Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 18 June 2021
Sec. Health Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic COVID-19 and Behavioral Sciences View all 35 articles

Risk-Aversion for Negative Health Outcomes May Promote Individual Compliance to Containment Measures in Covid-19 Pandemic

  • 1Scuola Universitaria Superiore IUSS Pavia, Pavia, Italy
  • 2Cognitive Computational Neuroscience Research Unit, IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy
  • 3Neurogenetic Research Center, IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy
  • 4Center for Neurocognitive Rehabilitation, Centro Interdipartimentale Mente/Cervello, University of Trento, Mattarello, Italy
  • 5Dementia Research Center, IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy
  • 6Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
  • 7Cognitive Psychology Center, IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy

First-person experience of stressful life events can change individuals' risk attitudes, driving to increased or decreased risk perception. This shift to more risk-averse or risk-loving behaviors may find a correlate in the individual psycho-socio-emotional profile. To this purpose, we aimed to estimate the relationship between differences in risk-taking attitudes toward possible negative health outcomes and psycho-socio-emotional dimensions modulating the experience of life-threatening situations, in the context of the Covid-19 pandemic. In March 2020, we launched the PsyCovid Study (https://wprn.org/item/428452) to assess psycho-socio-emotional changes due to Covid-19 pandemic in the Italian population. Additionally, we distributed to 130 participants the Covid-19 Risk Task, including monetary and health-related stimuli, estimating a measure of risk-aversion toward health and classifying participants on the basis of their risk-attitude profiles. The set of psycho-socio-emotional variables was reduced to three PCA components: Proactivity, Isolation, Inactivity. The individual degree of risk-aversion toward negative health outcomes was directly related to Proactivity, encasing empathic, social support and positive coping strategies, which may prompt individuals to put in place self-protection strategies toward possible negative health consequences. These findings indicate that a risk-averse profile toward possible negative health outcomes may be associated to higher levels of individual prosocial and proactive dispositions, possibly making individuals' more compliant with the social and hygienic guidelines and, thus, reducing their exposure to the SARS-CoV-2 infection.

Introduction

Italy was one of the first countries in the world, and the very first Western country, to be severely affected by the SARS-CoV2 virus, starting from February 2020. The first pandemic wave, which caused a rapid increase of positive cases and deaths in a few weeks, slowed down. However, in October 2020 the contagion curve raised again (https://www.epicentro.iss.it/coronavirus/sars-cov-2-dashboard) and the Covid-19 pandemic is still causing thousands of deaths worldwide every day (https://covid19.who.int/; https://covid.cdc.gov/covid-data-tracker/#demographics).

After the first lockdown (Mar-May, 2020), the incidence of psychiatric syndromes and psychosocial distress increased significantly in Italy (Sani et al., 2020), as well as in all other countries (Serafini et al., 2020; Torales et al., 2020). In addition, such a new growth of contagions further boosts people's experience of anxiety and distress (Mazza et al., 2020). In this unpredictable context, interindividual differences in risk-taking attitudes – reflecting the subjective willingness to take risks - might represent a crucial variable capable to modulate decision-making and risk-taking behaviors toward negative health outcomes, which also concern the individuals' compliance with Covid-19 containment measures (e.g., social distancing, fiduciary isolation, mask use).

The literature about risk-taking attitude and behaviors in relation to life-threatening events indicates that first-person experience of extremely stressful events can change risk attitudes by either decreasing or increasing individual risk tolerance, namely making people have more risk-averse (Holt and Laury, 2002; Shupp et al., 2017; Jakiela and Ozier, 2018) or risk-prone (Orri Stefánsson and Bradley, 2019; Galandra et al., 2020) attitudes. However, previous studies on this topic have often used tasks including hypothetical monetary stimuli (i.e., simulations of monetary rewards, opposed to real monetary stimuli which allow the subjects to gain or lose a real payoff) (Xu et al., 2016), preventing conclusions about real life contexts and decisions, as well as people's choices in relation to non-monetary outcomes.

Recently, we developed the Covid-19 Risk Task (Galandra et al., 2020), starting from the Holt-Laury Paired Lottery Task (Holt and Laury, 2002), including novel ecological stimuli beside the standard monetary lotteries.

