Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 26 September 2022
Sec. Children and Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Behavioural Patterns and Mental Health in Early Life View all 4 articles

Physical activity intervention promotes working memory and motor competence in preschool children

\nJing-Yi ZhangJing-Yi Zhang1Qi-Qi ShenQi-Qi Shen1Dong-Ling WangDong-Ling Wang1Jin-Mei HouJin-Mei Hou1Tong XiaTong Xia1Shou QiuShou Qiu2Xiao-Ye WangXiao-Ye Wang2Si-Bo ZhouSi-Bo Zhou2Wen-Wen YangWen-Wen Yang2Si-Yu HengSi-Yu Heng2Can-Can LuCan-Can Lu2Lei Cui
Lei Cui1*Heng-Chan Yin
Heng-Chan Yin1*
  • 1College of P.E. and Sports, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 2Branch School of Mingguang, Beijing Normal University Kindergarten, Beijing, China

Objective: This study investigated the effects of 12 weeks of specifically designed physical activity intervention on working memory and motor competence in preschool children and explored the correlation between working memory changes and motor competence changes by the intervention.

Methods: Four classes of preschool children were grouped into an intervention group and a control group. Children in the intervention group received a 12-week physical activity intervention, while children in the control group followed their daily routine as usual. Before and after the intervention period, children were assessed with the 1-back task and Movement Assessment Battery for Children, second edition (MABC-2) to measure their working memory and motor competence, respectively.

Results: Regarding working memory, the accuracy on the 1-back task increased significantly in the intervention group relative to the control group. The intervention group demonstrated a greater decrease in response time from pre- to posttest than the control group, but the difference was not statistically significant. Regarding motor competence, children's manual dexterity, aiming and catching and total score increased significantly in the intervention group relative to the control group, while no significant difference in static and dynamic balance was observed between the two groups. Furthermore, the correlation results showed that changes in the efficacy and efficiency of working memory were positively related to changes in static and dynamic balance and the total score on the MABC-2.

Conclusion: These findings demonstrated that 12 weeks of specifically designed physical activity intervention could improve preschool children's efficacy of working memory as well as manual dexterity, aiming and catching and global motor competence. The improvement in the efficacy and efficiency of working memory was positively related to the improvement in static and dynamic balance and global motor competence.

Introduction

Working memory, as one of the key components of executive function, refers to the collection of cognitive processes that temporarily retain information in an accessible state, which are suitable for carrying out any mental task (14). Working memory is essential for a wide range of cognitive abilities, such as understanding written or spoken language, reasoning, and making plans and decisions (5), particularly those that require dealing with interference, conflict, or distraction (6), and it predicts important cognitive and academic outcomes in children. Working memory deficits, on the other hand, have negative repercussions. Preschool children are in a critical period of working memory development. Improving their working memory has become a concerning problem. Meta-analytic studies have found that working memory can be trained (79). Computer-based training, educational programs, and physical activities have all been offered as ways to foster children's working memory (1014). Physical activity interventions that are consistently challenging, playful, enjoyable, and cognitively enriched appear to have a more positive impact on children's working memory (13, 15).

Motor competence is defined as the ability to perform a wide range of motor skills that require motor coordination and control and that primarily include fundamental locomotor (e.g., jumping and running) and object-control skills (e.g., catching and throwing) (16). These competences are continuously developed in the early years and are further refined into context- and sport-specific skills (17). Good motor competence is seen to be vital for children's physical, social, and psychological development (18) as well as being the foundation for an active lifestyle, whereas poor motor competence is linked to less involvement in sports activities (19). The initial years of a person's life are especially important for developing their motor competence, which does not develop naturally or automatically over time but is learned, practiced, and developed (20, 21). According to this view, intervention programs with structured learning environments and specific purposes are critical for motor competence development in children (2224). Placing children in environments with sensory, cognitive and social stimulation could inspire their exploration, curiosity and enjoyment in as early as preschool, which could lead to optimal development (25).

Collectively, early childhood is a period when children's physical, motor and cognitive abilities are especially rapidly developed (26). Carrying out well directed and planned Physical Education programs is essential for children's physical, social, and emotional wellbeing, and for increasing adherence to physical exercise (27). Studies have mostly explored the influences of physical activity on either motor competence or working memory independently; however, evidence on the effects of specifically designed interventions on both motor competence and working memory is still lacking. Furthermore, the relationship between motor competence and working memory is still unclear.

As a result, the goal of this study was to determine whether a specifically designed physical activity intervention may help preschool children improve their working memory and motor competence. There were two hypotheses of this study: (1) a long-term physical activity intervention may cause changes in working memory and motor competence, and (2) working memory changes caused by physical activity intervention are associated with motor competence changes.

Materials and methods

Participants and study design

Four classes of children from a kindergarten school located in Beijing were invited to participate in the study. Among a total of 126 preschool children aged 4 to 5 years, 17 were excluded for the following reasons: (a) their parents did not sign a consent form (N = 2); (b) they did not complete related tests for various reasons (i.e., developmental delays or not attending school during the testing period) (N = 8); and (c) their attendance rates fell below 70% (N = 7). The final sample consisted of 109 participants. The guardians of all the participants were fully informed of the experimental contents and signed written informed consent.

