Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Public Health, 08 September 2021
Sec. Children and Health

Shaping Healthy Eating Habits in Children With Persuasive Strategies: Toward a Typology

  • 1Department of Communication, Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
  • 2Department of Media and Communication, Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Munich, Germany

There is an abundance of evidence that the presentation of unhealthy foods (UHFs) in different media has the power to shape eating habits in children. Compared to this rich body of work with regard to the effects of UHF presentations, studies testing the effects of healthy foods (HFs) are less conclusive. In particular, while the persuasive mechanisms behind HF presentations are well-understood, we lack insights about the role of messages factors, that is, how are (and should) HFs (be) presented in order to foster healthy eating habits in children. This paper tackles this research gap by suggesting the Persuasive Strategies Presenting Healthy Foods to Children (PSPHF) typology, classified along three pillars: (a) composition-related characteristics, (b) source-related characteristics, and (c) information-related characteristics. Against the background of the PSPHF typology, we review the available empirical evidence, outline pressing research gaps, and discuss implications for researchers, health promoters, and program planers.

Food is an essential part of our life, and our food environment shapes preferences and eating behaviors. The social environment and cultural environment are essential factors in shaping the eating behaviors of children (1). Yet, food preferences can also be formed in a mediated environment (2). The depiction of foods in the media has been heavily criticized in the past (3). Content analyses of traditional TV commercials (3, 4), of online content (5), of embedded forms of advertising (6), and food depictions within entertaining content like TV series (7), or movies (8), indicate a dominant focus on food low in nutritional value and high in fat, salt, and/or sugar. Keller and Schulz (9) thus ascertain that the media presents a distorted view of the types and proportions of foods that should be eaten.

The lack of foods high in nutritional value (e.g., fruits and vegetables) and the simultaneously high focus on fast food, candy, soft drink, alcohol, and salted snacks in mass media are a cause for concern. Particularly, children are not consuming the recommended fruit and vegetable ratios, but tend to overeat sugary and salty products (10). The predominant unhealthy food (UHF) depictions in the media might reinforce the already-existing eating preferences of children. This is indicated by a recent meta-analysis (11) and two literature reviews (12, 13) demonstrating that the eating habits of children are heavily affected by food presentations in the media. In the long run, this can negatively affect the weight of the children, increasing the likelihood of obesity and overweight, which raises serious implications for long-term health concerns (14).

Existing research has shown that children respond to the presentation of unhealthy snacks in their corresponding food behaviors and preferences (1518). Fostering healthy food (HF) behaviors through promotional efforts has proven to be less effective and not as straightforward. Presentations of HF in media content targeted at children, while able to increase the hypothetical liking of these foods (19, 20), however, have limited or even backfiring effects on food choices (17, 21). Researchers have argued that the mere presentation of HFs is not sufficient to shape eating habits among children. By contrast, food presentations need to be connected to specific persuasive strategies (22).

A systematic analysis of such persuasive strategies is highly warranted. We have a good grasp of the underlying mechanisms and of the individual susceptibility factors explaining the effects of food presentations on children (23, 24). However, despite these efforts, and the general literature on message factors in health communication (25, 26), we lack a comprehensive overview of the message factors that can be applied to HF-related media content targeted at children. Research on message factors from other areas such as non-food products or research on adults cannot be generalized to the presentation of HFs to children. In this paper, we therefore suggest a comprehensive typology of persuasive strategies, the Persuasive Strategies Presenting Healthy Foods to Children (PSPHF) typology. We present an integration of the available empirical findings into our typology and discuss how different persuasive strategies in connection with HF presentations can shape attitudinal, intentional, and behavioral outcomes in children. The PSPHF typology has not only important theoretical and methodological implications for future research, but also potentially informative to practitioners and policy regulators. That is, in contrast to individual (e.g., age) and contextual (e.g., parents, culture) factors that also shape healthy eating habits of children, persuasive strategies can be strategically implemented by content creators.

Theoretical Background

To gain insights into the underlying mechanisms of food presentations, Folkvord and colleagues (24) developed a theoretical framework focusing on the effectiveness of promotional strategies. Based on the Cue Reactivity Theory (27), and on the Processing of Commercialized Media Content model (PCMC) (28), the Reactivity of Embedded Food Cues in Advertising Model (REFCAM) (24) suggests that specific foods integrated into editorial content, such as product placements or food integrated into advergames, influence children in a two-step process. In a first step, physiological (i.e., heart rate) (29) and/or psychological reactions (i.e., thought about foods) (30) of the children are influenced by the presentation of foods. Focusing on embedded foods, the model assumes that decreased cognitive processing, thus enhanced automatic processing, can influence the eating behavior of children in a next step (24). The authors describe the relationship between children's reactions toward the presented foods (i.e., cue reactivity) and children's eating behavior as being reciprocal, thus, as an “incentive-sensitization process” (p. 27) (24). Moreover, based on the Differential Susceptibility to Media Effects Model (31), the authors assume that individual susceptibility factors, such as children's Body Mass Index (BMI), are influencing this two-step process. Very briefly, the authors mention that message factors, i.e., “the level of integration of food cues,” plays a role in children's reactions as these factors are influencing “the level of elaboration” (p. 28) (24). However, this is not discussed in more detail in the REFCAM.

Focusing on the effectiveness of healthy food presentations in media, Folkvord (23) established another theoretical framework. Similar to the REFCAM (24), the Promotion of Healthy Foods Model describes a two-step process: In a first step, the attention as well as the “reinforced value of the [HF] (i.e., liking and wanting)” has to be influenced before the food presentations contribute to a healthy eating behavior (p. 114). Again, the author suggests a reciprocal relationship between the reinforced value as well as the food intake. Individual susceptibility factors (e.g., BMI) as well as contextual factors (e.g., parental background) (23) of the children are again considered in the model.

In light of the existing theoretical conceptualizations (23, 24) as well as the available empirical evidence (17, 18, 30, 32), we have a good understanding of the underlying processes behind the effectiveness of food presentations in the media. However, when it comes to HF presentations, we particularly lack insights into how variations in content shape the openness of the children to HF options. That is, we need a typology that can be used to describe how media messages regarding HFs should be arranged to successfully increase the attractiveness of these foods for children. More precisely, we lack an in-depth understanding of most important message factors, thus, the most effective “level of integration of HF cues” (p. 28) (24).