Briefly, the Holt-Laury Paired Lottery Task is a classical decision-making task, widely used to eliciting risk preferences and attitudes. It is based on a multiple prize list (MPL) design in which the subject is asked to consider a list of 10 ordered paired lotteries, A and B – i.e., a series of consecutive choices between two outcomes – reported on ten different rows in a table, and indicate which, between lotteries A and B, the subject would accept for each row. In any row, Lottery A represents a safer choice than Lottery B, as the expected payoff of the latter increases at a higher rate than the former. The row at which subjects switch from the safe to the risky lottery (i.e., the switch from A to B) is thus used as a proxy of risk aversion (see details in Materials and Methods section).

Starting from this design, the two ecological versions (Health Status and Employment Status conditions) of the Covid-19 Risk Task (Galandra et al., 2020) were specifically related to risk-taking attitudes toward different real-life domains, concerning health and employment outcomes in the Covid-19 pandemic time. Results highlighted that individuals are more prone to undertake risky behaviors when presented with ecological stimuli (e.g., choosing between two different medical or employment conditions), rather than hypothetical monetary materials (i.e., choosing between two different lotteries) (Galandra et al., 2020). These findings underlined that, when facing ecological stimuli related to a real emergency situation, peoples' decisions for non-monetary outcomes are similar to decisions undertaken in presence of real monetary lotteries putting real payoffs at stake (Xu et al., 2016), and producing a larger shift in risk-taking attitudes (Galandra et al., 2020). Briefly, triggers of real-life experiences as stimuli, also in non-monetary domains, appear more effective to investigate realistic risk-related behaviors, and facilitate the interpretation and contextualization of results.

It is well-known that psychosocial and emotional factors (e.g., loneliness, empathy, coping style, anxiety and mood alterations) influence our perceptions of events (Galandra et al., 2020; Serafini et al., 2020), and represent crucial determinants in risky decision-making (Charpentier et al., 2017; Zhu and Wang, 2017; Taylor, 2020) especially in extremely stressful and life-threatening situations (Brooks et al., 2018; Safi-Keykaleh et al., 2020). Into the context of Covid-19 pandemic, we showed that the perception of the outbreak impact for health could be modulated by the degree of loneliness and distress (Cerami et al., 2020b), as well as by proactive and prosocial attitudes, including empathy, social support and positive coping strategies (Cerami et al., 2020a). Additionally, age may as well have a role in modulating risk-attitude toward negative health outcomes, as young people might perceive themselves having better chances to rapidly recover from Covid-19 or not having severe long-term consequences.

In light of these considerations, we explored the relationship linking individual risk-taking attitude toward health to psycho-socio-emotional dimensions modulating the experience of life-threatening situations and age, in the context of the Covid-19 pandemic. To this purpose, we hypothesized that a more risk-averse attitude toward possible negative health outcomes may be related to superior prosocial dispositions and proactive coping styles, enhancing people's readiness to actively put in place self-protection strategies to cope with such a long-term stressful and health-threatening situation, like the Covid-19 pandemic.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The present study included 130 volunteers (89 females, mean age = 38.5 y.o., sd = ±9.3 y.o.) from the general population, who took part to the PsyCOVID Study [https://wprn.org/item/428452; (Cerami et al., 2020b)] and additionally completed the Covid-19 Risk Task (https://psyarxiv.com/5n942/). While the aim of the PsyCOVID Study was collecting multidimensional data, including health status and psycho-socio-emotional variables in Italian residents, the purpose of Covid-19 Risk Task survey was to delineate specific profiles of risk-taking behaviors in working adults (age range = 25–64 y.o.). Both the PsyCOVID Study (Cerami et al., 2020b) and the Covid-19 Risk Task (Galandra et al., 2020) surveys have been implemented on Google Forms and distributed via written invitations through e-mails and Whatsapp.

At the beginning of the survey, we presented the general aim of the study, the commitment required to participants, and information about the research team. Participants had to read and provide their informed consent by clicking a box. After providing informed consent, participants were directed to the survey. Participants did not receive any incentive to take part in the study. Eligibility criteria were the age (18 y.o. or older), the ability to provide an informed consent and the place of residence (Italy).

All participants provided their consent to the experimental procedure, which was approved by the IUSS-University of Pavia Ethics Committee.

