Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 27 August 2019
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuroeducation: Translating Lab Insights into Classroom Practice View all 12 articles

Implications of Unconnected Micro, Molecular, and Molar Level Research in Psychology: The Case of Executive Functions, Self-Regulation, and External Regulation

  • 1School of Education and Psychology, University of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain
  • 2School of Psychology, University of Almería, Almería, Spain
  • 3Center of Research of Psychology, University of Almería, Almería, Spain
  • 4School of Psychology, University of Granada, Granada, Spain

The proliferation of research production in Psychology as a science has been increasing exponentially. This situation leads to the necessity of organizing the research production into different levels of analysis that make it possible to delimit each research domain. The objective of this analysis is to clearly distinguish the different levels of research: micro-analysis, molecular, and molar. Each level is presented, along with an analysis of its benefits and limitations. Next, this analysis is applied to the topics of Executive Functions, Self-Regulation, and External Regulation. Conclusions, limitations, and implications for future research are offered, with a view toward a better connection of research production across the different levels, and an allusion to ethical considerations.

Introduction

With the recent proliferation of a large volume of research in Neuroscience, Cognitive Neuropsychology and Psychology, referring to the neurological substratum of basic psychological processes, we find an epistemic imbalance where the more global, contextualized approaches to explaining behavior give way to approaches based on analysis and study of basic processes. Based on observation of this reality, the aim of the following theoretical analysis is: (1) to define the different levels that make up psychological research, as well as the scientific domain inherent to each, with their limitations and benefits; and (2) to apply this classification to a recent research topic of great academic, investigative, and professional impact: executive functions and self-regulation.

The development of human knowledge in an empirical scientific format, as we know it today, is relatively recent in human history, given its complexity and the cognitive repertoires and high-level technology that are required to produce it. It is not surprising, therefore, that the initial formats of knowledge were conceptual constructions taken from related facts, which were used to develop concepts (definitions, characteristics), and finally, patterns or probabilistic predictions in the form of principles. Fables, proverbs and parables are good examples of this kind of commonplace psychological knowledge. All such elaborations have a common denominator: they start from direct observation of reality, based on unfiltered samples of behavior, from which they propose probabilistic relations and common-sense predictions. This type of understanding, therefore, is a form of unscientific, untested, folk knowledge.

With the historical appearance of the scientific-positivist paradigm, production of human knowledge in a scientific format has greatly increased, both in quality and in quantity. This production, however, has clearly been conditioned by the resources or technological developments available at the moment. Commonplace human knowledge thus tends to increasingly match contributions of what we call scientific knowledge. Consequently, in today’s Knowledge Society heavily laden with this type of knowledge and the scientific-technological component any analysis of reality or of any issue is usually connected to some type of evidence or some scientific-technical grounding. This does not mean that scientific knowledge is infallible or unquestionable (the overtrust bias); instead, such knowledge is subject to being disproven, restricted or verified, to ensure that it attains a higher level of consistency than folk knowledge, which is elaborated unsystematically by a human cognitive system, with many inaccuracies, inconsistencies and biases. On the other hand, scientific knowledge would have been obtained using technological instruments and techniques that provide for greater accuracy in data collection, processing, and analysis. This fact has led to a perception of data obtained through high level techniques as being “more scientific” (the technology bias). As a result, recent scientific studies involving complex techniques (scanners, computed tomography, etc.) would seem to have more scientific value. Psychology as a science is not invulnerable to this scenario. In this line, McCabe and Castel (2008) demonstrated that, in the area of cognition, the inclusion of brain images in an article results in increased scores for its scientific reasoning. In this case, the authors argue that this might be explained by “people’s affinity for reductionistic explanations of cognitive phenomena” (p. 343).

In light of this, it seems appropriate to reflect on the different levels and formats of psychology research – to clarify their different theoretical and empirical domains, and so encourage a better connection and integration between them, in both conceptual and applied knowledge. In order to illustrate these concepts, examples related to executive functions, their associated behavioral and contextual variables/models and their relationship with ADHD will be discussed.

Levels and Formats of Psychology Research

Definition of Levels and Formats

The different levels of research have coexisted within Science from the beginning, and they contribute complementary elements to the analysis of phenomena. In the case of Psychology, one may establish three levels of analysis of human behavior. To grasp this more clearly, we use the journey metaphor (Pintrich, 2000a, b) adapted. See Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Levels of research and working formats in the “building” of Psychological Research.

(1) Level of micro-analysis of behavior (biological analysis or the “hardware” of behavior). As the name indicates, it focuses on the smallest, most discrete, most basic level of the phenomenon being studied. In the case at hand, we may consider this level of analysis as referring to the biological, neuronal substrata or underpinnings of human conduct. Hence, the name neuropsychology, referring to the neuronal-behavioral connection. Typical of this domain of analysis is identifying areas of the brain, their connections, explanations, regularities, and biological or neuronal models of behavior. Consequently, this domain studies behavioral problems or disorders that stem from biological or physiological processes. One highly relevant, current line of research at this level is that of the neurological foundations of psychological processes and executive functions (Lukito et al., 2018; Rubia, 2018).

(2) Level of molecular analysis of behavior (functional analysis or the “software” of behavior). In this case, the analysis focuses on constructing behavioral models of the types of learning that are operational in human beings. Examples at this level would be theories, laws and models of learning – studied profusely and mastered by psychologists– in order to explain different types of human behavior, and thereby enable precise behavioral analyses of problems and behavioral disorders at the molecular level, or mid-level of complexity. These models also serve to promote and optimize processes of human development and learning. This is the level where we find models of self-regulation (Brown, 1998) and self-regulated learning (Zimmerman, 2000; Schunk, 2005).

(3) Level of molar analysis of behavior (interactive analysis or analysis of behavior in real, interactive contexts). Analysis at this level is necessarily interactive because the object of analysis is the subject-situation binomial, in interaction, in real contexts. At this level we find the contextualized, applied models of behavior, typical of the applied disciplines of Psychology, such as Educational Psychology, Social Psychology and Clinical and Health Psychology. These disciplines seek to explain, model and predict the influence of factors relating to the subject and to the situation, and to estimate the proportion of variance that is explained by the two groups of factors, in order to determine likely behaviors. In our specific topic area, it would be Level 3 of the theoretical model referred to as the “Theory of Self-Regulated vs. Externally-Regulated Learning,” or SRL vs. ERL Theory (de la Fuente, 2017).

Issues Associated With the Different Levels of Research Inference

As we have seen, each level of research involves certain characteristics, benefits and limitations that should be made explicit in order to adequately contextualize research production. Lacking this, inferences may be drawn that are not appropriate to the particular domain of scientific production; in other words, data and models from one level may be used to try to explain phenomena beyond the scope or domain of the investigation in question, using high-level inferences. For this reason, it is valuable to recall the benefits and limitations inherent to each level of research.

Level of Micro-Analysis

As a benefit, this level ascertains precise information about the biochemical, psychophysiological, and neurological areas and processes of behavior. It is therefore essential for constructing models and information related to disorders and problems of a neuropsychological and neurophysiological nature. However, this detailed, discrete vision of behavior has the limitation that linear implications may not be extracted and applied to the molecular level, much less to the molar. As in the Table 1 metaphor, knowledge of how the car engine is wired does not help us understand any substantial variance of the car’s total behavior (the subject), or how the trip transpires along the different roads (context). It is obvious that a good engine increases the likelihood of a good car and a good trip, but there are many intervening (mediating) variables between the car engine and the car’s behavior on the road that need to be understood in order to have a complete view of the phenomenon being studied – such as the style of driving and the road conditions.

Molecular Analysis Level

This level of analysis has the benefit of ascertaining mid-range information and enabling the creation of mid-level explanatory models. Following the Table 1 metaphor, this level would provide knowledge about the car’s behavior, understood as the set of factors referring to and mediated by the subject who drives the car. In this case we are considering the driver’s manner of driving. It is obvious that a good engine increases the likelihood of a good driving style, but we understand that they are two different variables; they cannot be further reduced, since a good engine can be part of a car with other differentiating characteristics. A good engine with an inefficient braking system or downforce, associated with a reckless driving style, only increases the chances of a traffic accident. Nonetheless, this level of analysis also has limitations or restrictions in that it contributes no information about the characteristics of the context. Consequently, we need a further level of analysis in order to offer a contextualized, interactive view of the object of study.