A Typology of Message Factors

Based on content analyses that investigated the presentation of foods (4, 6, 8, 9, 33), and based on current literature reviews regarding persuasive techniques used in food promotions with children (34), we have identified three pillars of persuasive strategies as our PSPHF typology: (a) composition-related characteristics, (b) source-related characteristics, and (c) information-related characteristics.

Composition-related characteristics are composed of the modality of (visual, audio, audiovisual) (35), the centrality of (foreground, background) (36), duration of (36), and interaction with (37) foods. This pillar mainly focuses on theoretical and empirical assumptions of obtrusiveness and awareness of HF presentations as motivational factors to contribute to healthier eating. Closely connected to the effectiveness of interactive elements, source-related characteristics include strategies that are directly related to the source who is presenting the message. This second pillar describes who is providing the message (38), but also other source-related aspects, such as how many characters/endorsers are presented in connection with foods (39). While the majority of effects might depend again partly on the awareness and obtrusiveness as motivational factors, the effectiveness of who is presented in connection with an HF depends on the relationship of children with the presenter (40). Emotional factors can be described as the main drivers of the effectiveness of these strategies. Lastly, the information-related characteristics, i.e., which information is connected with a HF presentation, include aspects of which arguments are presented to portray the importance of consuming a specific food, and also how the information is presented (41). Depending on the strategic integration of the information, cognitive but also emotional components act as motivational factors. The message factors are intertwined; thus, a combination is possible, and effects may not be independent (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Typology of persuasive strategies.

The three dimensions proposed capture important possibilities of how HFs can be integrated in the media in order to influence healthy eating habits of children. These components are especially important as soon as children are able to understand the content (i.e., have the ability to process the provided information) and to build relationships with, e.g., the characters presented in this content. Based on the current empirical evidence, this developing process begins at the age of three and is seen as completed at the approximate age of 16 when adolescents have reached the end of the so-called reflective phase (42). Of course, the following strategies might differ in their effectiveness depending on the developmental stages of children. However, this paper does not specifically predict for which age group-specific HF presentations are effective. The paper rather aims to provide an overview of possible effective strategies based on the available empirical evidence and current theoretical assumptions.

Literature Review

Composition

Research on embedded brands highlights the relevance of composition factors for the effectiveness of cue integrations in entertaining content. In particular, four aspects have been identified: (1) the modality (35), (2) the centrality (36), (3) the duration of a presentation (35), and (4) interactive elements of the presentation (43).

Modality

Three presentation modalities are typically distinguished: (a) visual, (b) verbal, or (c) audiovisual presentations (35). The order of these modality types already reflects their rising obtrusiveness. Recall ability of information and conscious awareness are considered as relevant when making product decisions (35, 44). Thus, particularly, audiovisual presentations are considered as effective with regard to awareness and memory measures due to the double modality of the presentation (35). Of course, there is also an argument to be made about the effectiveness of unobtrusive presentations. This is founded on the mere exposure effect, which is based on the theoretical concept of a non-associative learning process (45). The mere exposure effect shows a positive affective effect due to multiple, unobtrusive stimulus presentations on the stimulus evaluation without explicit memory traces (46).

Yet, conscious awareness for HF might be particularly relevant (47). Especially when being asked to choose between a HF and an UHF option, conscious awareness for the former might be crucial, as children have to act against their inherent preference to choose the latter (48). Children have to activate their inhibitory control and consciously remind themselves of the healthier option. Along these lines, Charry (19) has indicated that multimodal, i.e., audiovisual HF presentations are more effective in creating HF intentions in children compared to unimodal, i.e., visual presentations. She explains this effect due to the higher level of attention multimodal food presentations create. We thus follow Charry's (19) (p. 611) recommendation: “that screenwriters of popular programmes should be advised to use audio-visual supports, not those that are merely visual, when integrating HF consumption messages into their shows for pre-adolescents.” It should be noted that this study (19) was focusing on the intentional and not actual behavior of the children. Thus, a systematic analysis of how modality drives HF choices in children is a gap in research that should be addressed in the future.

Centrality

To evaluate how central a cue is presented, research often distinguishes whether or not the cue is presented (a) in the background (i.e., second image plane), (b) as a central presentation (i.e., first image plane), and (c) as a closeup (i.e., depiction on the first image plane and on more than 50% of the screen) (6).

Based on the theory of selective attention, “we perceive and remember only those objects and details that receive focused attention” (p. 1059) (49). Presentation centrality might be relevant for young children, as they have only a limited attention span (28) and thus might focus particularly on foods in the foreground. With regard to HF presentations, we again stress the role of conscious awareness for HF (47), in order to assure that children are able to activate their inhibitory control and consciously remind themselves of healthier food options (48). We would thus argue for a central and prominent integration of HF instead of a mere fruit platter in the background. Studies that systematically test these assumptions for HF presentations in children are still missing.

Duration

How long a certain cue is shown within an entertaining context also speaks to the obtrusiveness of a presentation. For brand placements, we again see that longer brand presentations lead to higher levels of awareness and memory. This, however, also coincides with more counterarguing against the presented brand and more negative brand associations (36). Yet, for HF presentations, obtrusiveness is, as highlighted already, relevant in order to create awareness and to positively influence HF choices (47). Hence, following this line of argumentation, we would suggest that for HF presentations more is actually more and we recommend longer air-time for fruit and vegetables (8). However, again we clearly lack empirical evidence for these assumptions with children as the target group.

Interaction

In addition to the already-mentioned composition factors, interaction with a cue (in our case a food) is regarded as important (37). Interaction is defined as the action of a character who is using, holding, handling (e.g., preparing and requesting), or eating a product (50). In the PCMC model, the authors (28) postulate that children allocate more cognitive resources toward more interactive than non-interactive presentations.