Measures

Risk-Taking Attitude Toward Health

Risk-taking attitude toward health was estimated as a result of the Health Status condition (HSc) of the Covid-19 Risk Task (Galandra et al., 2020). The HSc Covid-19 Risk Task represents a modified version of the classical Holt-Laury Paired Lottery Task (Holt and Laury, 2002) and was specifically developed to assess risk-taking attitude toward negative health outcomes, in the context of Covid-19 pandemic. Briefly, it includes two series of 10 paired Lotteries, A and B, presented on 10 consecutively rows in a table. In any row, Lottery A and Lottery B reflect different health outcomes (Series 1 Lottery A: Symptomatic SARS-CoV2 infection without hospitalization – Type II Diabetes Mellitus, Lottery B: Shoulder Fracture – Symptomatic SARS-CoV2 infection with hospitalization; Series 2 Lottery A: Psoriasis – Asymptomatic SARS-CoV2 infection, Lottery B: Cold – Symptomatic SARS-CoV2 infection without hospitalization) [see Supplementary Tables 1, 2 and Galandra et al. for further details about stimuli selection and stimuli appearance (Galandra et al., 2020)].

In this task, participants have to make a choice between Lottery A and Lottery B. In any row, Lottery A always reports the “safe” choice while Lottery B represents the “risky” choice, as Lottery A has less payoffs variability than Lottery B. The 10 rows differed in terms of probability of “winning the higher prize” – i.e., the probability to undergo the less severe negative outcome in terms of health care – in each lottery. In the first row, the probability of winning the higher prize is 10%, while for the subsequent nine rows, the probability to obtain the better outcome progressively increases by 10% so that by row nine there is a 90% chance of winning the higher prize, and row 10 is a choice between two certain winnings.

A risk-neutral individual usually selects Lottery A for the first four choices, either A or B for choice five (i.e., 50–50%) and then switches over Lottery B for the last four choices. Considering the utility function

u(x)=x r1

where x represents the prize and r represents the constant relative risk aversion coefficient (CRRA) (Holt and Laury, 2002; Albert and Duffy, 2012), risk-neutral conditions are defined by r = 0, while risk-loving and risk-averse conditions by, respectively, r > 0 and r < 0. In the present work, we characterized the individual risk-taking profile on the basis of Albert's r cut-offs (Albert and Duffy, 2012) and, thus, we identified participants' as risk-loving (r < −0.15, n. of safe choices: 0–3), risk-neutral (−0.15 < r > 0.15, n. of safe choices: 4), mildly risk-averse (0.15 < r > 0.68, n. of safe choices: 5–6) and highly risk-averse (r > 0.68, n. of safe choices: >6).

Psycho-Socio-Emotional Dimensions

In the PsyCOVID study, we collected a set of measures reflecting psycho-socio-emotional dimensions, with a battery of validated questionnaires in Italian language. In particular, we used the Empathic Concern and Perspective Taking sub-scales of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index – IRI (Davis, 1983) to describe, respectively, emotional and cognitive dimensions of empathic abilities. Loneliness was investigated with the Italian Loneliness Scale – ILS (Zammuner, 2008), which includes three sub-scales: Emotional, Social and General Loneliness. Coping strategies were assessed with the short version of the Coping Orientation to the Problems Experienced – COPE-NVI-25 (Foà et al., 2015), measuring different coping styles toward problems and stressful events, reflected in 5 scale sub-scores (Positive attitude, Problem orientation, Transcendence orientation, Social support, Avoidance strategies). Finally, we collected information about individuals' ability to identify and describe emotions experienced by one's self or others with the Toronto Alexithymia Scale – TAS-20 (Bressi et al., 1996).

Statistical Analyses

We performed statistical data analysis using SPSS (https://www.spss.it/) and set statistical significance at p < 0.05 for all tests.

First, we carried out descriptive statistics on: (i) socio-demographic variables, reporting mean and standard deviation for pseudo-continuous measures and frequency and percentage for categorical descriptors, and (ii) risk-taking attitudes toward health, reporting frequency and percentage of different risk profiles (risk-loving, risk-neutral, mildly risk-averse, highly risk-averse). To this purpose, we computed a measure of risk-taking attitude toward health (mHSc) as the mean of number of safe choices between Series 1 and 2.