Level of Molar Analysis

The benefit of this level of analysis lies in its effort to explain subjects’ behavior in interaction with characteristics of the real context, paying special attention to the causative role of the context in the behavior being analyzed. The limitation of this level is the difficulty of descending from here to connect with the micro-analysis level. In the Table 1 metaphor, this type of analysis would be equivalent to constructing models that explain and predict the role of the road (traffic signs, design, camber, course) in subjects’ behavior, and how subjects interact with such contextual characteristics.

Based on the above analysis, we can identify the most common research errors that result from not clearly establishing the contributions and limitations of the domain of one’s object of study.

Compartmentalization Error

This error refers to not clearly defining the connections, differences, and continuity between similar constructs, adopted at different levels. In practice, this results in research using different conceptual labels for similar or equivalent constructs that emerge at different research levels. For example, adopting labels such as executive functions in reference to the self-regulation construct without previously defining the models and the domain or theoretical level at which these appear leads to conceptual confusion between constructs and their levels of application. A visual illustration would be researchers working on three different floors of a building (micro-analysis, molecular analysis, molar analysis), all of whom are studying the behavior of a car, but unaware of what the researchers on the other floors are working on (see Table 1).

Overgeneralization Error

In this case research conclusions or consequences are inferred at a level or domain distant from the data or evidence provided. For example, since basic research on attentional difficulties has shown that smells contribute to learning, one might conclude that this variable ought to be included in teaching-learning processes at school. This is a high-level inference, given that this evidence can easily be applied and integrated into discrete, basic instrumental models of learning, basic learning processes through classic laboratory conditioning of human beings. However, it is much more complicated to apply this to cognitive, meta-cognitive and meta-motivational models that explain processes involved in the construction of complex knowledge, in real contexts of formal learning.

Fuzziness Error

This refers to defining conceptually similar constructs, taken from one’s own level of research or another, and using it to justify results at a different level of research. For example, to assume that inattention results directly and exclusively from dysregulation of executive functions, as a neurological construct, when the evidence has consistently shown that strategic processes are involved, including meta-attention and behavioral, emotional and cognitive dysregulation. As advise Lilienfeld et al. (2015), clarity and rigor are essential in disciplines as psychology and psychiatric where many of the phenomena studied are “open concepts” that favor misunderstanding.

The Case of Executive Functions, Self-Regulation, and External Regulation

Levels of Analysis

Micro-Analysis Level of the Problem (Behavioral “Hardware”): Executive Functions

Definition and typology

Executive functions refer to higher-order control processes involved in the regulation of thought and action. Barkley (1997a) defined executive functions as “those types of actions we perform to ourselves and direct at ourselves so as to accomplish self-control, goal-directed behavior, and the maximization of future outcomes” (p. 57). Historically, there has been great interest and proliferation of studies focused on determining the role of the frontal lobes in human behavior, where consideration of that role has evolved from negligent to being central to the explanation of self-regulated human behavior (Fuster, 1989, 1997, 2002; Casey, 1992; Barkley, 1997a, b; Elliot, 2003; Eshel et al., 2007; Ardilla Ardila and Ostrosky Solís, 2008; García-Molina et al., 2009). Luria (1966) proposed three complementary functional units: (1) the limbic system, in charge of motivation and warning; (2) posterior cortical areas (including visual, auditory, and general sensory regions), in charge of stimuli recollection, information processing and storage; and (3) the frontal cortex, in charge of executive functioning, control and monitoring of the activities we perform. Lezak (1982) defined executive functions as the mental components that enable goal-setting, planning strategies and the effective performance of actions. Baddeley (1996) grouped executive functions into cognitive processes for inhibition, cognitive flexibility, behavior planning and organization, and verbal fluency. The concept of executive functions comes mainly from neurological research on disorders suffered by persons with injuries to the brain cortex. Injuries to these lobes have a great impact on the life of these individuals, such as personality changes, difficulty with autonomy, and emotional and affective regulation (Baddeley and Wilson, 1988).

Baggetta and Alexander (2016) carried out a systematic review of 106 empirical studies where they established the common denominator of executive functions as cognitive processes that: (1) guide action and are essential to learning tasks and everyday actions; (2) help monitor these types of task; (3) control not only the cognitive but also the emotional and behavioral domains of human action. These authors report that the model most often adopted in research in that of Miyake et al. (2000), an integrating model with three executive components, distinct yet related to each other: (1) Updating information; (2) Inhibiting information; (3) Cognitive flexibility. Metcalfe and Mischel (1999) established two executive systems, referring to cognitive and emotional behaviors (Hongwanishkul et al., 2005; Brock et al., 2009; Prencipe et al., 2011; Arias and de los Ángeles, 2012, p. 44; Anderson and Bolden, 2018).

A recent work (Tirapu-Ustarroz et al., 2018) has analyzed, through a review of studies with exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, the model models structural models underlying the existing empirical evidence. These authors conclude that the executive functions models evolve with age and incorporates planning factors in the later ages analyzed.

Neurocognitive treatment

Rubia (2018) carried out a review addressing the cognitive neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Their findings revealed functional brain abnormalities in ADHD subjects. Specifically, brain networks related to cognitive control, attention, timing, and working memory were impaired. In addition, distorted activity in brain regions that process motivation and emotion control and in the default mode network, was reported. On the other hand, it is interesting to note that, according to these abnormal activity patterns, classification of ADHD using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data achieved satisfactory classification accuracies of over 80%.

In order to treat frontal functional impairment, transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) seems to be a promising tool in the improvement of ADHD symptoms and cognitive functions. However, larger clinical trials of repeated stimulation with and without cognitive training are needed to test clinical efficacy and potential costs in non-targeted brain functions. Regarding NF, its improvements in cognition and ADHD symptoms may be due to the placebo effect, since no differences in accuracy between experimental and control conditions have been found. Neurotherapeutics seems attractive for ADHD due to its safety and potential longer-term neuroplastic effects, which drugs cannot offer. However, short- and longer-term clinical and cognitive efficacy must be subjected to thorough testing, as well as the potential for individualized treatment.

This level of analysis, therefore, is optimal for explaining disorders based on neurological problems and brain injuries. There has been prolific evidence in recent years of the important role of the frontal cortex and the functional role of “executive functions” for predicting performance (Gathercole and Pickering, 2000a, b; Blair and Razza, 2007; Bull et al., 2008; Cleary and Chen, 2009; Agostino et al., 2010; Molfese et al., 2010; Monette et al., 2011; De los Ángeles, 2012; Diamond, 2012, 2013; Ramírez, 2014; Arán and López, 2016; Diamond and Ling, 2016; Cid-Sillero et al., 2018; Díez and Bausela, 2018; Follmer, 2018; Jacoby and Lavidor, 2018). An impairment in executive functioning has also been related to antisocial and delinquent behaviors (De Brito and Hodgind, 2009; Pera-Guardiola et al., 2016; Gil-Fenoy et al., 2018). Additionally, executive functions are associated with social competence and behavioral problems in preschool education (Romero-López et al., 2015), or in the delayed sleep-wake phase disorder (Wilhelmsen-Langeland et al., 2019).

Neuropsychological concept of “executive functions” and its limitations

“In the cognitive neuroscience literature, the term executive function is sometimes used interchangeably with self-control. The conceptual overlap between executive function and self-control is plain: core executive functions include top–down inhibitory control, working memory, and the cognitive flexibility to switch perspectives when demands require doing so” (El Haj and Allain, 2012; Gaillardin and Baudri, 2018; Duckworth et al., 2019, p. 376). Even though executive functions and their components (working memory, inhibitory control, and planning) have a proven neuropsychological contribution to self-regulation, and there is an established relationship between them (Diamond, 2013; Canet et al., 2016), we do not find a linear relationship between the conceptual models of research in the micro-, molecular-, and molar- analysis domains. Furthermore, despite this important evidence, models at the micro-analysis level overlook a large number of variables that molecular research has established in the explanation of behavior, such as the mediating role of personality, cognitive variables, learning strategies, learning styles, motivational style and coping strategies, not to mention the role of context (Meltzer, 2007; Best et al., 2011). For this reason, in the case of ADHD, the neuropsychological concept of “executive functions,” although adequate in its domain of micro-analysis, ought to be completed and connected to contributions from research at further levels of psychological research, mentioned above. Servera-Barceló (2005) comment “we express our desire that ADHD stop being a mere list of symptoms with a vague factorial support, to go on to incorporate those neurobehavioral characteristics that have received the support of the works empirical studies developed largely within the model of self-regulation” (p. 365).