There are three theoretical explanations for why interactive placements are particularly successful. First, interaction with food presents the utilization of the product (51). This, in turn, facilitates knowledge about the characteristics of the product. The observation of how a media character interacts with a product may then lead to a modeling behavior (52). Second, the concept of Para-social Interaction (PSI) (40) and the Balance Model (37) suggests that if media characters evaluate products, this could affect product assessments of viewers because of the connection to the presented characters (36, 37). Thus, when children build a relationship with a character, and this character interacts with a product, the young audience is likely to adopt the behavior of the character due to this identification process. Third, interactive placements can create a meaning transfer from the presented character to the product. Therefore, likable characters, which are typically found in content targeted at children, can transfer their popularity onto the product. This is based on the theory of Evaluative Conditioning (53). Conditioning occurs when a liked character repeatedly is associated with a product (53), and it has been shown to positively influence the product evaluation.

Some effect studies on both product presentations and UHF placements with adults (36, 37) and children (43) have shown that interactive and prominent presentations trigger product choices to a higher extent compared to non-interactive presentations. One recent study revealed that showing HFs in a social context led to higher memory among children. Moreover, presenting HFs in a gregarious context led to the best memory of the HFs (54). Along the same lines, Spielvogel et al. (32) revealed that interactive presentations compared to non-interactive ones are more effective in arousing visual attention for HFs. This underlines the importance of interactive integrations of HFs in content targeted at children. Still, more studies are needed to replicate these first findings.

Source

When investigating the effects of interactive elements, the source, i.e., characters or endorsers, interacting with products seems to be a possible important motivator. As described earlier, based on the concept of PSI (40) and the Balance Model (37), characters who interact with products might influence the healthy eating habits of children. This process might be based on the liking (55), the similarity evaluation (56), the physical attractiveness (57), and also on the perceived credibility (58) of these characters. Two components seem especially important: (1) who is presented in connection with the HFs (38) and (2) how many characters are connected to the product (59).

Types of Characters/Endorsers

According to the Social Cognitive Theory (51), children learn and in a further step model behaviors observed in their surroundings. This modeling behavior can be also influenced via symbolic learning, thus, with behaviors presented by media characters (60). This behavior might vary according to the characters presented with HFs. Based on Friedman and Friedman (38) media, characters can be categorized into three important categories: peers, celebrities, and experts.

Peers. Especially when it comes to eating behaviors, peers have been found to be effective (60, 61). Peers can be defined “as children roughly the same age as the children themselves” (p. 42) (22). Studies argue that with increasing age parents get less important, while the influence of peers increases (62). This is argued based on the goal of affiliation (i.e., positive relationship with friends, gaining popularity) (63). Moreover, the perceived similarity between a peer and a child can act as an important driver to copy a behavior (58). In this regard, studies revealed that presenting peers in connection with HFs can act as a driver for healthy eating habits in children (22, 64). Thus, connecting HFs in media with peers might be one effective strategy. However, research should replicate these first results and study in more detail the underlying mechanism.

Celebrities. Celebrities are widely used when presenting foods to children (5). Studies agree that celebrities or other prominent/popular characters (65) are very attractive promotional figures. Thus, licensed characters (i.e., Ronald McDonald) (65), traditional celebrities (e.g., top-athletes) (66), or the so-called social media influencers (67) have the potential to impact the eating habits of children. The effectiveness is driven by the assumption that people tend to feel the wish to emulate the lifestyle of these popular media figures (68). Many studies already revealed the effectiveness of prominent characters for UHF (69). Testing the effects on attitudes and intentional behaviors, some studies showed positive effects of HFs in connection with popular characters (70). However, testing real behaviors or attentional effects, the connection of popular characters with HFs compared to UHFs showed mostly less effectiveness or non-significant effects (7173). There is a big variety of celebrity endorsers (e.g., licensed figures, traditional celebrities, or social media influencers); thus, investigating these different types in connection with HFs seems essential to gain a better picture of which celebrities can be deemed as effective. Overall, connecting HFs with celebrities could be a good strategy to influence the healthy eating habits of children.

Experts. Another type of character that is theoretically assumed as being effective is experts (38). In the field of health communication, experts can be defined as medical doctors, or other persons working in health-related jobs (i.e., dietitian) (58). The effectiveness of experts is theoretically based on the perceived competence or knowledge (58) and the perceived authority (61). Taking nutritional advice from an unfamiliar source certainly has a larger impact if this source has relevant credentials. According to one study comparing the three types of social endorsers (peers, celebrities, and experts), experts proofed particularly effective in shaping the healthy eating habits of children (74). However, the study used only unfamiliar characters, which might have led to the ineffectiveness of celebrities and peers (15).

In sum, connecting HFs with media characters might be a good strategy to motivate children to eat HFs. Liking, familiarity, credibility, or attractiveness of the social endorsers (75) are the factors that have the ability to shape the overall effectiveness of HF presentations regardless of the type of character.

Consensus of Characters/Endorsers

Based on assumptions of conformity concepts (76), majority-biased transmission (39), as well as on the spiral of silence (77), the modeling of behaviors might increase when a majority of people are conducting a specific behavior. Thus, not only the type of endorser might influence healthy eating habits of children but also how many characters endorse eating HFs. There exist many assumptions why people tend to model the behavior of a majority. For example, the “copy-the-majority tactic” describes that people tend to behave according to a majority because they simply think it is the majority (78); or random copying means that “if observers copy an individual at random, the likelihood to copy a majority member exceeds that of copying a minority member” (p. 65) (39). Along the same lines, other social factors might influence the copying of the majority such as the wish for prestige (79).

Some studies revealed that presenting a majority of characters or transporting social norms of a majority in media connected with HFs can shape the healthy eating behaviors of the children (22, 61). Therefore, this might be a fruitful avenue to increase HF consumption of children with different sources.

Information

The level of integration can not only vary based on compositional factors or source-related factors. Also which information is connected with a HF presentation can influence the reactions of the children (30, 80). Current studies can be roughly classified along with two forms of presentation: (1) what information is connected with HFs (i.e., emphasis frames) and (2) how identical chunks of information are presented (i.e., equivalence frames) (41). While emphasis frames highlight some information regarding HF while not mentioning others, equivalence frames describe the same information in different ways.