Then, we performed a Principal Component Analysis (PCA) on psycho-socio-emotional variables in order to identify a smaller set of predictors. In particular, after assessing the suitability of the correlation matrix (Keiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy = 0.661; Bartlett's test of sphericity < 0.001), we performed a PCA on the scores of 11 variables, including the three sub-scales of the ILS (General Loneliness, Emotional Loneliness, Social Support), the IRI perspective-taking and emotional concern sub-scores, the five scores of coping styles assessed with the COPE-NVI-25 (Positive attitude, Problem orientation, Transcendence orientation, Social support, Avoidance strategies) and the global score of the TAS-20. Both the scree plot and the Kaiser-Guttman criterion (i.e., components with eigenvalue >1) converged in determining the number of components to be retained (=3). We used an orthogonal rotation (Varimax) to facilitate the interpretation of the resulting components (Abdi and Williams, 2010).

We then explored the relationship linking the loading factors of the three independent components to the measure of risk-taking attitude toward health (mHSc). We finally assessed the relationship between mHSc and age.

Results

Tables 1, 2 illustrate, respectively, the socio-demographic characteristics of study sample (n = 130) and the relative distribution of risk-taking profiles toward health.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the sample.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Risk-taking profiles toward health.

As reported in Table 2, about half of the sample (67/130, 51.6%) showed a risk-loving profile, while 19 out of 130 subjects showed a risk-neutral profile and a third of the sample 44/130 included mildly to highly risk-averse individuals.

PCA reduced the initial dataset of 11 psycho-socio-emotional variables into 3 components explaining the 59.317% of the total variance. This result was in line with findings reported in the overall sample of the PsyCOVID study, including a total number of 1,258 participants (Cerami et al., 2020b). The first component (C1: Proactivity) included active, problem oriented and social support coping strategies, plus variables related to empathy, suggesting an internal locus of control. The second component (C2: Isolation) encompassed the two loneliness scores. Finally, the third component (C3: Inactivity) suggested an external locus of control, with transcendent and avoidant coping strategies, alexithymia and social loneliness sub-score.

To explore the relationship between C1, C2 and C3 to the measure of risk-taking attitude toward health (mHSc), we computed a correlation analysis (Spearman's rank correlation coefficient), which revealed a significant positive relationship between mHSc and C1 (r = 0.25, p = 0.005). No significant correlation was found between mHSc and the other two components C2 (=-0.5, p = 0.601) and C3 (=-0.1, p = 0.270). This result suggests that the increase of risk-aversion toward possible negative health outcomes is related to prosocial and proactive dispositions reflecting an internal locus of control.

Finally, the correlation analysis (Spearman's rank correlation coefficient) between mHSc and age highlighted a positive significant association (r = 0.2, p = 0.026) indicating that the risk aversion toward negative health outcomes increase with age.

Discussion

Covid-19 pandemic is putting the whole society to the test. Social distancing, fear of contagion and job uncertainty became part of our lives. In such an unpredictable and stressful situation, personal resources needed to promote psychosocial adaptation and emotional balance may be lacking, and this in turn may affect routines and habits related to everyday life and work (Cellini et al., 2020; León-Zarceño et al., 2021). In particular, into the context of Covid-19 pandemic, the risk of falling ill is weighed against the risk of losing the job and, thus, possibly compromising the living standards (Godinic et al., 2020; Rutter et al., 2020). In addition, people who work, find themselves having to organize and manage job activities and childcare as best as possible in this uncertain situation, sometimes without sufficient support of the institutions, trying to appropriately balance needs and sustainability (Del Boca et al., 2020a,b; Leduc and Liu, 2020; Blum and Dobrotić, 2021; Ruffolo et al., 2021).

As we reported by analyzing the baseline findings of the PsyCOVID study (Cerami et al., 2020b), loneliness and distress, but also empathic skills and proactive coping strategies, represent psychosocial and emotional determinants shaping individual judgments and perceptions, as well as risky decision-making processes (Charpentier et al., 2017; Zhu and Wang, 2017; Taylor, 2020).

Investigating interindividual differences in risk-taking attitudes toward negative health outcomes through an ad-hoc developed risk task – Covid-19 Risk Task (Galandra et al., 2020) in 130 Italian workers (89 females, age range 25–64) collected among the PsyCOVID study participants, we found an opposite pattern of risk-taking profiles in health vs. monetary condition with more frequent risk-loving behaviors in playing health-related lotteries. Thus, we decided to further explore and report in the present work the relationship between individual differences in risk-taking attitude toward health and psycho-socio-emotional variables modulating the individual experience during life-threatening situations, and in turn people's resilience to Covid-19 pandemic.