Molecular Analysis Level (Behavioral “Software”): Self-Regulation

Definition and typology

Self-Regulation refers to both unconscious and conscious processes that affect the ability to control responses (Baumeister and Heatherton, 1996; Brown, 1998; Black and Allen, 2017; McClelland et al., 2018). This process has overarching effects on an individual’s ability to tolerate unmet wants or needs, handle disappointments and failures, and work toward success. The ability to self-regulate is the foundation for compliance with accepted standards of conduct at home, school, and later, in the workplace (vanDellen and Hoyle, 2008). This personal variable, therefore, has recently been considered a meta-skill or an ability to manage other skills (de la Fuente, 2017). Self-regulation is often thought of as:

(1) Cognitive self-regulation (meta-cognitive skills), the degree to which persons can be self-reflective, and can plan and think ahead. Persons with these strengths are in control of their thoughts. They monitor their behavior, evaluate their abilities, and are able to adjust their behavior, if necessary. For example, if a self-regulated youngster knows there is an upcoming test, he or she chooses to study to be ready for the test, instead of hanging out with friends.

(2) Social-emotional self-regulation (meta-emotional skills) is the ability to inhibit negative responses and delay gratification. An individual with this ability is able to control his or her emotional reactions to positive and negative situations, as in the case of a child who can resist his immediate inclination to erupt into anger when a peer cuts in front of him in the lunch line.

(3) Behavior self-regulation (meta-behavioral skills). Brown (1998, p. 62) saw self-regulation as an individual’s capacity to plan, monitor and direct his or her behavior in changing situations.”

Barkley’s (1998) model was initially based on behavioral inhibition, but eventually the limitations that were detected and new lines of research led him to advance toward self-regulation. The main components of the model are: behavioral inhibition processes, the very concept of self-regulation/self-control, the EF involved, and motor control. The model is applied to the behavior and characteristics of children with ADHD (for a model review, see Servera-Barceló, 2005). Complementarily, the Zimmerman model (Zimmerman and Labuhn, 2012; Panadero, 2017) of Self-Regulated Learning has clearly shown that the self-control phase is the intermediate phase of self-regulation learning behavior. However, in the contexts of Personality or Clinical Psychology, Self-Control and Self-Regulation behavior are still considered equivalent, when the first is part of the second (Wiese et al., 2018; Duckworth et al., 2019).

Recent research has consistently reported that the self-regulation variable is a flourishing, positive, linear predictor of mental health, and a negative predictor of procrastination behaviors (Garzón-Umerenkova et al., 2018). In adolescents at risk for social exclusion, it is also a predictor of resilience (Artuch-Garde et al., 2017). It has further been established as a positive predictor of satisfaction in learning and performance (de la Fuente et al., 2015), as well as a negative predictor of anxiety (Ferrari et al., 2009; de la Fuente et al., 2017b) and the perception of maladaptive behaviors (de la Fuente et al., 2008; Ferrari et al., 2009). In complementary fashion, self-regulation has also been identified as an important procedural variable involved in adolescents’ competency in an interaction with alcohol, predicting low alcohol use (Ferrari et al., 2009; de la Fuente et al., 2017a). It has further been verified as a mediating variable in the effect produced by a mindfulness program (de la Fuente et al., 2018).

At the level applied to scholastic and academic learning, there is also an enormous amount of evidence regarding the importance and role of self-regulation during learning, i.e., self-regulated learning, in predicting motivation, learning and achievement (Schunk, 2005; Zimmerman and Labuhn, 2012; de la Fuente et al., 2014; Panadero and Alonso-Tapia, 2014; Greene et al., 2015, 2018; Bol et al., 2016; Sperling et al., 2016; Peters-Burton and Botov, 2017; Deekens et al., 2018; Greene, 2018; Li et al., 2018). In summary, the effects of self-regulation in scholastic and academic learning are (Schunk and Greene, 2018): (1) Higher academic achievement. Young people who are self-regulated are more likely to perform well in school; (2) School engagement. Adolescents who delay gratification and adjust their behavior are more likely to be engaged in school. Moreover, such students tend to work harder than their peers who lack self-regulating abilities do; (3) Peer social acceptance. Self-regulation is also linked to favorable perceptions by others. Children and adolescents who are able to control impulses and reflect on their actions are more likely to have friends and to get along with others; (4) Avoidance of negative behaviors. Self-regulated adolescents are less likely to engage in substance abuse, truancy, and violence; (5) Healthy eating patterns. Adolescents who are able to regulate their behavior are more likely to have healthy eating habits (Schutz and Davis, 2000; Strauman, 2002; Terry and Leary, 2011; Miller et al., 2018; Rademacher and Koglin, 2018).

Conclusion

Despite the important contributions of this variable, its theoretical domain does not allow for any accurate understanding of the effects of context, that is, of the educational environment, whether on academic behavior, or on behaviors related to health, work or social life. It is therefore necessary to include research at the next level in order to understand the interactive combination of subject × context.

Level of Molar Analysis (Real Interactive Behavior): Self- vs. External-Regulation

Definition

Despite abundant prior evidence, most models in use today whether produced at the level of micro-analysis or molecular analysis have paid little or no attention to the combined factors of self-regulation, that is, to the analysis of personal factors × contextual factors. Consequently, beginning with an awareness of the deeply interactive nature of behavior regulation, the theory of Self- vs. Externally-Regulated Behavior has been formulated at this level of psychological analysis (de la Fuente, 2017). See Tables 2, 3.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Conceptual continuum and typologies of each self-regulatory behavior.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Conceptual continuum of the Externally-Regulated Learning (ERL) context dimension.

This theoretical model postulates a combination of the subject’s level of self-regulation (SR), non-regulation (NR) or dysregulation (DR), with the context, which may be regulatory (RC), non-regulatory (NRC), or dysregulatory (DRC). This concept has the advantage of modeling the potential combinations of the regulatory level of the person and of the context, when explaining and predicting behaviors. Basically, the model can establish and predict the role of certain contexts by increasing or decreasing the likelihood of typical ADHD or attention deficit disorder (ADD) behaviors, assuming that these cannot be explained exclusively on the basis of neurological and individual characteristics. The model seeks to answer the following question: would the same person with low self-regulation (ADHD or ADD) behave the same way in a context that promotes self-regulation as they would in a context promoting dysregulation? The answer is that it seems unlikely. The model thus categorizes four possible effects resulting from the different combinations of levels of personal regulation (self-regulation) and a regulating context (promoting self-regulation). The proposed relationships have been tested with consistent effects in university teaching-learning processes (de la Fuente et al., 2015, 2017c), and are summarized in Table 4 below.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Combination types between levels of variables in the Theory of Self- vs. Externally-Regulated Learning.

(1) Low personal self-regulation (personal non-regulation/dysregulation) with a non-regulatory context (non-regulatory/dysregulatory context). Think of a subject with ADHD/ADD, characterized by openness to experience associated risk behaviors, who lives in a family with divorced parents, receives contradictory instruction from them, lacks an organized schedule, has distracting daily activities and lacks externally-induced motivation for learning. What likelihood is there that medication alone can correct the problem? According to the proposed theoretical model, this combination would probably result in low achievement and a good number of socio-personal problems. This thesis would be consistent with results that find a significant correlation between ADHD and delinquent behaviors, substance abuse and personal failure (Holmes et al., 2019; Sadek, 2019) assigning it some degree of causality but without in depth analysis of any other explanatory variables, with unexplained latent effects in this association relationship.

(2) Low personal self-regulation (personal non-regulation/dysregulation) with a medium/high regulatory context. Think of a subject with ADHD/ADD characterized by openness to experience associated risk behaviors, who lives in a family with congruent parenting behaviors, with shared educational criteria and instruction, family organization in support of the child, a well-organized schedule, control over distracting activities, as well as externally-induced motivation and capacity for learning. What likelihood is there that an educational psychology intervention will enhance the effect of medication? This thesis would be consistent with evidence showing that the combined effects of medication and educational psychology intervention produce better effects, though without reaching an optimal level of achievement, learning, and development (Sherman et al., 2008; Dvorsky and Langberg, 2016).

