Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 August 2020
Sec. Gender, Sex and Sexualities
This article is part of the Research Topic Gender Equality and Women's Empowerment in Education View all 16 articles

Mindfulness and Empathy: Mediating Factors and Gender Differences in a Spanish Sample

  • 1Department of Science Education, Faculty of Education, University of Burgos, Burgos, Spain
  • 2Department of Specific Didactics, Faculty of Education, University of Burgos, Burgos, Spain

Numerous research studies link mindfulness training to improved empathy. However, few studies focus on the mediating factors of empathy. This work has three objectives: (a) to analyze the possible mediation of mindfulness as a feature in this relation, (b) to analyze the mindfulness factors that mediate in the increase of empathy and (c) to analyze the moderating role of gender. The sample was composed of 246 Spanish-speaking university students (M = 24.08 years, SD = 8.43). The instruments used were the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) and the Toronto Empathy Questionnaire (TEQ). For data analysis, the indirect effect was calculated using 10000 bootstrap samples for the bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals (BCI). The improvement of empathy is mediated by the changes in mindfulness trait (B = 0.233, p < 0.001), disappearing in the presence of this mediator, the direct effect of mindfulness practice on empathy (B = 0.161, p = 0.394). We did not find a differential functioning of this mediation according to gender. Observing and describing are the FFMQ factors that mediate significantly between mindfulness practice and empathy.

Introduction

Mindfulness has experienced a boom in recent years with interventions that have proven effective in different contexts and ages (Coholic and Eys, 2015; Ted Ng et al., 2016; Zeng et al., 2017). Health-related areas are those, either as a complementary therapy to treatment or as an improvement in general well-being, on which the main research in this field has been focused, for example, the importance of a normalizing, accepting, non-judgmental attitude to decrease anxiety and depression, and to foster wellbeing, reduce stigma and prejudice (Barcaccia et al., 2019; Salvati et al., 2019).

However, in recent years its practice has extended to other areas such as organization, schools and sports (De Allicon, 2020; Vickery and Dorjee, 2016; Doron et al., 2020; Emerson et al., 2020).

Viciana et al. (2018) in their review of scientific production in mindfulness for the decade 2007–2017 in the Web of Science, report a rather scarce production with respect to studies that focus on mindfulness within the university environment. They point out that, of the total of 652 articles in any language, only 96 would be related to the field of education. The research on mindfulness in the university environment is reduced to 24 articles, with a total sample of 4119 students.

The main results in the university population point to its effectiveness in promoting health, general well-being, increased life satisfaction and a decrease in states of anxiety and depression (Dvorakova et al., 2017). Benefits have also been noted in knowledge retention (Ramsburg and Youmans, 2014), and significant improvements in reading and science scores (Bakosh et al., 2016).

Mindfulness is defined as focusing attention in an intentional way on the present moment without judging (Kabat-Zinn, 1990). It has been conceptualized as having several elements such as acceptance, non-judging, and non-reactivity. This multi-faceted conceptualization of mindfulness allows for a more complete understanding of its relationships with other variables, and thus Baer et al. (2006) developed the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) that provides the most comprehensive coverage of the construct (Peters et al., 2015). The FFMQ arises from the exploratory factor analysis of the five most popular questionnaires that had been developed on mindfulness (Baer et al., 2006). It provides a broader assessment of mindfulness in its attentional, self-awareness and emotional regulation facets compared to other popular scales such as the MAAS, which evaluates only the component of mindfulness focused on the present by a single factor (Zhuang et al., 2017). In addition, it has demonstrated sensitivity to differences in mindfulness practice, allowing for discrimination between meditators and non-meditators (Cebolla et al., 2012). It consists of five dimensions: observing - the ability to perceive and recognize internal or external stimuli -, describing - to label with words the living experience -, act with awareness, not-judging of inner experience - equable vision before the thoughts, sensations or emotions that are perceived - and not-reactivity to inner experience - the latter refers to the distance with the experience and a period of time in which one does not react to the stimulus (Baer et al., 2006).

On an emotional level, mindfulness facilitates the development of a feeling of unconditional love, compassion and forgiveness toward ourselves and others (Gilbert, 2010; Neff, 2011; Siegel, 2007). With respect to oneself, self-compassion allows us to understand our own suffering from a broad and transcendent perspective, perceiving human imperfections and feeling empathy toward others (Wu et al., 2019). With respect to others, compassion would turn the suffering of others into a vehicle for connection, rather than isolation (Salzberg, 2011). McCullough et al. (1997) highlight the emotional dimension of empathy as the most important in the development of forgiveness toward others. This affective disposition creates a bond on which empathy is constructed, since it helps us to perceive our shared humanity. Worthington (2006) in his model REACH on forgiveness to others raises empathy as the core of the model, facilitating a positive emotional response and compassion toward a person or an event that hurt us. Thus, empathy seems to play a key role in forgiveness and compassion toward oneself and others, allowing the maintenance, reconciliation and repair of social relationships.

This paper aims to deepen the mechanisms through which the practice of mindfulness could foster our empathic ability. Empathy refers to the ability to be affected by and share the emotional state of another, to evaluate the reasons for the other’s state and to identify with the other, adopting their perspective (De Waal, 2008). It is an essential component of social cognition, contributing to and enhancing our ability to understand, respond to and adapt to the emotions of others, to be effective in emotional communication and to engage in prosocial behavior (Spreng et al., 2009). While there has been an evolution in definitions according to different approaches and perspectives, research emphasizes the distinction between cognitive and emotional components (Rankin et al., 2005). Nevertheless, it has been difficult to find consensus between the theoreticians and investigators about if the processes that are related to empathy – perspective taking, sympathy, personal anguish, emotional contagion or theory of mind – are part of an affective perception or a cognitive understanding (Spreng et al., 2009). Thus, it is evidenced a great difficulty in finding agreement on the interrelated processes that contribute to empathy. In this work we have conceptualized empathy as an emotional process mainly, not subordinated to a cognitive understanding, although this can help to improve understanding and behavior (Spreng et al., 2009).