Due to the convenience and not disturbing the kindergarten's schedule, the experiment was organized by class. Four classes (Class 1–4) were divided into two groups based on children's physical activity levels (a survey from their guardians) and pretest scores to ensure homogeneity, with every two classes being combined as a whole. As a result, Class 1 and Class 3 combined to form a single group, and Class 2 and Class 4 combined to form the other group. All dependent variables of the two groups remained no significant difference on pre-test. Then, the two groups were randomly designated the intervention group (N = 57) and control group (N = 52). No significant differences in sex, age or dominant hand were observed between the two groups. Detailed sample characteristics are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics.

The experiment used a 2 (group: intervention group, control group) × 2 (time: pre, post) mixed factorial design with time as a within-subjects factor and group as a between-subjects factor. The 1-back task and Movement Assessment Battery for Children, second edition (MABC-2) were used to measure children's working memory and motor competence, respectively, both before and after the intervention.

Physical activity intervention procedures

During the 12-week intervention period, children in the intervention group received physical activity interventions (three 40-min sessions weekly), while children in the control group engaged in regular activities as usual. For most of the time, the intervention was conducted on Tuesday, Wednesday, and Friday. We conducted the intervention on Tuesday instead of Monday because the kindergarten pre-arranged their own activity on Monday. And as we tried to avoid four consecutive days' lack of intervention during a week, we did not arrange the intervention on three consecutive days. However, in some cases (i.e., the intervention day is on a public holiday), the arrangement would be adjusted, but three intervention days a week need to be covered. The interventions were carried out at the outdoor playground at the school; however, in some cases of severe weather, they were carried out in a multipurpose room with ample space. All the interventions were conducted in the morning, and the instructors are sport psychology students with qualified teacher certifications.

The activities included two types of games. Type 1 games mainly focused on motor learning with the purpose of allowing children to acquire fundamental movement skills, including locomotor (e.g., jumping, running, hopping, leaping, sliding, and galloping) and object control skills (e.g., catching, throwing, dribbling, rolling, kicking, and striking), to set the foundation for more challenging and complex games soon afterward. Type 2 games were based on type 1 games but incorporated more cognitive rules that were specifically designed to foster children's cognitive abilities with the purpose of promoting working memory and motor competence jointly. Since cognitive abilities are distinguishing but also integrating, and they influence and are influenced by each other, cognitive rules in the games included but were not limited to the core elements of working memory. A summary of the main games used is featured in Table 2. All games were designed for the children's enjoyment, enthusiasm, and engagement.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of six main type 2 games in the physical activity intervention.

The 12 weeks of intervention were divided into 3 phases. In the first 4 weeks (Phase 1), type 1 games were introduced because simple, less difficult motor games were assumed to be sufficiently challenging for the children since many of the fundamental skills being introduced were not yet familiar to them. In the middle 4 weeks (Phase 2), type 2 games were included, with some type 1 games still remaining because after 4 weeks of practice in phase 1, some motor skills had become automated, and more difficult games were needed. In the last 4 weeks (Phase 3), there remained a combination of type 1 and type 2 games, but the difficulty level continued to rise to increase the chance of reaching the children's optimal challenge level (28, 29).

For each 40-min intervention session, a 5-min warm-up, 30-min formal activities, and a 5-min cool-down were needed. During the warm-up and cool-down, children were mainly led to stretch their whole body with low-intensity activities for the sake of fully preparing for the intervention (warm-up) and relaxing their body (cool-down). Furthermore, the practice of deep breathing was also included in the cool-down part to help them calm down and prepare for the next routine of kindergarten. The 30-min formal activities were pre-arranged games with higher intensity. Throughout the session, the children's heart rates (HR) were monitored by polar watch or a manual check of their radial arteries to ensure moderate intensity physical activity (60% to 69% HRmax).

Data acquisition

Working memory assessment

A modified 1-back task was applied to measure the participants' working memory before and after the intervention. The 1-back task is taken from the n-back paradigm, which is widely used as a measure of working memory. In the n-back task, subjects identify over consecutive trials whether the current stimulus matches a stimulus presented n trials before (30). Based on preschoolers' cognitive abilities, we adopted a computerized pictorial 1-back task to test their working memory, presented on E-prime 1.0.

The target stimuli were composed of 5 different geometric shapes of different colors to suit the children's cognitive level. Before the experiment task, the experimenter fully explained the rules of the game with the help of the geometric pictures printed on the white paper. After the children reported that they fully understood the rules, they began to work on the experimental task. The task involved practice and formal sessions with 12 and 48 trials, respectively. For each trial, the target stimulus was presented on a screen for 3000 ms, which was followed by a white blank screen presented for 1000 ms. Children were required to make judgments as accurately and quickly as possible by pressing a button. All trials were presented in randomized order, and for each stimulus, the children were asked to determine whether the picture presented on the screen was the same as the one that had been shown previously. Response accuracy (ACC) and reaction time (RT) during formal sessions were collected to assess working memory performance. The higher the ACC, the higher the efficiency, in contrast, the lower the RT, the higher the efficacy.