(3) Medium/High personal self-regulation (SR) with a non-regulatory context. Think of a subject with ADHD/ADD with personal characteristics of medium/high regulation (SR), who lives in a family with divorced parents, receives contradictory instruction from them, lacks an organized schedule, has distracting daily activities and lacks externally-induced motivation for learning. What likelihood is there that self-regulated behavior will persist over time? According to the theoretical model, this combination would probably result in underachievement in comparison to the subject’s potential, and in certain socio-personal problems. This thesis would be consistent with evidence that refers to persons with ADHD having maladjusted behaviors as a consequence of a non-regulatory or dysregulatory context (Beattie, 2012). In this case, it is the context that is actively promoting dysregulation. We might consider that certain false positives fall into this case, children who are restless and lost, who have been socialized in a paradigm of dispersed attention, and are untrained in daily tasks, although they have potential for such training.

(4) Medium/High personal self-regulation (SR) with a medium/highly-regulatory context (RC). Think of a subject with a medium/high level of regulation, who lives in a family with congruent parenting behaviors, with shared educational criteria, family organization in support of the child, a well-organized schedule, control over distracting activities, mediation in external activities and friendships, as well as externally-induced motivation and capacity for learning. This thesis would be consistent with evidence that shows this to be best combination for optimal learning and academic achievement (de la Fuente et al., 2017c; Fong et al., 2018). In the sphere of developmental psychology, some recent studies have established the role of the family as an external regulatory context, and precursor of self-regulation and of executive function (Berner et al., 2010), consistent with postulates of the theoretical model presented here.

Conclusion

Introducing contextual variables into the study of self-regulatory processes seems to be essential, when we realize their role in determining behavior, whether in teaching processes (Vermunt, 1989; Vermunt, 2007; Timmons et al., 2016; Fryer and Vermunt, 2018), or in other contexts (Spruce and Bol, 2015).

Assessment of Constructs

In Psychology, one of the parameters used for clearly defining a particular psychological construct has been the analysis of instruments associated with measuring that construct. This enables us to accurately understand the factors that make up the construct, as well as the theoretical-empirical adequacy of how the concept is formulated. Therefore, analyzing the different instruments in use should allow us to delimit the differences more accurately between the domains and/or levels of the constructs involved.

Assessment in Micro-Analysis: Executive Functions (see Supplementary Material Anex 1)

Esopo et al. (2018) state that “executive control is a broad term commonly referring to the maintenance and execution of high-level plans and goals, and involves planning, cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, and working memory processes” (p. 31). They have summarized different inventories of executive functions. These include (see Supplementary  Material Anex 1).

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function – Adult Version, BRIEF-A (Roth et al., 2005, 2013)

This self-report measure of executive function contains 75 items that assess attention regulation, inhibitory and emotion control, working memory, planning, and organization behaviors.

Consideration of Future Consequences (CFCs) (Strathman et al., 1994)

Consideration of Future Consequences quantifies the extent to which individuals consider potential future outcomes of their current behavior and is predictive of a number of health behaviors (Chapman, 2005), making it especially relevant for adherence. The CFC is a common, cross-culturally validated measure with attractive psychometric properties.

Deferment of Gratification Scale (DGS) (Ray and Najman, 1986)

As an alternative measure to the CFC, the Deferment of Gratification Scale (DGS) assesses the ability to resist the temptation of an immediate reward and instead wait for a larger, reward later (Carducci, 2009). The DGS comprises two factors relevant to adherence behavior: controlling impulses and planning and waiting. It was specifically designed to target intertemporal economic behavior, originally to explain social mobility or lack thereof. Participants choose ratings from “strongly disagree” (0) to “strongly agree” (5) on 12 items. Six items are ‘positive’ (such as, “I am good at saving my money”) and six are reversed (for example, “I agree with the philosophy ‘eat, drink and be merry, for tomorrow we may all be dead.”’).

Today, assessment of executive functions has evolved toward a virtual reality format, thereby improving its ecological validity and minimizing error factors (Díaz-Orueta et al., 2012, 2013; Bombín-González et al., 2014; Climent-Martínez et al., 2014; Azouvi et al., 2015; Iriarte et al., 2016; Nir-Hadad et al., 2017).

Assessment in Molecular Analysis: Self-Regulation (see Supplementary Material Anex 1)

Adolescent self-regulatory inventory

This is a 36-item questionnaire used to measure self-regulation of teens. Respondents rate how true each item is for them, ranging from 1 (not true of me at all) to 5 (quite true of me). A sum or average of the items is calculated. After reverse coding items 1, 2, 5, 6, 7, 8, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 18, 19, 21, 34, 35, higher scores indicate ability to self-regulate. The range of response is: (1) Not at all true of me; (2) Not very true of me; (3) Neither true nor untrue of me; (4) Somewhat true of me; (5) Quite true of me.

Self-regulation questionnaire (SRQ) (Brown et al., 1999)

This is a 63-item questionnaire used to measure self-regulation of teens. The questionnaire includes seven subscales that measure seven steps of self-regulation: receiving relevant information, evaluating the information and comparing it to norms, triggering change, searching for options, formulating a plan, implementing the plan, assessing the plan’s effectiveness. The short version has 17 items, for university students (Pichardo et al., 2014; Garzón-Umerenkova et al., 2017).

Assessment in Molar Analysis: Assessment of Self- vs. External-Regulation (see Supplementary Material Anex 1)

This level of assessment adopts the principles of interactivity by involving a joint assessment process, including both self-assessment and other-assessment of the teaching process and the learning process, under the premise that both of these will have either a regulatory nature (high score), an a-regulatory nature (mid-range score) or a dysregulatory nature (low score).

The Interactive Assessment of the Teaching-Learning Process, IATLP (de la Fuente and Martínez-Vicente, 2007), is based on the conception that self-regulation and self-regulation during the learning process may be induced externally, through regulatory teaching (de la Fuente and Justicia, 2003). The psychometric characteristics of this instrument are consistent and have been reported previously (de la Fuente et al., 2010).

Discussion and Conclusion

The conceptual analysis offered here leads to several aspects worth mentioning for present and future research.

(1) We have observed how, at the micro-analysis level of basic psychological research (neuropsychological level), the construct most used is that of Executive Functions; at the molecular level of research, models use the construct Self-Regulation (or Self-Regulated learning), and at the molar level of research while maintaining the SR construct the context has also been incorporated in constructs, i.e., Self-Regulated vs. Externally-Regulated Learning. The concept of Executive Functions may be considered an essentially psychoneurological or psychiatric concept, that is, a medical concept, and therefore it minimizes the role of other personal and contextual variables, as well as being based on the assumption of the very concept of a “disorder” in connection with executive functions.

(2) It is clearly evident that some overlap exists between constructs and levels of psychological research, leading to confusion in the short term, and in the medium term fuzziness as to the proper connections between the different levels of research. If the executive functions construct is assumed to have certain elements in common with the self-regulation construct (SR), more behavioral in nature, and the similarities and differences are not clearly established, it may result in conceptual absorption and supplantation between different theoretical models that are complementary and belong to different research domains, as has been shown in this paper. A similar case has been shown between self-control and self-regulation, in which, in addition, it is intended to explain the performance in the absence of context variables or the teaching process (Duckworth et al., 2019) The impression is that each level of research and conceptual domain seeks to explain the totality of the problem, when this is only possible through integration and complementarity between the three levels of research.

(3) Adopting an approach from the exclusive perspective of micro-analysis while adequate for explaining neuropsychological disorders is plainly insufficient at the molecular level, and especially at the molar level, where contextual factors are essential for explaining behavioral variability and the subject × context interaction. In such significant behaviors as antisocial conduct, when basic research (micro-analysis level) takes a leap into psycho-educational or psychosocial implications (molar level), without considering possible subject × context interactions, it becomes too easy to conclude, risking attributional bias, that differences in executive functions in subjects with and without behavior or personality disorders explain these complex behaviors (for a review, see De Brito and Hodgind, 2009). While acknowledging the importance of contributions from micro-analysis, the fact that nothing else is known about the subjects’ characteristics or about their context must be recognized as an important limitation. If not, the socialization processes of these subjects, and what we know of their explanatory value (molecular level of psychological analysis), might be considered void, not to mention the influence of context in these behaviors (molar level of psychological analysis). From there, it would be easy to infer a deficit in executive functions as the cause of these maladjusted behaviors, disregarding any other factors (Pellerone et al., 2016). From a neurological and clinical point of view, this may be understandable (Anderson and Bolden, 2018), but from the psycho-educational and psychosocial viewpoint, it is simply unacceptable.