Emphasis Frames

There are many persuasive strategies with the goal to emphasize some aspects of a message while not mentioning others. Based on the Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) (81), people base product decisions either more on affects or cognitions. More precisely, while affects describe positive or negative emotions toward a product, cognitions include positive or negative attributes which are accredited to a product (82). In the research area of health communication, some empirical evidence has shown that positive affective cues are especially effective when promoting health-related behaviors (30, 83, 84), while cognitive cues fall short in comparison (30, 84) in adults as well as in children. Thus, emphasizing the taste and affective components connected to a food proves more impactful than highlighting nutritional facts (i.e., “full of vitamins”).

Another persuasive strategy is the use of threat or fear appeals. The Protection Motivation Theory (85) describes that fear or threat appeal is especially effective if (a) the threat is perceived as realistic, (b) the fulfillment of the threat is appraised as likely, (c) the presented solution for the portrayed problem is evaluated as efficient, and (d) the presented solution is assessed as realizable. A study with children showcased threat appeals connected to obesity are an effective strategy in influencing the healthy eating behavior of children (86). Thus, while ethically questionable (87), threat appeals seem to be effective in influencing the healthy eating behaviors of children.

Often health-related content also emphasizes the positive effects of a specific behavior on the appearance of an individual, or on the health of an individual (88). While some studies showed the effectiveness of such appearance frames in adults (89), this strategy has not been studied in children, leaving this as a yet unexamined line of research.

Furthermore, when advertising UHFs, positive outcomes of consumptions, such as fun, vitality, or sociability, are typically emphasized (90). These strategies do also have the potential to present HFs in persuasive ways. However, connecting HFs with positive outcomes such as fun, vitality, or sociability has not been properly investigated to date.

Equivalent Frames

One persuasive strategy often used when presenting health-related topics is to either present the gain of engaging in a specific behavior or the loss when not following this behavior (25, 26). According to the Prospect Theory (91), emphasizing the losses are especially effective for deductive behaviors (e.g., getting a mammography), while using gain frames are more effective for preventative behaviors (e.g., preventing obesity by eating healthy). This assumption is based on different degrees of risk assessment of these behaviors: While detection behaviors potentially involve high financial costs as well as possible negative consequences and are therefore evaluated as risky, prevention behaviors involve little financial costs and not conducting this behavior could be risky (92). Existing studies already indicate that presenting gain arguments in connection with HF presentations in media presents a good strategy to influence intentions and real consumption behavior of the children positively (80, 93). Therefore, more gain arguments should be used in HF presentations.

Another persuasive strategy based on equivalent framing is to either present a reward or a punishment for a specific behavior. This follows the classic assumptions of the Social Cognitive Theory that deduces that negative consequences make a certain behavior unappealing, while receiving rewards positively reinforces the conducted actions (2). This assumption has been tested for nutritional behavior in adults, particularly regarding alcohol portrayals in the media. Bahk (94), for instance, found that compared to showing no depictions of alcohol consumption, the presence of negative consequences deteriorated attitudes toward alcohol. Yet, the mere absence of negative consequences and the presentation of positive consequences improved the alcohol evaluations of the participants (95). A recent study furthermore indicates that either positive or negative consequences affect what behavioral expectancies viewers link to the consumption of alcohol (96). These results highlight the relevance of consequence portrayals. We are not, however, aware of any studies that employ this technique in a content setting targeted at HF and children. Still, from these first results, we conclude that presenting HFs as rewards might act as a good motivational cue for children.

For an overview of the empirical evidence of HFs presentations and its effects on children and the remaining research gaps see Tables 1, 2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Overview of evidence on the persuasive strategies regarding the effects of healthy foods on children.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overview of Evidence on the Persuasive Strategies Regarding the Effects of Healthy Foods (HF) on Children.

Discussion

The aim of this article was to provide researchers, health promoters, and program planners with a holistic “blueprint” of possibilities to integrate HFs within the media content of children, the PSPHF typology. The review of theoretical assumptions and empirical evidence also provides a valuable overview of the complexity of the effects of HF media presentations on children. Significant gaps still remain to understand the effectiveness of different message factors in connection with HFs.

We lack research looking into how composition-related factors of HFs shape cognitive, attitudinal, and behavioral outcomes in children. This is surprising because composition-related factors are the most basic characteristics of messages, and they are also comparatively easy to vary. Cognitive outcomes, prompted by composition-related factors, are particularly relevant, because they may drive attitudinal and behavioral outcomes. Some studies showed that audiovisual presentations (19) and interactive elements can contribute to some positive effects in children (32, 60). Research using eye-tracking, or heart rate measures (29), might lead to even more insights into how these factors influence the reactions of children toward HFs. To our knowledge, only two studies used eye-tracking measures to investigate the effects of HFs on children (32, 54). Other studies should follow these examples.

With regard to source-related aspects, studies with regard to HFs in audiovisual media are still inconclusive. First empirical evidence shows some positive effects with regard to peers (22), experts (74), and celebrities (70) presentations with HFs. However, not all source-related factors have been systematically studied to date. In this context, evaluative outcomes seem to be most pressing. Depending on the type of endorsers presented in connection with HFs, different underlying mechanisms might be prevalent (75). To gain better insights into how and why specific endorsers are especially effective, it seems important to conduct qualitative studies to gain a deeper understanding of these processes as well as physiological studies that highlight cue reactivity responses of children to different characters. Since especially familiar and popular endorsers seem to be effective (60), studies should first set their focus on endorsers children already know. From the current research, using a majority of endorsers seems to be especially effective or at least not harming the effects of HF presentations (22, 61).

The third pillar, information-related characteristics, can be described as the most heterogeneous one. While some presentation strategies have been found to influence the healthy eating behavior of children positively (e.g., gain framing, affective cognitive arguments) (30, 80, 93), other components have been hardly (e.g., threat appeals) (86), or not at all studied (appearance- vs. health-framing, reward presentations) (64, 89). Thus, qualitative studies are needed as a first step to gain detailed insights into how children evaluate and react when being exposed to these message factors.

