ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 13 June 2025

Sec. Children and Health

Volume 13 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2025.1539922

This article is part of the Research TopicThe Impact of Climate and Environmental Change on Epigenetics and Pediatric HealthView all 5 articles

Education and urbanization improves cognitive function affected by altitude of adolescents: a cross-section study in Tibet, China

Yangzong Ciren&#x;Yangzong Ciren1Jianhong Gong&#x;Jianhong Gong2Furong Li&#x;Furong Li1Xiaofeng Gong&#x;Xiaofeng Gong1Dunzhu DanzengDunzhu Danzeng1Yanping NingYanping Ning3Tianheng WangTianheng Wang4Xiaobing TianXiaobing Tian5Quzha Silang
Quzha Silang1*
  • 1Medical College, Tibet University, Lhasa, China
  • 2School of Medicine, Shanxi Datong University, Datong, Shanxi, China
  • 3The People’s Hospital of Tibet Autonomous Region, Lhasa, China
  • 4Health Service Center of Jiri Community of Lhasa City, Lhasa, China
  • 5School of Public Health, North Sichuan Medical College, Nanchong, Sichuan, China

Background: The aim of the study was to examine the cognitive function of Tibetan adolescents at different altitudes and evaluate the influence of education and urbanization on child’s cognition at high altitudes.

Methods: A cross-sectional study was conducted between October 2015 and September 2016 in four counties in Tibet. The study population included 324 adolescents living at an altitude of 2,800 m, including 227 who grew up at altitudes of 4,300 m, and 732 adolescents attending three high-altitude boarding schools (one at 4,300 m and two at 4,500 m), including 119 who grew up at an altitude of 2,800 m. Fluid reasoning was assessed using Standard Progressive Matrices (SPMs). The background information was collected using self-administered questionnaires.

Results: A multilevel linear regression model was used to determine the effects of altitude, education, and urbanization on fluid reasoning. In the fixed-effects model, the average SPM score of adolescents from low altitudes who attended kindergarten was 40.41. Scores of students who moved from high to low altitudes, came from high altitudes, and moved from low to high altitudes were 2.66, 4.71, and 6.70 points lower, respectively, than those of students from low altitudes, and 2.50 points lower in students who had not attended kindergarten than those who had. In the random-effects model, the scores of those who only went to first grade were 2.93 points lower. Students from County 3 had average scores of 1.89 higher than did students from the other three counties.

Conclusion: High altitudes may negatively affect the cognition of Tibetan adolescents. This effect was reduced by moving to a lower altitude, increasing the number of years of formal education, attending kindergarten, and living in a more urbanized location. Even at high altitude, moving to higher altitudes should be avoided.

1 Introduction

High altitude is defined as the level at which the human oxygenated hemoglobin concentration falls below 90%, corresponding to an altitude of at least 2,500 m. Notably, more than 81.6 million people live at high altitudes (1). Children living at high altitudes are at risk for impaired cognitive development due to intermittent or relatively chronic hypoxia (2).

Childhood is an important and sensitive period for cognitive development (3). Studies of native Highlander children from South America and migrant children from Europe have highlighted an association between high residential altitude and impaired cognitive function and neurodevelopment (48). However, the mechanism by which chronic hypoxia at high altitudes affects cognition remains unclear, but it may be related to the insufficiency in cerebral blood flow and the toxicity of reactive oxygen species generated due to chronic hypoxia (911). Moreover, education, including school and pre-school attendance, has consistently been shown to have positive effects on a child’s cognitive development (12). Children living in more urbanized areas may have more opportunities to get a high-quality education as well as greater access to modern technology. Psychosocial stimuli, such as computer use and high-quality education, may have beneficial effects on a child’s cognitive development.

Chinese Tibetans have lived on the Qinghai-Tibet Plateau for thousands of years, and their adaptation to this low-oxygen environment has reached a steady state. The negative impact of high altitude on the growth of Tibetan children and adolescents has also been consistently confirmed in previous studies (1316). As far as we know, few study to date has investigated the association between altitude and cognitive development among native Tibetan adolescents. In the past two decades, Tibet has witnessed a huge development in elementary education and urbanization. However, to our knowledge, no studies have examined whether these changes have beneficial effects on children’s cognitive development.

