Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 17 March 2020
Sec. Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic Emotional Disturbance and Brain Imaging in Neuropsychiatric Disorders View all 17 articles

Acute and Chronic Effects of Betel Quid Chewing on Brain Functional Connectivity

Adellah Sariah,&#x;Adellah Sariah1,2†Shuixia Guo,&#x;Shuixia Guo3,4†Jing Zuo&#x;Jing Zuo5†Weidan PuWeidan Pu6Haihong LiuHaihong Liu7Edmund T. Rolls,Edmund T. Rolls8,9Zhimin XueZhimin Xue1Zhening LiuZhening Liu1Xiaojun Huang*Xiaojun Huang1*
  • 1Mental Health Institute of the Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 2Department of Mental Health and Psychiatric Nursing, Hubert Kairuki Memorial University, Dar es Salaam, Tanzania
  • 3MOE-LCSM, School of Mathematics and Statistics, Hunan Normal University, Changsha, China
  • 4Key Laboratory of Applied Statistics and Data Science, Hunan Normal University, Changsha, China
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, Brain Hospital of Hunan Province, Changsha, China
  • 6Medical Psychological Institute, Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 7Mental Health Center of Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 8Oxford Centre for Computational Neuroscience, Oxford, England
  • 9Department of Computer Science, University of Warwick, Coventry, England

Background: The active alkaloid in Betel quid is arecoline. Consumption of betel quid is associated with both acute effects and longer-term addictive effects. Despite growing evidence that betel quid use is linked with altered brain function and connectivity, the neurobiology of this psychoactive substance in initial acute chewing, and long-term dependence, is not clear.

Methods: In this observational study, functional magnetic resonance imaging in a resting-state was performed in 24 male betel quid-dependent chewers and 28 male controls prior to and promptly after betel quid chewing. Network-based statistics were employed to determine significant differences in functional connectivity between brain networks for both acute effects and in long-term betel users versus controls. A support vector machine was employed for pattern classification analysis.

Results: Before chewing betel quid, higher functional connectivity in betel quid-dependent chewers than in controls was found between the temporal, parietal and frontal brain regions (right medial orbitofrontal cortex, right lateral orbital frontal cortex, right angular gyrus, bilateral inferior temporal gyrus, superior parietal gyrus, and right medial superior frontal gyrus). In controls, the effect of betel quid chewing was significantly increased functional connectivity between the subcortical regions (caudate, putamen, pallidum, and thalamus), and the visual cortex (superior occipital gyrus and right middle occipital gyrus).

Conclusion: These findings show that individuals who chronically use betel quid have higher functional connectivity than controls of the orbitofrontal cortex, and inferior temporal and angular gyri. Acute effects of betel quid are to increase the functional connectivity of some visual cortical areas (which may relate to the acute symptoms) and the basal ganglia and thalamus.

Introduction

Betel quid (BQ) is a psychotropic substance, extensively consumed by more than 600 million people worldwide (1). Right after consumption, users of BQ have reported experiencing decreased thinking ability, disturbed mental processes, increased vigilance, body relaxation, enhanced motor responses, and a boosted sense of wellness (2). Substance dependence features including tolerance, craving, and drug-seeking behaviors as well as withdrawal symptoms have been acknowledged by habitual users of BQ (3). Many psychoactive substances act on the brain's reward pathway during acute administration, an effect that may be different in habitual users (4). The basal ganglia, extended amygdala, and the prefrontal cortex have been implicated in the initial stages, development, and habitual use of addictive substances (5). During the initial stages, the individual engages in voluntary substance use behaviors (6). Such behaviors may be accompanied by intense feelings which once experienced, may enhance recurrent substance use (7). Arecoline is the principal active component in BQ (8). It facilitates the release of dopamine (DA) (9) by binding to M5 muscarinic acetylcholine receptors on GABA terminals on DA neurons in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) (10). DA concentration is increased in the VTA and other projection areas through a series of mechanisms carried out by the mesocorticolimbic system [VTA, nucleus accumbens (NAc), and prefrontal cortex (PFC)], which is considered to be a principal pathway of drug reward (11). Additionally, cholinergic and inhibitory GABA'ergic inputs greatly regulate the mesolimbic dopaminergic neurons (12), which are known for their important role in processing rewards, reinforcement learning, (13) and dependence (14). Moreover, acute administration of psychoactive drugs has been found to activate brain areas connected to the mesocorticolimbic neural networks, implicated in drug rewards (15). Therefore, the need to use psychoactive substances repetitively and the compulsivity that is demonstrated in individuals addicted to drugs may be elucidated by the involvement of the reward and habit pathways in the brain (5). In contrast to the increased dopaminergic transmission in the NAc during acute exposure to drugs, chronic drug use is linked with less rewarding effects which result from reduced DA levels (16, 17). Chronic drug use is known to diminish the ability of the brain to control drug use behaviors, leading to increased risk for compulsive behavior that characterizes addiction (6). At first, it was believed that losing control over drug use stemmed from impairment in the subcortical reward brain region. However, findings from addiction studies have demonstrated the crucial role of the PFC in modulating the limbic reward regions and executive functions. Disruption of the PFC has been associated with loss of inhibitory control observed in drug-addicted individuals who have relapsed (18).

Resting-state functional connectivity (FC) studies have found that the majority of addictive drugs lead to reward, emotional and cognitive dysregulation (19). The mesocorticolimbic (MCL) system has been implicated to play an important role in drug addiction. The interaction among and between the MCL regions and other subcortical and cortical structures that manifests as circuit-level FC alterations have been observed in the reward circuit of drug addicts (19). The principal reward network is connected to other reward brain areas encompassing the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC), and medial PFC (mPFC) (20). For instance, increased resting-state FC was observed between NAc and the ventral mPFC (vmPFC) (rostral ACC and mPFC) of heroin addicts (21). Likewise, abstinent cocaine-users displayed greater resting-state FC between the ventral striatum and the vmPFC (22). Apart from increased striatal-PFC FC, a study investigating FC in prescription opioid users found reduced FC between NAc and the subcortical (hippocampus and amygdala) and cortical (cingulate, parietal, prefrontal) regions (23).

Drugs of addiction are also characterized by emotional dysregulation emanating from altered FC between the amygdala and PFC regions (19). Interaction of the amygdala with mPFC, hippocampus, cingulate, and insula regions has been linked with emotional processing and regulation, and generation of affective states (24). The hippocampus (involved in memory and learning) and the dorsal ACC (involved in cognitive control) are thought to be impaired in addiction, where a greater saliency value of drugs accompanied by a weaker inhibitory control leads to compulsive drug-seeking behavior (25, 26). The amygdala and its connections are fundamental elements perpetuating drug use, and previous studies propose that aberrant amygdala-mPFC FC may play a crucial role in emotional dysregulation frequently observed in drug addicts (19). Reduced FC strength was reported between the amygdala and regions of mPFC (including vmPFC and rostral ACC) in individuals addicted to cocaine (27), and heroin abusers (28). Similarly, extensive reduction of FC was displayed between the amygdala and several regions, including ventrolateral, medial, and dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC) regions in individuals addicted to prescription-opioid (23). In this study, longer periods of opioid use were linked with greater amygdala-vmPFC (specifically the subgenual ACC) FC reductions.

