebra finch. FASEB J (2005) 19:848–50. doi:10.1096/fj.04-2874fje

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Boucher J. Lost in a sea of time: Time parsing and autism. In: Hoerl C, McCormack T, editors. Time and Memory: Issues in Philosophy and Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2001). p. 111–36.

Google Scholar

31. Wimpory D, Nicholas B, Nash S. Social timing clock genes and autism: a new hypothesis. J Intellect Disabil Res (2002) 46:352–8. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2788.2002.00423.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Weisman O, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R. Oxytocin administration to parent enhances infant physiological and behavioral readiness for social engagement. Biol Psychiatry (2012) 72:982–9. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.06.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Insel TR. Oxytocin – a neuropeptide for affiliation: evidence from behavioral, receptor autoradiographic, and comparative studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology (1992) 17(1):3–35. doi:10.1016/0306-4530(92)90073-G

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Modahl C, Fein D, Waterhouse L, Newton N. Does oxytocin deficiency mediate social deficits in autism? J Autism Dev Disord (1992) 2(3):449–51. doi:10.1007/BF01048246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Modahl C, Green LA, Fein D, Morris M, Waterhouse L, Feinstein C, et al. Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children Biol. Psychiatry (1998) 43(4):270–7.

Google Scholar

36. Guedeney A, Guedeney N, Tereno S, Dugravier R, Greacen T, Welniarz B, et al. Infant rhythms versus parental time: promoting parent-infant synchrony. J Physiol Paris (2011) 105(4–6):195–200. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2011.07.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Lickliter R, Bahrick LE, Honeycutt H. Intersensory redundancy facilitates prenatal perceptual learning in bobwhite quail (Colinus virginianus) embryos. Dev Psychol (2002) 38:15–23. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.1.15

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Tordjman S. At the crossroads between psychoanalysis and neuroscience: the importance of subjectivity. J Physiol Paris (2010) 104(5):232–42. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2010.08.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Wallon H. L’enfant Turbulent. Paris: PUF (1984).

Google Scholar

40. Haag G, Botbol M, Graignic R, Perez-Diaz F, Bronsard G, Kermarrec S, et al. The autism psychodynamic evaluation of changes (APEC) scale: a reliability and validity study on a newly developed standardized psychodynamic assessment for youth with pervasive developmental disorders. J Physiol Paris (2010) 104(6):323–36. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2010.10.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Price DA, Close GC, Fielding BA. Age of appearance of circadian rhythm in salivary cortisol values in infancy. Arch Dis Child (1983) 58:454–6. doi:10.1136/adc.58.6.454

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Joseph D, Chong NW, Shanks ME, Rosato E, Taub NA, Petersen SA, et al. Getting rhythm: how do babies do it? Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed (2014) 100(1):F50–4. doi:10.1136/archdischild-204-306104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Lavelli M, Fogel A. Developmental changes in the relationship between the infant’s attention and emotion during early face-to-face communication: the 2-month transition. Dev Psychol (2005) 41(1):265. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.265

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Trevarthen C, Aitken KJ. Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2001) 2:3–48. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00701

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Shultz S, Vouloumanos A, Bennett RH, Pelphrey K. Neural specialization for speech in the first months of life. Dev Sci (2014) 17(5):766–74. doi:10.1111/desc.12151

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Meltzoff AN, Kuhl PK, Movellan J, Sejnowski TJ. Foundations for a new science of learning. Science (2009) 325:284–8. doi:10.1126/science.1175626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Kuhl PK. Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code. Nat Rev Neurosci (2004) 5:831–43. doi:10.1038/nrn1533

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Goldstein MH, King AP, West MJ. Social interaction shapes babbling: testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2003) 100:8030–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.1332441100

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Delaherche E, Chetouani M, Mahdhaoui A, Saint-Georges C, Viaux S, Cohen D. Interpersonal synchrony: a survey of evaluation methods across disciplines. IEEE Trans Affect Comput (2012) 3:349–65. doi:10.1109/T-AFFC.2012.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Crown CL, Feldstein S, Jasnow MD, Beebe B, Jaffe J. The cross-modal coordination of interpersonal timing: six-week-olds infants’ gaze with adults’ vocal behavior. J Psycholinguist Res (2002) 31(1):1–23. doi:10.1023/A:1014301303616