The primary objective of this study was to investigate the cognitive function of Tibetan adolescents residing at different altitudes. The secondary objective of this study was to evaluate the influence of education, represented by grade, pre-school attendance, and urbanization indicated by the county where the school is located, on child cognition at high altitudes.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Study design and setting

This cross-sectional study was conducted between October 2015 and September 2016 in four counties of Tibet. Primary schools in Tibet are dispersed across rural areas, while middle schools are situated in towns with only one school per county. Stratified random cluster sampling was used to select participants using an altitude cutoff point of 2,800 meters. Three counties out of 28 located at altitudes of ≥4,300 m and one out of 13 counties at altitudes ≤2,800 m were, respectively, selected. In each county, only one town where the middle school is situated was chosen as the study setting. The detailed information is presented in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Background characteristics of the four counties.

2.2 Sample size

The sample size was calculated using the formula for comparing two means (17), with a significance level set at 0.0083 (adjusted from 0.05 due to multiple comparisons among the four groups, as 0.0083 = 0.05/6, to control the type I error rate). Other parameters used in the sample size calculation included a power of 80%, a variance in cognitive scores of 64, a mean difference in cognitive scores between adolescents at low and high altitudes of 5, and a design effect of 1.7 (All parameters were selected in accordance with the findings of our preliminary study). Using these parameters, the minimum sample size required for each group was determined to be 94. Therefore, the total sample size required for the four groups was 376.

2.3 Study participants

Attendees at the low-altitude school included 356 adolescents, including 105 in the (local) low-altitude group and 251 in the residing at high altitude but studying at low altitude group (indicated as “high to low” group afterward). Of these, 97 (92.4%) and 227 (90.4%) consented to participate in the study, respectively, resulting in a response rate of 91.0%.

A total of 1,709 students attended the high-altitude school in County 2. However, none of the third-grade students were available for the study because they needed to prepare for graduation examinations. To achieve a balance in grades with the low-altitude school, four of the 15 first-grade classes and three of the 12 s-grade classes were randomly selected. These seven classes included 313 students, of whom 306 (97.8%) consented to participate, all having grown up at high altitudes.

A total of 1,726 students attended the high-altitude school in County 3, but only second-grade students were available at the time of this study because third-grade students needed to prepare for the graduation examination, and first-grade students had to take the examination. To achieve a balance in grades with the school from County 2, four of the 14 s-grade classes were randomly selected. Of the 182 students in these four classes, 177 (97.3%) consented to participate; two students were excluded because of missing data, and all of these students grew up at high altitudes.

In total, 786 students attended a high-altitude middle school in County 4. To achieve a balance in grades with the school in County 2, two of the seven first-grade classes, two of the six second-grade classes, and two of the six third-grade classes were randomly selected. Of the 262 students in these classes, 255 (97.3%) consented to participate. However, four students were excluded due to missing data. Consequently, there were 132 in the local low-altitude group and 119 in the group residing at low altitude but studying at high altitude, hereafter referred to as the “low to high” group. Thus, 1,062 students participated in the study, and six were excluded owing to missing data. None of the participants met the exclusion criteria.

Chinese Tibetan students aged below 18 years who were studying in a research setting were included in this study. Students with significant physiological disabilities or a history of neurological or psychiatric illness were excluded from the study.

This study was approved by the local Ethics Committee of Tibet University. The parents of all the enrolled students provided written informed consent.

2.4 Instrument and data collection

2.4.1 Background characteristics

Data regarding the students’ background characteristics were collected using a self-administered questionnaire. The main variables included school, grade, age, sex, birth order, history of kindergarten attendance, education level of the students’ parents, marital status of the parents, monthly household income, and number of children in the family.

2.4.2 Cognitive measurement

Human cognition has several components. Fluid reasoning refers to the complex cognitive capacity to reason and solve novel problems independent of prior knowledge. It has been shown to be an important predictor of academic achievement in children (12, 18). Fluid reasoning in this study was measured using the Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM), a feasible instrument for use in surveys of a relatively large sample.