Apart from reward and emotional deficits, individuals addicted to psychoactive drugs are known to display neural dysfunction linked with cognitive control (29). The cognitive control network includes the ACC, lateral PFC, and parietal areas (19). For instance, decreased resting-state FC was observed between the ACC and dlPFC of heroin users relative to controls (21). Moreover, substantially decreased FC was observed within and between lateral PFC and parietal regions, such that decreased interhemispheric connectivity between lateral PFC areas was associated with a greater frequency of self-reported cognitive deficits (30) in cocaine addicts. A similar characteristic was displayed in abstinent heroin users such that FC was reduced between the lateral PFC and parietal regions. The observed reduction in FC matched a reduction in gray matter density in the same regions, with a longer duration of use predicting a greater reduction in both parameters (31).

A number of psychoactive substances have been linked with FC alterations in addicts. Specifically, compared to healthy controls, cocaine users displayed decreased FC within corticostriatal reward circuitry (27, 32), which has been associated with compulsive use of drugs and relapse (32). Apart from the reward circuitry, users of cocaine demonstrated altered FC between vital regions in the salience network and cortical regions (involved in decision making) (33); and within cortical brain areas involved with executive control (such as the cognitive control and attentional salience networks) (30, 33). Similarly, cocaine dependence has been associated with disruption among the default mode, salience and emotional networks where cocaine-dependent individuals displayed decreased connectivity between rostral ACC and salience network; posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and executive control network (ECN); and bilateral insula and default mode network (DMN) (34). Moreover, compared to controls, the ventral striatum of individuals with cocaine dependence exhibited reduced FC with the hippocampal, parahippocampal gyrus, vmPFC, and increased FC with the visual cortex (35). In alcohol dependence, individuals are often characterized by an impulsive drive to consume alcohol and a lack of self-control towards its consumption despite negative consequences (36). Evidence shows that individuals with alcohol dependence showed increased within-network FC in the salience network (SN) (including insula, hippocampus, and temporal lobe); anterior DMN (including superior frontal gyrus (SFG), ACC, medial frontal gyrus (MFG), and superior medial gyrus); posterior DMN (involving middle cingulate cortex, PCC, precuneus, insula, caudate, superior temporal gyrus (STG), and thalamus); orbitofrontal cortex (OFCN) (including middle and superior orbital gyrus, insula, amygdala); amygdala-striatum (ASN) (including putamen, amygdala, caudate, hippocampus, and inferior temporal gyrus (IFG); and left executive control (LECN) networks (consisting of the angular gyrus) (36). Relative to controls, cannabis abusers demonstrated increased resting-state FC of subcortical regions. Specifically, the cannabis abusers displayed greater local functional connectivity density (lFCD) than controls in the ventral striatum (NAc location), midbrain (SN/VTA location), brainstem, and thalamus (37). Results from a study investigating FC of the DMN revealed increased FC in the right hippocampus, while reduced FC was found in the right dorsal ACC and left caudate of chronic heroin users relative to controls (21). Increased FC was observed between dorsal ACC (dACC)-right anterior insula (AI), the dACC-thalamus, the dACC-left AI, and the right AI-left AI of nicotine addicts. Increased FC was associated with risky decision making (38).

The effect of BQ use on brain functional connectivity during acute administration is not well understood. Studies investigating the acute effects of BQ have largely focused on the frontal and default mode networks, giving subcortical regions less attention. For instance, based on previous findings from independent component analysis (ICA), acute use of BQ among naïve chewers was associated with increased and decreased FC in the frontal and default mode networks respectively (39). Evidence from addiction studies has linked initial drug use with activation of the reward pathway which primarily involves the interaction between subcortical and frontal cortical structures (6). Additionally, resting-state fMRI studies investigating chronic effects of BQ in the brain have yielded inconsistent results. For instance, compared to controls, individuals with betel quid dependence (BQD) had decreased FC in the DMN (40, 41), parietal network (42), and between the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and DMN (43); while increased connectivity was mostly displayed in networks including the visual (41), frontoparietal, occipital/parietal, frontotemporal, temporal/limbic, and frontotemporal/cerebellum (42), and between the ACC and regions of the reward network (41). The different FC results may have been influenced by differences in subjects across studies (sample size, age, gender, variations in BQ preparation, dependence level, duration of BQ exposure and the use of other substances, e.g., alcohol and tobacco) and use of different analysis methods, such as ICA (39, 40, 42), functional connectivity density mapping (44), graph theoretical analysis (GTA), and network-based statistics (NBS) (41).

Persistent psychoactive substance use has been linked with impaired brain function which disrupts the ability to wield self-control over drug use behaviors that typifies addiction (6).

Previous neuroimaging studies have investigated separately the acute and chronic effects of BQ on brain functional connectivity. Specifically, acute and chronic effects of BQ were mostly explored in the DMN and different parts of the brain respectively. This is the first study to examine both the acute and chronic effects of BQ concurrently. We explored the whole brain rather than predefined systems, with the aim of elucidating the impact of both initial and chronic BQ use on brain FC. Specifically, our first aim was to examine functional connectivity during initial BQ use among naïve chewers. Second, we aimed to explore the differences in FC between the naïve and BQ dependent chewers. We used NBS to identify FC differences (45). The results may provide further evidence and understanding of the neural mechanisms involved during initial BQ chewing and BQD.

Materials and Methods

Aim, Design, and Setting of the Study

This is an observational neuroimaging study that aimed to examine the effects of both acute and chronic BQ chewing in the whole brain. Recruitment of participants and data collection was carried out between January 2015 and March 2016 at the Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, located in Changsha city, Hunan Province, China.

Characteristics of Participants

All participants in this study were male. The following criteria for inclusion and exclusion of participants have been described in our previous papers (39, 42). Twenty-five individuals with BQD had to meet the following inclusion criteria: (1) 18–40 years of age; (2) Han Chinese ethnicity; (3) accomplished nine or more years of education; (4) right-hand dominant; (5) diagnosed with BQD as individuals using BQ at least 1 day at a time for more than 3 years and with a score of 5 or higher on the Betel Quid Dependence Scale (BQDS). The BQDS is a 16-item self-administered tool made up of three parts: physical and psychological urgent need, increasing dose and maladaptive use (46). Exclusion criteria included: (1) a history of neurological disorder or other serious physical illness; (2) a history of any mental disorders; (3) a history of substance abuse other than BQ; (4) a contraindication to MRI.

Thirty healthy controls were enrolled from the community in the Changsha City area. The inclusion and exclusion criteria for controls corresponded to those of the BQD group. The only exception was that controls would not have a diagnosis of BQD or have a family history of psychiatric illness amongst their first-degree relatives. All study participants were asked not to use any psychoactive substance during the 24-h period prior to scanning.