Different studies on empathy seem to indicate that we are born with a biological predisposition to be empathic; nevertheless, its level of development is influenced and determined by the environment. To increase empathy in people would be a great benefit for society with respect to the diminution of antisocial behaviors (Winning and Boag, 2015). In this sense, to advance toward a society of solidarity and understanding it would be key to foster empathy.

Some research related to empathy suggests contradictory results regarding the influence of gender. The initial studies that examined these differences (Hoffman, 1977) show higher levels of empathy in women. Hoffman suggests that women have a greater tendency to put themselves in the place of the other, as well as a greater emotional resonance. Eisenberg and Lennon (1983) in an attempt to replicate these results found that women obtained higher scores when empathy was assessed through a questionnaire, but found no differences when physiological correlates or facial gestures were measured. This led to the assumption that the differences between men and women were due to gender role stereotypes, according to which when completing the questionnaires women tended to present themselves as more empathetic because this is what is expected of them regardless of the fact that there are no differences in reactivity. Later research supports these conclusions: Michalska et al. (2013) found that women scored higher than men on an empathy questionnaire, a difference that increased with age. However, they found no gender differences in pupillary reaction or neural or hemodynamic response, suggesting dissociation between the questionnaires and neurophysiological response. Recent studies have found gender differences in the influence of mindfulness on other constructs such as aggression or rumination (Peters et al., 2015; Eisenlohr-Moul et al., 2016) so the role of gender as a moderator in the effects of mindfulness on empathy will be explored. In recent years, mindfulness-based interventions have been proposed as a means to improve empathy responses (Fulton and Cashwell, 2015; Kemper and Khirallah, 2015; Foukal et al., 2016; Lamothe et al., 2016; Bellosta-Batalla et al., 2017). Empathy involves the ability to understand and share the feelings of others. Thus, the basic attentional processes involved in mindfulness (observing, describing and acting with awareness) could be key in the development of empathy. The capacity to be aware of what is happening in the present moment, and to observe and describe the emotions in oneself, would make the appearance of these capacities more likely in relationships with others. Jinpa et al. (2009) propose that people would be able to observe and describe their own suffering and would become aware of their desire to alleviate it, extending it to the suffering of others in a natural way. Also, the capacities of non reactivity and non judging, can be key in the empathic development since they would allow to distance themselves from the strong emotions when not reacting in excess, making possible to understand, to take care of and to respond properly to the other’s feelings (Wallmark et al., 2012). In this line, Bishop et al. (2004) raises mindfulness as a metacognitive capacity that orients people not only to their own affective state, but also to contextual stimuli in a non-reactive and acceptance way. Moreover, from a physiological point of view, it has been established that the practice of mindfulness causes changes in the same brain areas that are related to empathy: the prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insula (Cahn and Polich, 2006; Lutz et al., 2008; Chiesa and Serretti, 2010; Shamay-Tsoory, 2011). Hölzel et al. (2011) found that the daily practice of 30 min of meditation increased the density of gray matter in the brain regions associated with empathy. Numerous studies have found this benefit (Greason and Cashwell, 2009; Birnie et al., 2010; De la Fuente-Anuncibay et al., 2019; Ngô, 2013; Shapiro et al., 1998). Nevertheless, several studies question this positive effect in the improvement of empathy when practicing mindfulness (Beddoe and Murphy, 2004; Dekeyser et al., 2008; Ridderinkhof et al., 2017).

Although there are many aspects to investigate (Fulton and Cashwell, 2015; Bibeau et al., 2016), there are theoretical as well as empirical evidences that support the connection between mindfulness and empathy (Jones et al., 2019). Most of studies explain this relation from correlations or regressions (Krasner et al., 2009; Fulton and Cashwell, 2015) being the therapeutic contexts, associated to health variables, and development of empathy skills in health professionals those who have centered the greater efforts (Aiken, 2006; Miró et al., 2015; Wang, 2006).

The popularization of mindfulness practice has increased in non-formal settings, but there is little research that attempts to explain the mechanisms through which mindfulness practice influences empathy outside of closed or structured programs (Barbosa et al., 2013; Jones et al., 2019), or the differential effects based on gender.

On the other hand, most of the research has been carried out in Anglo-Saxon areas. In this respect, De la Fuente-Anuncibay et al. (2019) in their study with English students, pointed out modifications in the mindfulness trait and their influence in the development of empathy, pointing out the necessity to extend it to samples of other countries, although we do not have evidences of this type in other contexts like the Spanish-speaking one.

The present study addresses the practice in an informal or non-therapeutic context (leisure, meditation centers, courses, etc.), which has been little studied previously, and focused on university students in Spain. The objectives set in this study focus on verifying that mindfulness practices produce changes in the cognitions of the mindfulness construct, which are those that influence the improvement of empathy.

As illustrated in Figure 1, the total effect (c) refers to the relationship between mindfulness practice and empathy without controlling for the mediator (Hypothesis 1). The direct effect (c´) refers to the relationship between mindfulness practice and empathy after controlling for the mediator (Hypothesis 4). Finally, a total indirect effect (ab) refers to the role of the mediator in the relationship between mindfulness practice and empathy (Hypothesis 2 and 3).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hypothesized mediation model: Indirect effect of mindfulness practice on empathy through mindful trait and total effect of independent variable on dependent variable.

The second objective will be to establish which are the factors of mindfulness trait that have more influence on the development of empathy in Spanish-speaking contexts. Mindfulness is a metacognitive factor that precedes other cognitive-affective processes, so we consider that it can function as a mediator and that the five facets of mindfulness could be influencing the increase of empathy skills. Thus, we expect to obtain similar results to the study by De la Fuente-Anuncibay et al. (2019) in English-speaking samples, in which observing, describing, and not-reactivity were predictors of empathy (Hypothesis 5).