Implications for Future Research

Our suggested typology, along with the discussion of prior research, bears a number of important theoretical and methodological implications. In fact, with the exception of interactive elements, most composition-related factors are relevant for creating awareness for HF presentations. Based on the available empirical evidence, one could argue that awareness alone is not sufficient to shape the healthy eating preferences of children. Composition-related factors may help to foster and support the effectiveness of source- and information-related characteristics. Source-related factors, by contrast, clearly directly affect cognitive, attitudinal, and behavioral outcomes. They relate to cognitive outcomes because endorsers, such as celebrities or popular characters, drive attention. However, they also directly affect associative processes (i.e., connect foods to evaluations) and stimulate heuristic decision-making, and thus impact evaluative outcomes such as liking and behaviors. Information-related characteristics matter for argument-based strategies, most likely under situations of high processing motivation. Since we lack studies to verify these assumptions, as a further step, the presented strategies should be tested with a series of empirical studies. Furthermore, content analyses could give insights about how these strategies have already been used for the presentation of UHF and HF. Besides the obvious lack of research for some characteristics with respect to some outcomes, we particularly identify four pressing research gaps.

First, further research should set its focus on healthy eating behaviors in children as an outcome variable. Studies suggest that attitudes and intentions (19, 20, 70, 93) can be influenced with HF presentations, while studies measuring eating behaviors are less conclusive (17, 21, 22). To counteract the rising overweight and obesity in children, influencing the behavior of the children seems to be one key aspect.

Second, we lack research comparing the effectiveness of composition-, source-, and information-related characteristics. For instance, some composition-related characteristics may be more relevant for cognitive outcomes compared to others. That is, the factors that drive the obtrusiveness of composition-related characteristics to children are far from being fully understood. Moreover, we lack studies on perceptual processes, using eye-tracking studies and designs systematically varying composition-related characteristics. By the same token, there is a dearth of studies systematically comparing the effects of source-related characteristics. Although there are some preliminary studies comparing the effects of peers, celebrities, and experts (74), we lack a deeper understanding of why some sources may be more consequential for cognitive, attitudinal, or behavioral outcomes than others.

Third, an important avenue to investigate is the interplay of different message factors. Composition-related characteristics may be important boundary conditions for the effectiveness of source- and information-related characteristics. That is, composition-related characteristics drive attention, and such attention can further support how, for instance, expert statements are processed, or messages are understood and stored in the memory of the children. Along the same lines, specific source-related characteristics may further support the persuasiveness of arguments made about healthy nutrition. However, the three characteristics may also work in opposite directions. One might argue that using the most effective persuasive strategies all in one can act as the ultimate motivator for healthy eating habits. However, this high persuasion attempt might also lead to reactance, especially if children gain the impression that they should be influenced by these presentations (97). Thus, investigating the interplay and closely connected to this level of persuasion attempt can lead to important insights.

Fourth, the effectiveness of composition-, source-, and information-related characteristics may depend on the individual susceptibility as well as contextual factors. According to the REFCAM (24) and the Promotion of HFs Model (23), factors such as the BMI or parental mediation styles play a key role. Studies showed that these components can shape how children react to HF presentations (16, 21, 22). Likewise, the developmental stage of children may greatly matter as well (30), which is far from being fully understood. Especially, information-related characteristics demand cognitive resources and skills that develop over time (42). Also, persuasion knowledge depends on the cognitive development of children and may predict how source- and information-related characteristics are processed and understood.

Practical Implications

For health promoters, and content creators, the PSPHF typology provides an overview of the different factors that can (and also should) be taken into account when HFs are integrated in media targeted at children. Our overview suggests that the mere presentation of HFs is not sufficient to generate desired outcomes. Since message factors can be directly influenced as compared to individual susceptibility characteristics and social context factors, our PSPHF typology gives clear hints about concrete stylistic elements, program plots, or relevant verbal or visual integrations. Our overview of the existing research helps to determine for which characteristics are backed up with the existing empirical research, and which are not. This helps practitioners to build their content decisions on a clear body of scientific evidence.

Conclusion

Health promoters and content creators who aim to foster healthy eating habits of children face the challenge that the key factors driving HF choice, such as individual or social aspects, cannot be directly influenced. Message factors are thus the most important vehicle to influence HF behaviors in children. That is, message factors can be directly influenced and therefore used to counteract to the rising obesity and overweight in children. We have suggested the PSPHF typology to enable a systematic overview of message factors. Our typology clearly shows that several significant blind spots remain when trying to understand how messages can and should be drafted in order to foster healthy habits among children. At the same time, it demonstrates the empirical evidence, therefore informing not only researchers but also content creators. It is our hope that the PSPHF typology, as a general framework, sparks research in this area, potentially leading to substantial updates and also revisions of the typology.

Author Contributions

AB, BN, and JM contributed to the conception of the review. AB conceptualized the first draft of the review and the typology. AB and BN drafted the initial manuscript and reviewed and revised the manuscript. JM wrote sections of the manuscript and reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Conflict of Interest

The research was supported by funds of the Oesterreichischen Nationalbank (Austrian Central Bank, Anniversary Fund, Project Number: 17 715).

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Birch LL, Fisher JO. Development of eating behaviors among children and adolescents. Pediatrics. (1998) 101(Pt. 2):539–49.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

2. Bandura A. Social cognitive theory in cultural context. Appl Psychol. (2002) 51:269–90. doi: 10.1111/1464-0597.00092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Kelly B, Freeman B, King L, Chapman K, Baur LA, Gill T. Television advertising, not viewing, is associated with negative dietary patterns in children: TV ads associated with poor diets in children. Pediatr Obes. (2016) 11:158–60. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Vilaro MJ, Barnett TE, Watson AM, Merten JW, Mathews AE. Weekday and weekend food advertising varies on children's television in the USA but persuasive techniques and unhealthy items still dominate. Public Health. (2017) 142:22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2016.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Coates AE, Hardman CA, Halford JCG, Christiansen P, Boyland EJ. Food and beverage cues featured in youtube videos of social media influencers popular with children: an exploratory study. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:2142. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Naderer B, Matthes J, Spielvogel I. How brands appear in children's movies. A systematic content analysis of the past 25 Years. Int J Advert. (2019) 38:237–57. doi: 10.1080/02650487.2017.1410000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Mayrhofer M, Naderer B, Binder A. Unhealthy fun: food and beverage references in comedy series. J Mass Commun Q. (2020) 97:257–77. doi: 10.1177/1077699019844768