Limitations, Implications, and Future Prospects

The essential limitation of this paper is that its focus is only analytical; it lacks an empirical meta-analysis of the publications produced at each level. This meta-analysis remains pending and it is necessary to do it in the future, to provide empirical evidence to the analyses made in this theoretical review. Nonetheless, we can infer certain implications from the foregoing analysis:

(1) Each level of Psychology research and analysis must refocus and address the level and research domain to which it belongs: micro-analysis, molecular analysis, or molar analysis. For this reason, new researchers must be taught the benefits and limitations of each level in question, so that they might find their place at their level of choice and perform their scientific work within their sphere of competence. In our case, we understand that while the construct of executive functions is attractive to researchers due to the halo effect coming from neurological support, the self-regulation construct possesses greater behavioral explanatory power (by incorporating the three levels of regulation, a-regulation, and dysregulation). Furthermore, it can (now) also incorporate the external component of regulation, through the construct of external regulation (regulatory, a-regulatory, and dysregulatory contexts), making it possible to carry out interactive, subject × context analyses, typical of Psychology’s domain as a behavioral science.

(2) It is essential that we work toward creating cross-level research that makes connections and validates the relationships between conceptual and empirical models at the different levels (Moore et al., 2018; Musso et al., 2019; Sánchez et al., 2019). Only in this way can we verify the external validity of each construct, not only between constructs at the same level, but also between constructs in different research levels and domains. Some prior studies have already begun to investigate in this direction, connecting executive functions – > self-regulation – > external regulation (Calkins and Marcovitch, 2010; Follmer and Sperling, 2016, 2018a,b,c; Aadland et al., 2018; Baumeister et al., 2018; Rutherford et al., 2018; Wagner et al., 2018; Wimmer et al., 2019). However, studies are needed that evaluate the relevance and potential role of context in self-regulating behavior, and its role as a regulatory/non-regulatory/dysregulatory agent, all of this in different behavioral issues, as established by SRL vs. ERL theory. Lacking this, just as we speak of neuromyths in education (Howard-Jones, 2014), alluding to erroneous educational conceptions about the brain and its functioning, unsupported by neurological science, we might also speak of psychomyths in neurology, alluding to the myths of neurology that psycho-educational science does not support at molecular and molar levels. The very concept of neuro-education attempts to establish a connection between the two distal levels, micro-analysis/neurology and molar analysis/education (for a detailed analysis, see Pallarés-Dominguez, 2016), without regard for the qualitative leap and the difficulty involved in connecting and conceptually integrating the two levels of research. “Executive functioning lays the basis for behavioral self-regulation in real-life contexts, such as adapting one’s behavior according to social standards and working consistently toward long-term goals despite the temptations of short-term pleasures (it is important to note that several terms with overlapping definitions, such as behavioral self-regulation, effortful control, and self-control, are used in the literature with regard to the construct we are referring to as behavioral self-regulation)” (Gunzenhauser et al., 2017, pp 30–31).

(3) Research ethics must be kept in mind, reminding us that publications that introduce and promote psychological constructs must be unequivocally backed by evidence presented in empirical research. If this is not the case, opinion trends and new myths will be created, based on a false belief in the unlimited explanatory power of Neurology and Psychiatry, to the detriment of important, consolidated scientific contributions from other levels of Psychology research.

Author Contributions

JF have made the general analysis and first version of the manuscript. MG-T has made the second review of the Self-regulation and Externally regulation sections of ADHD. MA-S has performed a review of the neuroanalysis level of ADHD. JM-V has carried out the carried out the revision bibliographic by sections. FP-S has reviewed the professional applicability of the manuscript. MV has carried out a review of the section on professional ethics and future vision of research.

Funding

This study was supported by R&D Project EDU2011-24805, PGC2018-094672-B-I00 (Ministry of Science and Education, Spain), and UAL18-SEJ-DO31-A-FEDER (University of Almería), and the European Social Fund.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01919/full#supplementary-material

References

Aadland, K. N., Aadland, E., Andersen, J. R., Lervåg, A., Moe, V. F., Resaland, G. K., et al. (2018). Executive function, behavioral self-regulation, and school related well-being did not mediate the effect of school-based physical activity on academic performance in numeracy in 10-year-old children. The active smarter kids (ASK) study. Front. Psychol. 9:245. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agostino, A., Johnson, J., and Pascual-Leone, J. (2010). Executive functions underlying multiplicative reasoning: problem type matters. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 105, 285–305. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, J., and Bolden, J. (2018). The Role of Executive Functions in Depression and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Symptomatology. Ph. D Thesis. University of Tennessee: Knoxville, TN.

Google Scholar

Arán, V., and López, M. B. (2016). Predictores de la comprensión lectora en niños y adolescentes: el papel de la edad, el sexo y las funciones ejecutivas [Predictors of reading comprehension in children and adolescents: the role of age, sex, and executive functions]. Cuadernos Neuropsicol. 10, 24–44. doi: 10.7714/CNPS/10.1.202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardilla Ardila, A., and Ostrosky Solís, F. (2008). Desarrollo Histórico de las Funciones Ejecutivas [Historical development of executive functions]. Revista Neuropsicología, Neuropsiquiatría y Neurociencias 8, 1–21.

Google Scholar

Arias, G., and de los Ángeles, M. (2012). Las funciones ejecutivas cálidas y el rendimiento académico [Warm executive functions and academic performance]. Doctoral thesis, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid.

Google Scholar

Artuch-Garde, R., González-Torres, M. C., de la Fuente, J., Vera, M. M., Fernández-Cabezas, M., and López-García, M. (2017). Relationship between resilience and self-regulation: a study of Spanish youth at risk of social exclusion. Front. Psychol. 8:612. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azouvi, P., Vallat-Azouvi, C., Millox, V., Darnoux, E., Ghout, I., Azerad, S., et al. (2015). Ecological validity of the dysexecutive questionnaire: results from the PariS-TBI study. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 25, 864–878. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2014.990907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A. Hum. Exp. Psychol. 49A, 5–28. doi: 10.1080/713755608

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A., and Wilson, B. B. (1988). Frontal amnesia and dysexecutive syndrome. Brain Cogn. 7, 212–230. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(88)90031-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baggetta, P., and Alexander, P. A. (2016). Conceptualization and operationalization of executive function. Mind Brain Educ. 10, 10–33. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (1997a). ADHD and the Nature of Self-Control. New York, NY: Guilford.

Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (1997b). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive function: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 121, 65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (1998). “A theory of ADHD: inhibition, executive functions, self-control, and time,” in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorders: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment, ed. R. A. Barkley (New York, NY: Guilford), 225–262.

Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Heatherton, T. D. (1996). Self-regulation failure: an overview. Psychol. Inq. 7, 1–15. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0701_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., and Vohs, K. D. (2018). “Self-regulation and the executive function: the self as controlling agent,” in Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, 2nd Edn, eds A. W. Kruglanski and E. T. Higgins (New York, NY: The Guilford Press).

Google Scholar

Beattie, D. M. N. (2012). An Exploration of the Relationship Between Parental Stress and Psychopatology within the Context of Childhood Attention Deficyt and Hyperactivity. Ph. D. Thesis. University of Soudhapton: Soudhapton.

Google Scholar

Berner, A., Carlson, S. M., and Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Dev. 81, 326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., and Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learn. Individ. Diff. 21, 327–336. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Black, S., and Allen, J. D. (2017). Part 2: goals, mindset, and self-regulation. Ref. Librar. 58, 159–173. doi: 10.1080/02763877.2016.1221787

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C., and Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. 78, 647–663. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bol, L., Campbell, K. D. Y., Perez, T., and Yen, C. J. (2016). The effects of self-regulated learning training on community college students’ metacognition and achievement in developmental math courses. Comm. Coll. J. Res. Pract. 40, 480–495. doi: 10.1080/10668926.2015.1068718

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bombín-González, I., Cifuentes-Rodríguez, A., Climent-Martínez, G., Luna-Lario, P., Cardas-Ibáñez, J., Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., et al. (2014). Validez ecológica y entornos multitarea en la evaluación de las funciones ejecutivas. [Ecological validity and multitask environments in assessment of executive functions.]. Rev. Neurol. 59, 77–87.

Google Scholar

Brock, L., Rimm-Kaufman, S., Nathanson, L., and Grimm, K. (2009). The contributions of “hot” and “cool” executive function to children’s academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Child. Res. Q. 24, 337–349. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, J. M. (1998). “Self-regulation and the addictive behaviours,” in Treating Addictive Behaviors, 2nd Edn, eds W. R. Miller and N. Heather (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 61–73. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-1934-2_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, J. M., Miller, W. R., and Lawendowski, L. A. (1999). “The self-regulation questionnaire,” in Innovations in Clinical Practice: A Source Book, eds L. Vandecreek and T. L. Jackson (Sarasota, FL: Professional Resources Press), 281–293.