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Stormark KM, Braarud HC. Infants’ sensitivity to social contingency: a “double video” study of face-to-face communication between 2-and 4-month-olds and their mothers. Infant Behav Dev (2004) 27(2):195–203. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Feldman R. Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2007) 48(3–4):329–54. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Dumas GJ, Nadel R, Soussignan J, Martinerie L, Garnero L. Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PLoS One (2010) 5(8):e12166. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0012166

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Saint-Georges C, Mahdhaoui A, Chetouani M, Cassel R, Laznik MC, Apicella F, et al. Do parents recognize autistic deviant behavior long before diagnosis? Taking into account interaction using computational methods. PLoS One (2011) 6(7):e22393. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0022393

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Guinchat V, Chamak B, Bonniau B, Bodeau N, Perisse D, Cohen D, et al. Very early signs of autism reported by parents include many concerns not specific to autism criteria. Res Autism Spectr Disord (2012) 6:589–601. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2011.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Rogers T, Roberts W, Brian J, Szatmari P. Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. Int J Dev Neurosci (2005) 23(2–3):143–52. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Elsabbagh M, Johnson MH. Getting answers from babies about autism. Trends Cogn Sci (2010) 14(2):81–7. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Stern D. Le monde interpersonnel du nourrisson. Paris: PUF (1989).

Google Scholar

59. Cohen D, Cassel RS, Saint-Georges C, Mahdhaoui A, Laznik MC, Apicella F, et al. Do parentese prosody and fathers’ involvement in interacting facilitate social interaction in infants who will later develop autism? PLoS One (2013) 8(5):e61402. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0061402

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Watanabe K. Behavioral speed contagion: automatic modulation of movement timing by observation of body movements. Cognition (2008) 106:514–24. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Mauk MD, Buonomano DV. The neural basis of temporal processing. Annu Rev Neurosci (2004) 27:307–40. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144247

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Xavier J, Tilmont E, Bonnot O. Children’s synchrony and rhythmicity in imitation of peers: toward a developmental model of empathy. J Physiol Paris (2013) 107(4):291–7. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.03.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Trevarthen C. what is like to be a person who knows nothing? Defining the active intersubjective mind of a newborn human being. Infant Child Dev (2011) 20:119–35. doi:10.1002/icd.689

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Nadel J. Imiter Pour Grandir. Développement du bébé et de l’enfant avec autisme. Paris: Dunod (2011).

Google Scholar

65. Nadel-Brulfert J, Baudonnière PM. The social function of reciprocal imitation in 2-years-old peers. Int J Behav Dev (1982) 5:95–109. doi:10.1177/016502548200500105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Nadel J, Baudonnière PM, Fontaine AM. Les comportements sociaux imitatifs. Recherches Psychologie Sociale (1983) 5:15–29.

Google Scholar

67. Ikegami T, Iizuka H. Turn-taking interaction as acooperative and co-creative process. Infant Behav Dev (2007) 30:278–88. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.02.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Rizzolatti G, Craighero L. The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci (2004) 27:169–92. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Guionnet S, Nadel J, Bertasi E, Sperduti M, Delaveau P, Fossati P. Reciprocal imitation: toward a neural basis of social interaction. Cereb Cortex (2012) 22:971–8. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr177

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Decety J, Chaminade T, Grezes J, Meltzoff AN. A PET exploration of the neural mechanisms involved in reciprocal imitation. Neuroimage (2002) 15:265–72. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0938

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Iacoboni M. Neural mechanisms of imitation. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2005) 15:632–7. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Nettl B. An ethnomusicologist contemplates universals in musical sound and musical culture. In: Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S, editors. The Origins of Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2000). p. 463–72.

