- Department of Sociology, University of North Carolina at Charlotte, Charlotte, NC, United States
In the contemporary United States, many individuals suffer from a lack of close relationships, negatively affecting their happiness. At the same time, many individuals do not feel a loving relationship with God/Spirit (i.e., religious love). In the 1950s, theorist Pitirim Sorokin posited that love, particularly religious love, acts as a transformative energy capable of fostering resilience in the modern rational world, and scholars hypothesized a link between religious love and happiness. However, the topic remains understudied. This study analyzes United States data from the Global Flourishing Study and presents results from linear regression with robust standard errors. The findings indicate that a central component of religious love, love by God, mitigates the adverse effects of lacking close relationships. This study calls on researchers to further investigate the significant yet understudied role of religious love in individuals’ lives.
1 Introduction
Sociologists and social scientists have long recognized the importance of emotions in shaping individual interactions and societal structures (63, 64). However, the empirical examination of love within society remains limited, with most studies focusing on romantic, parental, or sexual love (1). Research on religious love—love by God or the Divine—is particularly scarce. Yet, Sorokin’s (2) theory of love suggests that religious love has the potential to foster resilience to societal stressors and ultimately promote greater happiness.
One notable social stressor is the absence of close relationships. Extensive research highlights the critical role of close social relationships in fostering happiness (3–5). These relationships act as buffers during stressful situations (6, 7). Most studies on close relationships explore their supportive versus detrimental characteristics, their evolution across the life course, and their impact on wellbeing, happiness, and health (7–9).
Despite these advancements, scholars have rarely assessed whether the relationship between close relationships and happiness is influenced by perceptions of love by God. This study proposes that love by God may serve as a buffer, protecting individuals without close relationships from the negative effects on happiness. Using U.S. data from the Global Flourishing Study, this research examines whether love by God moderates the relationship between close social relationships and happiness. This inquiry is especially relevant in light of two pressing trends in the United States: (1) increased social disconnectedness, and (2) declining belief in God (10, 11). The tested hypothesis was preregistered with OSF Registries at: https://osf.io/rz23g.
1.1 Relationships, love by God, and happiness
Pitirim Sorokin developed a multidimensional theory of love, which included religious love, conceptualized as love by God or a divine, spiritual being (12, 13). While religions have created distinct traditions to honor this form of love, Sorokin argued that religious love itself is universal. He described it as a transformative energy, capable of reshaping humanity by fostering love, compassion, and a sense of unity with both the divine and others (14, 15). Sorokin further theorized that modernization has shifted society away from “ideational truth,” which incorporates supraconscious, spiritual perceptions, toward “sensate truth,” which relies on sensory perception and measurable reality (16). This transition, he contended, has led to a decline in social harmony and integration, as humanity lost its connection to supraconscious love (17). Similarly, scholars argue that capitalism and modern development are antithetical to love, as the focus on reason and efficiency undermines human relationships (18). According to Sorokin, love in general, and religious love in particular, serves as a force for social integration (19), as God is a member of a person’s social network that can offer support and even combat loneliness (20).
The lack of harmony and social integration, as theorized by Sorokin, becomes particularly evident when individuals lack close, supportive interpersonal relationships. In fact, love is often defined as a desire to have a close connected ongoing relationship with another (21). Thus, close relationships are built on a foundation of love, even when the actors do not consciously acknowledge the underlying love. Indeed, Sternberg (22) identifies three components of love: intimacy, commitment, and passion, two of which are stable in most close relationships. The first component, intimacy, represents connectedness and bondedness. It is reflected in the desire to support another and the ability to count on another. The second component of love, commitment, reflects the decision to be committed to another, though the length of the commitment is variable. Thus, close social relationships are founded upon love. This conception of love aligns with Sorokin’s broader conceptualization of love which includes psychological love—empathy, devotion, and respect–and social love—including interconnectedness and caring for others (23, 24).
Close relationships are associated with greater wellbeing, happiness, and health outcomes (5, 25–33). Researchers have emphasized that social connection is essential for humans, as it motivates behavior and enhances wellbeing (34). Importantly, some scholars suggest that the mere existence of at least one meaningful social bond is more critical to happiness than the number of relationships (4). Conversely, a lack of close relationships is a key predictor of poor health, diminished wellbeing, and unhappiness (35–38).
Despite these findings, no research has examined whether religious love, specifically love by God, moderates the association between close social relationships and happiness. Sorokin theorized that religious love fosters social integration and cohesion, which in turn enhances happiness. This idea is supported by studies showing that religious love promotes health and wellbeing, including happiness, and protects against poor health and psychological distress (1, 39, 40).
Religious love may moderate the association between personal relationships and happiness because, as Sorokin posited, it serves as a transformative energy. In this sense, God or Spirit acts as an omnipresent force, guiding individuals’ interactions, perceptions, and behaviors across social contexts. Dill (41), for instance, found in a qualitative study of Black youth that their faith and relationship with God provided a sense of protection, allowing them to turn their worries over to God. Similarly, Upenieks et al. (42) observed in a study of student-athletes that a positive relationship with God helped mitigate the negative impact of low trait courage on wellbeing. While this study did not explicitly examine love, it underscores how a relationship with God can foster resilience.
Building on these findings, this study proposes that individuals who lack close, supportive social relationships may not necessarily experience unhappiness if they perceive love by God. When traditional social support is absent, the perception of being loved by God or Spirit may offer a sense of protection and connection. Religious love, therefore, has the potential to provide resilience against the adverse effects of social disconnection. Therefore, the study assesses the following hypothesis:
Love by God will moderate the relationship between close relationships and happiness.