BQ can induce some physiological and psychological changes to users. But a half fruit of BQ is unlikely to induce severe adverse effects in healthy young men, even if used for the first time. We recorded the participants' heart rates and blood pressures just before the first scan and right after the second scan (about 30 min after using the betel quid) so as to monitor and rule out any physiological changes to users. Statistical analysis showed that there were no significant differences between the first and second measures of heart rate or blood pressure (Table 1 below). It has been reported that “The onset (of physiological effect) was within 2 min after chewing, peak effect was reached within 4–6 min and the effect lasted for an average of 16.8 min (47).” The absence of changes in heart rate or blood pressure may have resulted from the long interval (about 30 min) between the betel quid chewing and the recording. We also administered behavior questionnaires including the Beck Depression Inventory and Beck Anxiety Inventory before betel quid chewing to assess the participants' emotional status.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Demographics and clinical characteristic of participants.

This study was conducted in accordance with recommendations of the Helsinki Declaration established in 1964, and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Approval to conduct this study was obtained from the Ethics Committee of the Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University. Before inclusion in the study, written informed consent was provided by each participant.

Image Acquisition and Preprocessing

HC1 and BQD1 were defined as controls and participants with BQD respectively, who were scanned before BQ chewing. HC2 and BQD2 were defined as controls and participants with BQD respectively scanned after BQ chewing. In theory, we can compare any pair of conditions. However, in the main text, we describe the results for HC1 versus HC2 and HC1 versus BQD1. The results for other comparisons are shown in the supplementary materials. The following explanation about image acquisition and preprocessing parameters have also been described in our previous papers (39, 42). Resting-state fMRI scans were carried out for all participants before and after BQ chewing. HC1 and BQD1 were asked to chew the dried BQ along with its husk and swallow the saliva quickly in no more than 3 min. The BQ was an industrially wrapped product that has been described before by (48). Subsequently, the residual BQ was spat out, and 3 min later participants underwent the second fMRI scan which resulted in HC2 and BQD2.

Resting-state images were obtained from a Philips Gyroscan Achieva 3.0-T scanner in the axial direction. The following imaging parameters were used for the gradient-echo echo-planar imaging sequence: matrix size = 64 × 64, repetition time = 2,000 ms, echo time = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, gap = 0 mm, field of view = 24 cm × 24 cm, number of slices = 36, and slice thickness = 4 mm. Earplugs and foam pads were utilized to lessen scanner noise and head motion respectively. Participants were asked to lie flat on their back motionless with their eyes closed. The maximum time for each resting-state fMRI scan was 500 s, and generally, 250 image volumes were acquired.

The Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPARSF) toolbox (49) was utilized to preprocess the fMRI imaging data by way of Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8) (50). The first 10 images were removed to allow for scanner adjustment and for participants to gain familiarity with the scanner environment. Slice-timing correction and realignment for the head motion were performed to the residual 240 image volumes. The following measures were taken to minimize the effect of head motion on functional connectivity: First, the following criteria had to be met for realignment (1): a maximum displacement in the x, y, or z-axis of less than 2 mm and (2) angular rotation about each axis of less than 2°. Initially, the scan was performed among 25 BQD and 30 HCs, nevertheless, 1 BQD participant and 2 HCs had to be excluded from the study owing to greater than 2° and 2mm of rotations and translations respectively during fMRI scanning. Second, we utilized the Friston 24-parameter model (51) to regress out head motion effects from the realigned data (i.e., 6 head motion parameters, 6 head motion parameters one-time point before, and the 12 corresponding squared items) based on recent reports that higher-order models demonstrate benefits in removing head motion effects (52). Third, the head motion was also controlled at the group-level by using the mean framewise displacement (FD) as a covariate. These measures were strict enough to control artifacts caused by head movements. We compared head motion between the BQD1-2 and HC1-2, which was measured by mean FD derived from Jenkinson's formula (53), and no difference was detected between the groups. A scrubbing procedure was performed, where we calculated DVARS (a temporal derivative of time courses and variance across voxels) (54) to measure the rate of change of the BOLD signal across the entire brain for each frame of data. This revealed only a very small proportion of our data had movement contamination. When we compared the results obtained from the original data and the movement scrubbed data, there were no notable differences. Thus, all the results are obtained from the original data in this paper.

The data were spatially normalized into standard coordinates using the Montreal Neurological Institute echo-planar imaging template in the SPM package and was then resampled into 3 mm × 3 mm × 3 mm voxels. The preprocessed images were smoothed using a 4mm Gaussian kernel before the statistics. Subsequently, the BOLD signal of each voxel was first detrended to eliminate any linear trend. These signals were then passed through a band-pass filter of 0.01–0.08 Hz to decrease low-frequency drift and high-frequency physiological noise. Lastly, nuisance covariates (Friston 24-head motion parameters, white matter and cerebrospinal signals) were regressed out from the BOLD signals.

Whole-Brain Functional Network Construction

The revised automated anatomical labeling atlas (AAL2) (55) was used to parcellate the brain into 94 regions of interest (ROI) (47 in each hemisphere). The mean time courses were obtained from each ROI by extracting the signal average of all voxels within the region. The AAL2 atlas provides an upgraded parcellation of the orbitofrontal cortex for the automated anatomical labeling atlas (56). The new parcellation of the orbitofrontal cortex is based on anatomical evidence (57). The anatomical regions defined in each hemisphere and their labels in the AAL2 are provided in the supplementary materials (Table S4).

Acute Impact of BQ

The acute impact of BQ was estimated by comparing HC1 versus HC2. Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated between all pairs of ROIs, to acquire 94 × 94 correlation matrices rij,i, j = 1, 2….,94, indicating the FC strength for each pair (connectivity between any two brain regions) of regions for each participant. Then, the FC with significant differences (p-value of less than 0.05) before and after chewing BQ were selected by performing a paired t-test and the difference network was constructed. The number of all FCs for each node in the different network was defined as the degree of this brain region.

Differences of Functional Connectivity in Chronic BQ Users and Controls

These differences were estimated by comparing the difference between HC1 vs BQD1. The 94 × 94 Pearson correlation coefficients were initially calculated between all pairs of ROIs. Then, FCs with significant differences (p-value of less than 0.05) between HC1 and BQD1 were selected by performing a two-sample t-test and the difference network was constructed. The number of all FCs for each node in the difference network was defined as the degree of this brain region.

Network-Based Statistics

At present, studies using neuroimaging data to construct functional or structural networks are many, and most of them are aimed at finding different connections between the two groups of networks. When we test each connection in the graph of the network at the same time, the family-wise error rate is generated. The network-based statistic is an effective way to control the family-wise error rate, depending on the degree of association between the connections of interest (45). The specific steps are as follows. First, a Fisher's r to z transform for each connection in the network was performed, and a t-test was performed for the differences between the two groups for each connection. The test statistic computed for each pairwise association was then thresholded to formulate a set of suprathreshold links. Components present in the set of suprathreshold links were ascertained using a breadth-first search, and the number of links they comprise (or size) was stored. Thereafter, permutation testing was used to assign a p-value controlled for the FWE to each connected component based on its size. A total of M random permutations was created independently, where for each permutation, a random exchange was done for the group to which every participant belongs. The required test statistic for each permutation was recalculated, then the same threshold was applied to define a set of suprathreshold links. The maximal component size in the set of suprathreshold links extracted from each of the M permutations was ascertained and stored, thereby earning an empirical estimate of the null distribution of maximal component size. Lastly, to estimate the p-value of an observed component of size k, the total number of permutations was detected showing a greater maximal component size than k and normalizing by M. Networks with significant inter-group differences were detected if the p-value was smaller than the given 0.05 threshold.