Google Scholar

73. Patel AD, Iversen JR, Bregman MR, Schulz I. Experimental evidence for synchronization to a musical beat in a nonhuman animal. Curr Biol (2009) 19:827–30. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.038

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Feldman R. Parent-infant synchrony biological foundations and developmental outcomes. Curr Dir Psychol Sci (2007) 16:340–5. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00532.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Winkler I, Háden GP, Ladinig O, Sziller I, Honing H. Newborn infants detect the beat in music. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2009) 106:2468–71. doi:10.1073/pnas.0809035106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Provasi J, Bobin-Begue A. Spontaneous motor tempo and rhythmical synchronisation in 2-1/2 and 4-year-old children. Int J Behav Dev (2003) 27:220–31. doi:10.1080/01650250244000290

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Bobin-Bègue A, Provasi J, Marks A, Pouthas V. Influence of an auditory tempo on the endogenous rhythm of non-nutritive sucking. Eur Rev Appl Psychol (2006) 56:239–45. doi:10.1016/j.erap.2005.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Trevarthen C, Stuart Daniel S. Disorganized rhythm and synchrony: early signs of autism and Rett syndrome. Brain Dev (2005) 27:S25–34. doi:10.1016/j.braindev.2005.03.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Sandercock R, Jones W, Klin A, Shultz S. Contingent smiling behavior in 2- to 5-month-old infants with and without ASD during dyadic interactions. International Meeting for Autism Research (IMFAR). Atlanta, GA (2015).

Google Scholar

80. Jones W, Klin A. Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature (2013) 504(7480):4427–31. doi:10.1038/nature12715

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

82. Malow B, Adkins K, McGrew S, Wang L, Goldman S, Fawkes D. Melatonin for sleep in children with autism: a controlled trial examining dose, tolerability and outcomes. J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 42(8):1729–37. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1418-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Goldbeter A. La vie Oscillatoire. Paris: Odile Jacob (2010).

Google Scholar

84. Winnicott DW. Collected Papers: Through Pediatrics to Psychoanalysis. London: Tavistock Publications (1958).

Google Scholar

85. Fureix C, Benhajali H, Henry S, Bruchet A, Prunier A, Ezzaouia M, et al. Plasma cortisol and faecal cortisol metabolites concentrations in stereotypic and non-stereotypic horses: do stereotypic horses cope better with poor environmental conditions? BMC Vet Res (2013) 9:3. doi:10.1186/1746-6148-9-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Gabriels RL, Agnew JA, Pan Z, Holt KD, Reynolds A, Laudensiager ML. Elevated repetitive behaviors are associated with lower diurnal salivary cortisol levels in autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychol (2013) 93(2):262–8. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.02.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Adler BA, Wink LK, Early M, Shaffer R, Minshawi N, McDougle CJ, et al. Drug-refractory aggression, self-injurious behavior, and severe tantrums in autism spectrum disorders: a chart review study. Autism (2015) 19(1):102–6. doi:10.1177/1362361314524641

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Minshawi NF, Hurwitz S, Morriss D, McDougle CJ. Multidisciplinary assessment and treatment of self-injurious behavior in autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability: integration of psychological and biological theory and approach. J Autism Dev Disord (2014). doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2307-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Stigler KA. Psychopharmacologic management of serious behavioral disturbance in ASD. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am (2014) 23(1):73–82. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2013.07.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Horrigan JP, Barnhill LJ. More on melatonin. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (1997) 36(8):1014. doi:10.1097/00004583-199708000-00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Hayashi E. Effect of melatonin on sleep-wake rhythm: the sleep diary of an autistic male. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2000) 54:383–4. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1819.2000.00725.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Jan JE, Freeman RD, Wasdell MB, Bomben MM. A child with severe night terrors and sleep-walking responds to melatonin therapy’. Dev Med Child Neurol (2004) 46(11):789. doi:10.1017/S0012162204231358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Gupta R, Hutchins J. Melatonin: a pancea for desperate parents? (Hype or truth). Arch Dis Child (2005) 90(9):986–7. doi:10.1136/adc.2005.075218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Andersen IM, Kaczmarska J, McGrew SG, Malow BA. Melatonin for insomnia in children with autism spectrum disorders. J Child Neurol (2008) 23(5):482–5. doi:10.1177/0883073807309783