As a third objective, because previous work has demonstrated gender differences in empathy (Michalska et al., 2013) and gender differences in the effects of mindfulness on other variables such as aggressiveness (Eisenlohr-Moul et al., 2016) we also explored gender as a moderator of the effects of mindfulness on the empathy (Hypothesis 6).

Materials and Methods

Participants

An convenience sample of 246 volunteer students from the University of Burgos (Spain) belonging to 7 degrees was selected: Degree in Social Education (n = 80), Degree in Pedagogy (n = 102), Degree in Early Childhood Education Teacher (n = 25), Master’s Degree in Inclusive Education and Society (n = 18), Course on Pedagogical and Didactic Qualification in Vocational Training (n = 15), and Master’s Degree in Teacher Training in Compulsory Secondary and Upper Secondary School Education (n = 6). The gender distribution was 35 men and 211 women with an average age of 22.23 years (SD = 4.76). The distribution of the sample presents a representative percentage of the university population in the area studied, characterized by a greater presence of women. In this sense, the OECD (2016) points out great differences according to the areas, greater in education (81%) with respect to health or social services (72%), these percentages being very similar to other European countries such as Ireland and the United Kingdom. The sampling was non-probabilistic convenience.

With regard to the practice of mindfulness, it was included in the group of people who practiced or had practiced mindfulness for non-therapeutic purposes in the last three years. The sample was divided into two groups according their responses to the question “Have you had any mindfulness training?”, separating those who had never practiced mindfulness and those who had practiced it in informal contexts, that is, outside closed programs with a predetermined number of sessions.

Instruments

The scales used were the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) (Baer et al., 2006) and the Toronto Empathy Questionnaire (TEQ) (Spreng et al., 2009) selected for presenting adequate psychometric characteristics. The FFMQ was designed to measure mindfulness with psychometric guarantees from a factor analysis of the five most used scales that measured mindfulness trait, or the tendency to be more aware in daily life, consolidating a new 39-item questionnaire. It consists of five subscales: observing, describing, act with awareness, not-judging of inner experience and not-reactivity to inner experience (Baer et al., 2006). These are grouped into a general factor of second order that collects mindfulness cognitions in a single dimension. It has psychometric guarantees, adequate reliability, convergent and discriminatory validity, showing sensitivity to differences in the practice of mindfulness (Cebolla et al., 2012). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.888 for the FFMQ (observing = 0.803, describing = 0.926, act with awareness = 0.846, not-judging of inner experience = 0.861 and not-reactivity to inner experience = 0.640).

The FFMQ has been validated in many countries, including France, the Netherlands, Germany, China, Norway and Chile (Rodríguez, 2017).

The TEQ (Spreng et al., 2009) is a short questionnaire that measures empathy as a primary emotional process, as a broad construct. It is a reliable scale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.69) robust, with internal consistency and test-retest reliability (Spreng et al., 2009). By means of a factorial analysis from the most used questionnaires to evaluate empathy, Spreng et al. (2009) obtained a one-dimensional factor that contemplates 16 items with contents on empathy such as emotional contagion and responses to the emotional state of the other or of help, altruism or activation of the sympathetic system.

Both scales have been used in previous studies with similar college populations (Quezada et al., 2012; Schmidt and Vinet, 2015; Mathad et al., 2017).

Procedure

The data was collected by the research team on paper and participation was voluntary. The research was approved by the Bioethics Committee of the University of Burgos. The principles of anonymity and confidentiality of the data of the study participants were taken into account. To obtain information, students completed the questionnaires in a single phase of about 25–30 min.

Results

Preliminary Analysis

Means, standard deviations, skewness and kurtosis values, and correlations for major study variables are summarized in Table 1. We checked skewness and kurtosis in our mediators and criterion variables. Following Curran et al. (1996), skewness > ± 2 and kurtosis > ± 7 would indicate non-normal distribution. The results indicated that all measures were normally distributions, except non-reactivity to inner experience (Kurtosis = 8.18) dimension. However, as show Ng and Lin (2016) in your simulation, unlike classic methods, bootstrap methods do not rely on the assumption of asymptotic normality of the parameter estimates and offer better results. Even with a small sample size, bootstrap methods offer satisfactory control over Type I error and desirable statistical power (Ng and Lin, 2016). Moreover, PROCESS provides a method for testing of indirect effects that minimizes bias in results that can arise from non-normal sampling distributions (Hayes, 2018).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations.

As can be seen, empathy correlates positively with the mindfulness trait (r = 0.249, p < 0.0001). Of the dimensions of FFMQ, empathy was positively correlated with Observing (r = 0.270, p < 0.0001) and Describing (r = 0.235, p < 0.0001). The internal consistency indices (Cronbach’s alpha) of all variables in the study range from 0.640 to 0.926.

Mediation Model

The first objective of this work is to confirm, from the total measurement of the mindfulness trait, the relationship between practicing mindfulness (in informal contexts) and improving empathy. Our hypothesis is that, in the presence of the mindfulness trait mediator, the direct effect of mindfulness practice on the improvement of empathy disappears.

We carried out a mediation analysis with the macro PROCESS for SPSS (Model 4), developed by Hayes (2018) in order to address the hypotheses of this study (Hypothesis 1 to H4). Mediation analyses test the effect of X (mindfulness practice) on Y (empathy) through the mediating variables (mindfulness trait). In this sense, mediation analysis allows us to assess how mindfulness practices influence empathy.

In our case, PROCESS generates coefficients using ordinary least squares (OLS) regression. We performed the analysis with 10000 bootstrap samples, with which it is not necessary to satisfy the assumptions of OLS (normality, lack of multicollinearity, etc.).