Support Vector Machine (SVM) Classifier

In order to study how much difference there is between the different groups, we employed a widely used SVM classifier. SVM is a learning machine for two-class problems for pattern classification analysis.

We used an SVM toolkit called libsvm composed by Chih-Jen Lin from Taiwan University (58) (http://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/~cjlin/libsvm/). Specifically, the whole brain FC was applied to the raw input matrix. Features that appeared statistically significant (a smaller p-value than the threshold for a two-sample t-test) were picked. Various types of kernel (linear, t=0; polynomial, t=1; radial basis function, t=2) and different trade-off parameter C (0.001, 0.01, 0.1, 1, 10, 100, 1,000, 10,000) were tried to attain the highest accuracy rate. A leave-one-subject-out cross-validation technique was applied to ascertain how the test performs as well as to validate the classifier, where the classifier was trained on all subjects except one, who was then used for the test data. The mean discrimination accuracy, sensitivity, specificity and AUC (area under ROC curve) were obtained for the entire sample.

Selecting the generalization rate as the statistic, permutation tests were employed to estimate the statistical significance of the observed classification accuracy. In the permutation testing, the class labels of the training data were randomly permuted prior to training. Cross-validation was then performed on the permuted training set, and the permutation was repeated 100 times. The p-value represents the probability of observing a classification prediction rate in the permutation testing no less than the discrimination accuracy. If the p-value is smaller than the significance level, we reject the null hypothesis that the classifier could not learn the relationship between the data and the labels reliably and declare that the classifier learns the relationship with a probability of being wrong of at most p.

Results

The mean age for BQD chewers and HC was 23.5 ± 3.88 years and 24.9 ± 2.60 years respectively. Individuals with BQD displayed a mean BQDS score of 7.58 ± 2.17 and a mean duration of BQ use of 7.13 ± 3.79 years (Table 1).

Acute Impact of BQ

We used the NBS method to assess specific network connections for acute impact. Compared with HC1, HC2 displayed higher functional connectivity strength between subcortical regions including the basal ganglia and thalamus, and occipital brain regions, after chewing BQ as shown in Figure 1A. The brain regions comprised of 37 nodes and 55 connections that included many connections involving the basal ganglia (corrected p value < 0.001). The ROIs with the highest degree are shown in Figure 1B including bilateral caudate (CAU), thalamus (THA), left putamen (PUT), bilateral superior occipital gyrus (SOG) and middle occipital gyrus (MOG). Because the right caudate has the biggest degree in Figure 1, it was used as an example to calculate the connectivity between the right caudate and the other voxels in the whole brain. We performed this analysis to show whether the voxel-wise analysis is consistent with the NBS analysis. We used a pairwise t-test to compare the difference between HC1 and HC2. The t map (link-wise FDR corrected, q=0.05) is shown in Figure 2. A significant difference was detected with connectivity involving the caudate and putamen. No association was found between the baseline scores and FCs.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 (A) Network-based functional connectivity differences after acute administration of betel quid to naive participants. Orange indicates an increase in functional connectivity which was found between the subcortical and occipital brain regions. (B) The degree of different areas for acute use (ROIs with the highest degree). CAU, caudate; L/R, left/right; MOG, middle occipital gyrus; PUT, putamen; SOG, superior occipital gyrus; THA, thalamus.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 The t map (link-wise FDR corrected, q=0.05) in Healthy Controls before and after acute betel quid chewing. A significant difference was detected with connectivity between the caudate and putamen. The colorbar represents the t value for each region of interest (ROI).

Differences of Functional Connectivity in Chronic BQ Users and Controls

Using the NBS method, higher functional connectivity strength in BQD1 compared with HC1 was found between the temporal, parietal and prefrontal brain regions, as shown in Figure 3. The regions comprised of 55 nodes and 79 connections (corrected p-value is 0.035). The ROIs with the highest degree are shown in Figure 3B including the right medial OFC, right lateral OFC, angular gyrus, superior parietal gyrus (SPG), SFG, and bilateral ITG. There was no association between the baseline scores and FCs.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 (A) Network-based connection differences for chronic betel quid users minus controls. Orange indicates an increase in functional connectivity which was found between the frontal, parietal and temporal brain regions. (B) The degree of different areas for chronic use [regions of interest (ROIs) with the highest degree] The right OFCmed is the combination of the sum degree of rectus, OFCmed, OFCant, and OFCpost). ANG, angular gyrus; ant/post, anterior/posterior; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; L/R, left/right; med/lat, medial/lateral; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; SPG, superior parietal gyrus.

Significant links during acute and chronic BQ chewing are provided in supplementary materials 456 (Figure S1 and Table S2).

Network-Based Classification

Receiver operating characteristic curves (ROC) were charted for network classification analysis, so as to ascertain whether graph-based network metrics might act as biomarkers for discriminating different groups. The ROC analysis was performed for each metric (i.e., one-dimensional characteristic) displaying significant differences between groups. For each particular metric, a range of thresholds was employed to allocate each participant into either the first or the second group. An initial linear discriminant analysis was carried out to yield an overall estimate of group separation. The highest degrees of separability (AUC) were observed between HC1 and HC2 (0.7551), as well as between HC1 and BQD2 (0.8646). HC2 versus BQD2 and BQD1 versus BQD2 portrayed the lowest AUC (Table S3 and Figure S2 in supplementary materials).

Discussion

This study identifies the neurobiological effects involved in the initial acute effects of BQ chewing and in differences in those who are BQ dependent from controls when BQ was not being administered. For the purpose of this study, we only compared HC1 versus HC2, and HC1 versus BQD1, because our interest lies in the acute impact of BQ chewing, and in differences between long-term BQ and non-BQ users. NBS was used because it is a more sensitive approach than FDR.

The results from the SVM showed that the biggest difference between groups was observed between healthy controls who did not chew BQ and individuals who chewed BQ. For instance, HC1 versus BQD2 and HC1 versus HC2 have greater AUC values compared to those who chewed BQ (HC2 versus BQD2 and BQD1 versus BQD2). A small difference was detected between those who chewed BQ. Previous studies have also reported the difference in connectivity between BQ dependent chewers and healthy controls (4143), as well as between non-chewer healthy controls and healthy controls who chewed BQ (39). This is consistent with the hypotheses that BQ can alter the brain once it is consumed; or with the hypothesis that there are differences between individuals that lead some to consume BQ. AUC has previously been suggested as the preferred measure of diagnostic accuracy in psychiatry and forensic psychology, with reports considering AUC values greater than 0.7 as having strong effects in test performance (59).