The SPM is language-free and suitable for children aged ≥6 years. It consists of visually presented geometric analog-like problems. A matrix of geometric figures is presented with one missing entry, and the correct missing entry must be selected from a set of alternatives. There were five sets (A–E) consisting of 12 items each, with a maximum score of 60. The degree of item difficulty increased from Set A to Set E. Before administering the SPM, students were encouraged to complete the instrument to the best of their abilities. Researchers explained the importance of the study and its future benefits to Tibetan children, but informed the students that their scores would not affect their school performance.

The nature of the SPM and instructions for its completion were explained, and the first two items were solved. The SPM was then administered to the students in each class, with each class consisting of approximately 40 students under the guidance of well-trained teachers. A maximum time period of 60 min was allowed. The completed answer sheets were then delivered to the study center. Each student’s score was subsequently confidentially divulged.

The Chinese language questionnaire, which included questions about social and demographic characteristics, including educational background, was translated into Tibetan by an expert in Tibetology working at the Tibet Academy of Social Sciences and back-translated by another expert in Tibetology at the same institution to validate the translation. Both experts were fluent in Chinese and Tibetan.

2.5 Statistical analysis

Fluid reasoning (SPM) scores were reported as means ± standard deviations and compared using an independent sample t-test or ANOVA across groups. Cross-tabulation was performed across the four altitude groups and other categorical variables, with associations determined using chi-square tests. Variables with a significance level < 0.10 in the univariate analysis were tested in the multivariate analysis.

A multilevel linear regression model was employed to examine the effects of altitude on the cognition of Tibetan adolescents, considering grade as the second-level factor and school as the third-level factor. The validity of this model was assessed. The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC), representing the degree of correlation among data within a cluster, was calculated using a model with an intercept and a level two factor, or both level two and level three factors. The ICC values ranged from 0 to 1, with 0 indicating no cluster effect and values greater than 0 confirming the suitability of the model for multilevel regression analysis. After demonstrating the validity of the multilevel linear regression model, multivariate analysis was conducted using a linear regression model that included only the intercept. The independent variable “altitude” was then added to the model. Next, the first-level variable “grade” and second-level variable “school” with random intercept and random slope were added to the model in a stepwise manner. Finally, covariates were individually added to the model. When one model was a special case of another, the best model was chosen by comparing the log-likelihoods of these two models using a log-likelihood ratio test. If the test results are not statistically significant, a more complicated model may not be necessary. The assumption of normality and homoscedasticity of random effects was checked. All statistical analyses were performed using R 4.0.3, with p < 0.05 regarded as statistically significant.

3 Results

3.1 Background characteristics of participants

The background characteristics of the participants in each altitude group, as well as those of their parents and families, are presented in Table 2. There were no significant differences in students’ gender or the marital status of their parents across the four groups. However, adolescents in the high-altitude group were generally older. Adolescents in the low-altitude group were more likely to have attended kindergarten, have a lower birth order, have parents with higher levels of education, and come from households with fewer children and higher total monthly income. Participants who moved from low to high altitudes had higher birth orders, parents with lower levels of education, and came from households with more children.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Background characteristics of the adolescents in the four altitude groups.

3.2 Altitude and cognitive function

The average fluid reasoning (SPM) score was the highest in the low-altitude group and lowest in the high-altitude group (Table 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Average fluid intelligence (Standard Progressive Matrices) scores of the adolescents in the four altitude groups.

3.3 Univariate analysis of factors affecting cognitive function

Table 4 shows the results of the univariate analysis of the factors affecting the participants’ fluid reasoning (SPM) scores. These scores were not significantly affected by the sex, age, or marital status of the participants. Factors significantly associated with higher fluid reasoning (SPM) scores included lower birth order, a history of attending kindergarten, parents with an educational level of middle school and above, families with one child, and monthly family income ≥5,000 RMB.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Univariate analysis of factors affecting fluid reasoning (Standard Progressive Matrices) scores of the participants.