Google Scholar

Bull, R., Espy, K. A., and Wiebe, S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at 7 years. Dev. Neuropsychol. 33, 205–228. doi: 10.1080/87565640801982312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calkins, S. D., and Marcovitch, S. (2010). “Emotion regulation and executive functioning in early development: integrated mechanisms of control supporting adaptive functioning,” in Human Brain Development. Child Development at the Intersection of Emotion and Cognition, eds S. D. Calkins and M. A. Bell (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 37–57. doi: 10.1037/12059-003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canet, L., Introzzi, I., Andrés, M. L., and Stelzer, F. (2016). La contribución de las funciones ejecutivas a la autorregulación. [The contribution of Executive functions to Self-regulation.]. Panam. J. Neuropsychol. 10, 106–128. doi: 10.7714/CNPS/10.2.206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carducci, B. J. (2009). The Psychology of Personality: Viewpoints, Research, and Applications. Malden, MA: John Wiley & Sons.

Google Scholar

Casey, R. (1992). The role of the frontal lobe in the regulation of cognitive development. Brain Cogn. 20, 51–73. doi: 10.1080/87565640801982312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, G. B. (2005). Short-term cost for long-term benefit: time preference and cancer control. Health Psychol. 24(4, Suppl.), S41–S48. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.24.4.S41

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cid-Sillero, S., Santiago-Ramajo, S., and Martín-Lobo, M. P. (2018). Relation between executive functions and empathy and their influence on academic performance in students of basic vocational training. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 16, 517–536.

Google Scholar

Cleary, T. J. Y., and Chen, P. P. (2009). Self-regulation, motivation and math achievement in middle school: variations across grade level and math context. J. Sch. Psychol. 47, 291–314. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2009.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Climent-Martínez, G., Luna-Lario, P., Bombín-González, I., Cifuentes-Rodríguez, A., Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., and Díaz-Orueta, U. (2014). Evaluación neuropsicológica de las funciones ejecutivas mediante realidad virtual. [Neuropsychological assessment of executive functions through virtual reality]. Rev. Neurol. 58, 465–475.

Google Scholar

De Brito, S., and Hodgind, S. (2009). “Executive functions of persistent violent offenders: a critical review of the literature,” in The Neurobiological Basis of Violence: Science and Rehabilitation, eds S. Hodgins, E. Viding, and A. Plodowski (West Sussex: Oxford University Press), 167–199.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J. (2017). Theory of self- vs. externally-regulated learningTM: fundamentals, evidence, and applicability. Front. Psychol. 8:1675. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Cubero, I., Sánchez-Amate, M. C., Peralta, F. J., Garzón, A., and Fiz-Pérez, J. (2017a). The adolescent’s competency for interacting with alcohol as a determinant of intake: the role of self-regulation. Front. Psychol. 8:1800. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01800

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., and Justicia, F. (2003). Regulación de la enseñanza para la autorregulación del aprendizaje en la Universidad [Regulating teaching for self-regulated learning at University]. Aula Abierta 82, 161–171.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., López-García, M., Vera, M. M., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., and Zapata, L. (2017b). Personal self-regulation, learning approaches, resilience and test anxiety in psychology students. Estud. Educ. 32, 9–26. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., López-García, M., Zapata, L., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Vera, M. M., Solinas, G., et al. (2014). Competency to study and learn in stressful contexts: fundamentals of the e-Coping with academic stress utility. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 12, 717–746. doi: 10.14204/ejrep.34.14034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Mañas, I., Franco, C., Cangas, A., and Soriano, E. (2018). Differential effect of level of self-regulation and mindfulness training on coping strategies used by university students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15, 2230. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15102230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., and Martínez-Vicente, J. M. (2007). Scales for the Interactive Assessment of the Teaching Learning Process, IATLP. Almería: Education & Psychology I+D+I.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Peralta, F. J., and Sánchez-Roda, M. D. (2008). Autorregulación personal y percepción de los comportamientos desadaptativos. [Personal self-regulation and perception of maladaptive behaviors.]. Psicothema 21, 548–554.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Sander, P., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Vera, M., Garzón-Umerenkova, A., and Fadda, S. (2017c). Combined effect of levels in personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching on meta-cognitive, meta-motivational, and academic achievement variables in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 8:232. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Sander, P., Justicia, F., Pichardo, M. C., and García-Berbén, A. B. (2010). Validation study of the scale for assessment of the teaching-learning process, student version (ATLP-S). Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 8, 815–840.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Zapata, L., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Sander, P., and Cardelle-Elawar, M. (2015). The role of personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching to predict motivational-affective variables, achievement, and satisfaction: a structural model. Front. Psychol. 6:399. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De los Ángeles, M. (2012). Las funciones ejecutivas cálidas y el rendimiento académico [Hot executive functions and academic performance]. Doctoral dissertation, University of Madrid: Madrid.

Google Scholar

Deekens, V. M., Greene, J. A., and Lobczowski, N. G. (2018). Monitoring and depth of strategy use in computer-based learning environments for science and history. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 88, 63–79. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 21, 335–341. doi: 10.1177/0963721412453722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A., and Ling, D. S. (2016). Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 34–48. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz-Orueta, U., García-López, C., Crespo-Eguílaz, N., Sánchez-Carpintero, R., Climent, G., and Narbona, J. (2013). AULA virtual reality test as an attention measure: convergent validity with conners’ continuous performance test. Child Neuropsychol. 20, 328–342. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2013.792332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Díaz-Orueta, U., Iriarte, Y., Climent-Martínez, G., and Banterla, F. (2012). An ecological virtual reality test with distractors for attention in children and adolescents. J. Virt. Real. 5, 1–20.

Google Scholar

Díez, E., and Bausela, E. (2018). Funciones ejecutivas y la competencia para resolver problemas matemáticos en educación primaria [Executive functions and the competence to solve mathematical problems in primary education]. Panam. J. Neuropsychol. 12, 43–57.

Google Scholar

Duckworth, A. L., Taxer, J. L., Eskreis-Winkler, L., Galla, B. M., and Gross, J. J. (2019). Self-control and academic achievement. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 70, 373–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dvorsky, M. R., and Langberg, J. M. (2016). A review of factors that promote resilience in youth with ADHD and ADHD symptoms. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 19, 368–391. doi: 10.1007/s10567-016-0216-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El Haj, M., and Allain, P. (2012). What do we know about the relationship between source monitoring deficits and executive dysfunction? Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 22, 449–472. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2012.658267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliot, R. (2003). Executive functions and their disorders. Br. Med. Bull. 65, 49–59. doi: 10.1093/bmb/65.1.49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eshel, N., Nelson, E. E., Blair, R. J., Pine, D. S., and Ernst, M. (2007). Neural substrates of choices selection in adults and adolescents: development of the ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulated cortices. Neuropsychologia 45, 1270–1279. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esopo, K., Mellowb, D., Thomasc, C., Uckatd, H., Abrahame, J., Jainf, P., et al. (2018). Measuring self-efficacy, executive function, and temporal discounting in Kenya. Behav. Res. Ther. 101, 30–45. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, J. R., Stevens, E. B., and Jason, L. A. (2009). The role of self-regulation in abstinence maintenance: effects of communal living on self-regulation. J. Groups Addict. Recov. 4, 32–41. doi: 10.1080/15560350802712371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Follmer, D. J. (2018). Executive function and reading comprehension: a meta-analytic review. Educ. Psychol. 53, 42–60. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2017.1309295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Follmer, D. J., and Sperling, R. A. (2016). The mediating role of metacognition in the relationship between executive function and self-regulated learning. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 86, 559–575. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Follmer, D. J., and Sperling, R. A. (2018c). Interactions between reader and text: contributions of cognitive processes, strategy use, and text cohesion to comprehension of expository science text. Learn. Individ. Diff. 67, 177–187. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2018.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Follmer, D. J., and Sperling, R. A. (2018a). A latent variable analysis of the contribution of executive function to adult readers’ comprehension of science text: the roles of vocabulary ability and level of comprehension. Read. Writ. 32, 377–403. doi: 10.1007/s11145-018-9872-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Follmer, D. J., and Sperling, R. A. (2018b). Examining the role of self-regulated learning microanalysis in the assessment of learners’ regulation. J. Exp. Educ. 87, 269–287. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2017.1409184

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fong, W., Mejía, L. P., and Amaya, N. (2018). Metacognition and its association with teacher quality and student attitude in engineering students. Contemp. Eng. Sci. 11, 3035–3041. doi: 10.12988/ces.2018.87306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fryer, L. K., and Vermunt, J. D. (2018). Regulating approaches to learning: testing learning strategy convergences across a year at university. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 88, 21–41. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuster, J. M. (1989). The Prefrontal Cortex. New York, NY: Raven Press.