Effects of Acute BQ Chewing on Brain Functional Connectivity

Increased functional connectivity among naive BQ chewers was mostly observed between subcortical regions including the basal ganglia and thalamus (CAU, PUT.L, and THA) and the visual cortex (SOG and MOG.R). This effect of BQ is consistent with previous reports about increased activity in these regions during acute cocaine administration (60). For the majority of psychoactive drugs, the acute effects involve the activation of reward pathways (6). The reward pathways in the brain include the basal ganglia (including the striatum), the limbic system (amygdala) and parts of the PFC (61). The basal ganglia are known for modulating the rewarding effects of drug use and also play a role in habit formation (dorsal striatum) (5). The striatum is involved in reward-related learning, as well as contributing to the development and maintenance of addictive behaviors (62). In particular, the putamen and caudate of naive BQ chewers demonstrated significantly increased FC, portraying the role of the striatum in the reward pathway during acute drug administration (6). DA neurons in the VTA play an important role in processing drug rewards (63), and increased DA in the striatum has been linked with subjective feelings of pleasure, euphoria, or a “high” resulting from drug use (64) and alcohol-associated cues (65). Our results agree with previous studies where activation of the ventral striatum significantly correlated with smoking motivation for pleasurable relaxation (66), supporting the reported psychological effects experienced immediately after BQ chewing (2). Similarly, acute alcohol influences neuronal activity in the ventral striatum which is known to project to regions believed to regulate motor responses, motivation and executive functions (67). Compared to non-users, individuals with substance use disorders displayed reduced functional connectivity between the nucleus accumbens and the frontal cortical regions responsible for controlling cognition (68). Such findings provide evidence that differences in the connections between the reward processing and cognitive-behavioral control areas may play a crucial role in the development of substance use disorders including betel quid dependence.

Compared to the frontal and striatal brain areas, the visual cortex (69) has received attention to a lesser degree in substance use and addiction neuroimaging studies. Our study found significantly increased FC involving the visual cortex of controls immediately after BQ chewing, which is consistent with results by Huang et al. (39) and also backing up the reported heightened alertness experienced by BQ chewers (8). Increased FC in the visual cortex has also been demonstrated after acute alcohol consumption (70). We suggest that BQ may enhance alertness which in turn activates the visual cortex, however, this requires further investigation. Drug cue exposure studies have also documented activation of the visual cortex in substance abusers when presented with visual drug cues as compared to neutral cues (71).

Differences of Functional Connectivity in Chronic BQ Users and Controls

Our study found significantly higher FC in the networks involving the right medial OFC, right lateral OFC, and right SFG of BQ dependent individuals. Analogous results have been reported by (39). The OFC, whose disruption leads to maladaptive and impulsive decision making (72), is known for its function in signaling the value of expected outcomes or consequences (73), motivational behavior (74), salience attribution (75), emotional regulation, and decision making (together with the amygdala and insula) (61, 76, 77). It has numerous projections to the striatum (75), and each of its sub-regions performs distinct functions; for example, the OFCmed is known for its role in monitoring reward stimuli whereas the lateral OFC evaluates punishing stimuli (78). For instance, individuals carrying out a monetary decision-making task exhibited activation of the OFCmed to positive reward outcome whereas activation of the lateral OFC was observed during monetary loss outcome (79). Addiction studies have also shown that the enhanced expectation value of a drug in the reward (ventral pallidum, NAc, and VTA), motivation (medial OFC, VTA, ventral ACC, dorsal striatum, SN, and motor cortex), and memory (medial OFC, amygdala, dorsal striatum, and hippocampus) circuits overcomes the control circuit (dlPFC, inferior frontal cortex, ACC, and lateral OFC) resulting in compulsive drug use and loss of control (80). Furthermore, compared to controls, individuals with cocaine dependency showed significantly decreased interhemispheric resting-state functional connectivity of the prefrontal cortex and the dorsal attention network encompassing medial premotor and posterior areas, as well as the bilateral frontal (30). Additionally, resting-state studies have portrayed dysfunctional network connectivity across the brain during both acute and chronic nicotine exposure, where the effects mostly appeared to involve the networks linked with attention, cognitive control, ACC, and insula (81). This relates to the enhanced attention that is commonly experienced by smokers (81) and continues to support the notion that substance use is associated with various alterations in connectivity between significant regions of the brain. The brain reward system is not only activated by the drugs, but also stimuli associated with the substance's rewarding effects, such as drug-associated cues. These stimuli can trigger the urge to use drugs (incentive salience) by activating the DA system on their own. The DA levels tend to persist even after the rewarding effects of the drugs have declined (5). Imaging studies of cocaine-addicted individuals have reported higher activity in the PFC during drug expectation than during drug administration (15, 60). This is in agreement with our hypothesis that the expectation value of BQ after 24 h of abstinence in dependent individuals was enhanced and may have contributed to the observed increased FC found in this investigation. Our study found increased FC in the amygdala whose key role is to control stress reactions and negative emotions (82). Therefore, the 24 h abstinence in BQ dependent individuals may have triggered unpleasant feelings associated with withdrawal symptoms which are believed to originate from reduced activation in the reward network of the basal ganglia and increased activation of the stress system including stress neurotransmitters (corticotropin-releasing factor, norepinephrine, and dynorphin) (83) in the amygdala (84). There is evidence that all abused substances tend to disrupt the dopamine reward system when used for a long time (6). For instance, addicts in imaging studies have constantly demonstrated long-term decreased D2 dopamine receptor, compared with non-addicts (85). The overall loss of reward sensitivity may explain compulsive drug use seen in addicts as a way to experience the pleasurable feelings the reward system formerly exerted (86). The desire to get rid of the negative feelings accompanying withdrawal may therefore reinforce continual drug use like the one demonstrated in individuals with BQD. Additionally, compared to HC1 versus HC2, the FC observed in the subcortex between HC1 and BQD1 groups was not significantly increased. This may be due to reduced D2 dopamine receptors (85) in individuals with BQD, which makes them less sensitive to BQ and therefore increases their compulsivity.

We also found increased FC in the right medial SFG, which mirrors a reduced efficiency of response inhibition processes in the PFC (87). The SFG is involved in planning, and motivation, as well as contributing to both stimulation and inhibition of craving. Its activation during responding to smoking cues versus neutral cues is highly correlated with participants' reports of craving (88), suggesting that time spent without BQ may have stimulated craving and thus activated the SFG in the BQD group.

Increased FC was observed in the right angular gyrus and right SPG of BQ dependent individuals. Our results are similar to other cocaine studies that have demonstrated increased FC between frontal–temporal and frontal–parietal brain regions of abstinent chronic cocaine users (89). Similarly, compared to people who have never smoked and former nicotine smokers, current smokers had greater connectivity in the right superior parietal lobe located in the dorsal attention network (90). The dorsal attention network has been linked with attention processing, predominantly in employing top-down control over fundamental sensory operations including visual information and maybe a crucial location for distorting attention (91). Therefore, greater connectivity in this area may indicate an increased propensity to focus one's attention on external signals (90), which may make it harder to abstain from BQ. The angular gyrus plays a crucial role in comprehension, reasoning (92), attention, language memory and self-awareness as well as providing information about self-awareness in the default mode network (DMN) (93). Addicts with dysfunctional DMN may exhibit impairment in disease awareness, need for professional help, and/or drug-seeking behavior (94) which supports what is often depicted in BQD behavior.

The results from this study also demonstrated increased FC in the right ITG. The ITG and SPG are involved in visual and auditory processing (95), and the increased FC in these regions in this study may relate to a perceived improvement in visual and auditory abilities in chronic BQ chewers. Such experiences may facilitate maintenance of BQ chewing, thus leading to dependent behavior.

