Your new experience awaits. Try the new design now and help us make it even better

EDITORIAL article

Front. Educ.

Sec. Teacher Education

Volume 10 - 2025 | doi: 10.3389/feduc.2025.1720577

This article is part of the Research TopicThe Important Role of the Early School Years for Reading, Writing and Math Development: Assessment and Intervention at School EntryView all 15 articles

Editorial: The Important Role of the Early School Years for Reading, Writing, and Math Development: Assessment and Intervention at School Entry

Provisionally accepted
  • 1University of Education Upper Austria, Linz, Austria
  • 2Johannes Kepler Universitat Linz, Linz, Austria
  • 3Universitat Graz, Graz, Austria
  • 4Bergische Universitat Wuppertal, Wuppertal, Germany

The final, formatted version of the article will be published soon.

Children start formal schooling in reading, writing, and mathematics with substantially varying learning prerequisites (Davis-Kean et al., 2022;Merrell, 2023). While some children barely know any letters or numbers, others can already read, write, and calculate with great fluency. Lack of letter or number knowledge at entry does not mean that children lack foundational experiences with literacy and numeracy. They bring rich, practice-based resources, including ways of engaging with language, quantity, and space that are often undervalued in school (Moll et al., 1992;Nunes & Bryant, 2015;Rosa & Orey, 2016). Children with less favorable learning conditions are at higher risk for disadvantageous learning outcomes and lack educational success (Davis-Kean, 2022;Sparks et al., 2014;Stanley et al., 2017). As early literacy and numeracy, as well as their precursors, are associated with family background variables, these unequal starting conditions set the stage for later social and ethniccultural educational disparities (e.g., Bradley & Corwyn, 2002;Davis-Kean, 2022;Skopek & Passaretta, 2021). Addressing these differences between children requires instruction that builds on sound diagnostic information on their learning prerequisites. The availability of objective, reliable, and valid information regarding students' learning prerequisites should enable teachers to implement targeted interventions and adapt their instruction to meet the individual needs of young learners (Mandinach, 2012). This should result in optimal learning support, compensate for unequal starting conditions, and counteract educational inequalities. These assumptions correspond to the concept of data-based decision making (DBDM; Mandinach, 2012;Souvignier & Förster, in press). In short, DBDM is a process that covers several central steps, starting with the assessment of data on students' learning prerequisites, then analyzing data to derive instructional interventions, and finally evaluating the interventions' effects. The 14 articles of this Research Topic address aspects of the DBDM process around the time of school entry (from the last preschool year to grade 2), primarily focusing on assessment and assessmentbased interventions.Generally, assessments must meet standard quality criteria (objectivity, reliability, and validity) to be effectively used for instructional adaptations. In this context, the question arises as to what aspects of learning prerequisites should be assessed to optimize the prediction of future learning outcomes.Moreover, assessments must be economical so that they can be used in the classroom context. Therefore, technology-enhanced assessments that facilitate test administration, scoring, and reporting of results are increasingly being developed. Furthermore, the simple provision of assessment data may not be sufficient to ensure that teachers can derive instructional interventions with manageable effort (Hebecker et al., 2022). Finally, interventions should be effective. This editorial summarizes the findings of the articles in this Research Topic along the DBDM steps: (1) providing basic knowledge for the development of assessments, (2) providing evidence that assessments are sound, (3) providing evidence on how to support teachers in interpreting and using data for instruction, and (4) providing evidence on effective interventions. Unless otherwise justified by content, we present the studies in each of the following sections according to the domains of reading, writing, and mathematics. The following articles lay the conceptual groundwork for the design of assessments by examining which early skills predict later outcomes and how these indicators are associated with sociodemographic factors.In their article "Morphological awareness predicts reading comprehension in first-grade students", Sparks and Metsala focus on a specific predictor of reading comprehension. Their study of Englishspeaking children from Canada contributes to the understanding of the role of morphological awareness in predicting reading success. They investigated whether morphological awareness uniquely contributes to the prediction of reading after controlling for oral language skills. Their findings indicate that morphological awareness assessed in grade 1 accounts for unique variance in reading comprehension in grade 2.Sigmund and colleagues investigate "Early cognitive predictors of spelling and reading in Germanspeaking children". Their article contributes to the understanding of early spelling and reading skills predictors by explicitly investigating whether predictors assessed before school entry show differential predictive validity for reading and spelling skills in grade 1. Specifically, their results based on a German sample underscore the importance of phonological