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Matthes J, Naderer B. Sugary, fatty, and prominent: food and beverage appearances in children's movies from 1991 to 2015. Pediatr Obes. (2019) 14:1–7. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Keller SK, Schulz PJ. Distorted food pyramid in kids programmes: a content analysis of television advertising watched in Switzerland. Eur J Public Health. (2011) 21:300–5. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckq065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Geller KS, Dzewaltowski DA. Longitudinal and cross-sectional influences on youth fruit and vegetable consumption. Nutr Rev. (2009) 67:65–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2008.00142.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Sadeghirad B, Duhaney T, Motaghipisheh S, Campbell NRC, Johnston BC. Influence of unhealthy food and beverage marketing on children's dietary intake and preference: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized trials: meta-analysis of unhealthy food and beverage marketing. Obes Rev. (2016) 17:945–59. doi: 10.1111/obr.12445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Dixon H, Niven P, Scully M, Wakefield M. Food marketing with movie character toys: effects on young children's preferences for unhealthy and healthier fast food meals. Appetite. (2017) 117:342–50. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Smith R, Kelly B, Yeatman H, Boyland E. Food marketing influences children's attitudes, preferences and consumption: a systematic critical review. Nutrients. (2019) 11:875. doi: 10.3390/nu11040875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. World Health Organization (WHO). Obesity and Overweight. (2020) Available online at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight

Google Scholar

15. Boyland EJ, Harrold JA, Kirkham TC, Halford JCG. The extent of food advertising to children on UK television in 2008. Int J Pediatr Obes. (2011) 6:455–61. doi: 10.3109/17477166.2011.608801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Halford JC, Boyland EJ, Hughes GM, Stacey L, McKean S, Dovey TM. Beyond-brand effect of television food advertisements on food choice in children: the effects of weight status. Public Health Nutr. (2008) 11:897–904. doi: 10.1017/S1368980007001231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Folkvord F, Anschütz DJ, Buijzen M, Valkenburg PM. The effect of playing advergames that promote energy-dense snacks or fruit on actual food intake among children. Am J Clin Nutr. (2013) 97:239–45. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.047126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Folkvord F, Anschütz DJ, Wiers RW, Buijzen M. The role of attentional bias in the effect of food advertising on actual food intake among children. Appetite. (2015) 84:251–8. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Charry KM. Product placement and the promotion of healthy food to pre-adolescents: when popular TV series make carrots look cool. Int J Advert. (2014) 33:599–616. doi: 10.2501/IJA-33-3-599-616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Dias M, Agante L. Can advergames boost children's healthier eating habits? A comparison between healthy and non-healthy food: advergames: boosting children's eating habits. J Consumer Behav. (2011) 10:152–60. doi: 10.1002/cb.359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Naderer B, Matthes J, Binder A, Marquart F, Mayrhofer M, Obereder A, et al. Shaping children's healthy eating habits with food placements? Food placements of high and low nutritional value in cartoons, children's BMI, food-related parental mediation strategies, and food choice. Appetite. (2018) 120:644–53. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Binder A, Naderer B, Matthes J. Do children's food choices go with the crowd? Effects of majority and minority peer cues shown within an audiovisual cartoon on children's healthy food choice. Soc Sci Med. (2019) 225:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.01.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Folkvord F, editor. The Psychology of Food Marketing and (Over)eating. 1st ed. Routledge (2019). Available online at: https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/9781000517668 doi: 10.4324/9780429274404-1 (accessed January 30, 2020)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Folkvord F, Anschütz DJ, Boyland E, Kelly B, Buijzen M. Food advertising and eating behavior in children. Curr Opin Behav Sci. (2016) 9:26–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. O'Keefe DJ, Jensen JD. The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encouraging disease detection behaviors: a meta-analytic review. J Commun. (2009) 59:296–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2009.01417.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. O'Keefe DJ, Jensen JD. The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encouraging disease prevention behaviors: a meta-analytic review. J Health Commun. (2007) 12:623–44. doi: 10.1080/10810730701615198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Jansen A. A learning model of binge eating: cue reactivity and cue exposure. Behav Res Ther. (1998) 36:257–72. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00055-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Buijzen M, Van Reijmersdal EA, Owen LH. Introducing the PCMC model: an investigative framework for young people's processing of commercialized media content. Commun Theory. (2010) 20:427–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2010.01370.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Nederkoorn C, Smulders FTY, Jansen A. Cephalic phase responses, craving and food intake in normal subjects. Appetite. (2000) 35:45–55. doi: 10.1006/appe.2000.0328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Naderer B, Binder A, Matthes J, Mayrhofer M. Healthy, sweet, brightly colored, and full of vitamins: cognitive and affective persuasive cues of food placements and children's healthy eating behavior. Int J Advert. (2020) 39:1–19. doi: 10.1080/02650487.2020.1735140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Valkenburg PM, Peter J. The differential susceptibility to media effects model. J Commun. (2013) 63:221–43. doi: 10.1111/jcom.12024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Spielvogel I, Matthes J, Naderer B, Karsay K. A treat for the eyes. An eye-tracking study on children's attention to unhealthy and healthy food cues in media content. Appetite. (2018) 125:63–71. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.01.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Araque-Padilla R, Villegas-Navas V, Montero-Simo M-J. Non-branded food placements in children's entertainment programs: a content analysis. Health Commun. (2019) 34:1222–9. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2019.1587690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Jenkin G, Madhvani N, Signal L, Bowers S. A systematic review of persuasive marketing techniques to promote food to children on television: persuasive TV food marketing to children. Obes Rev. (2014) 15:281–93. doi: 10.1111/obr.12141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Gupta PB, Lord KR. Product placement in movies: the effect of prominence and mode on audience recall. J Curr Issues Res Advert. (1998) 20:47–59. doi: 10.1080/10641734.1998.10505076

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Russell CA. Investigating the effectiveness of product placements in television shows: the role of modality and plot connection congruence on brand memory and attitude. J Consum Res. (2002) 29:306–18. doi: 10.1086/344432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Russell CA, Stern BB. Consumers, characters, and products: a balance model of sitcom product placement effects. J Advert. (2006) 35:7–21. doi: 10.2753/JOA0091-3367350101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Friedman HH, Friedman L. Endorser effectiveness by product type. J Advert Res. (1979) 19:63–71.