A number of limitations are noted. First, our study was cross-sectional: we only observed functional connectivity differences in naive and chronic BQ chewers, but cannot infer causality. Future longitudinal neuroimaging BQ studies are crucial to consider the mechanisms fundamental for the neuro-transition from initial BQ use to dependence. Second, the use of other substances, such as cigarettes or alcohol could have influenced the results even though all recruited participants met the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Third, for BQ dependent individuals, we did not take into account the influence of craving on functional connectivity and the duration since last BQ use; which might influence the results. Fourth, for the investigation of the acute effects of chewing betel, it would be useful in future studies to have a control group that did not chew betel but was otherwise scanned similarly.

Conclusion

This is the first study to examine the acute and chronic effects of BQ concurrently. In controls the effect of acute BQ chewing significantly increased functional connectivity between subcortical regions (including the caudate, putamen, pallidum and thalamus); and visual brain regions (including the bilateral superior occipital gyrus and right middle occipital gyrus networks). These increases may relate to the acutely rewarding and visual effects of betel produced by its arecoline. In habitual users of betel, networks comprising the right medial OFC, right lateral OFC, bilateral inferior temporal gyrus, right angular gyrus, superior parietal gyrus, and right medial superior frontal gyrus had higher functional connectivity as compared to the controls before BQ chewing. These differences may be related to the craving for betel.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

XH designed the study and collected data. XH and SG analyzed the data. AS, XH, and SG prepared the first draft of the manuscript. AS, XH, SG, ER, and ZL critically revised the content of the manuscript. WP, JZ, HL, and ZX actively participated in writing and revising the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by the China Precision Medicine Initiative (2016YFC0906300) and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant nos. 81561168021, 81471362, 81671335, 81701325, 81801353, 11671129, 31671134).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge Professor Zhong He from the Department of Radiology, Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University for his assistance in imaging data acquisition, and Professor Jianfeng Feng from the Centre for Computation Systems Biology, Fudan University, for revising this paper and his guidance in fMRI data analysis.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00198/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

BQ, Betel quid; BQD, Betel quid dependence; HC, Healthy controls; FC, Functional connectivity; DA, Dopamine; OFC, Orbitofrontal cortex; CAU, Caudate; THA, Thalamus; PUT, Putamen; SOG, Superior occipital gyrus; MOG, Middle occipital gyrus; SPG, Superior parietal gyrus; SFG, Superior frontal gyrus; ITG, Inferior temporal gyrus; ACC, Anterior cingulate cortex.

References

1. Boucher BJ, Mannan N. Metabolic effects of the consumption of Areca catechu. Addict Biol (2002) 7(1):103–10. doi: 10.1080/13556210120091464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Osborne PG, Chou T-S, Shen T-W. Characterization of the Psychological, Physiological, psychological and EEG profiles of Acute Betel Quid Intoxication naive subjects. PloS One (2011) 6(8):1–11. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023874

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Garg A, Chaturvedi P, Mishra A, Datta S. A review on harmful effects of pan masala. Indian J Cancer (2015) 52(4):663. doi: 10.4103/0019-509X.178449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Nestler EJ. Is there a common molecular pathway for addiction? Nat Neurosci (2005) 8(11):1445–9. doi: 10.1038/nn1578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (HHS), Office of the Surgeon General. Facing addiction in America: The Surgeon General's Report on Alcohol, Drugs, and Health. Washington, DC: HHS; (2016), 1–413.

Google Scholar

6. Volkow ND, Morales M. The Brain on Drugs: From Reward to Addiction. Cell (2015) 162(4):712–25. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.07.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Everitt BJ. Neural and psychological mechanisms underlying compulsive drug seeking habits and drug memories - indications for novel treatments of addiction. Eur J Neurosci (2014) 40(1):2163–82. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12644

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Chu N-S. Effects of betel chewing on the central and autonomic nervous systems. J Biomed Sci (2001) 8(3):229–36. doi: 10.1007/BF02256596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Bendor J, Lizardi-Ortiz JE, Westphalen RI, Brandstetter M, Hemmings HC, Sulzer D, et al. AGAP1/AP-3-dependent endocytic recycling of M5 muscarinic receptors promotes dopamine release. EMBO J (2010) 29(16):2813–26. doi: 10.1038/emboj.2010.154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Yang K, Buhlman L, Khan GM, Nichols RA, Jin G, McIntosh JM, et al. Functional Nicotinic Acetylcholine Receptors Containing 6 Subunits Are on GABAergic Neuronal Boutons Adherent to Ventral Tegmental Area Dopamine Neurons. J Neurosci (2011) 31(7):2537–48. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3003-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Sulzer D. How Addictive Drugs Disrupt Presynaptic Dopamine Neurotransmission. Neuron (2011) 69(4):628–49. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Shina JH, Adrovera MF, Wessb J, Alvareza VA. Muscarinic regulation of dopamine and glutamate transmission in the nucleus accumbens. PNAS (2015) 112(26):8124–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515368112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Saddoris MP. Rapid dopamine dynamics in the accumbens core and shell Learning and action. Front Biosci (2013) E5(1):273–88. doi: 10.2741/E615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Wise RA. Roles for nigrostriatal—not just mesocorticolimbic—dopamine in reward and addiction. Trends Neurosci (2009) 32(10):517–24. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kufahl PR, Li Z, Risinger RC, Rainey CJ, Wu G, Bloom AS, et al. Neural responses to acute cocaine administration in the human brain detected by fMRI. NeuroImage (2005) 28(4):904–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Wise RA. Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat Rev Neurosci (2004) 5(6):483–94. doi: 10.1038/nrn1406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Tobler PN. Adaptive Coding of Reward Value by Dopamine Neurons. Science (2005) 307(5715):1642–5. doi: 10.1126/science.1105370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nat Rev Neurosci (2011) 12(11):652–69. doi: 10.1038/nrn3119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sutherland MT, McHugh MJ, Pariyadath V, Stein EA. Resting state functional connectivity in addiction: lessons learned and a road ahead. NeuroImage (2012) 62(4):2281–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry analysis. Lancet Psychiatry (2016) 3(8):760–73. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00104-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Ma N, Liu Y, Fu X-M, Li N, Wang C-X, Zhang H, et al. Abnormal Brain Default-Mode Network Functional Connectivity in Drug Addicts. PloS One (2011) 6(1):e16560. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Wilcox CE, Teshiba TM, Merideth F, Ling J, Mayer AR. Enhanced cue reactivity and fronto-striatal functional connectivity in cocaine use disorders. Drug Alcohol Depend (2011) 115(1–2):137–44. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Upadhyay J, Maleki N, Potter J, Elman I, Rudrauf D, Knudsen J, et al. Alterations in brain structure and functional connectivity in prescription opioid-dependent patients. Brain (2010) 133(7):2098–114. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Stein JL, Wiedholz LM, Bassett DS, Weinberger DR, Zink CF, Mattay VS, et al. A validated network of effective amygdala connectivity. NeuroImage (2007) 36(3):736–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat Neurosci (2005) 8(11):1458–63. doi: 10.1038/nn1584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci (2005) 8:1481–9. doi: 10.1038/nn1579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Gu H, Salmeron BJ, Ross TJ, Geng X, Zhan W, Stein EA, et al. Mesocorticolimbic circuits are impaired in chronic cocaine users as demonstrated by resting-state functional connectivity. NeuroImage (2010) 53(2):593–601. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Wang W, Wang Y, Qin W. Changes in functional connectivity of ventral anterior cingulate cortex in heroin abusers. Chin Med J (2010) 123(12):1582–8. doi: 10.3760/cma.j.issn.0366-6999.2010.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Goldstein RZ, Leskovjan AC, Hoff AL, Hitzemann R, Bashan F, Khalsa SS, et al. Severity of neuropsychological impairment in cocaine and alcohol addiction: association with metabolism in the prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia (2004) 42(11):1447–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Kelly C, Zuo X-N, Gotimer K, Cox CL, Lynch L, Brock D, et al. Reduced Interhemispheric Resting State Functional Connectivity in Cocaine Addiction. Biol Psychiatry (2011) 69(7):684–92. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Yuan K, Qin W, Dong M, Liu J, Sun J, Liu P, et al. Gray matter deficits and resting-state abnormalities in abstinent heroin-dependent individuals. Neurosci Lett (2010) 482(2):101–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hu Y, Salmeron BJ, Gu H, Stein EA, Yang Y. Impaired Functional Connectivity Within and Between Frontostriatal Circuits and Its Association With Compulsive Drug Use and Trait Impulsivity in Cocaine Addiction. JAMA Psychiatry (2015) 72(6):584. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Cisler JM, Elton A, Kennedy AP, Young J, Smitherman S, Andrew James G, et al. Altered functional connectivity of the insular cortex across prefrontal networks in cocaine addiction. Psychiatry Res: Neuroimaging (2013) 213(1):39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Liang X, He Y, Salmeron BJ, Gu H, Stein EA, Yang Y. Interactions between the Salience and Default-Mode Networks Are Disrupted in Cocaine Addiction. J Neurosci (2015) 35(21):8081–90. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3188-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Zhang S, Li C-SR. Ventral striatal dysfunction in cocaine dependence – difference mapping for subregional resting state functional connectivity. Transl Psychiatry (2018) 8(1):119. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0164-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Zhu X, Cortes CR, Mathur K, Tomasi D, Momenan R. Model-free functional connectivity and impulsivity correlates of alcohol dependence: a resting-state study: Functional connectivity and impulsivity correlates of alcohol dependence. Addict Biol (2017) 22(1):206–17. doi: 10.1111/adb.12272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Manza P, Tomasi D, Volkow ND. Subcortical Local Functional Hyperconnectivity in Cannabis Dependence. Biol Psychiatry: Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging (2018) 3(3):285–93. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Wei Z, Yang N, Liu Y, Yang L, Wang Y, Han L, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity between the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and thalamus is associated with risky decision-making in nicotine addicts. Sci Rep (2016) 6(1):1–9. doi: 10.1038/srep21778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Huang X, Liu Z, Mwansisya TE, Pu W, Zhou L, Liu C, et al. Betel quid chewing alters functional connectivity in frontal and default networks: A resting-state fMRI study: Betel Quid Alters Network Connectivity. J Magnetic Reson Imaging (2016) 45(1):157–66. doi: 10.1002/jmri.25322