3.4 Validity of the multilevel linear regression model

The multilevel linear regression model with intercept and grade as second-level factors yielded an ICC of 0.098, indicating that this model was appropriate for the present study. The previous model, which included school as a third-level factor, yielded an ICC of 0.126, suggesting that multilevel linear regression with grade as the second-level variable and school as the third-level variable was also valid for the present study.

3.5 Variables affecting child cognitive development

After the model refinement process, in the fixed-effects part of the final model (Table 5), the intercept was 40.41, which corresponds to the average fluid reasoning (SPM) score of all adolescents at low altitudes who attended kindergarten. The coefficient estimate of altitude in students who moved from high to low was −2.66, indicating that the average fluid reasoning (SPM) score was 2.66 points lower when compared with that of students in the low-altitude group. The coefficients of altitude in students at high altitude and in those who moved from low to high altitude were −4.71 and −6.70, respectively, indicating that the average scores of fluid reasoning (SPM) were 4.71 and 6.70 points lower in these students than in those in the low-altitude group. The average fluid reasoning (SPM) scores were 2.50 points lower in students who had not attended kindergarten than in those who had attended kindergarten. The intercept and slopes of altitude, as well as the history of attending kindergarten, were statistically significant (p < 0.05).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Multilevel linear regression model predicting Standard Progressive Matrices SPM scores of Tibetan adolescents.

Unlike conventional linear regression models that include only intercepts at the individual level, the three-level random intercept model incorporates intercepts at three levels: individual, grade, and school. This is because intercepts can vary across grades and schools. The assessment of the random effects in the current final model showed that the coefficient for the first grade was −2.93, representing the intercept at the grade level for the first grade. Consequently, students in the first grade at all schools, who came from low altitudes and had attended kindergarten, had an average fluid reasoning (SPM) score of 37.48 (40.41–2.93). However, this coefficient was not statistically significant for students in the second and third grades, as the 95% confidence intervals for the intercepts of these grades included zero. Thus, students in the second and third grades at all schools, who came from low altitudes and attended kindergarten, had an average fluid reasoning (SPM) score of 40.41.

The coefficient for school from County 3 is 1.89, indicating that the intercept at the school level for this school is 1.89. Students in all grades in County 3, who came from low altitudes and had a history of preschool attendance, therefore, had an average SPM score of 42.3 (40.41 + 1.89). However, for students from schools in the first, second, and fourth counties, this coefficient was not statistically significant, as the 95% confidence interval for their intercepts included zero.

An example of the cumulative effects of grade and school is illustrated by assessing students in the first grade from County 3 who came from low altitudes and had a history of preschool attendance. The intercept for these students was 39.37 (40.41–2.93 + 1.89), indicating that their average SPM score was 39.37.

4 Discussion

The average SPM score was the highest in the low-altitude group, followed by the high-to-low-altitude, high-altitude, and low-to-high-altitude groups. In addition to altitude, kindergarten attendance affected fluid reasoning (SPM) scores as a covariate. Both grade and school influenced participants’ fluid reasoning (SPM) scores as high-level factors. Those who attended only first grade had lower average fluid reasoning (SPM) scores, whereas students from County 3 had higher average fluid reasoning (SPM) scores.

The results of this study showed that cognitive function was compromised in Tibetan adolescents living at high altitudes, with fluid reasoning (SPM) scores inversely correlating with altitude. Although the mechanism by which chronic hypoxia at high altitudes affects cognition remains unclear, changes in cognitive function caused by chronic hypoxia at high altitudes may be related to insufficiency in cerebral blood flow induced by cerebral blood flow automatic regulation dysfunction (9, 10). The effect of hypoxia on cognitive function might also be associated with the toxicity of reactive oxygen radicals produced by the nervous system in response to chronic hypoxia (11). Furthermore, our finding that fluid reasoning (SPM) scores were lowest in students who moved from low to high altitude may suggest that children at high altitudes should not be relocated to even higher altitudes, thereby preventing more severe hypoxia at these higher altitudes. After excluding other influencing factors, the average fluid reasoning (SPM) score of students who moved from high to low altitude was found to lie between the scores of the low- and high-altitude groups, suggesting that the effects of high altitude–related hypoxia on cognitive function might be reversible. The compromised cognitive function of children living at high altitudes could be improved by relocating them to lower altitudes.