Google Scholar

39. Haun DBM, van Leeuwen EJC, Edelson MG. Majority influence in children and other animals. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2013) 3:61–71. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Rubin RB, McHugh MP. Development of parasocial interaction relationships. J Broadcast Electr Media. (1987) 31:279–92. doi: 10.1080/08838158709386664

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Matthes J. Framing. Nomos (2014). Available online at: http://www.nomos-elibrary.de/index.php?doi=10.5771/9783845260259 (accessed January 27, 2020).

Google Scholar

42. John DR. Consumer socialization of children: a retrospective look at twenty-five years of research. J Consum Res. (1999) 26:183–213. doi: 10.1086/209559

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Naderer B, Matthes J, Zeller P. Placing snacks in children's movies: cognitive, evaluative, and conative effects of product placements with character product interaction. Int J Advert. (2018) 37:852–70. doi: 10.1080/02650487.2017.1348034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Brennan I, Babin LA. Brand placement recognition: the influence of presentation mode and brand familiarity. J Promot Manage. (2004) 10:185–202. doi: 10.1300/J057v10n01_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Bornstein RF, D'Agostino PR. Stimulus recognition and the mere exposure effect. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1992) 63:545–52. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Zajonc RB. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1968) 9(Pt. 2):1–27. doi: 10.1037/h0025848

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Naderer B, Binder A, Matthes J, Spielvogel I, Forrai M. Food as an eye-catcher. An eye-tracking study on children's attention to healthy and unhealthy food presentations as well as non-edible objects in audiovisual media. Pediatr Obes. (2020) 15:e12591. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Desor JA, Maller O, Turner RE. Taste in acceptance of sugars by human infants. J Comp Physiol Psychol. (1973) 84:496–501. doi: 10.1037/h0034906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Simons DJ, Chabris CF. Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception. (1999) 28:1059–74. doi: 10.1068/p2952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Naderer B, Matthes J, Marquart F, Mayrhofer M. Children's attitudinal and behavioral reactions to product placements: investigating the role of placement frequency, placement integration, and parental mediation. Int J Advert. (2018) 37:236–55. doi: 10.1080/02650487.2016.1218672

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Bandura A, Grusec JE, Menlove FL. Observational learning as a function of symbolization and incentive set. Child Dev. (1966) 37:499. doi: 10.2307/1126674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Bloesch EK, Davoli CC, Roth N, Brockmole JR, Abrams RA. Watch this! Observed tool use affects perceived distance. Psychon Bull Rev. (2012) 19:177–83. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0200-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Schemer C, Matthes J, Wirth W, Textor S. Does “Passing the Courvoisier” always pay off? Positive and negative evaluative conditioning effects of brand placements in music videos: does “Passing the Courvoisier” Always Pay Off? Psychol Market. (2008) 25:923–43. doi: 10.1002/mar.20246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Samson L, Nanne AJ, Buijzen M. Remember the motivationally-relevant appeals? The influence of social and sensory appeals on memory for pronutritional messages promoting healthy foods. Int J Advert. (2020) 40:1–20. doi: 10.1080/02650487.2020.1833675

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Moyer-Gusé E. Toward a theory of entertainment persuasion: explaining the persuasive effects of entertainment-education messages. Commun Theory. (2008) 18:407–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00328.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Cohen J. Defining identification: a theoretical look at the identification of audiences with media characters. Mass Commun Soc. (2001) 4:245–64. doi: 10.1207/S15327825MCS0403_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Tiggemann M, Wilson-Barrett E. Children's figure ratings: relationship to self-esteem and negative stereotyping. Int J Eat Disord. (1998) 23:83–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

58. Wilson BJ. Designing media messages about health and nutrition: what strategies are most effective? J Nutr Educ Behav. (2007) 39:S13–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2006.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Cialdini RB, Goldstein NJ. Social influence: compliance and conformity. Annu Rev Psychol. (2004) 55:591–621. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Cruwys T, Bevelander KE, Hermans RCJ. Social modeling of eating: a review of when and why social influence affects food intake and choice. Appetite. (2015) 86:3–18. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.08.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Hang H, Davies I, Schüring J. Children's conformity to social norms to eat healthy: a developmental perspective. Soc Sci Med. (2020) 244:112666. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Kohlberg L. From Is to Out: How to Commit the Naturalistic Fallacy and Get Away With It in the Study of Moral Development. New York, NY: Academic Press (1971).

Google Scholar

63. Slaughter V, Imuta K, Peterson CC, Henry JD. Meta-analysis of theory of mind and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Dev. (2015) 86:1159–74. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Laureati M, Bergamaschi V, Pagliarini E. School-based intervention with children. Peer-modeling, reward and repeated exposure reduce food neophobia and increase liking of fruits and vegetables. Appetite. (2014) 83:26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.07.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Roberto CA, Baik J, Harris JL, Brownell KD. Influence of licensed characters on children's taste and snack preferences. Pediatrics. (2010) 126:88–93. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-3433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Boyland EJ, Harrold JA, Dovey TM, Allison M, Dobson S, Jacobs M-C, et al. Food choice and overconsumption: effect of a premium sports celebrity endorser. J Pediatr. (2013) 163:339–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.01.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Berryman R, Kavka M. ‘I Guess A Lot of People See Me as a Big Sister or a Friend': the role of intimacy in the celebrification of beauty vloggers. J Gender Stud. (2017) 26:307–20. doi: 10.1080/09589236.2017.1288611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Hirschman EC, Thompson CJ. Why media matter: toward a richer understanding of consumers' relationships with advertising and mass media. J Advert. (1997) 26:43–60. doi: 10.1080/00913367.1997.10673517