Motor competence assessment

The MABC-2 was used for each participant to measure their motor competence before and after the intervention period. It is a revision of the MABC, one of the most broadly used assessment tools by psychologists, physiotherapists, and educational professionals (31), which assesses children's motor competence on eight tasks for three age bands (3~6 years, 7~10 years, and 11~16 years) (32). These tasks are categorized to assess three motor skills: manual dexterity, aiming and catching, and static and dynamic balance. Their scores are combined into a total score as a general indicator of global motor competence.

During the test, for each test item, children were allowed to make two attempts, with their best attempt used as the score. An F (failure) score was given when a child failed to complete a task or and an R (refusal) score was given when a child refused to perform a task.

Raw item scores were converted into age-adjusted standard scores using the officially licensed system (Standardized Assessment System for Children's Motor Coordination, Chinese version; https://movementabc.online/yjy/#/home). In the system, F and R scores would automatically convert into 1, which is the minimum standard score. Three standard scores that assess manual dexterity, aiming and catching, and static and dynamic balance separately, and a total standard score that assesses global motor competence were used as the dependent measures.

Statistical analysis

Analyses were conducted using SPSS 26.0 (International Business Machines Corp., NY, USA).

For all of the variables, independent-sample t tests were used to assess whether participants in the two groups differed on baseline scores. A repeated-measures ANOVA was then applied to explore task performance changes in the intervention group and control group from pre- to postintervention. A simple effect test was subsequently performed when the interaction effect was significant.

Finally, a Pearson correlation analysis was used to estimate the relationship between changes in working memory and changes in motor competence with physical activity intervention for children in the intervention group.

For all statistical results, p ≤ 0.05 (*) was considered significant, p ≤ 0.01 (**) was considered highly significant, p ≤ 0.001 (***) was considered strongly significant and p > 0.05 was considered non-significant.

Results

Baseline characteristics

An independent-sample t test of the pretest scores showed no significant differences between the two groups with respect to the 1-back task (ACC) (t = −0.616, p = 0.539), 1-back task (RT) (t = −0.070, p = 0.944), manual dexterity (t = −0.072, p = 0.943), aiming and catching (t = −0.749, p = 0.455), static and dynamic balance (t = 1.457, p = 0.148), and total score (t = 0.760, p = 0.449), indicating group homogeneity before the physical activity intervention was implemented (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Baseline characteristics of participants.

Working memory results

For the ACC of the 1-back task, the results revealed a significant main effect of time [F(1, 107) = 38.654, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.265], main effect of the group [F(1, 107) = 11.171, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.095], and time × group interaction [F(1, 107) = 12.810, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.107] (see Table 4). Further results of simple effect analysis showed a significant difference between pre- and posttest results for the intervention group with higher ACC found for posttest than pretest results [F(1, 107) = 50.291, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.320] but no significant difference observed between pre- and posttest results in the control group [F(1, 107) = 3.327, p = 0.071, ηp2 = 0.030]. Moreover, there was no significant difference between the intervention group and control group for the pretest [F(1, 107) = 0.380, p = 0.539, ηp2 = 0.004], but there was a significant difference for the posttest [F(1, 107)= 29.613, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.217], with the ACC of the intervention group being significantly higher than that of the control group (see Figure 1).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. 1-back task performance.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Changes in 1-back task performance. (A) Simple effect analysis results in 1-back task (ACC). (B) Simple effect analysis results in 1-back task (RT). **p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001.

Regarding RT of the 1-back task, there was a significant main effect of time [F(1, 107) = 28.971, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.213], indicating that the RT changed significantly between pre- and posttests (Table 4). Further analysis showed that the RT decreased significantly for the posttest relative to the pretest, both in the intervention group [F(1, 107) = 9.646, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.162] and control group [F(1, 107) = 20.623, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.083], but no significant difference between the two groups was observed [F(1, 107) = 0.993, p = 0.321, ηp2 = 0.009] for the posttest (Figure 1). Independent sample t test results showed no significant difference between the intervention group and control group in terms of changes in RT (posttest – pretest) (t = 0.891, p = 0.375).

Motor competence results

For manual dexterity, the results revealed a significant main effect of group [F(1, 107) = 5.777, p = 0.018, ηp2 = 0.051] and a time × group interaction [F(1, 107) = 9.564, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.082], and no significant main effect of time was found [F(1, 107) = 0.096, p = 0.757, ηp2 = 0.001] (Table 5). Further results of simple effect analysis showed a significant difference between pre- and posttest results for the intervention group [F(1, 107) = 6.069, p = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.054] with higher posttest than pretest scores, but no significant difference was observed between pre- and posttest results for the control group [F(1, 107) = 3.700, p = 0.057, ηp2 = 0.033]. Moreover, no significant difference between the intervention group and control group on the pretest [F(1, 107) = 0.005, p = 0.943, ηp2 < 0.001] was observed, but there was a significant difference for the posttest [F(1, 107) = 11.646, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.098], with scores of the intervention group being significantly higher than those of the control group (Figure 2).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. MABC-2 performance.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Changes in MABC-2 performance. (A) Simple effect analysis results in manual dexterity. (B) Simple effect analysis results in aiming and catching. (C) Simple effect analysis results in static and dynamic balance. (D) Simple effect analysis results in total score. *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001.