In detail, half sample showed a risk-loving attitude toward negative health outcomes in the HSc condition of the Covid-19 Risk Task despite the greatest part maintained a risk-averse profile in the monetary condition [mildly to highly risk-averse 108/130 (83.1%); risk-neutral 11/130 (8.5%); risk-lovers 11/130 (8.5%)] (Galandra et al., 2020). This evidence further confirmed the shift in risk-taking attitude and behaviors when people are facing or have recently faced extremely stressful conditions, as underlined in previous studies (Brooks et al., 2018; Cerami et al., 2020b). In this case, people were more prone to undertake a risky decision – i.e., half of participants showed a risk-loving profile – when they had to choose between different medical conditions, including the risk to develop Covid-19 symptomatology. Moreover, young compared to older adults might perceive themselves as less vulnerable to the infection or having better chances to recover from Covid-19. For these reasons they might be more willing to undertake risky decisions for their health in order to obtain the best possible outcome. Our results support this hypothesis by showing that the increase of risk aversion toward negative health outcome is positively associated with age. Similar results have been previously provided in adolescents and young adults that perceived themselves less at risk of infection compared to their relatives (Yang et al., 2020) and took the pandemic less seriously (Commodari and La Rosa, 2020). Consistently, young adults with higher risk perception reported stronger desire to contribute in the reduction of contagion and to protect their loved ones compared to peers with lower risk perception (Yang et al., 2020).

In line with recent findings (Commodari et al., 2020), that highlighted the role of psychological variables such as empathy, self-efficacy and imagination in promoting an overall healthy behavior and a better compliance with Covid-19 containment measures, we provided evidence that people's risk-taking profile toward health is related to individual differences in psycho-socio-emotional variables. Indeed, our data revealed that the individual degree of aversion toward risk for health is positively related to a PCA component – i.e., Proactivity – encasing proactive, problem oriented and social support coping strategies, plus superior empathic skills (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Proactivity, risk-taking attitude and compliance with containment measures. The figure illustrates the relationship between Proactivity and risk-taking attitude toward negative health outcomes, and the possible consequences in the individual compliance with infection containment measures, in the context of Covid-19 pandemic.

To put it differently, we observed that people with a risk-loving profile toward health (51.6% of our sample) showed an inferior degree of Proactivity than more risk-averse individuals, and thus displayed a lower expression of empathic concern and perspective-taking, a less use of positive coping styles, and lower degrees of perceived social support. These dimensions have been related to the health locus of control, which impact on how people approach their own health and health-related life decisions (Kesavayuth et al., 2020). Moreover, a higher risk tolerance toward health problems – which is conceptually similar to the risk-loving attitude assessed by the health condition of the Covid-19 Risk Task – has been associated to chance health locus of control (Wallston et al., 1978), namely believing that an external force (e.g., the fate) governs our health status.

Individuals showing a greater risk-aversion toward possible negative health outcomes revealed superior prosocial dispositions and perceived social support, as well as positive coping styles might be characterized by and internal health locus of control (i.e., believing that there is a direct link between one's behavior and health status) which may enhance individuals' readiness to actively put in place self-protection behaviors (e.g., social distancing, mask use, hand hygiene) to cope with the distress and the threat that Covid-19 pandemic posed on our lives for an indefinite period of time. Adopting a positive coping style encourages to better assess information coming from the environment, reducing anxious, fearful and depressive feelings to stressful condition and finally promoting adherence to regulations and directives (Ding et al., 2020).

Major limitations to the present work of course refer to the lack of a longitudinal perspective and the adoption of a small sample size. Indeed, the cross-sectional nature of the study design prevents any kind of causal conclusion about possible changes of individual risk-taking attitudes as a consequence of life-threatening and stressful experiences, like that of the Covid-19 pandemic, overtime. In addition, the small sample size and the selective age range may hinder the generalization of these findings to the general population. Thus, only future replication studies, using same tasks on larger samples and including younger (<18 y.o.) and older (>65 y.o.) individuals, will be able to confirm the reliability of present results and overcome limitations of the cross-sectional study design. Importantly, despite we found an association between risk-taking attitudes toward negative health outcomes and proactivity suggesting a relationship with individual compliance to regulations aimed at containing the pandemic spread, our findings are not sufficient to explain individual behaviors put in place and compliance to government directives. Further studies specifically exploring risk-attitude profiles and compliance to hygienic and social recommendations are recommended.