processing, intelligence, and letter knowledge for predicting grade 1 spelling after accounting for preschool spelling and grade 1 reading skills. In contrast, only rapid automatized naming (RAN) emerged as a significant predictor of reading once preschool reading and grade 1 spelling were controlled for.The article by Banfi and colleagues titled "Longitudinal predictors of reading and arithmetic at different attainment levels" stands out by addressing both reading and mathematics, examining not only children at risk for academic failure but also high achievers. Their study advances the understanding of the cognitive foundations underlying the association between reading and arithmetic fluency. Drawing on longitudinal data of over 300 students in Grades 1, 2, and 3 from the UK and Austria, the authors identify both domain-general predictors (e.g., nonverbal IQ) and domain-specific predictors (e.g., phonological awareness for reading and magnitude processing for arithmetic). Notably, most predictors proved to be equally important for high and low achievers.Concluding this section, the study by Schöfl, Weber, and colleagues titled "Language abilities and phonological information processing mediate the association of spelling with bilingualism and socioeconomic status" is placed last because it extends the focus to the socioeconomic and migration-related factors underlying differences in spelling achievement and examines how these effects are mediated by language abilities and phonological information processing. Using an Austrian sample, the authors found that language skills assessed at the very beginning of grade 1 mediate the association between migration-related bilingual status and spelling skills in grade 2, and that the link between socioeconomic status and spelling is partly mediated by language skills and phonological information processing. These findings highlight the importance of promoting language skills and phonological information processing in preschool and at the beginning of primary school to reduce social and ethnic-cultural disparities. The following articles examine the reliability and validity of assessments, as well as the potential effects of test administration mode, to determine whether the studied instruments provide consistent and accurate measures on which instructional decisions can be based. Schöfl, Steinmair, and colleagues report in their article "Using an app-based screening tool to predict deficits in written word spelling at school entry" results on the validity and diagnostic accuracy of an efficient and cost-free screening tool for identifying spelling problems in community school settings.They used an Austrian sample to examine the predictive validity of different precursors of word spelling, measured at school entry, for predicting spelling difficulties at the end of grade 2. Their results indicate that a screening consisting of the two measures letter knowledge (an aspect of phonological information processing) and sentence repetition (a measure of grammatical knowledge) has an acceptable diagnostic accuracy.Chatzaki and colleagues explore the assessment of early mathematical competencies through a playful, game-based approach in their article "Exploring the potential of a game-based preschool assessment of mathematical competencies using a linear board game called the House of Numbers".Drawing on data from a sample of German preschoolers, they examine the reliability and validity of the game-based test. Their results suggest that the House of Numbers is a valid and reliable tool for assessing the mathematical abilities of preschoolers. In addition, they found that the game-based test is an appropriate screening tool with high diagnostic validity for identifying children at risk of low math performance.Cruz and colleagues also focus on assessing mathematics skills in the preschool years. In their article "Assessment of math abilities before school entry: a tool development", using a Portuguese sample, the authors provide support for the reliability and validity of a developed screening tool that covers several precursors of mathematics skills assessed during the last year before school entry. They reported high internal consistency of the screening measures and found support for the validity of the screening for predicting grade 1 mathematics achievement.While these studies validate or develop new assessment instruments for early literacy and numeracy, Seifert and colleagues focus on a methodological aspect that cuts across assessment context, namely, the potential influence of test administration mode on assessment outcomes. In their article "Unveiling mode effects in grade 1 vocabulary assessment: the intriguing influence of test mode", the authors investigate whether the digital administration of a well-established paper-and-pencil test of receptive vocabulary produces comparable results to the traditional format. In their study with Austrian grade 1 students, they found positive effects of the digital mode, which highlights the need to consider the test mode in the field application of the test and emphasizes the need for further investigations to better understand mode effects. They conclude that ensuring comparability across paper and digital forms will require cross-mode score linking and measurement invariance testing. The following articles address questions regarding how assessment results can be reported and how they can be effectively aligned with instruction to support data-based decision-making in education.Extending beyond the validation of assessments, the contributions by Stuhr and colleagues and Ufer and colleagues