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Zhu X, Zhu Q, Jiang C, Shen H, Wang F, Liao W, et al. Disrupted Resting-State Default Mode Network in Betel Quid-Dependent Individuals. Front Psychol (2017) 8:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00084/full

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Weng J-C, Chou Y-S, Huang G-J, Tyan Y-S, Ho M-C. Mapping brain functional alterations in betel-quid chewers using resting-state fMRI and network analysis. Psychopharmacology (2018) 1–15. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-4841-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Huang X, Pu W, Liu H, Li X, Greenshaw AJ, Dursun SM, et al. Altered Brain Functional Connectivity in Betel Quid-Dependent Chewers. Front Psychiatry (2017) 8:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00239/full

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Liu T, Li J, Zhao Z, Zhong Y, Zhang Z, Xu Q, et al. Betel quid dependence is associated with functional connectivity changes of the anterior cingulate cortex: a resting-state fMRI study. J Trans Med (2016) 14(1):1–13. doi: 10.1186/s12967-016-0784-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Liu T, Li J, Zhang Z, Xu Q, Lu G, Huang S, et al. Altered Long- and Short-Range Functional Connectivity in Patients with Betel Quid Dependence: A Resting-State Functional MRI Study. Cell Physiol Biochem (2016) 40(6):1626–36. doi: 10.1159/000453212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Zalesky A, Fornito A, Bullmore ET. Network-based statistic: Identifying differences in brain networks. NeuroImage (2010) 53(4):1197–207. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Lee C-Y, Chang C-S, Shieh T-Y, Chang Y-Y. Development and validation of a self-rating scale for betel quid chewers based on a male-prisoner population in Taiwan: The Betel Quid Dependence Scale. Drug Alcohol Depend (2012) 121(1–2):18–22. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.07.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Chu N-S. Neurological aspects of areca and betel chewing. Addict Biol (2002) 7(1):111–4. doi: 10.1080/13556210120091473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Zhang X, Reichart PA. A review of betel quid chewing, oral cancer and precancer in Mainland China. Oncol (2007) 43(5):424–30. doi: 10.1016/j.oraloncology.2006.08.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Yan. DPARSF: a MATLAB toolbox for “pipeline” data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Front Syst Neurosci (2010) 4(13):1–7. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00013/abstract

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Flandin G, Friston KJ. Statistical parametric mapping. Scholarpedia (2008) 3(4):6232. doi: 10.4249/scholarpedia.6232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Friston KJ, Williams S, Howard R, Frackowiak RSJ, Turner R. Movement-Related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn Reson Med (1996) 35(3):346–55. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910350312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Yan C-G, Cheung B, Kelly C, Colcombe S, Craddock RC, Martino AD, et al. A Comprehensive Assessment of Regional Variation in the Impact of Head Micromovements on Functional Connectomics. (2014) 40:183–201. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S. Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images. NeuroImage (2002) 17(2):825–41. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. (2013) 28:2142–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Rolls ET, Joliot M, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Implementation of a new parcellation of the orbitofrontal cortex in the automated anatomical labeling atlas. NeuroImage (2015) 122:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain. NeuroImage (2002) 15(1):273–89. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Chiavaras MM, LeGoualher G, Evans A, Petrides M. Three-Dimensional Probabilistic Atlas of the Human Orbitofrontal Sulci in Standardized Stereotaxic Space. NeuroImage (2001) 13(3):479–96. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Chang C-C, Lin C-J. Training ν-Support Vector Classifiers: Theory and Algorithms. Neural Comput (2001) 13:2119–47. doi: 10.1162/089976601750399335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Rice ME, Harris GT. Comparing effect sizes in follow-up studies: ROC Area, Cohen's d, and r. Law Hum Behav (2005) 29(5):615–20. doi: 10.1007/s10979-005-6832-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Kufahl P, Li Z, Risinger R, Rainey C, Piacentine L, Wu G, et al. Expectation Modulates Human Brain Responses to Acute Cocaine: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. Biol Psychiatry (2008) 63(2):222–30. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Rolls ET. The Brain, Emotion, and Depression. Oxford: Oxford University Press; (2018).