In conclusion, the present study highlights how shifts in risk-taking attitudes by preferring possible negative health outcomes are related to the psychological and socio-emotional individual profile. This is of extreme importance in the context of the present Covid-19 pandemic in which individual behaviors may dramatically influence the well-being of the whole community. Excessive risk tolerance toward negative health outcomes together with the believe that individual actions and compliance to social and hygienic guidelines – e.g., respecting the social distancing, wearing the mask, or washing hands properly – are not necessarily linked to negative health consequences may cause the whole community to be more exposed to the SARS-CoV2 diffusion. Since the psycho-socio-emotional profile in risk-loving people is characterized by a lower degree of empathic dispositions and perceived social support, beside a scarce use of positive coping strategies, novel and multi-domain intervention strategies should be developed to overcome the psychosocial crisis that is spreading all over the world. Such interventions should promote positive attitude and resilience to the crisis and self-efficacy in adhering to the restrictive measures to contain virus contagion. Specific interventions including psychoeducational and metacognitive approaches, as well as mindfulness trainings, may also help to increase self-awareness and improve the empowerment of empathic and social skills in order to reduce emotional distress and perceived isolation and boost social support in individuals in daily life and crisis times. In the meantime, the scientific community should be better aware of the psychosocial impact that the Covid-19 pandemic is going to have to Western and Eastern populations (AlHumaid et al., 2020; Chew et al., 2020; Dawson and Golijani-Moghaddam, 2020; Rodríguez-Rey et al., 2020; Xiong et al., 2020). Parallelly, governments should consider the need of allocating the available economic resources to large-scale psychological interventions, with the aim to increase people's resilience according to the needs of psychosocial well-being in the whole society and the specific requirements of some fragile populations.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by IUSS-University of Pavia Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

CG, CCe, and CCr participated in study design and performed data analysis and interpretation. CCe and CCr drafted the main manuscript. All authors participated in data collection and reviewed the manuscript.

Funding

Mondino Foundation IRCCS was funded by Italian Ministry Ricerca Corrente 2020.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Pigmento-LAB ETS members for their valuable contribution in subject recruitment. We thank William Dello Russo and Bella Parlapiano for their suggestions for the graphical design of the on-line survey.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.666454/full#supplementary-material

References

Abdi, H., and Williams, L. J. (2010). Principal component analysis. WIREs Comp. Stat. 2, 433–459. doi: 10.1002/wics.101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albert, S. M., and Duffy, J. (2012). Differences in risk aversion between young and older adults. Neurosci. Neuroecon. 2012:10.2147/NAN.S27184. doi: 10.2147/NAN.S27184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

AlHumaid, J., Ali, S., and Farooq, I. (2020). The psychological effects of the COVID-19 pandemic and coping with them in Saudi Arabia. Psychol. Trauma 12, 505–507. doi: 10.1037/tra0000623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blum, S., and Dobrotić, I. (2021). Childcare-policy responses in the COVID-19 pandemic: unpacking cross-country variation. Eur. Soc. 23, S545–S563. doi: 10.1080/14616696.2020.1831572

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bressi, C., Taylor, G., Parker, J., Bressi, S., Brambilla, V., Aguglia, E., et al. (1996). Cross validation of the factor structure of the 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale: an Italian multicenter study. J. Psychosom. Res. 41, 551–559. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(96)00228-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, S. K., Dunn, R., Amlôt, R., and Rubin, G. J. (2018). Running title: infectious disease outbreaks. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 60, 248–257. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0000000000001235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cellini, N., Canale, N., Mioni, G., and Costa, S. (2020). Changes in sleep pattern, sense of time and digital media use during COVID-19 lockdown in Italy. J. Sleep Res. 29:e13074. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cerami, C., Canevelli, M., Santi, G., Galandra, C., Dodich, A., Cappa, S., et al. (2020a). Identifying frail populations for disease risk prediction and intervention planning in the Covid-19 era: a focus on social isolation and psychosocial vulnerability. PsyArXiv [preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/fpvkr