For aiming and catching, the results revealed a significant main effect of group [F(1, 107) = 7.920, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.069] and time × group interaction [F(1, 107) = 7.564, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.066], but no significant main effect of time was observed [F(1, 107) = 1.174, p = 0.281, ηp2 = 0.011] (Table 5). Further results of simple effect analysis showed a significant difference between pre- and posttest results for the intervention group [F(1, 107) = 7.703, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.067] with higher posttest than pretest scores, but no significant difference was observed between pre- and posttest scores in the control group [F(1, 107) = 1.328, p = 0.252, ηp2 = 0.012]. Moreover, there was no significant difference between the intervention group and control group for the pretest [F(1, 107) = 0.562, p = 0.455, ηp2 = 0.005], but there was a significant difference for the posttest [F(1, 107) = 17.040, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.137], with scores in the intervention group being significantly higher than those in the control group (Figure 2).

For static and dynamic balance, the results revealed a significant time × group interaction [F(1, 107) = 6.135, p = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.054] (Table 5). The time × group interaction effect was further analyzed using simple effect analysis. The results showed that for the intervention group, the posttest scores were much higher than the pretest scores, but the difference was not statistically significant [F(1, 107) = 2.040, p = 0.156, ηp2 = 0.019]; there was a significant decrease in posttest scores compared to pretest scores in the control group [F(1, 107) = 4.248, p = 0.042, ηp2 = 0.038]. Moreover, there was no significant difference between the two groups after the experiment [F(1, 107) = 1.987, p = 0.162, ηp2 = 0.018] (Figure 2).

In terms of total score, the results revealed a significant main effect of group [F(1, 107) = 5.007, p = 0.027, ηp2 = 0.045] and time × group interaction [F(1, 107) = 23.157, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.178], but no significant main effect of time was observed [F(1, 107) = 0.115, p = 0.735, ηp2 = 0.001] (Table 5). Further results of simple effect analysis showed a significant improvement for the posttest from the pretest in the intervention group [F(1, 107) =13.905, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.115], while there was a significant decrease from the posttest to the pretest [F(1, 107) = 9.566, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.082] in the control group. Moreover, there was no significant difference between the two groups for the pretest [F(1, 107) = 0.578, p = 0.449, ηp2 = 0.005], while a significant difference between them for the posttest was observed [F(1, 107) = 20.091, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.158], with scores in the intervention group being significantly higher than those in the control group (Figure 2).

Correlations between working memory and motor performance changes

Correlation analysis results of changes in working memory and motor competence for children in the intervention group showed significant positive correlations between changes in the static and dynamic balance and 1-back (ACC) (r = 0.289, p = 0.029) and changes in the total score and 1-back (ACC) (r = 0.293, p = 0.027) (Table 6). Additionally, significant negative correlations were found between changes in the static and dynamic balance and 1-back (RT) (r = −0.419, p = 0.001) and changes in the total score and 1-back (RT) (r = −0.341, p = 0.009) (Table 6). The results indicated that the improvements of the efficacy (increase in ACC) and efficiency (decrease in RT) of working memory were positively related to the improvement of skills of static and dynamic balance and global motor competence. However, one thing should also be noticed that except for the moderate correlation between changes in static and dynamic balance and 1-back (RT), the other correlations are weak.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Correlations between motor performance and working memory changes.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to examine the effects of an physical activity intervention on promoting working memory and motor competence in preschool children. We found that physical activity intervention significantly increased the efficacy of working memory, manual dexterity, aiming and catching, and global motor competence. Furthermore, there was a significant positive correlation between improved working memory performance (both ACC and RT) and improved static and dynamic balance as well as global motor competence for the intervention group.

Effects of the physical activity intervention on working memory

Our findings showed that a 12-week physical activity intervention specifically tailored to preschool children enhanced their working memory, which is consistent with previous research (33).

According to meta-analytical studies, a favorable impact on working memory was observed after an extended period of increased physical activity (34). Training studies have also evidenced a relationship between physical activity programs and improved working memory (33, 35, 36). In addition, among the various intensities of physical activity, moderate-intensity physical activity is considered to be the most effective at improving cognition (37, 38). In our study, the physical activity intervention lasted 12 weeks, with moderate intensity throughout, resulting in better working memory performance for children in the intervention group than in the control group.

Except for the positive influences of physical activity itself, cognitive elements engaged in the intervention were also an important contributor to improving participants' working memory. Previous studies have revealed that cognitively enriched programs have the potential to improve cognitive abilities than physical activities with less cognitive content. Mirko Schmidt et al. (28) compared the effects of a 6-week combined physical-cognitive intervention vs. a sedentary cognitive intervention and a control group and found that children from two cognitively engaged groups improved their working memory performance compared to children in the control group. A pilot study from Valentina Biino et al. (39) investigated the influences of cognitively enriched physical activity on motor skills and executive functions in preschool children and found that after a 12-week intervention, children who received the cognitively enriched physical activity intervention showed significant improvements in working memory and gross motor skills compared to those who were assigned to swimming and the control group. These studies suggested that physical activity intervention with cognitive engagement is a viable way to improve working memory at as early as preschool age. The games we utilized in our study incorporated rich cognitive elements into physical activity games that targeted promoting motor and working memory abilities jointly. For example, in a game called “Delayed Imitation”, teachers performed a sequence of specified movements in the first four beats, and children were asked to imitate these movements in the following four beats. The game required children to use a variety of cognitive abilities, including working memory, which helped them keep the sequence of movements in mind before imitating successively. With continuous practice of these games, the children's working memory improved.