also explore how assessment outcomes might be translated into instructional guidance. In their article "Exploration of latent early literacy profiles in German kindergarten children using a newly developed app", Stuhr et al. use app-assessed data from a German preschool sample to identify latent literacy profiles based on precursors of reading, such as word awareness, phonological awareness, and alphabet knowledge, and suggest profile-specific recommendations on dialogic book reading. This should facilitate the transformation from assessment results to instructional activities.They emphasize the need for experimental and implementation-focused studies to evaluate the effectiveness of the tailored recommendations.In their article, "I have three more than you, you have three less than me? Levels of flexibility in dealing with additive situations", Ufer et al. assess the flexibility in dealing with additive situations in a sample of second and third-grade German students and develop a model distinguishing discrete levels of flexibility. The authors discuss that assigning students to levels may help teachers make use of assessment results, especially when these are linked to suggestions for instructional adaptations. Powell and colleagues also focus on an intervention to promote mathematics skills, but address the preschool age. In their article "Investigation of the initial feasibility of extended mathematics readalouds used by kindergarten teachers", they investigate the feasibility and effects of a mathematicsfocused read-aloud program in a US sample. They found no significant effect of the training, but they found evidence that implementation fidelity is significantly associated with outcomes. This highlights the need for high program fidelity when specific programs are used within instructional interventions to achieve desired effects. While the preceding studies focus on evaluating the effects of specific interventions, Lembke and colleagues address a central prerequisite for their sustainable impact in practice, namely, implementation fidelity. In their article "Data-based individualization in early writing: the importance and measurement of implementation fidelity", they focus on implementing a data-based individualization approach to promote early writing. Using a US sample, the authors monitor the implementation of DBDM, describe challenges for teachers in implementing DBDM in their classroom, and provide evidence on how to improve implementation fidelity to ensure that evidence-based practices can take effect in everyday school life. A close examination of the articles in this Research Topic reveals three messages that are largely aligned. Firstly, it is essential to assess early and precisely. Brief, reliable screeners of phonological, letter, language, and early numerical competencies can forecast later literacy and numeracy, and identify both risk and high attainment. Secondly, the data must be made actionable in an instructional context. While technological advancements and profiling tools can enhance the feasibility of this process, it is imperative to obtain explicit validation for the translation of results into concrete teaching methodologies. Thirdly, alignment of assessment and intervention within sustained data-based decision-making cycles has been demonstrated to engender promising long-term and transfer effects.Implementing well-specified instructional responses with high fidelity and prioritizing language and phonological appears to support for socio-demographically disadvantaged learners has been shown to engender equity gains. Complementarily, assessment and intervention should be guided by an assetsbased stance that values children's existing literacies and everyday numerical-spatial competencies, ensuring that data use supports responsive instruction rather than reproducing deficit framings (Moll et al., 1992;Nunes & Bryant, 2015).To facilitate the implementation of this approach, we advocate integrating low-stakes screening procedures at school entry into the regular curriculum, in accordance with the principles of the Multi-Tiered System of Supports (MTSS) and Response to Intervention (RTI) frameworks (Fuchs & Fuchs, 2006;McIntosh & Goodman, 2016). MTSS/RTI represents a tiered model of early identification and support that aligns universal screening, progress monitoring, and data-based instructional adaptations.These screening procedures should be complemented by the implementation of DBDM routines that follow the same tiered principles of the MTSS/RTI framework and are supported by professional learning opportunities focusing on the interpretation and use of assessment data.In the context of research, three priorities emerge: firstly, the experimental evaluation of profile-based recommendations; secondly, the establishment of cross-mode equivalence in assessments; and thirdly, the design of studies that explicitly target implementation fidelity and equity impacts. Overall, the reviewed studies collectively illustrate that there is a clear and direct path from early assessment to measurable learning gains in the early school years, provided that the loop from measurement to instruction is effectively closed.

Keywords: data-based decision making (DBDM), early literacy, Early numeracy, screening, implementation fidelity, assessment mode effects, Equity, Kindergarten

Received: 08 Oct 2025; Accepted: 14 Oct 2025.

Copyright: © 2025 Schöfl, Weber, Seifert and Förster. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

* Correspondence: Christoph Weber, christoph.weber@ph-ooe.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.