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Galli-Carminati G, Deriaz N, Bertschy G. Melatonin in treatment of chronic sleep disorders in adults with autism: retrospective study. Swiss Med Wkly (2009) 139(19–20):293–6.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Jan JE, Espezel H, Appleton RE. The treatment of sleep disorders with melatonin. Dev Med Child Neurol (1994) 36:97–107. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.1994.tb11818.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Ishizaki A, Sugama M, Takeuchi N. Usefulness of melatonin for developmental sleep and emotional/behaviour disorders – studies of melatonin trial on 50 patients with developmental disorders. No To Hattatsu (1999) 31(5):428–37.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Paavonen EJ, Nieminen-von Wendt T, Vanhala R, Aronen ET, von Wendt L. Effectiveness of melatonin in the treatment of sleep disturbances in children with Asperger disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol (2003) 13(1): 83–95. doi:10.1089/104454603321666225

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Giannotti F, Cortesi F, Cerquiglini A, Bernabei P. An open-label study of controlled-release melatonin in treatment of sleep disorders in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord (2006) 36(6):741–52. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0116-z

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. De Leersnyder H, Zisapel N, Laudon M. Prolonged-release melatonin for children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Pediatr Neurol (2011) 45:23–6. doi:10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2011.02.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. MacArthur AJ, Budden SS. Sleep dysfonction in Rett syndrome: a trial of exogenous melatonin treatment. Dev Med Child Neurol (1998) 40:186–92. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.1998.tb15445.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Garstang J, Wallis M. Randomized controlled trial of melatonin for children with autistic spectrum disorders and sleep problems. Child Care Health Dev (2006) 32(5):585–9. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2214.2006.00616.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Wasdell MB, Jan JE, Bomben MM, Freeman RD, Rietveld WJ, Tai J, et al. A randomized, placebo-controlled trial of controlled release melatonin treatment of delayed sleep phase syndrome and impaired sleep maintenance in children with neurodevelopmental disabilities. J Pineal Res (2008) 44(1):57–64. doi:10.1111/j.1600-079X.2007.00528.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Wirojanan J, Jacquemont S, Diaz R, Bacalman S, Anders TF, Hagerman RJ, et al. The efficacy of melatonin for sleep problems in children with autism, fragile X syndrome, or autism and fragile X syndrome. J Clin Sleep Med (2009) 5(2):145–50.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Wright B, Sims D, Smart S, Alwazeer A, Alderson-Day B, Allgar V, et al. Melatonin versus placebo in children with autism spectrum conditions and severe sleep problems not amenable to behaviour. management strategies: a randomised controlled crossover trial. J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41(2):175–84. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1036-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Cortesi F, Giannotti F, Sebastiani T, Panunzi S, Valente D. Controlled-release melatonin, singly and combined with cognitive behavioural therapy, for persistent insomnia in children with autism spectrum disorders: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. J Sleep Res (2012) 21(6):700–9. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01021.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Gringas P, Gamble C, Jones AP, Wiggs L, Williamson PR, Sutcliffe A, et al. Melatonin for sleep problems in children with neurodevelopmental disorders: randomised double masked placebo controlled trial. BMJ (2012) 345:1–16. doi:10.1136/bmj.e6664

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Jan JE, O’Donnel ME. Use of melatonin in the treatment of paediatric sleep disorders. J Pineal Res (1996) 21:193–9. doi:10.1111/j.1600-079X.1996.tb00286.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Jan JE, Connolly MB, Hamilton D, Freeman RD, Laudon M. Melatonin treatment of non-epileptic myoclonus in children. Dev Med Child Neurol (1999) 41(4):255–9. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.1999.tb00593.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Phillips L, Appleton RE. Systematic review of melatonin treatment in children with neurodevelopmental disabilities and sleep impairment. Dev Med Child Neurol (2004) 46:771–5. doi:10.1017/S001216220400132X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Braam W, Smits MG, Didden R, Korzilius H, Van Geijlswijk M, Curfs LMG. Exogenous melatonin for sleep problems in individuals with intellectual disability: a meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol (2009) 51:340–9. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03244.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Guénolé F, Godbout R, Nicolas A, Franco P, Claustrat B, Baleyte JM. Melatonin for disordered sleep in individuals with autism spectrum disorders: systematic review and discussion. Sleep Med Rev (2011) 15(6):379–87. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2011.02.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Doyen C, Mighiu D, Kaya K, Colineaux C, Beaumanoir C, Mouraeff Y, et al. Melatonin in children with autistic spectrum disorders: recent and practicaldata. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2011) 20(5):231–9. doi:10.1007/s00787-011-0162-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Rossignol DA, Frye RE. Melatonin in autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol (2011) 53:783–92. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.03980.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Reading R. Melatonin in autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Care Health Dev (2012) 38(2):301–2.