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Dixon H, Scully M, Niven P, Kelly B, Chapman K, Donovan R, et al. Effects of nutrient content claims, sports celebrity endorsements and premium offers on pre-adolescent children's food preferences: experimental research: child responses to food pack promotions. Pediatr Obes. (2014) 9:47–57. doi: 10.1111/j.2047-6310.2013.00169.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Smits T, Vandebosch H. Endorsing children's appetite for healthy foods: celebrity versus non-celebrity spokes-characters. Communications. (2012) 37:371–91.doi: 10.1515/commun-2012-0021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Coates AE, Hardman CA, Halford JCG, Christiansen P, Boyland EJ. Social media influencer marketing and children's food intake: a randomized trial. Pediatrics. (2019) 143. doi: 10.1542/peds.2018-2554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Folkvord F, de Bruijne M. The effect of the promotion of vegetables by a social influencer on adolescents' subsequent vegetable intake: a pilot study. IJERPH. (2020) 17:2243. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17072243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Murphy G, Corcoran C, Tatlow-Golden M, Boyland E, Rooney B. See, Like, share, remember: adolescents' responses to unhealthy-, healthy- and non-food advertising in social media. IJERPH. (2020) 17:2181. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17072181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Binder A, Naderer B, Matthes J. Experts, peers, or celebrities? The role of different social endorsers on children's fruit choice. Appetite. (2020) 155:104821. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Kruglanski AW, Raviv A, Bar-Tal D, Raviv A, Sharvit K, Ellis S, et al. Says who?: Epistemic authority effects in social judgment. In: Zanna MP, editor. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Elsevier (2005). p. 345–92. Available online at: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0065260105370067 (accessed October 28, 2020).

Google Scholar

76. Asch SE. Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychol Monogr Gen Appl. (1956) 70:1–70. doi: 10.1037/h0093718

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Noelle-Neumann E. The spiral of silence a theory of public opinion. J Commun. (1974) 24:43–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1974.tb00367.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Laland KN. Social learning strategies. Anim Learn Behav. (2004) 32:4–14. doi: 10.3758/BF03196002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Chudek M, Heller S, Birch S, Henrich J. Prestige-biased cultural learning: bystander's differential attention to potential models influences children's learning. Evol Hum Behav. (2012) 33:46–56. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Binder A, Naderer B, Matthes J. The effects of gain- and loss-framed nutritional messages on children's healthy eating behaviour. Public Health Nutr. (2020) 23:1726–34. doi: 10.1017/S1368980019004683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Petty RE, Cacioppo JT. The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In: Petty RE, Cacioppo JT. editor. Communication and Persuasion. New York, NY: Springer New York (1986). p. 1–24. Available online at: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-1-4612-4964-1_1 (accessed January 22, 2020).

Google Scholar

82. Fabrigar LR, Petty RE. The role of the affective and cognitive bases of attitudes in susceptibility to affectively and cognitively based persuasion. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. (1999) 25:363–81. doi: 10.1177/0146167299025003008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Hensels IS, Baines S. Changing ‘gut feelings' about food: an evaluative conditioning effect on implicit food evaluations and food choice. Learn Motiv. (2016) 55:31–44. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2016.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Lawton R, Conner M, McEachan R. Desire or reason: predicting health behaviors from affective and cognitive attitudes. Health Psychol. (2009) 28:56–65. doi: 10.1037/a0013424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Rogers RW. A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change1. J Psychol. (1975) 91:93–114. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1975.9915803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Charry KM, Demoulin NTM. Behavioural evidence for the effectiveness of threat appeals in the promotion of healthy food to children. Int J Advert. (2012) 31:773–94. doi: 10.2501/IJA-31-4-773-794

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Hastings G, Stead M, Webb J. Fear appeals in social marketing: strategic and ethical reasons for concern. Psychol Mark. (2004) 21:961–86. doi: 10.1002/mar.20043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Scheufele DA. Framing as a theory of media effects. J Commun. (1999) 49:103–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1999.tb02784.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Aubrey JS. Looking good versus feeling good: an investigation of media frames of health advice and their effects on women's body-related self-perceptions. Sex Roles. (2010) 63:50–63. doi: 10.1007/s11199-010-9768-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Halford JCG, Boyland EJ. The marketing of foods and non-alcoholic beverages to children. Setting the research agenda. Appetite. (2013) 62:182–4. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Kahneman D, Tversky A. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica. (1979) 47:263. doi: 10.2307/1914185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Rivers SE, Salovey P, Pizarro DA, Pizarro J, Schneider TR. Message framing and pap test utilization among women attending a community health clinic. J Health Psychol. (2005) 10:65–77. doi: 10.1177/1359105305048556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Wyllie J, Baxter S, Kulczynski A. Healthy kids: examining the effect of message framing and polarity on children's attitudes and behavioral intentions. J Advert. (2015) 44:140–50. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2015.1018462

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Bahk CM. The impact of presence versus absence of negative consequences in dramatic portrayals of alcohol drinking. J Alcohol Drug Educ. (1997) 42:18–26.

Google Scholar

95. Kulick AD, Rosenberg H. Influence of positive and negative film portrayals of drinking on older adolescents' alcohol outcome expectancies. J Appl Social Pyschol. (2001) 31:1492–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb02684.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Mayrhofer M, Naderer B. Mass media as alcohol educator for everyone? Effects of portrayed alcohol consequences and the influence of viewers' characteristics. Media Psychol. (2019) 22:217–43. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2017.1378112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Friestad M, Wright P. The persuasion knowledge model: how people cope with persuasion attempts. J Consum Res. (1994) 21:1. doi: 10.1086/209380

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: food advertising, healthy food behavior, message factors, persuasive strategies, children, typology

Citation: Binder A, Naderer B and Matthes J (2021) Shaping Healthy Eating Habits in Children With Persuasive Strategies: Toward a Typology. Front. Public Health 9:676127. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.676127

Received: 04 March 2021; Accepted: 03 August 2021;
Published: 08 September 2021.

Edited by:

Frans Folkvord, Tilburg University, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Helen Dixon, Cancer Council Victoria, Australia
Ifeoma Adaji, University of Saskatchewan, Canada

Copyright © 2021 Binder, Naderer and Matthes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alice Binder, alice.binder@univie.ac.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.