Notably, in our study, the improvement in working memory in the physical activity intervention was mainly reflected in efficacy (ACC) rather than efficiency (RT). It may be that compared to RT, ACC is a more sensitive and reliable measure of working memory tasks in childhood (40, 41), which differs from tasks for young adults, where RTs are more reliable than accuracies under the same conditions (42, 43). Beyond the fact that the physical activity intervention did not significantly enhance the efficiency of working memory, we also found that compared to performance on the pretest, the efficiency of working memory for children in both groups was significantly enhanced with the posttest. Since previous studies have found that rapid improvements in working memory tasks occur during preschool and early school years (26, 44, 45), it is reasonable that regardless of whether an intervention was received, children's performance on RT for the 1-back task significantly improved.

Effects of the physical activity intervention on motor competence

The results showed that in general, the 12-week PA intervention improved children's motor competence. This is consistent with previous reviews that indicated positive effects of preschool-based interventions on fundamental movement skill proficiency in early childhood (21, 46, 47), suggesting that specific interventions could positively affect children's motor competence. However, we also noticed that static and dynamic balance were not improved after the intervention. This may be due to the emergence of a ‘ceiling effects.' Ceiling effects occur when the scores of a relatively large proportion of a sample are in the upper range of the measurement scale (48, 49). There are four tasks used to assess static and dynamic balance, and the sum of these four tasks' standard scores is equal to the final standard score for static and dynamic balance. As we observed, a large proportion of children in the intervention group reached ‘ceiling effects' on the pre-test (the same as children in the control group), with about 47.4% and 71.9% of them receiving the highest score on the ‘walking heels raised' and ‘jumping over cord' tasks, respectively. This resulted in a relatively high score for static and dynamic balance on the pretest. Therefore, although the children's performance improved on the posttest, there was no significant difference identified. Since the proportion of children who reached the highest score on the two tasks is similar between the two groups, there was no significant difference both on pre-test and post-test for static and dynamic balance.

The first years of life are an essential period for developing motor competence. A series of studies showed that children who learned motor skills under specialist guidance showed greater growth in motor competence than children engaged in free play (5052). This indicates that in addition to natural development and maturation, continuous interaction with a stimulating and supportive social and physical environment as well as professional instructions also benefit children's motor competence (46, 53, 54). The physical activity intervention used in our study is based on fundamental movement skills requiring motor coordination and control, providing many opportunities for participants to learn and practice under the guidance of professional instructors in attractive environments and settings to can maximize their development of motor competence.

The results also showed that for participants in the control group, there was no difference in manual dexterity, aiming and catching between the pretest and posttest. Because of the significant decrease in static and dynamic balance, the total score also decreased significantly. These results surprised us because it was expected that due to normal development and maturation, children's motor competence should increase even without specific intervention (55). Possible reasons for the decrease in static and dynamic balance may include a mis-estimation of the children's abilities as a result of ‘ceiling effects,' and bias in the process of converting raw scores to age-adjusted standard scores. Another possible reason that cannot be ignored may be that the pretest occurred at the beginning of a new semester after a long summer holiday, when children were expected to have engaged in more physical activity; therefore, they had many opportunities to acquire better motor competence. The posttest occurred at the end of the semester. The semester ran from summer to winter, and children's physical activity decreased during this period, so their performance on some of the tasks declined. This indicated that without adequate physical activity experience, children's motor competence may decrease.

Correlations between working memory and motor performance

The correlation results showed that the improvement of working memory was positively related to the improvement of static and dynamic balance and global motor competence, indicating that the development of motor competence and the maturation of working memory are interrelated (56). From a neuroscientific point of view, their connection may be due to the fact that they share overlapping neural mechanisms and draw on common resources (5761). Research has found that working memory and its neural circuitry follow a trajectory similar to that of motor competence (45), as the basal ganglia, prefrontal cortex and cerebellum are coactivated for top-down control of behavior in both complex cognitive and movement tasks (62, 63). Research from the behavioral sciences has also found that interventions aimed at promoting motor skills could also improve the performance of working memory (6366), and the program of motor activities linked to executive functions significantly improved both motor competence and executive functions (67), indicating a positive relation between working memory and motor competence.

In this study, the games we used in the intervention involved the deep engagement of motor and cognitive abilities, which contributed to the joint improvement of motor competence and working memory. Type 1 games involved process of motor learning and control. When participating in these games, children managed new stimuli, meaning that to perform motor skills better, cognitive abilities such as working memory were activated (62). Type 2 games combined motor skills and cognitive rules to provide opportunities to practice motor competence and working memory at the same time. As the games became more difficult, children gained experience working with new stimuli and practicing complex motor skills, which led to the improvement of their motor competence and working memory.