The number of years of formal education completed by individuals has been found to correlate positively with their cognitive function (19). Similarly, the current study found that average fluid reasoning (SPM) scores were lower in first-grade students than in those in second and third grades, indicating that a shorter duration of education might negatively affect children’s cognition. Individuals with more years of schooling may have processed cognitive tasks in a more efficient manner (20). Average fluid reasoning (SPM) scores tended to increase from second to third grade, but this trend was not statistically significant. As this study included only middle school students, the effects of formal education duration on children’s cognition were restricted. Additional studies that include participants with a wider range of years of schooling are required.

Pre-school attendance has been found to improve motor, cognitive, and psychosocial development and reduce the need for special education in primary schools (21). Similarly, the present study found that fluid reasoning (SPM) scores were higher in students who had attended kindergarten. Activities in kindergarten are likely to stimulate motor activity, cognitive development, and pro-social behaviors, with years spent in kindergarten being associated with reduced rates of motor, cognitive, and psychosocial impairments (22). However, the present study did not evaluate the years spent in kindergarten, indicating a need for studies assessing the effects of the duration of preschool attendance on child cognitive development at high altitudes. These findings suggest that the negative effects of high altitude on children’s cognitive development can be mitigated by encouraging longer formal education, including kindergarten attendance.

As a high-level factor, school also influenced the fluid reasoning (SPM) scores, with students from County 3 having a higher average score than those from other counties. Differences among schools might not only reflect differences in education quality but also differences among counties. County 3, part of Lhasa, the capital city of the Tibet Autonomous Region, is more urbanized than the other three counties included in this study. Although County 2 is similar to County 3 in terms of distance to Lhasa, a high mountain between County 2 and Lhasa makes County 3 more urbanized than County 2. The increased availability of resources, such as high-quality education and psychosocial stimuli, in more urbanized locations may improve children’s cognitive development. Policies that promote the quality of education and programs that enrich psychosocial stimuli in rural areas may help overcome compromised child cognitive development in rural high-altitude locations.

Although children from low-income families tend to have lower general cognitive abilities than those from higher-income families (23), the present study found that family income did not affect child cognition. Poverty at an early age and later during middle childhood was found to equally affect cognition in children aged 7–14 years (23). The present study was cross-sectional in design and did not investigate household income during early childhood. However, poverty may contribute directly to poor childhood outcomes through poor housing conditions, lack of access to health care, poor nutrition, and an inability to afford tuition costs (24). Because of compulsory education, as well as government spending on the education of rural students in Tibet, all participants received free education and accommodation, regardless of income, unless their parents were government employees. This may have contributed to the lack of correlation between average fluid reasoning (SPM) scores and monthly family income.

Parental educational levels represent personal resources and problem-solving abilities of parents (24). Parental, especially maternal, educational levels have been found to affect child cognition (25). The present study found that parents’ education levels were unrelated to the average fluid reasoning (SPM) scores of their children, perhaps because the educational levels of most parents of these students were lower than middle school, making it harder to detect the effect of parental educational level on child cognition.

Children’s nutritional status may serve as a confounding factor influencing cognitive development. Therefore, this was carefully considered during both the design and data collection stages of our study. However, as previously mentioned, all participants except those whose parents were government employees receive free education and accommodation. Based on our observations and feedback from students and teachers, the meals provided by the school were nutritionally balanced. If malnutrition still occurred in some children, it could potentially be attributed to hypoxia at high altitudes, as high altitudes may impair children’s ability to absorb and utilize nutrients from food (26). Consequently, controlling for nutritional status might partially mitigate the effects of altitude on cognitive development. Thus, nutritional status was not controlled for in this study.

4.1 Generalizability of the study results

Due to strict adherence to the random sampling procedure and a sufficient sample size, the sample of the current study was representative. Therefore, the results of the current study can be generalized to adolescents in the Tibet Autonomous Region.