Google Scholar

116. Guénolé F, Baleyte JM. Meta-analysing the effectiveness of melatonin for sleep-disturbed individuals with autism spectrum conditions: should Rett syndrome be included? Dev Med Child Neurol (2011) 53(11):1063. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04071.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Guénolé F, Baleyte JM. Effects of melatonin should be studied separately in each neurodevelopmental disorder and with specific sleep diagnoses. Pediatr Neurol (2012) 46(1):60. doi:10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2011.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Lord C. What is melatonin? Is it a useful treatment for sleep problems in autism? J Autism Dev Disord (1998) 28(4):345–6. doi:10.1023/A:1026020600723

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Dawson G, Rogers SJ, Munson J, Smith M, Winter J, Greenson J, et al. Randomized controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: the early start Denver model. Pediatrics (2010) 25(1):e17–23. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-0958

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Rogers SJ, Dawson G. Early Start Denver Model for Children with Autism: Promoting Language, Learning, and Engagement. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2010).

Google Scholar

121. Feldman R, Masalha S. Parent-child and triadic antecedents of children’s social competence: cultural specificity, shared process. Dev Psychol (2010) 46(2):455–67. doi:10.1037/a0017415

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Leerkes EM, Blankson AN, O’Brien M. Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social-emotional functioning. Child Dev (2009) 80(3):762–75. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01296.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Paavola L, Kemppinen K, Kumpulainen K, Moilanen I, Ebeling H. Maternal sensitivity, infant co-operation and early linguistic development: some predictive relations. Eur J Dev Psychol (2006) 3(1):13–30. doi:10.1080/17405620500317789

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Shonkoff JP, Phillips DA, editors. From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press (2000).

Google Scholar

125. Lewy A, Dawson G. Social stimulation and joint attention deficits in young autistic children. J Abnorm Child Psychol (1992) 20:555–66. doi:10.1007/BF00911240

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorder, biological rhythms, motor, emotional and relational rhythms, synchronization of rhythms, melatonin, Early Start Denver Model, therapeutics

Citation: Tordjman S, Davlantis KS, Georgieff N, Geoffray M-M, Speranza M, Anderson GM, Xavier J, Botbol M, Oriol C, Bellissant E, Vernay-Leconte J, Fougerou C, Hespel A, Tavenard A, Cohen D, Kermarrec S, Coulon N, Bonnot O and Dawson G (2015) Autism as a disorder of biological and behavioral rhythms: toward new therapeutic perspectives. Front. Pediatr. 3:1. doi: 10.3389/fped.2015.00001

Received: 20 August 2014; Accepted: 05 January 2015;
Published online: 23 February 2015.

Edited by:

Roberto Canitano, University Hospital of Siena, Italy

Reviewed by:

Rajshekhar Bipeta, Gandhi Medical College and Hospital, India
Felipe Ortuño, Clínica Universidad de Navarra, Spain
Roberto Canitano, University Hospital of Siena, Italy

Copyright: © 2015 Tordjman, Davlantis, Georgieff, Geoffray, Speranza, Anderson, Xavier, Botbol, Oriol, Bellissant, Vernay-Leconte, Fougerou, Hespel, Tavenard, Cohen, Kermarrec, Coulon, Bonnot and Dawson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sylvie Tordjman, Hospitalo- Universitaire de Psychiatrie de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent, 154 rue de Châtillon, Rennes 35200, France e-mail: s.tordjman@yahoo.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.