Conclusion

In summary, our study indicated that a 12-week specifically designed physical activity intervention could improve preschool children's working memory and motor competence. Additionally, the improvement in the efficiency and the efficacy of working memory was found to be positively related to the improvement in static and dynamic balance and global motor competence.

Study limitations

The present results suggest an effective way of promoting preschool children's working memory and motor competence jointly. In addition to the contributions of this study, we must consider some limitations and thus view the results with caution. In our study, only two groups were set, so as to compare the effects of a specifically designed physical activity intervention and simple physical activity on working memory and motor competence, without comparing the effects of specifically designed physical activity interventions and simple working memory training (i.e., computer-based working memory training) on working memory. A simple working memory training group could be added in future studies to further explore the effects of the physical activity interventions and simple working memory training. Furthermore, our study only considered the influence of two factors (time and group) on the tested variables; other factors, such as gender and socioeconomic status, should be better considered in future research.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

H-CY and LC designed the experiment and guided the intervention. J-YZ implemented the intervention, collected and analyzed data, and wrote the manuscript. Q-QS, J-MH, and TX implemented the intervention and collected data. D-LW collected data and edited the manuscript. SQ, X-YW, S-BZ, W-WY, S-YH, and C-CL assisted to implement the intervention. All the authors have read and approved of the final version.

Funding

This study was funded by the Key Program of the National Social Science Foundation (grant number 19ATY010).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Principle of Beijing Normal University Kindergarten, Shan Huang, for her support of our research; Assistant Di Wu for assisting with our demands; and Beijing Normal University Kindergarten (Mingguang) Teachers Ran Li and Tian-Xin Lou for their assistance during the data collection procedure.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Cowan N. Visual and auditory working memory capacity. Trend Cognit Sci. (1998) 2:77. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01144-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Ozonoff S, Strayer DL. Further evidence of intact working memory in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2001) 31:257–63. doi: 10.1023/A:1010794902139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Verté S, Geurts HM, Roeyers H, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA. (2006). The relationship of working memory, inhibition, and response variability in child psychopathology. J Neurosci Methods. (2005) 151:5–14. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2005.08.023.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Stöckel T, Hughes CM. The relation between measures of cognitive and motor functioning in 5- to 6-year-old children. Psychol Res. (2016) 80:543–54. doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0662-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. (2013) 64:135–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Kane MJ, Conway AR, Miura TK, Colflesh GJ. Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: a question of construct validity. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. (2007) 33:615–22. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev Psychol. (2013) 49:270–91. doi: 10.1037/a0028228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Sala G, Gobet F. Working memory training in typically developing children: A meta-analysis of the available evidence. Dev Psychol. (2017) 53:671–85. doi: 10.1037/dev0000265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kassai R, Futo J, Demetrovics Z, Takacs ZKA. meta-analysis of the experimental evidence on the near- and far-transfer effects among children's executive function skills. Psychol Bull. (2019) 145:165–88. doi: 10.1037/bul0000180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Karbach J, Unger K. Executive control training from middle childhood to adolescence. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:390. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Mackey AP, Hill SS, Stone SI, Bunge SA. Differential effects of reasoning and speed training in children. Dev Sci. (2011) 14:582–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01005.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Tomporowski PD, Pesce C. Exercise, sports, and performance arts benefit cognition via a common process. Psychol Bull. (2019) 145:929–51. doi: 10.1037/bul0000200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Diamond A, Ling DS. (2016). Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2015) 18:34–48. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. (2011) 333:959–64. doi: 10.1126/science.1204529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Takacs ZK, Kassai R. The efficacy of different interventions to foster children's executive function skills: a series of meta-analyses. Psychol Bull. (2019) 145:653–97. doi: 10.1037/bul0000195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Hackfort D. Dictionary of Sport Psychology: Sport, Exercise, and Performing Arts. London: Academic Press (2019).

Google Scholar

17. Lubans DR, Morgan PJ, Cliff DP, Barnett LM, Okely AD. Fundamental movement skills in children and adolescents: review of associated health benefits. Sports Med. (2010) 40:1019–35. doi: 10.2165/11536850-000000000-00000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Goodway JD, Ozmun JC, Gallahue DL. Understanding Motor Development: Infants, Children, Adolescents, Adults. Burlington, MA: Jones and Bartlett Learning (2019).

Google Scholar

19. Cobley S, Abraham C, Baker J. Relative age effects on physical education attainment and school sport representation. Phys Educ Sport Pedag. (2008) 13:267–76. doi: 10.1080/17408980701711983

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Robinson LE, Stodden DF, Barnett LM, Lopes VP, Logan SW, Rodrigues LP, et al. Motor competence and its effect on positive developmental trajectories of health. Sports Med. (2015) 45:1273–84. doi: 10.1007/s40279-015-0351-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Logan SW, Robinson LE, Wilson AE, Lucas WA. Getting the fundamentals of movement: a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of motor skill interventions in children. Child Care Health Dev. (2011) 38:305–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2011.01307.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Gallahue DL, Ozmun JC, Goodway J. Understanding Motor Development: Infants, Children, Adolescents, Adults. New York: McGraw-Hill (2012).