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cerami, C., Santi, G. C., Galandra, C., Dodich, A., Cappa, S. F., Vecchi, T., et al. (2020b). Covid-19 outbreak in Italy: are we ready for the psychosocial and the economic crisis? Baseline findings from the psycovid study. Front. Psychiatry 11:556. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charpentier, C. J., Aylward, J., Roiser, J. P., and Robinson, O. J. (2017). Enhanced risk aversion, but not loss aversion, in unmedicated pathological anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 81, 1014–1022. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chew, Q. H., Wei, K. C., Vasoo, S., Chua, H. C., and Sim, K. (2020). Narrative synthesis of psychological and coping responses towards emerging infectious disease outbreaks in the general population: practical considerations for the COVID-19 pandemic. Singapore Med. J. 61, 350–356. doi: 10.11622/smedj.2020046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Commodari, E., and La Rosa, V. L. (2020). Adolescents in quarantine during COVID-19 pandemic in Italy: perceived health risk, beliefs, psychological experiences and expectations for the future. Front. Psychol. 11:559951. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.559951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Commodari, E., La Rosa, V. L., and Coniglio, M. A. (2020). Health risk perceptions in the era of the new coronavirus: are the Italian people ready for a novel virus? A cross-sectional study on perceived personal and comparative susceptibility for infectious diseases. Public Health 187, 8–14. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2020.07.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, D. L., and Golijani-Moghaddam, N. (2020). COVID-19: psychological flexibility, coping, mental health, and wellbeing in the UK during the pandemic. J. Contextual Behav. Sci. 17, 126–134. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2020.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Boca, D., Oggero, N., Profeta, P., and Rossi, M. (2020a). Women's and men's work, housework and childcare, before and during COVID-19. Rev. Econ. Household 18, 1001–1017. doi: 10.1007/s11150-020-09502-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Boca, D., Oggero, N., Profeta, P., and Rossi, M. (2020b). Women's Work, Housework and Childcare, Before and During COVID-19. Available online at: https://covid-19.iza.org/publications/dp13409/ (accessed May 26, 2021).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ding, Y., Xu, J., Huang, S., and Xie, S. (2020). Risk perception and depression in public health crises: evidence from the COVID-19 crisis in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 17:5728. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17165728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foà, C., Tonarelli, A., Caricati, L., and Fruggeri, L. (2015). COPE-NVI-25: validazione italiana della versione ridotta della coping orientation to the problems experienced (COPE-NVI). Psicol. Salute 2, 123–140. doi: 10.3280/PDS2015-002007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galandra, C., Cerami, C., Santi, G., Dodich, A., Cappa, S., Vecchi, T., et al. (2020). Job loss and health threatening events modulate risk-taking behaviours in the Covid-19 emergency. Sci. Rep. 10:22236. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-78992-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godinic, D., Obrenovic, B., and Khudaykulov, A. (2020). Effects of economic uncertainty on mental health in the COVID-19 pandemic context: social identity disturbance, job uncertainty and psychological well-being model. Int. J. Innov. Econ. Dev. 6, 61–74. doi: 10.18775/ijied.1849-7551-7020.2015.61.2005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt, C. A., and Laury, S. K. (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. Am. Econ. Rev. 92, 1644–1655. doi: 10.1257/000282802762024700

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakiela, P., and Ozier, O. (2018). The impact of violence on individual risk preferences: evidence from a natural experiment. Rev. Econ. Stat. 101, 547–559. doi: 10.1162/rest_a_00763

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kesavayuth, D., Poyago-Theotoky, J., Tran, D. B., and Zikos, V. (2020). Locus of control, health and healthcare utilization. Econ. Model. 86, 227–238. doi: 10.1016/j.econmod.2019.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leduc, S, and Liu, Z. (2020). The uncertainty channel of the coronavirus. Fed. Reserve Bank San Francisco Econ. Lett. 7, 1–05. doi: 10.24148/wp2021-03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

León-Zarceño, E., Moreno-Tenas, A., Boix Vilella, S., García-Naveira, A., and Serrano-Rosa, M. A. (2021). Habits and psychological factors associated with changes in physical activity due to COVID-19 confinement. Front. Psychol. 12:620745. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.620745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazza, C., Ricci, E., Biondi, S., Colasanti, M., Ferracuti, S., Napoli, C., et al. (2020). A nationwide survey of psychological distress among italian people during the covid-19 pandemic: immediate psychological responses and associated factors. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 1–14. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17093165