Furthermore, this technique allows introducing dichotomous variables into the model (in this case, having or not practicing mindfulness), without the restrictions and disadvantages that others, such as Structural Equation Modeling - SEM-, through Weighted Least Squares (WLS) estimation present.

Applying this method, we generate 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals for indirect effects. Hayes and Preacher (2010) recommend the use of bootstrapping techniques to obtain confidence limits for indirect effects. Bootstrapping is a non-parametric sampling technique that resamples several times, improving the power of a model, to estimate the indirect effect. The Bootstrapping IC is the most recommended way to evaluate indirect effects (Preacher et al., 2007). The indirect effect is statistically significant if the intervals do not include zero.

The results showed significant total effects of mindfulness practice on empathy (Hypothesis 1: β = 0.375, p = 0.043). The total indirect effects (ab) were statistically significant, since the 95% confidence interval (CI) of the point estimate did not cross zero (β = 0.214, BootSE = 0.085, Boot95% CI = 0.074, 0.404). So, there was a significant indirect pathway: the mindfulness practice was a significant predictor of mindfulness trait (Hypothesis 2: β = 0.916, p < 0.0001), which was itself a significant predictor of empathy (Hypothesis 3: β = 0.233, p = 0.0002). Considering the influence of the mediator, that is, considering this significant indirect route, the direct effect (c’) of mindfulness practice on empathy was not significant (Hypothesis 4: β = 0.161, p = 0.394), according to our approach (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mediation model (PROCESS, Model 4).

As can be seen, all the hypotheses are confirmed as expected. Figure 2 shows the results of the mediation model, which indicate that the indirect effect is significant.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Results of the mediation model: Indirect effect of mindfulness practice on empathy through mindful trait and total effect of independent variable on dependent variable (standardized regression coefficients). *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001.

The second objective of the study is to analyze what mindfulness factors are mediating the overall effect between mindfulness practice and empathy.

The SPSS macro PROCESS, developed by Hayes (2018) was again used to check which FFMQ dimensions are mediating the relation between mindfulness practice and empathy (Figure 3). The indirect effect was also calculated using 10000 bootstrap samples for the bootstrap confidence intervals (CIs).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Results of the mediation model: Indirect effect of mindfulness practice on empathy through each mindfulness facets separately (Standardized regression coefficients). ***p < 0.001, *p < 0.05, +p < 0.10.

Table 3 shows the results of the indirect mediation routes for each of the mindfulness factors. These results indicated the indirect coefficient was significant (B = 0.244, BootSE = 0.09, Boot95% CI = 0.068, 0.448).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mediation model.

Only the indirect effects of the observe and describe dimensions are significant. The factors acting with awareness, not-judging of inner experience and non-reactivity to inner experience, would not work as mediating variables in the relationship between the practice of mindfulness and empathy.

Analyzing the significant results, the indirect effects indicate that the practice of mindfulness was a significant predictor of observation (B = 0.773, p < 0.001) and this was a significant predictor of empathy (B = 0.235, p < 0.001).

Also, it has been found that the practice of mindfulness was a significant predictor of the ability to describe (B = 0.611, p < 0.001) and this in turn was a significant predictor of empathy (B = 0.164, p < 0.05). The direct effect of the practice of mindfulness on the improvement of empathy also disappears (B = 0.131, p = 0.486), if the mediation of the FFMQ factors in that relation is taken into account.

Moderated Mediation Model

Finally, the present study expected that gender would moderate the indirect association between mindfulness practice and empathy via mindfulness trait. To test the moderated mediation hypothesis (Figure 4), the present study estimated parameters regression with PROCESS macro (Model 59) by Hayes (2018).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Moderated mediation model (model 59).

The results of the analysis allow us to conclude that gender was not a moderator in the proposed model. None of the interactions between the predictors variables and gender was statistically significant (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of the moderated mediation analysis (PROCESS, Model 59).

Discussion

The findings found in the present work seem to indicate that mindfulness practices developed in informal settings in Spanish-speaking contexts are a significant predictor of mindfulness trait, and that these are a significant predictor of the development of empathy. Therefore, our first research objective is fulfilled. Mindfulness would be a metacognitive capacity that influences later cognitive processes and, theoretically, also behavior, increasing in the individual the capacity to pay attention to their own affective state in the present moment, and also orienting them to contextual stimuli in a curious, open and accepting way (Bishop et al., 2004). According to these results, it seems that becoming aware and accepting one’s own emotions could have healthy effects on attending to the other person’s emotional experiences (Trautwein et al., 2014).

In relation to empathy, the meta-analysis made by Konrath et al. (2011) in which 72 studies on empathy of university students in the United States between 1979 and 2009 are analyzed, indicates that the present university students are 40% less empathic than those of twenty or thirty years ago, which is detrimental in attitudes of understanding, compassion, preoccupation or empathy toward other people. The importance of empathy in the educational field is undeniable.

There are numerous experimental researches that have observed an increase of empathy through the practice of mindfulness (Birnie et al., 2010; Shapiro et al., 2011; Gockel et al., 2012; Barbosa et al., 2013) pointing out improvements in university students after the application of the program.

In this regard, a growing body of studies on the benefits of mindfulness has begun to show its potential as an intervention strategy to improve mental health and overall performance. Research with university students points to the need to increase this body of study, confirming the need to investigate aspects of positive functioning (Viciana et al., 2018).