Google Scholar

23. Flôres FS, Rodrigues LP, Copetti F, Lopes F, Cordovil R. Affordances for motor skill development in home, school and sport environments: a narrative review. Percept Mot Skills. (2019) 126:366–88. doi: 10.1177/0031512519829271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Niemistö D, Finni T, Haapala EA, Cantell M, Korhonen E, Sääkslahti A, et al. Environmental correlates of motor competence in children-the skilled kids study. Int J Environ Res Pub Health. (2019) 16:1989. doi: 10.3390./ijerph16111989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Ball NJ, Mercado E, Orduña I. Enriched environments as a potential treatment for developmental disorders: a critical assessment. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:466. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Zelazo PD, Müller U, Frye D, Marcovitch S, Argitis G, Boseovski J, et al. The development of executive function in early childhood. Monograph Soc Res Child Dev. (2003) 68:1–151. doi: 10.1111./j.0037-976X.2003.00261.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Arufe Giráldez V, Pena García A, Navarro Patón R. Efectos de los programas de Educación Física en el desarrollo motriz, cognitivo, social, emocional y la salud de niños de 0 a 6 años. Una revisión sistemática. Sport Sci. J. (2021) 7:448–80. doi: 10.17979/sportis.2021.7.3.8661

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Schmidt M, Mavilidi MF, Singh A, Englert C. Combining physical and cognitive training to improve kindergarten children's executive functions: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. (2020) 63:101908. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101908

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Pesce C, Crova C, Marchetti R, Struzzolino I, Masci I, Vannozzi G, et al. Searching for cognitively optimal challenge point in physical activity for children with typical and atypical motor development. Mental Health Phys Activity. (2013) 6:172–80. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2013.07.001.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Chatham CH, Herd SA, Brant AM, Hazy TE, Miyake A, O'Reilly R, et al. From an executive network to executive control: a computational model of the n-back task. J Cogn Neurosci. (2011) 23:3598–619. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Brown T, Lalor A. The movement assessment battery for children–second edition (MABC-2): a review and critique. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr. (2009) 29:86–103. doi: 10.1080/01942630802574908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kwok C, Mackay M, Agnew JA, Synnes A, Zwicker JG. (2019). Does the movement assessment battery for children-2 at 3 years of age predict developmental coordination disorder at 4.5 years of age in children born very preterm? Res. Dev Disabil. (2018) 84:36–42. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.04.003.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Mulvey KL, Taunton S, Pennell A, Brian A. Head, toes, knees, skip! improving preschool children's executive function through a motor competence intervention. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2018) 40:233–9. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2018-0007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Álvarez-Bueno C, Pesce C, Cavero-Redondo I, Sánchez-López M, Martínez-Hortelano JA, Martínez-Vizcaíno V, et al. (2017). The effect of physical activity interventions on children's cognition and metacognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr. (2017) 56:729–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.06.012.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Hansen AL, Johnsen BH, Sollers JJ, Stenvik K, Thayer JF. Heart rate variability and its relation to prefrontal cognitive function: the effects of training and detraining. Eur J Appl Physiol. (2004) 93:263–72. doi: 10.1007/s00421-004-1208-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Stroth S, Reinhardt RK, Thöne J, Hille K, Schneider M, Härtel S, et al. (2010). Impact of aerobic exercise training on cognitive functions and affect associated to the COMT polymorphism in young adults. Neurobiol Learn Mem. (2010) 94:364–72. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.08.003.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Arent SM, Landers DM. Arousal, anxiety, and performance: a reexamination of the Inverted-U hypothesis. Res Q Exerc Sport. (2003) 74:436–44. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2003.10609113.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Chang YK, Chu CH, Wang CC, Wang YC, Song TF, Tsai CL, et al. Dose-response relation between exercise duration and cognition. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. (2015) 47:159–65. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Biino V, Tinagli V, Borioni F, Pesce C. Cognitively enriched physical activity may foster motor competence and executive function as early as preschool age: a pilot trial. Phys Educ Sport Pedag. (2021) 17:1–19. doi: 10.1080/17408989.2021.1990249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Diamond A, Barnett WS, Thomas J, Munro S. Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science. (2007) 318:1387–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1151148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Diamond A, Kirkham N. Not quite as grown-up as we like to think: parallels between cognition in childhood and adulthood. Psychol Sci. (2005) 16:291–7. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01530.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Hockey A, Geffen G. The concurrent validity and test? Retest reliability of a visuospatial working memory task. Intelligence. (2004) 32:591–605. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Perrig WJ, Meier B. The concurrent validity of the N-back task as a working memory measure. Memory. (2010) 18:394–412. doi: 10.1080/09658211003702171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Carlson SM, Moses LJ. Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Dev. (2001) 72:1032–53. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Best JR, Miller PHA. Developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev. (2010) 81:1641–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Wick K, Leeger-Aschmann CS, Monn ND, Radtke T, Ott LV, Rebholz CE, et al. Interventions to promote fundamental movement skills in childcare and kindergarten: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. (2017) 47:2045–68. doi: 10.1007/s40279-017-0723-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Riethmuller AM, Jones R, Okely AD. Efficacy of interventions to improve motor development in young children: a systematic review. Pediatrics. (2009) 124:e782–92. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-0333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. French B, Sycamore NJ, McGlashan HL, Blanchard CC, Holmes NP. Correction: ceiling effects in the movement assessment battery for children-2 (MABC-2) suggest that non-parametric scoring methods are required. PLoS ONE. (2021) 16:e0245674. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0202689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. French B, Sycamore NJ, McGlashan HL, Blanchard CC, Holmes NP. Ceiling effects in the movement assessment battery for children-2 (MABC-2) suggest that non-parametric scoring methods are required. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0198426. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0198426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Mecías-Calvo M, Lago-Fuentes C, Arufe-Giráldez V, Navarro-Patón R. Study of motor competence in 4–5-year-old preschool children: are there differences among public and private schools? Children. (2021) 8:340. doi: 10.3390/children8050340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Robinson LE, Wadsworth DD, Peoples CM. Correlates of school-day physical activity in preschool students. Res Q Exerc Sport. (2012) 83:20–6. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2012.10599821.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Robinson LE, Webster EK, Logan SW, Lucas WA, Barber LT. Teaching practices that promote motor skills in early childhood settings. Early Childhood Educ J. (2012) 40:79–86. doi: 10.1007/s10643-011-0496-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Honrubia-Montesinos C, Gil-Madrona P, Losada-Puente L. Motor development among spanish preschool children. Children. (2021) 8:41. doi: 10.3390/children8010041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Arufe Giráldez V. Cómo debe ser el trabajo de Educación Física en Educación Infantil? How should Physical Education work in Early Childhood Education be? Retos. (2020) 37:588–96. doi: 10.47197/retos.v37i37.74177