Google Scholar

Fuster, J. M. (1997). The Prefrontal Cortex-Anatomy, Physiology, and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott-Raven.

Google Scholar

Fuster, J. M. (2002). Frontal lobe and cognitive development. J. Neurocytol. 31, 373–385.

Google Scholar

Gaillardin, F., and Baudri, S. (2018). Influence of working memory and executive function on stair ascent and descent in young and older adults. Exp. Gerontol. 106, 74–79. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2018.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Molina, A., Enseñat, A., Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., and Roig-Rovira, T. (2009). Maduración de la corteza prefrontal y desarrollo de las funciones ejecutivas durante los primeros cinco años de vida. Rev. Neurol. 48, 435–440. doi: 10.33588/rn.4808.2008265

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garzón-Umerenkova, A., de la Fuente, J., Amate, J., Paoloni, P. V., Fadda, S., and Pérez, J. F. (2018). A linear empirical model of self-regulation on flourishing, health, procrastination, and achievement, among university students. Front. Psychol. 9:536. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garzón-Umerenkova, A., de la Fuente, J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Zapata-Sevillano, L., Pichardo, M. C., and García-Berbén, A. B. (2017). Validation of the Spanish short self-regulation questionnaire (SSSRQ) through rasch analysis. Front. Psychol. 8:276. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gathercole, S. E., and Pickering, S. J. (2000a). Assessment of working memory in six and seven year old children. J. Educ. Psychol. 92, 377–390. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.2.377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gathercole, S. E., and Pickering, S. J. (2000b). Working memory deficits in children with low achievement in the national curriculum at 7 years of age. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 70, 177–194. doi: 10.1348/000709900158047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gil-Fenoy, M. J., García-García, J., Carmona-Samper, E., and Ortega-Campos, E. (2018). Antisocial behavior and executive functions in young offenders. Rev. Psicodidáct. 23, 70–76. doi: 10.1016/j.psicod.2017.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, J. A. (2018). Self-Regulation in Education. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Greene, J. A., Bolick, C. M., Caprino, A. M., Deekens, V. M., McVea, M., Yu, S. B., et al. (2015). Fostering high-school students’ self-regulated learning online and across academic domains. High Sch. J. 99, 88–106. doi: 10.1353/hsj.2015.0019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, J. A., Deekens, V. M., Copeland, D. Z., and Yu, S. (2018). “Capturing and modeling self-regulated learning using think-aloud protocols,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation of Learning and Performance, 2nd Edn, eds D. H. Schunk and J. A. Greene (New York, NY: Routledge), 323–337. doi: 10.4324/9781315697048-21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunzenhauser, C., Saalbach, H., and von Suchodoletz, A. (2017). Boys have caught up, family influences still continue: influences on executive functioning and behavioral self-regulation in elementary students in Germany. PsyCh. J. 6, 29–42. doi: 10.1002/pchj.157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, J., Bryant, A., and Gathercole, S. E. (2019). Protocol for a transdiagnostic study of children with problems of attention, learning and memory (CALM). BMC Pediatr. 19:10. doi: 10.1186/s12887-018-1385-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S. C., and Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: age-related changes and individual differences. Dev. Neuropsychol. 28, 617–644. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard-Jones, P. A. (2014). Neuroscience and education: myths and messages. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 817–824. doi: 10.1038/nrn3817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iriarte, Y., Diaz-Orueta, U., Cueto, E., Irazustabarrena, P., Banterla, F., and Climent, G. (2016). AULA-advanced virtual reality tool for the assessment of attention: normative study in Spain. J. Attent. Disord. 20, 542–568. doi: 10.1177/1087054712465335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacoby, N., and Lavidor, M. (2018). Null tDCS effects in a sustained attention task: the modulating role of learning. Front. Psychol. 9:476. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D. (1982). The problem of assessing executive functions. J. Psychol. 17, 281–297.

Google Scholar

Li, J., Ye, H., Tang, Y., Zhou, Z., and Hu, X. (2018). What are the effects of self-regulation phases and strategies for chinese students? a meta-analysis of two decades of research of the association between self-regulation and academic performance. Front. Psychol. 9:2434. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lilienfeld, S. O., Sauvigné, K. C., Lynn, S. J., Cautin, R. L., Latzman, R. D., and Waldman, I. D. (2015). Fifty psychological and psychiatric terms to avoid: a list of inaccurate, misleading, misused, ambiguous, and logically confused words and phrases. Front. Psychol. 6:1100. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lukito, S. D., O’Daly, O. G., Lythgoe, D. J., Whitwell, S., Debnam, A., Murphy, C. M., et al. (2018). Neural correlates of duration discrimination in young adults with autism spectrum disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their comorbid presentation. Front. Psychiatry 9:569. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luria, R. (1966). Human Brain and Psychological Processess. New York, NY: Harper and Row Publishers.

Google Scholar

McCabe, D. P., and Castel, A. D. (2008). Seeing is believing: the effect of brain images on judgements of scientific reasoning. Cognition 107, 343–352. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClelland, M., Geldhof, J., Morrison, F., Gestsdóttir, S., Cameron, C., Bowers, E., et al. (2018). “Self-regulation,” in Handbook of Life Course Health Development, eds N. Halfon, C. Forrest, R. Lerner, and E. Faustman (Cham: Springer), 275–297. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-47143-3_12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meltzer, L. (2007). Executive Function in Education: From Theory to Practice. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Metcalfe, J., and Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. Psychol. Rev. 106, 3–19. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, A. L., Gearhardt, A. G., Fredericks, E. M., Katz, B., Shapiro, L. K., Holden, K., et al. (2018). Targeting self-regulation to promote health behaviors in children. Behav. Res. Ther. 101, 71–81. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex frontal lobe tasks. A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molfese, V. J., Molfese, P. J., Molfese, D. L., Rudasill, K. M., Armstrong, N., and Starkey, G. (2010). Executive function skills of 6-8 year olds: brain and behavioral evidence and implications for school achievement. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 35, 116–125. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monette, S., Bigras, M., and Guay, M. C. (2011). The role of the executive functions in school achievement at the end of Grade 1. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 109, 158–173. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, D. A., Russell, A. E., Matthews, J., Ford, J. J., Rogers, M., Ukoumunne, O. C., et al. (2018). School-based interventions for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review with multiple synthesis methods. Rev. Educ. 6, 209–263. doi: 10.1002/rev3.3149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Musso, M. F., Boekaertsc, M., Segersd, M., and Cascallare, E. D. (2019). Individual differences in basic cognitive processes and self-regulated learning: their interaction effects on math performance. Learn. Individ. Diff. 71, 58–70. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2019.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nir-Hadad, S. H., Weiss, P. L., Waizman, A., Schwartz, N., and Kizony, R. (2017). A virtual shopping task for the assessment of executive functions: validity for people with stroke. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 27, 808–833. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2015.1109523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pallarés-Dominguez, D. (2016). Neuroeducación en diálogo: neuromitos en el proceso de enseñanza-aprendizaje y en la educación moral [Neuroeducation in dialogue: neuromites in the teaching-learning process and in moral education]. Pensamiento 273, 941–958.