Empathy is an essential element in interpersonal relationships and a central competence in the university environment (Centeno and Fernandez, 2020). The ability to put oneself in the place of the other -intellectually and emotionally- is a key factor for a more peaceful and supportive society, where relationships with others are established in a healthy way (Moya-Albiol, 2014). Research on the different forms of educating in empathy is seen as an area of special relevance due to its social and individual implications. In this sense, mindfulness is presented as a promising alternative since the attitudes and personal dispositions that it develops are intimately related to empathy. Empathy facilitates an approach to subjectivity and the way of understanding and feeling the world of others, regardless of whether it is different from our own. This approach is based on an attitude of acceptance and absence of judgment, which guarantees a sincere vision in which the image of the other is not altered by our own subjectivity. In the emotional sphere, empathy favors the establishment of an affective disposition of help. Furthermore, mindfulness helps us to recognize more subtle emotional states in ourselves and in others, facilitating our understanding of human emotions. Finally, the ability not to get carried away allows a distance to be established between others affectivity and one’s own, thus avoiding a fusion between the two. Further research is needed to establish the mechanisms through which mindfulness practices in university students improve empathy or other personal and social competencies that contribute to the development of their strengths and potential.

Despite the increase in the practice of mindfulness in informal or leisure contexts, there are hardly any studies on the effects for those who practice it for this purpose, since research has focused on therapeutic or formal contexts. The results of our study confirm that even informal practice of mindfulness can be considered a quality experience, because of the benefits it brings to the people who practice it, such as modifications in their mindfulness components or the increase of empathy. Gim (2009) consider contemplative practice as the essence of leisure because it creates a special experience that provides choice, freedom, tranquility, flow, and satisfaction. In line with our study, positive results are also obtained not only through greater internal balance but also by increasing well-being and a positive sense of health. With regard to the second objective, the dimensions of FFMQ that function as mediators between the practice of mindfulness and the improvement of empathy we have found two: observing and describing. No mediation on empathy was found in the dimensions of acting with awareness, non-judging of inner experience, and non-reactivity to inner experience. The current conception is that mindfulness consists of two distinct cognitive processes: attention focused on the present and acceptance of emotions. These two processes are often measured with FFMQ (Baer et al., 2006; Jones et al., 2019). The three FFMQ factors that shape present-centered attention are observing (attention to internal and external stimuli), describing (labeling and expressing experiences), and acting with awareness (attention in the present to one’s own behaviors rather than responding automatically). The two FFMQ factors that make up the acceptance of emotions are non-judging (adopting an unevaluated stance toward thoughts and emotions), and non-reactivity (letting emotions flow without being trapped by them). According to our results, the improvement of empathy due to informal mindfulness practices is due to processes related mainly to the attention focused on the present. And for this, it would not be necessary to act with awareness, but it would be enough to put the focus on the stimulation we experience and be able to label it and express it correctly. It seems clear then, that paying attention to internal processes has an influence on the sensitivity to other people’s needs or concerns.

If observing and describing one’s own emotions increases attention to the experiences of others, that is to say, empathy, other research (Neff and Beretvas, 2013) found that people who reported that they could label their own feelings correctly also reported that they could help others overcome difficult emotions, as well as the relationship between the degree to which people are kind to themselves and the kindness they have to another person.

In the same line, we find results in the works of Jones et al. (2019) and De la Fuente-Anuncibay et al. (2019) in English-speaking samples. In both studies they use FFMQ as a measure of mindfulness, with practice being the predictor variable. The scale used by Jones et al. (2019) to evaluate empathy was the Interpersonal Reactivity Scale (Davis, 1994) in a sample of American university students with little or no experience in the practice of mindfulness. Their results agree in that observing and describing were related to the improvement in the empathic abilities. On the other hand, they pointed out that adopting a non-evaluative posture toward emotions (Non-judging) negatively predicted empathy. On the other hand, the scale used by De la Fuente-Anuncibay et al. (2019) in a sample of English university students who made informal practices of mindfulness, was the TEQ (Spreng et al., 2009). Their results informed that, in addition to the dimensions of observing and describing, it influenced the dimension of not allowing to be caught by the own emotions (Non-reactivity) in the increase of empathy. We can suppose that the differences in some of our results, with respect to the work of Jones et al. (2019) are due to the scales used to evaluate empathy. The TEQ evaluates empathy as an emotional process and the content of the items has to do with the identification and understanding of others’ emotions, physiological activation, altruism, and the frequency of empathic and prosocial behaviors. IRI evaluates empathy around four factors: Perspective taking, Fantasy, Empathic concern and Personal distress. On the other hand, the only difference of the present work with the mentioned one of De la Fuente-Anuncibay et al. (2019) is that that one was carried out in an Anglo-Saxon country and this one in a Spanish-speaking context.

We have not found other investigations that use mindfulness trait as mediators that influence the increase of empathy, being this study a pioneer in this context.

Regarding the third objective, gender does not seem to function as a moderator between the practice of mindfulness and the changes it predicts in the mindfulness trait. Neither does it moderate between mindfulness trait and empathy, nor between mindfulness practice and empathy. This is consistent with previous research (Dean et al., 2017) that used structured mindfulness programs and did not find a different gendered functioning in the development of empathy. Our results support the idea that mindfulness training is equally effective in men and women, and has the same effects on empathy in both genders.

The findings of this paper should be interpreted with caution because of its limitations. Firstly, this study is transversal and therefore does not allow for the establishment of causality. Accordingly, researches with longitudinal designs are needed to provide more evidence for the hypotheses being raised. Another limitation could be reinforced by the use of random sampling, which is more powerful than the non-probability sampling used, thus avoiding the risk of bias. Finally, the limitation of the use of self-reports for the assessment of mindfulness and empathy should be established. Although both instruments are designed on the basis of factor analyses of the most commonly used questionnaires in both constructs and have sufficient psychometric guarantees, some authors point out deficiencies in them such as the influence of subjective perception or problems in establishing real change. Thus, situational assessments or the use of biological markers for empathy (Bellosta-Batalla et al., 2017; Breithaupt, 2009) or objective monitoring (Quintana and Rivera, 2012) and the use of behavioral measures of mindfulness practice (Grossman, 2011) are proposed.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Bioethics Committee of the University of Burgos (IR 15/2018). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

RF-A, ÁG-B, DO-S, and JP-R performed the conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, writing original draft preparation, review and editing, visualization, supervision, project administration, investigation, resources, and data curation.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Aiken, G. A. (2006). The Potential Effect of Mindfulness Meditation On the Cultivation of Empathy in Psychotherapy: A Qualitative Inquiry. Dissertation, University of Saybrook, California.