4.2 Limitations of this study

Because this study had a cross-sectional design, we could not assess causal relationships because the sequence of occurrence of each factor could not be determined. In addition, the effects of the quality of education, duration of formal education, and years of pre-schooling on the cognitive development of children at high altitudes have not been thoroughly assessed.

5 Conclusion

High altitudes may have a negative effect on the cognition of Tibetan adolescents. This effect may be reduced by moving to a lower altitude, increasing the number of years of formal education, attending kindergarten, or living in a more urbanized location. Individuals living at high altitudes should avoid moving to even higher altitudes.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: all data can be requested from corresponding author if someone uses them for related researches.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Life Science and Medical Ethics Review Committee, Tibet University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

YC: Writing – original draft, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software. JG: Data curation, Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. FL: Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. XG: Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. DD: Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – review & editing. YN: Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. TW: Formal analysis, Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. XT: Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. QS: Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grand number 81760598) and Natural Science Foundation of Lhasa (grand number LSKJ202305).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Tremblay, JC, and Ainslie, PN. Global and country-level estimates of human population at high altitude. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2021) 118:e2102463118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2102463118

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Niermeyer, S, Andrade Mollinedo, P, and Huicho, L. Child health and living at high altitude. Arch Dis Child. (2009) 94:806–11. doi: 10.1136/adc.2008.141838

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bidzan-Bluma, I, and Lipowska, M. Physical activity and cognitive functioning of children: a systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2018) 15:800. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15040800

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hogan, AM, Virues-Ortega, J, Botti, AB, Bucks, R, Holloway, JW, Rose-Zerilli, MJ, et al. Development of aptitude at altitude. Dev Sci. (2010) 13:533–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00909.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Hill, CM, Dimitriou, D, Baya, A, Webster, R, Gavlak-Dingle, J, Lesperance, V, et al. Cognitive performance in high-altitude Andean residents compared with low-altitude populations: from childhood to older age. Neuropsychology. (2014) 28:752–60. doi: 10.1037/neu0000065

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Virués-Ortega, J, Bucks, R, Kirkham, FJ, Baldeweg, T, Baya-Botti, A, Hogan, AM, et al. Changing patterns of neuropsychological functioning in children living at high altitude above and below 4000 m: a report from the Bolivian children living at altitude (BoCLA) study. Dev Sci. (2011) 14:1185–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01064.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Rimoldi, SF, Rexhaj, E, Duplain, H, Urben, S, Billieux, J, Allemann, Y, et al. Acute and chronic altitude-induced cognitive dysfunction in children and adolescents. J Pediatr. (2016) 169:238–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.10.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Wehby, GL. Living on higher ground reduces child neurodevelopment—evidence from South America. J Pediatr. (2013) 162:606–611.e1. e601. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.09.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Rudziński, W, Swiat, M, Tomaszewski, M, and Krejza, J. Cerebral hemodynamics and investigations of cerebral blood flow regulation. Nucl Med Rev Cent East Eur. (2007) 10:29–42.

Google Scholar

10. Virués-Ortega, J, Garrido, E, Javierre, C, and Kloezeman, KC. Human behaviour and development under high-altitude conditions. Dev Sci. (2006) 9:400–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00505.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kirkham, FJ, and Datta, AK. Hypoxic adaptation during development: relation to pattern of neurological presentation and cognitive disability. Dev Sci. (2006) 9:411–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00507.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Stevenson, HW, Parker, T, Wilkinson, A, Bonnevaux, B, and Gonzalez, M. Schooling, environment, and cognitive development: a cross-cultural study. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. (1978) 43:1–92. doi: 10.2307/1166040

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Dang, S, Yan, H, Yamamoto, S, Wang, X, and Zeng, L. Poor nutritional status of younger Tibetan children living at high altitudes. Eur J Clin Nutr. (2004) 58:938–46. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601915

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Argnani, L, Cogo, A, and Gualdi-Russo, E. Growth and nutritional status of Tibetan children at high altitude. Coll Antropol. (2008) 32:807–12.