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orri Stefánsson, H., and Bradley, R. (2019). What is risk aversion? Br. J. Philos. Sci. 70, 77–102. doi: 10.1093/bjps/axx035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Rey, R., Garrido-Hernansaiz, H., and Collado, S. (2020). Psychological impact and associated factors during the initial stage of the coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic among the general population in Spain. Front. Psychol. 11:1540. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruffolo, M., Price, D., Schoultz, M., Leung, J., Bonsaksen, T., Thygesen, H., et al. (2021). Employment uncertainty and mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic initial social distancing implementation: a cross-national study. Glob. Soc. Welfare 1:3. doi: 10.1007/s40609-020-00201-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutter, H., Wolpert, M., and Greenhalgh, T. (2020). Managing uncertainty in the covid-19 era. BMJ 370:m3349. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m3349

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Safi-Keykaleh, M., Khorasani-Zavareh, D., and Bohm, K. (2020). Factors affecting emergency medical technicians' on-scene decision-making in emergency situations: a qualitative study. Front. Emerg. Med. 4:e88.

Google Scholar

Sani, G., Janiri, D., Di Nicola, M., Janiri, L., Ferretti, S., and Chieffo, D. (2020). Mental health during and after the COVID-19 emergency in Italy. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 74:372. doi: 10.1111/pcn.13004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serafini, G., Parmigiani, B., Amerio, A., Aguglia, A., Sher, L., and Amore, M. (2020). The psychological impact of COVID-19 on the mental health in the general population. Qjm 113, 229–235. doi: 10.1093/qjmed/hcaa201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shupp, R., Loveridge, S., Skidmore, M., Lim, J., and Rogers, C. (2017). Risk, loss, and ambiguity aversion after a natural disaster. Econ. Disas. Clim. Change 1, 121–142. doi: 10.1007/s41885-017-0013-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, B. J. (2020). Risk-managing decision-making: a psycho-social rationality model. Br. J. Soc. Work. bcaa094. doi: 10.1093/bjsw/bcaa094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torales, J., O'Higgins, M., Castaldelli-Maia, J. M., and Ventriglio, A. (2020). The outbreak of COVID-19 coronavirus and its impact on global mental health. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 66, 317–320. doi: 10.1177/0020764020915212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallston, K. A., Strudler Wallston, B., and DeVellis, R. (1978). Development of the multidimensional health locus of control (MHLC) scales. Health Educ. Monogr. 6, 160–170. doi: 10.1177/109019817800600107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiong, J., Lipsitz, O., Nasri, F., Lui, L. M. W., Gill, H., Phan, L., et al. (2020). Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on mental health in the general population: a systematic review. J. Affect. Disord. 277, 55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, S., Pan, Y., Wang, Y., Spaeth, A. M., Qu, Z., and Rao, H. (2016). Real and hypothetical monetary rewards modulate risk taking in the brain. Sci. Rep. 6, 1–7. doi: 10.1038/srep29520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, X. Y., Gong, R. N., Sassine, S., and Jantchou, P. (2020). Risk perception of COVID-19 infection and adherence to preventive measures among adolescents and young adults. Children 7:311. doi: 10.3390/children7120311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zammuner, V. (2008). Italians-social and emotional loneliness: the results of five studies. World Acad. Sci. Eng. Technol. Int. J. Soc. Behav. Educ. Econ. Bus. Indus. Eng. 2, 416–428. Available online at: https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.192.9585&rep=rep1&type=pdf

Google Scholar

Zhu, Y., and Wang, C. (2017). The lonelier, the more conservative? A research about loneliness and risky decision-making. Psychology 08, 1570–1585. doi: 10.4236/psych.2017.810104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19, risk aversion, negative health outcomes, coping styles, loneliness, empathy

Citation: Cerami C, Galandra C, Santi GC, Dodich A, Cappa SF, Vecchi T and Crespi C (2021) Risk-Aversion for Negative Health Outcomes May Promote Individual Compliance to Containment Measures in Covid-19 Pandemic. Front. Psychol. 12:666454. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.666454

Received: 10 February 2021; Accepted: 10 May 2021;
Published: 18 June 2021.

Edited by:

Liz Steed, Queen Mary University of London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Elena Commodari, University of Catania, Italy
Valeria Verrastro, University of Catanzaro, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Cerami, Galandra, Santi, Dodich, Cappa, Vecchi and Crespi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chiara Crespi, chiara.crespi@unipv.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.