Google Scholar

62. Gerfen CR, Surmeier DJ. Modulation of Striatal Projection Systems by Dopamine. Annu Rev Neurosci (2011) 34(1):441–66. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Wise RA. Dopamine and reward: The anhedonia hypothesis 30 years on. Neurotoxicity Res (2008) 14(2–3):169–83. doi: 10.1007/BF03033808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Gatley SJ, Ding Y-S, Logan J, et al. Relationship between psychostimulant-induced “high” and dopamine transporter occupancy. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1996) 93(19):10388–92. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.19.10388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Heinz A. Correlation Between Dopamine D2 Receptors in the Ventral Striatum and Central Processing of Alcohol Cues and Craving. Am J Psychiatry (2004) 161(10):1783–9. doi: 10.1176/ajp.161.10.1783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Rose JE, Behm FM, Salley AN, Bates JE, Coleman RE, Hawk TC, et al. Regional brain activity correlates of nicotine dependence. Neuropsychopharmacology (2007) 32(12):2441. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Bjork JM, Gilman JM. The effects of acute alcohol administration on the human brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Neuropharmacology (2014) 84:101–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.07.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Motzkin JC, Baskin-Sommers A, Newman JP, Kiehl KA, Koenigs M. Neural correlates of substance abuse: Reduced functional connectivity between areas underlying reward and cognitive control: Neuropsychological Correlates of SUD. Hum Brain Mapp (2014) 35(9):4282–92. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Supèr H. Working Memory in the Primary Visual Cortex. Arch Neurol (2003) 60(6):809–12. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.6.809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Shokri-Kojori E, Tomasi D, Wiers CE, Wang G-J, Volkow ND. Alcohol affects brain functional connectivity and its coupling with behavior: greater effects in male heavy drinkers. Mol Psychiatry (2017) 22(8):1185–95. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Hanlon CA, Dowdle LT, Naselaris T, Canterberry M, Cortese BM. Visual cortex activation to drug cues: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging papers in addiction and substance abuse literature. Drug Alcohol Depend (2014) 143:206–12. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.07.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Schoenbaum G, Shaham Y. The role of orbitofrontal cortex in drug addiction: a review of preclinical studies. Biol Psychiatry (2008) 63(3):256–62. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Schoenbaum G, Roesch MR, Stalnaker TA. Orbitofrontal cortex, decision-making and drug addiction. Trends Neurosci (2006) 29(2):116–24. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Rolls ET. The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain Cognition (2004) 55(1):11–29. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00277-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang G-J, Swanson JM. Dopamine in drug abuse and addiction: results from imaging studies and treatment implications. Mol Psychiatry (2004) 9(6):557–69. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Rolls ET. The orbitofrontal cortex and emotion in health and disease, including depression. Neuropsychologia (2017) 1–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.09.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion decision making and the orbital fontal cortex. Cereb Cortex (2000) 10:395–07. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Kringelbach M. The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Prog Neurobiol (2004) 72(5):341–72. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Liu X, Powell DK, Wang H, Gold BT, Corbly CR, Joseph JE. Functional Dissociation in Frontal and Striatal Areas for Processing of Positive and Negative Reward Information. J Neurosci (2007) 27(17):4587–97. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5227-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Tomasi D, Telang F. Addiction: Beyond dopamine reward circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci (2011) 108(37):15037–42. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010654108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Fedota JR, Stein EA. Resting-state functional connectivity and nicotine addiction: prospects for biomarker development: Resting-state functional connectivity and nicotine. Ann NY Acad Sci (2015) 1349(1):64–82. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Davis M, Walker DL, Miles L, Grillon C. Phasic vs Sustained Fear in Rats and Humans: Role of the Extended Amygdala in Fear vs Anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology (2010) 35(1):105–35. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Plasticity of reward neurocircuitry and the “dark side” of drug addiction. Nat Neurosci (2005) 8(11):1442–4. doi: 10.1038/nn1105-1442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology (2010) 35(1):217. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Volkow ND, Tomasi D, Wang G-J, Logan J, Alexoff DL, Jayne M, et al. Stimulant-induced dopamine increases are markedly blunted in active cocaine abusers. Mol Psychiatry (2014) 19(9):1037–43. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.58

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Koob G. Drug Addiction, Dysregulation of Reward, and Allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology (2001) 24(2):97–129. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00195-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Ding W, Sun J, Sun Y, Chen X, Zhou Y, Zhuang Z, et al. Trait impulsivity and impaired prefrontal impulse inhibition function in adolescents with internet gaming addiction revealed by a Go/No-Go fMRI study. Behav Brain Functions (2014) 10(1):20. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-10-20

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Rose JE, McClernon FJ, Froeliger B, Behm FM, Preud'homme X, Krystal AD. Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation of the Superior Frontal Gyrus Modulates Craving for Cigarettes. Biol Psychiatry (2011) 70(8):794–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Ray S, Gohel S, Biswal BB. Altered Functional Connectivity Strength in Abstinent Chronic Cocaine Smokers Compared to Healthy Controls. Brain Connectivity (2015) 5(8):476–86. doi: 10.1089/brain.2014.0240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Claus ED, Weywadt CR. Resting-State Connectivity in Former, Current and Never Smokers. Nicotine Tob Res (2018) 1–32. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nty266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Vossel S, Geng JJ, Fink GR. Dorsal and Ventral Attention Systems: Distinct Neural Circuits but Collaborative Roles. Neuroscientist (2014) 20(2):150–9. doi: 10.1177/1073858413494269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Seghier ML. The Angular Gyrus: Multiple Functions and Multiple Subdivisions. Neuroscientist (2013) 19(1):43–61. doi: 10.1177/1073858412440596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci (2003) 100(1):253–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Goldstein RZ, Craig ADB, Bechara A, Garavan H, Childress AR, Paulus MP, et al. The Neurocircuitry of Impaired Insight in Drug Addiction. Trends Cogn Sci (2009) 13(9):372–80. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Battelli L, Alvarez GA, Carlson T, Pascual-Leone A. The Role of the Parietal Lobe in Visual Extinction Studied with Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation. J Cogn Neurosci (2009) 21(10):1946–55. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: functional brain imaging, basal ganglia, orbitofrontal cortex, betel quid, arecoline, resting-state fMRI

Citation: Sariah A, Guo S, Zuo J, Pu W, Liu H, Rolls ET, Xue Z, Liu Z and Huang X (2020) Acute and Chronic Effects of Betel Quid Chewing on Brain Functional Connectivity. Front. Psychiatry 11:198. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00198

Received: 11 October 2019; Accepted: 02 March 2020;
Published: 17 March 2020.

Edited by:

Maorong HU, Nanchang University, China

Reviewed by:

Xiangyu Long, University of Calgary, Canada
Wen Qin, Tianjin Medical University General Hospital, China
Yifeng Wang, Sichuan Normal University, China

Copyright © 2020 Sariah, Guo, Zuo, Pu, Liu, Rolls, Xue, Liu and Huang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xiaojun Huang, xiaojunh9@csu.edu.cn

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.