Google Scholar

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., and Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment 13, 27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakosh, L. S., Snow, R. M., Tobias, J. M., Houlihan, J. L., and Barbosa-Leiker, C. (2016). Maximizing mindful learning: mindful awareness intervention improves elementary school students’ quarterly grades. Mindfulness 7, 59–67. doi: 10.1007/s12671-015-0387-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbosa, P., Raymond, G., Zlotnick, C., Wilk, J., Toomey, R., and Mitchell, J. (2013). Mindfulness-based stress reduction training is associated with greater empathy and reduced anxiety for graduate healthcare students. Educ. Health 26, 9–14. doi: 10.4103/1357-6283.112794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barcaccia, B., Baiocco, R., Pozza, A., Pallini, S., Mancini, F., and Salvati, M. (2019). The more you judge the worse you feel. A judgemental attitude towards one’s inner experience predicts depression and anxiety. J. Individ. Diff. 138, 33–39. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.09.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beddoe, A. E., and Murphy, S. O. (2004). Does mindfulness decrease stress and foster empathy among nursing students? J. Nurs. Educ. Pract. 43, 305–312. doi: 10.3928/01484834-20040701-07

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellosta-Batalla, M., Pérez-Blasco, J., Cebolla, A., and Moya-Albiol, L. (2017). Empatía y mindfulness. Convergencia teórica. Rev. Latinoamericana Psicol. Posit. 3, 34–44.

Google Scholar

Bibeau, M., Dionne, F., and Leblanc, J. (2016). Can compassion meditation contribute to the development of psychotherapists’ empathy? A review. Mindfulness 7, 255–263. doi: 10.1007/s12671-015-0439-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birnie, K., Speca, M., and Carlson, L. E. (2010). Exploring self-compassion and empathy in the context of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR). Stress Health 26, 359–371. doi: 10.1002/smi.1305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 11, 230–241. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bph077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breithaupt, F. (2009). Kulturen Der Empathie. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.

Google Scholar

Cahn, B. R., and Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol. Bull. 132, 180–211. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cebolla, A., Garcia-Palacios, A., Soler, J., Guillén, V., Baños, R., and Botella, C. (2012). Psychometric properties of the Spanish validation of the five facets of mindfulness questionnaire (FFMQ). Eur. J. Psychiatry 26, 118–126. doi: 10.4321/s0213-61632012000200005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Centeno, R. P. R., and Fernandez, K. T. (2020). Effect of mindfulness on empathy and self-compassion: an adapted MBCT program on filipino college students. Behav. Sci. 10:61. doi: 10.3390/bs10030061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiesa, A., and Serretti, A. (2010). A systematic review of neurobiological and clinical features of mindfulness meditations. Psychol. Med. 40, 1239–1252. doi: 10.1017/s0033291709991747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coholic, D. A., and Eys, M. (2015). Benefits of an arts-based mindfulness group intervention for vulnerable children. Child Adolesc. Soc. Work J. 33, 1–13. doi: 10.1007/s10560-015-0431-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curran, P. J., West, S. G., and Finch, J. F. (1996). The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis. Psych. Meth. 1, 16–29. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.1.1.16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. H. (1994). Empathy: A Social Psychological Perspective. Boulder, CO: Westview.

Google Scholar

De Allicon, K. (2020). A mindfulness toolkit to optimise incident management and business continuity exercises. J. Bus. Contin. Emer. Plan. 13, 220–229.

Google Scholar

De la Fuente-Anuncibay, R., González-Barbadillo, A., Cubo, E., González-Bernal, J., and Pizarro Ruiz, J. P. (2019). Mediating effect of mindfulness cognition on the development of empathy in a university context. PLoS One 14:e0215569. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0215569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Waal, F. B. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 279–300. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dean, S., Foureur, M., Zaslawski, C., Newton-John, T., Yu, N., and Pappas, E. (2017). The effects of a structured mindfulness program on the development of empathy in healthcare students. Nursingplus Open 3, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.npls.2017.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dekeyser, M., Raes, F., Leijssen, M., Leysen, S., and Dewulf, D. (2008). Mindfulness skills and interpersonal behaviour. J. Individ. Differ. 44, 1235–1245. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doron, J., Rouault, Q., Jubeau, M., and Bernier, M. (2020). Integrated mindfulness-based intervention: effects on mindfulness skills, cognitive interference and performance satisfaction of young elite badminton players. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 47:101638. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.101638

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dvorakova, K., Kishida, M., Li, J., Elavsky, S., Broderick, P. C., Agrusti, M. R., et al. (2017). Promoting healthy transition to college through mindfulness training with first-year college students: pilot randomized controlled trial. J. Am. Coll. Health 65, 259–267. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2017.1278605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., and Lennon, R. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychol. Bull. 94, 100–131. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.94.1.100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Peters, J. R., Pond, R. S., and DeWall, C. N. (2016). Both trait and state mindfulness predict lower aggressiveness via anger rumination: a multilevel mediation analysis. Mindfulness 7, 713–726. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0508-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emerson, L. M., de Diaz, N. N., Sherwood, A., Waters, A., and Farrell, L. (2020). Mindfulness interventions in schools: integrity and feasibility of implementation. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 44, 62–75. doi: 10.1177/0165025419866906