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Behan S, Belton S, Peers C, O'Connor NE, Issartel J. Moving well-being well: investigating the maturation of fundamental movement skill proficiency across sex in Irish children aged five to twelve. J Sports Sci. (2019) 37:2604–12. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2019.1651144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Koziol LF, Lutz JT. From movement to thought: the development of executive function. Appl Neuropsychol Child. (2013) 2:104–15. doi: 10.1080/2162013748386.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Sergeant J. The cognitive-energetic model: an empirical approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2000) 24:7–12. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00060-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. (2000) 71:44–56. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD, et al. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. (2000) 41:49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Piek JP, Dyck MJ, Francis M, Conwell A. Working memory, processing speed, and set-shifting in children with developmental coordination disorder and attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2007) 49:678–83. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00678.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Michel E, Roethlisberger M, Neuenschwander R, Roebers CM. Development of cognitive skills in children with motor coordination impairments at 12-month follow-up. Child Neuropsychol. (2011) 17:151–72. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2010.525501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Leisman G, Moustafa AA, Shafir T. Thinking, walking, talking: integratory motor and cognitive brain function. Front Pub Health. (2016) 4:94. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Ludyga S, Gerber M, Kamijo K, Brand S, Pühse U. The effects of a school-based exercise program on neurophysiological indices of working memory operations in adolescents. J Sci Med Sport. (2018) 21:833–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.01001.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Koutsandréou F, Wegner M, Niemann C, Budde H. Effects of motor versus cardiovascular exercise training on children's working memory. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2016) 48:1144–52. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Hsieh SS, Lin CC, Chang YK, Huang CJ, Hung TM, City T. Effects of childhood gymnastics program on spatial working memory. Med Sci Sports Exer. (2017) 49:2537–4. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Ludyga S, Mücke M, Kamijo K, Andrä C, Pühse U, Gerber M, et al. The role of motor competences in predicting working memory maintenance and preparatory processing. Child Dev. (2020) 91:799–813. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Garcia Fernandez DA, Chávez Valenzuela ME, Cruz Chávez C, Guedea Delgado JC, Velázquez Saucedo G, Zubiaur González M, et al. (2018). Impacto de un programa de actividad motriz con funciones ejecutivas fortaleciendo el desarrollo integral del niño. Sport Sci J. (2018) 4:37–58. doi: 10.17979/sportis.4.1.2060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: physical activity, exercise intervention, working memory, motor competence, preschool children

Citation: Zhang J-Y, Shen Q-Q, Wang D-L, Hou J-M, Xia T, Qiu S, Wang X-Y, Zhou S-B, Yang W-W, Heng S-Y, Lu C-C, Cui L and Yin H-C (2022) Physical activity intervention promotes working memory and motor competence in preschool children. Front. Public Health 10:984887. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.984887

Received: 02 July 2022; Accepted: 31 August 2022;
Published: 26 September 2022.

Edited by:

Xia Jiang, Karolinska Institutet (KI), Sweden

Reviewed by:

Jianxiu Liu, Tsinghua University, China
Víctor Arufe Giráldez, University of A Coruña, Spain
Asgeir Mamen, Kristiania University College, Norway
Pay Preedy, Ohio Dominican University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Zhang, Shen, Wang, Hou, Xia, Qiu, Wang, Zhou, Yang, Heng, Lu, Cui and Yin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lei Cui, cuilei@bnu.edu.cn; Heng-Chan Yin, yinhengchan@bnu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.