Google Scholar

Panadero, A., and Alonso-Tapia, J. (2014). ¿Cómo autorregulan nuestros alumnos? Revisión del modelo cíclico de Zimmerman sobre estrategias de aprendizaje [How do our students self-regulate? Review of Zimmerman’s cyclical model of learning strategies]. An. Psicol. 30, 450–462. doi: 10.6018/analesps.30.2.167221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panadero, E. (2017). A review of self-regulated learning: six models and four directions for research. Front. Psychol. 8:422. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellerone, M., Iacolino, C., Mannino, G., Formica, I., and Zabbara, S. (2016). L’influenza di parenting su cognitive maladattive schema: una ricerca trasversale su un gruppo di adulti [The influence of parenting on cognitive maladaptive schema: a cross-sectional research on a group of adults]. J. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 10, 47–58. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S117371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pera-Guardiola, V., Batalla, I., Bosque, J., Kosson, D., Pifarré, J., Hernández-Rivas, R., et al. (2016). Modulatory effects of psychopathology on Wisconsin card sorting test performance in male offenders with antisocial personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 235, 43–48. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters-Burton, E. E., and Botov, I. S. (2017). Self-regulated learning microanalysis as a tool to inform professional development delivery in real-time. Metacogn. Learn. 12, 45–78. doi: 10.1007/s11409-016-9160-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pichardo, C., Justicia, F., de la Fuente, J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., and García-Berbén, A. B. (2014). Factor structure of the self-regulation questionnaire (SRQ) at Spanish Universities. Span. J. Psychol. 17, E62. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2014.63

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pintrich, P. R. (2000a). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: the role of goal orientation in learning and achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 92, 544–555. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.3.544

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pintrich, P. R. (2000b). “The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation, eds M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 451–502. doi: 10.1016/b978-012109890-2/50043-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prencipe, A., Kesek, A., Cohen, J., Lamm, C., Lewis, M. D., and Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 621–637. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rademacher, A., and Koglin, U. (2018). The concept of self-regulation and preschoolers’ social-emotional development: a systematic review. Early Child Dev. Care 1–19. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2018.1450251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramírez, Y. (2014). Predictores neuropsicológicos de las habilidades académicas [Neuropsychological predictors of academic skills]. Panam. J. Neuropsychol. 8, 155–170.

Google Scholar

Ray, J. J., and Najman, J. M. (1986). The generalizability of deferment of gratification. J. Soc. Psychol. 126, 117–119. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1986.9713578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romero-López, M., Pichardo, M. C., Inguglia, S., and Justicia, F. (2015). The role of executive function in social competence and behavioral problems in the last year of preschool. Ann. Psychol. 34, 490–499. doi: 10.6018/analesps.34.3.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, R. H., Lance, C. E., Isquith, P. K., Fischer, A. D., and Giancola, P. R. (2013). Confirmatory factor analysis of the behavior rating inventory of executive function-adult version in healthy adults and application to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 5, 425–434. doi: 10.1093/arclin/act031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, R. M., Isquith, P. K., and Gioia, G. A. (2005). BRIEF-A: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function–Adult Version: Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Google Scholar

Rubia, K. (2018). Cognitive neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and its clinical translation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 29:100. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutherford, T., Buschkuehl, T., Jaeggi, S. M., and Farkas, G. (2018). Links between achievement, executive functions, and self-regulated learning. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 32, 763–774. doi: 10.1002/acp.3462

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadek, J. (2019). Clinician’s Guide to ADHD Comorbidities in Children and Adolescents. Case Studios. London: Springer.

Google Scholar

Sánchez, M., Lavigne, R., Romero, J. F., and Elósegui, E. (2019). Emotion regulation in participants diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, before and after an emotion regulation intervention. Front. Psychol. 10:1092. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schunk, D. H. (2005). Self-regulated learning: the educational legacy of Paul R. Pintrich. Educ. Psychol. 40, 85–94. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep4002_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schunk, D. H., and Greene, J. A. (eds) (2018). Handbook of Self-Regulation of Learning and Performance, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Schutz, P. A., and Davis, H. A. (2000). Emotions and self-regulation during test taking. Educ. Psychol. 35, 243–256. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3504_03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Servera-Barceló, M. (2005). Modelo de autorregulación de Barkley aplicado al trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad: una revisión [Barkley’s model of self-regulation apllied to attention deficit activity disorder: a review”]. Rev. Neurol. 40, 358–368.

Google Scholar

Sherman, J., Rassmusen, C., and Baydala, L. (2008). The impact of teacher factors on achievement and behavioural outcomes of children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a review of the literatura. Educ. Res. 50, 347–360. doi: 10.1080/00131880802499803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperling, R. A., Ramsay, C. M., Reeves, P. M., Follmer, D. J., and Richmond, A. S. (2016). Supporting students’ knowledge construction and self-regulation through the use of elaborative processing strategies. Middle Sch. J. 47, 25–32. doi: 10.1080/00940771.2015.1135099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spruce, R., and Bol, L. (2015). Teacher beliefs, knowledge, and practice of self-regulated learning. Metacogn. Learn. 10, 245–277. doi: 10.1007/s11409-014-9124-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauman, T. J. (2002). Self-regulation and depression. Self Ident. 1, 151–157. doi: 10.1080/152988602317319339

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strathman, A., Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., and Edwards, C. S. (1994). The consideration of future consequences: weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66, 742–752. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.4.742

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terry, M. L., and Leary, M. R. (2011). Self-compassion, self-regulation, and health. Self Ident. 10, 352–362. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2011.558404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timmons, K., Pelletier, J., and Corter, C. (2016). Understanding children’s self-regulation within different classroom contexts. Early Child Dev. Care 186, 249–267. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2015.1027699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tirapu-Ustarroz, J., Cordero-Andrés, P., and Bausela-Herreras, E. (2018). Funciones ejecutivas en población infantil: propuesta de una clarificación conceptual e integradora basada en resultado de análisis factoriales [Executive functions in children: proposal for a conceptual and integrative clarification based on factor analysis results]. Panam. J. Neuropsychol. 12, 1–31. doi: 10.7714/CNPS/12.3.203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

vanDellen, M. R., and Hoyle, R. H. (2008). Possible selves as behavioral standards in self-regulation. Self Ident. 7, 295–304. doi: 10.1080/15298860701641108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermunt, D. H. M. (1989). “The interplay between internal and external regulation of learning, and the design of process-oriented instruction,” in Paper Presented at the Conference of the European Association of Research on Learning and Instruction, (Madrid).

Google Scholar

Vermunt, J. D. (2007). The power of teaching-learning environments to influence student learning. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. Monogr. Ser. 4, 73–90.

Google Scholar

Wagner, B., Fitzpatrick, J. P., Mazzucchelli, T., Symons, M., Olson, H. C., Jirikowic, T., et al. (2018). Study protocol for a self-controlled cluster randomised trial of the alert program to improve self-regulation and executive function in australian aboriginal children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. BMJ Open 8:e021462. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-021462

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiese, C., Tay, L., Duckworth, A. L., D’Mello, S., Kuykendall, L., Hofmann, W., et al. (2018). Too much of a good thing? Exploring the inverted-U relationship between self-control and happiness. J. Pers. 86, 380–396. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilhelmsen-Langeland, A., Saxvig, I. W., Johsen, E. H., Marszalekc, M. A., Isolde, M., Andersen, W., et al. (2019). Patients with delayed sleep-wake phase disorder show poorer executive functions compared to good sleepers. Sleep Med. 54, 244–249. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2018.10.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wimmer, L., Stockhausen, L. V., and Bellingrath, S. (2019). Effects of mindfulness training on regulatory and academic abilities in preadolescents: results from a pilot study. Open Psychol. 1, 69–93. doi: 10.1515/psych-2018-0006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). “Attaining self-regulation: a social cognitive perspective,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation, eds M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 13–40.

Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. J., and Labuhn, A. S. (2012). “Self-regulation of learning: process approaches to personal development,” in APA Educational Psychology, Handbook, Vol. 1, eds K. R. Harris, S. Graham, and T. Urdam (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 339–425.

Google Scholar

Keywords: research levels, psychology, executive functions, self-regulation, self-control, external regulation

Citation: de la Fuente J, González-Torres MC, Aznárez-Sanado M, Martínez-Vicente JM, Peralta-Sánchez FJ and Vera MM (2019) Implications of Unconnected Micro, Molecular, and Molar Level Research in Psychology: The Case of Executive Functions, Self-Regulation, and External Regulation. Front. Psychol. 10:1919. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01919

Received: 01 May 2019; Accepted: 05 August 2019;
Published: 27 August 2019.

Edited by:

Marta Portero Tresserra, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Spain

Reviewed by:

Ana Couceiro Figueira, University of Coimbra, Portugal
Giuseppe Mannino, Libera Università Maria SS. Assunta, Italy

Copyright © 2019 de la Fuente, González-Torres, Aznárez-Sanado, Martínez-Vicente, Peralta-Sánchez and Vera. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jesús de la Fuente, jdlfuente@unav.es; jfuente@ual.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.