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foukal, M. D., Lawrence, E. C., and Jennings, P. A. (2016). Mindfulness and mentoring satisfaction of college women mentoring youth: implications for training. Mindfulness 7, 1327–1338. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0574-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fulton, C. L., and Cashwell, C. S. (2015). Mindfulness-based awareness and compassion: predictors of counselor empathy and anxiety. Couns. Educ. Sup. 54, 122–133. doi: 10.1002/ceas.12009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. (2010). Compassion-Focused Therapy: Distinctive Features. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Gim, J. (2009). Toward a quality leisure experience: the practice of mindfulness. World Leis J. 51, 105–109. doi: 10.1080/04419057.2009.9674592

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gockel, A., Burton, D., James, S., and Bryer, E. (2012). Introducing mindfulness as a self-care andclinical training strategy for beginning social work students. Mindfulness 4, 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0134-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greason, P. B., and Cashwell, C. S. (2009). Mindfulness and counseling self-efficacy: the mediating role of attention and empathy. Couns. Educ. Sup. 49, 2–19. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6978.2009.tb00083.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, P. (2011). Defining mindfulness by how poorly I think I pay attention during everyday awareness and other intractable problems for psychology’s (re)invention of mindfulness: comment on Brown et al. (2011). Psychol. Assess. 23, 1034–1040. doi: 10.1037/a0022713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation and Conditional Process Analysis, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F., and Preacher, K. J. (2010). Quantifying and testing indirect effects in simple mediation models when the constituent paths are nonlinear. Multiv. Behav. Res. 45, 627–660. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2010.498290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Sex differences in empathy and related behaviors. Psychol. Bull. 84, 712–722. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.84.4.712

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hölzel, B. K., Carmody, J., Vangel, M., Congleton, C., Yerramsetti, S. M., Gard, T., et al. (2011). Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 191, 36–43. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jinpa, T., Rosenberg, E., McGonigal, K., Cullen, M., Goldin, P., and Ramel, W. (2009). Compassion Cultivation Training (CCT): An Eight-Week Course on Cultivating Compassionate Heart and Mind. Stanford, CA: Center for Compassion and Altruism Research and Education, Stanford University.

Google Scholar

Jones, S. M., Bodie, G. D., and Hughes, S. D. (2019). The impact of mindfulness on empathy, active listening, and perceived provisions of emotional support. Commun. Res. 46, 838–865. doi: 10.1177/0093650215626983

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full Catastrophe Living: Using The Wisdom of Your Body and Mind to Face Stress, Pain and Illness. New York, NY: Delacorte.

Google Scholar

Kemper, K. J., and Khirallah, M. (2015). Acute effects of online mind–body skills training on resilience, mindfulness, and empathy. J. Evid. Complement. Altern. Med. 20, 247–253. doi: 10.1177/2156587215575816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konrath, S. H., O’Brien, E. H., and Hsing, C. (2011). Changes in dispositional empathy in American college students over time: a meta-analysis. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 15, 180–198. doi: 10.1177/1088868310377395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krasner, M. S., Epstein, R. M., Beckman, H., Suchman, A. L., Chapman, B., Mooney, C. J., et al. (2009). Association of an educational program in mindful communication with burnout, empathy, and attitudes among primary care physicians. JAMA 302, 1284–1293. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamothe, M., Rondeau, É, Malboeuf-Hurtubise, C., Duval, M., and Sultan, S. (2016). Outcomes of MBSR or MBSR-based interventions in health care providers: a systematic review with a focus on empathy and emotional competencies. Complement Ther. Med. 24, 19–28. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2015.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Johnstone, T., and Davidson, R. J. (2008). Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: effects of meditative expertise. PLoS One 3:e1897. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathad, M. D., Pradhan, B., and Rajesh, S. K. (2017). Correlates and predictors of resilience among baccalaureate nursing students. J. Clin. Diagn. Res. JCDR 11:JC05. doi: 10.7860/JCDR/2017/24442.9352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCullough, M. E., Worthington, E. L. Jr., and Rachal, K. C. (1997). Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73, 321–336. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.2.321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michalska, K. J., Kinzler, K. D., and Decety, J. (2013). Age-related sex differences in explicit measures of empathy do not predict brain responses across childhood and adolescence. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 3, 22–32. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miró, M. T., Ibáñez, I., Felipe, I., and García, N. M. (2015). Entrenamiento en “open mindfuness: un estudio piloto. R. Psicot. 26, 145–159. doi: 10.33898/rdp.v26i102.51

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moya-Albiol, L. (2014). La Empatia: Entenderla Para Entender a Los Demás. Barcelona: Plataforma Actual.

Google Scholar

Neff, K. D. (2011). Self-Compassion. New York, NY: William Morrow.

Google Scholar

Neff, K. D., and Beretvas, S. N. (2013). The role of self-compassion in romantic relationships. Self Identity 12, 78–98. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2011.639548

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, M., and Lin, J. (2016). Testing for mediation effects under non-normality and heteroscedasticity: a comparison of classic and modern methods. Int. J. Q. Res. Educ. 3, 24–40.

Google Scholar

Ngô, T. L. (2013). Review of the effects of mindfulness meditation on mental and physical health and its mechanisms of action. Santé ment Que 38, 19–34. doi: 10.7202/1023988ar

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

OECD (2016). ). Panorama de La Educación. Indicadores de la OCDE 2016. Madrid: Ministerio de Educación, Cultura y Deporte.

Google Scholar

Peters, J. R., Smart, L. M., Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Geiger, P. J., Smith, G. T., and Baer, R. A. (2015). Anger rumination as a mediator of the relationship between mindfulness and aggression: the utility of a multidimensional mindfulness model. J. Clin. Psychol. 71, 871–884. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22189

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., and Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multiv. Behav. Res. 42, 185–227. doi: 10.1080/00273170701341316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quezada, C., Pablo Robledo, J., Roman, D., and Cornejo, C. (2012). Empathy and pitch convergence. RLA 50, 145–165.