Google Scholar

15. Bianba, BS, Berntsen, S, Andersen, LB, Stigum, H, Ouzhuluobu, A, Nafstad, P, et al. Exercise capacity and selected physiological factors by ancestry and residential altitude: cross-sectional studies of 9-10-year-old children in Tibet. High Alt Med Biol. (2014) 15:162–9. doi: 10.1089/ham.2013.1084

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Bianba, B, Yangzong, Y, Gonggalanzi, G, Berntsen, S, Andersen, LB, Stigum, H, et al. Anthropometric measures of 9- to 10-year-old native Tibetan children living at 3700 and 4300 m above sea level and Han Chinese living at 3700 m. Medicine. (2015) 94:e1516. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000001516

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. W, J, X, Z, W, Q, and K, D. Clinical epidemiology: Design, measurement and evaluation of clinical research. 3rd ed. Shanghai: Shanghai Science and Technology Press (2009). 161 p.

Google Scholar

18. Colom, R, Escorial, S, Shih, PC, and Privado, J. Fluid intelligence, memory span, and temperament difficulties predict academic performance of young adolescents. Pers Individ Dif. (2007) 42:1503–14. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.10.023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Lövdén, M, Fratiglioni, L, Glymour, MM, Lindenberger, U, and Tucker-Drob, EM. Education and cognitive functioning across the life span. Psychol Sci Public Interest. (2020) 21:6–41. doi: 10.1177/1529100620920576

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Du, C, Miyazaki, Y, Dong, X, and Li, M. Education, social engagement, and cognitive function: a cross-lagged panel analysis. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2023) 78:1756–64. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbad088

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Caniato, RN, Alvarenga, ME, Stich, HL, Jansen, H, and Baune, BT. Kindergarten attendance may reduce developmental impairments in children: results from the Bavarian pre-school morbidity survey. Scand J Public Health. (2010) 38:580–6. doi: 10.1177/1403494810376558

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hackman, DA, Gallop, R, Evans, GW, and Farah, MJ. Socioeconomic status and executive function: developmental trajectories and mediation. Dev Sci. (2015) 18:686–702. doi: 10.1111/desc.12246

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Maika, A, Mittinty, MN, Brinkman, S, and Lynch, J. Associations of early- and later-childhood poverty with child cognitive function in Indonesia: effect decomposition in the presence of exposure-induced mediator-outcome confounding. Am J Epidemiol. (2017) 185:879–87. doi: 10.1093/aje/kww195

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Shaw, DS, and Shelleby, EC. Early-starting conduct problems: intersection of conduct problems and poverty. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2014) 10:503–28. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153650

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. González, L, Cortés-Sancho, R, Murcia, M, Ballester, F, Rebagliato, M, and Rodríguez-Bernal, CL. The role of parental social class, education and unemployment on child cognitive development. Gac Sanit. (2020) 34:51–60. doi: 10.1016/j.gaceta.2018.07.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. San Miguel, JL, Spielvogel, H, Berger, J, Araoz, M, Lujan, C, Tellez, W, et al. Effect of high altitude on protein metabolism in Bolivian children. High Alt Med Biol. (2002) 3:377–86. doi: 10.1089/15270290260512855

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: education, urbanization, cognitive function, altitude, adolescents, Tibet

Citation: Ciren Y, Gong J, Li F, Gong X, Danzeng D, Ning Y, Wang T, Tian X and Silang Q (2025) Education and urbanization improves cognitive function affected by altitude of adolescents: a cross-section study in Tibet, China. Front. Public Health. 13:1539922. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2025.1539922

Received: 05 December 2024; Accepted: 27 May 2025;
Published: 13 June 2025.

Edited by:

Erin Kristin Zinkhan, The University of Utah, United States

Reviewed by:

Erica Mabel Mancera, National University of Colombia, Colombia
Santiago Ucrós, University of Los Andes, Colombia

Copyright © 2025 Ciren, Gong, Li, Gong, Danzeng, Ning, Wang, Tian and Silang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Quzha Silang, NTMzMTAwMDdAcXEuY29t

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.