Google Scholar

Quintana, M., and Rivera, O. (2012). Mindfulness training online for stress reduction, a global measure. Stud. Health Techonol. Informatics 181, 143–148. doi: 10.3233/978-1-61499-121-2-143

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramsburg, J. T., and Youmans, R. J. (2014). Meditation in the higher-education classroom: meditation training improves student knowledge retention during lectures. Mindfulness 5, 14–31. doi: 10.1007/s12671-013-0199-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rankin, K. P., Kramer, J. H., and Miller, B. L. (2005). Patterns of cognitive and emotional empathy in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Cogn. Behav. Neurol 18, 28–36. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000152225.05377.ab

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridderinkhof, A., de Bruin, E. I., Brummelman, E., and Bögels, S. M. (2017). Does mindfulness meditation increase empathy? An experiment. Self Identity 16, 251–269. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2016.1269667

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez, N. (2017). Mindfulness: instrumentos de evaluación. Una revisión bibliográfica. PSOCIAL 3, 46–65.

Google Scholar

Salvati, M., Chiorri, C., and Baiocco, R. (2019). The relationships of dispositional mindfulness with sexual prejudice and internalized sexual stigma among heterosexual and gay/bisexual men. Mindfulness 10, 2375–2384. doi: 10.1007/s12671-019-01215-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salzberg, S. (2011). Real Happiness. The Power of Meditation. New York, NY: Workman Publishing.

Google Scholar

Schmidt, C., and Vinet, E. V. (2015). Mindfulness: validation of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire (FFMQ) in chilean university students. Terapia Psicol. 33, 93–101.

Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2011). The neural bases for empathy. Neuroscientist 17, 18–24. doi: 10.1177/1073858410379268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K. W., Thoresen, C., and Plante, T. G. (2011). The moderation of mindfulness-based stress reduction effects by trait mindfulness: results from a randomized controlled trial. J. Clin. Psychol. 67, 267–277. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, S. L., Schwartz, G. E., and Bonner, G. (1998). Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on medical and premedical students. J. Behav. Med. 21, 581–599. doi: 10.1023/A:1018700829825

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, D. J. (2007). The Mindful Brain. Reflection and Attunement in the Cultivation of Well-Being. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Company.

Google Scholar

Spreng, R. N., McKinnon, M. C., Mar, R. A., and Levine, B. (2009). The Toronto Empathy Questionnaire: scale development and initial validation of a factor-analytic solution to multiple empathy measures. J. Pers. Assess. 91, 62–71. doi: 10.1080/00223890802484381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ted Ng, K. S., Chan, H. Y., Wee, S. T., Goh, L. G., Nur, F., Ying Tan, C. T., et al. (2016). Mindful awareness practice (MAP) to improve the cognition of singaporean elderly with mild cognitive impairment (MCI): a randomized controlled trial (rct). Alz Dem. 12, 1180–1181. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.07.118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trautwein, F.-M., Naranjo, J. R., and Schmidt, S. (2014). “Meditation effects in the social domain: self-other connectedness as a general mechanism?,” in Meditation–Neuroscientific Approaches and Philosophical Implications, eds S. Schmid and H. Walach (Cham: Springer), 175–198. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-01634-4_10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viciana, V., Fernández, A., Linares, M., Espejo, T., Puertas, P., and Chacón, R. (2018). Los Estudios Universitarios y el Mindfulness. Una revisión Sistemática. RINACE 16, 119–135. doi: 10.15366/reice2018.16.1.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vickery, C. E., and Dorjee, D. (2016). Mindfulness training in primary schools decreases negative affect and increases meta-cognition in children. Front. Psychol. 6:2025. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallmark, E., Safarzadeh, K., Daukantaitë, D., and Maddux, R. E. (2012). Promoting altruism through meditation: an 8-week randomized controlled pilot study. Mindfulness 4, 223–234. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0115-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S. J. (2006). Mindfulness Meditation: Its Personal and Professional Impact on Psychotherapists. Dissertation, University of Capella, Minneapolis.

Google Scholar

Winning, A. P., and Boag, S. (2015). Does brief mindfulness training increase empathy? The role of personality. J. Individ. Differ. 86, 492–498. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worthington, E. (2006). Perdón y Reconciliación. Nueva York: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Wu, Q., Chi, P., Zeng, X., Lin, X., and Du, H. (2019). Roles of anger and rumination in the relationship between self-compassion and forgiveness. Mindfulness 10, 272–278. doi: 10.1007/s12671-018-0971-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeng, X., Chan, V. Y. L., Liu, X., Oei, T. P. S., and Leung, F. Y. K. (2017). The four immeasurables meditations: differential effects of appreciative joy and compassion meditations on emotions. Mindfulness 8, 949–959. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0671-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhuang, K., Bi, M., Li, Y., Xia, Y., Guo, X., Chen, Q., et al. (2017). A distinction between two instruments measuring dispositional mindfulness and the correlations between those measurements and the neuroanatomical structure. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–9.

Google Scholar

Keywords: mindfulness, empathy, mediating effect, gender, FFMQ, TEQ

Citation: de la Fuente-Anuncibay R, González-Barbadillo Á, Ortega-Sánchez D and Pizarro-Ruiz JP (2020) Mindfulness and Empathy: Mediating Factors and Gender Differences in a Spanish Sample. Front. Psychol. 11:1915. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01915

Received: 26 March 2020; Accepted: 10 July 2020;
Published: 04 August 2020.

Edited by:

Nicole Farris, Texas A&M University–Commerce, United States

Reviewed by:

Wei Xu, Nanjing Normal University, China
Marco Salvati, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2020 de la Fuente-Anuncibay, González-Barbadillo, Ortega-Sánchez and Pizarro-Ruiz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Juan Pablo Pizarro-Ruiz, rjpizarro@ubu.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.