2 Methods
To assess the hypothesis, this study presents results from analysis of United States data from the first wave of the Global Flourishing Study, fielded in 2023 by Gallup, Inc. (43, 44). The Global Flourishing study was developed through a collaboration of researchers at Harvard University, Baylor University, Gallup, Inc., and the Center for Open Science. The United States data were collected with a probability-based sample of the civilian non-institutionalized population of adults, utilizing the Gallup web-based panel. Gallup, Inc. recruited panelists based on address or random digit dialing, with consideration of population characteristics based on census data (45). All members of the Gallup Panel were sent up to five invitations to participate in the survey. The United States response rate was 100%. The survey was administered in English. Gallup ensured quality responses by flagging surveys with: illogical or inconsistent responses, single response categories across several questions, and short time to completion. Surveys that presented multiple quality issues were excluded. Gallup also had security protocols in place to ensure that responses were unique and from valid devices. Sample weights were employed to adjust for the probability of selection and non-response (45).
The analyses focused on three primary variables. The dependent variable, happiness, was measured with the question, “In general, how happy or unhappy do you usually feel? “Responses were coded on an 11 point scale, where 0 represents extremely unhappy and 10 represents extremely happy. The primary independent variables were:
1. Close relationships: respondents were asked, “Is there any one special person you know that you feel very close to? For example, someone you can confide in and share your feelings with.” Responses were coded as 1 for No (no close relationship) and 0 for Yes.
2. Loved by God: respondents were asked, “I feel loved or cared for by God, the main God I worship, or the spiritual force that guides my life.” Loved by God was coded as 1 for Agree and 0 for Disagree, Unsure, or Not Relevant.
All models included controls for a series of variables related to both happiness and wellbeing which is a closely related construct (see Appendix A for the full list of variables). The literature has established that the quality of parental relationships predicts overall wellbeing (46–48). The quality of parental relationships was measured with two variables: relationship with mother and relationship with father. Both variables were measured on a 5-point scale, ranging from very bad to very good and were included in the analyses as categorical variables. For both variables, there was a category for “does not apply” to account for absent parents. The models also included a control for a measure of social support, where respondents reported the extent that they could count on people to help if they were in trouble, coded on an 11-point scale, ranging from 0 to 10.
The models also controlled for sense of country belonging, ranging from weak (0) to very strong (10) on an 11-point scale. At the country-level, sense of belonging is important for residents, particularly migrants undergoing an acculturation process (49, 50). A lack of country-connectedness can generate lower wellbeing (51). The models also controlled for perceived discrimination (coded as never, rarely, often, always, and missing) because perceptions of discrimination may impede social connections and wellbeing, particularly for individuals in marginalized positions (52).
Extensive research has found a relationship between religiosity/spirituality and both happiness and wellbeing (53–59). To ensure that the measure of loved by God was independent of overall religiosity, the study included controls for three measures: Religious service attendance, measured on a five-point scale ranging from never (1; the excluded category) to more than once a week (5); Frequency of prayer or meditation, measured on a four-point scale ranging from never (1, the excluded category) to more than once a day (4); and Religiosity, an index of four variables. The Global Flourishing Study incorporated measures that capture religiosity from a variety of previously developed and extensively tested scales (43). The religiosity measure included in this study was created with four variables. The first measure, religious beliefs and practices lie behind approach to life, was originally measured with four categories: agree, disagree, not relevant or unsure. It was recoded into a dichotomous measure of agree (1) versus disagree, not relevant, or unsure (0). The second measure, religion is an important part of daily life, was originally measured as yes, no, do not know, and refused. The variable was recoded to yes (1) versus no or do not know (0). Refused was recoded to missing. The third measure, finds strength or comfort in religion or spirituality, was originally measured as agree, disagree, not relevant, or unsure. This measure was dichotomized to compare agree (1) to disagree, not relevant, and unsure (0). The final measure, feels connected to a religion or form of spirituality was measured on a four-point scale, ranging from never (0) to always (4). A categorical principal component analysis demonstrated that the items fit together on a single construct with an eigenvalue above 1.0 and an alpha of 0.89. A factor analysis on a polychoric correlation matrix showed that the variables fit on a single factor with factor loadings for all variables above 0.9.
This study also included a control for parental love, one of the most studied forms of love. This variable was measured with three categories: Not Loved (excluded category), Loved by One Parent, and Loved by Two Parents. Parental love was created from two questions, (1) in general, did you feel loved by your mother when you were growing up? And (2) in general, did you feel loved by your father when you were growing up?
The final set of control variables included characteristics of individuals and families. Controls included age, gender (male, female, and other), foreign-born, race/ethnicity (White Non-Hispanic, Black Non-Hispanic, Asian Non-Hispanic, other and mixed race Non-Hispanic, and Hispanic) employment status (employed for an employer, self-employed, retired, unemployed, and out of the labor force), highest education (no high school diploma, high school graduate, Associate’s degree, Bachelor’s degree, and advanced degree), place (rural, small town, large city, and suburb), marital status (single and never married, married, separated or divorced, widowed, and domestic partner), and household income (categorized). See Appendix A for details and descriptive statistics.
The original data included responses from 38,312 individuals residing in the United States. The final working sample included 37,641 participants, after excluding 671 participants who were missing values on the dependent variable, the two primary independent variables or any continuous control variable. For all categorical control variables, an extra category for missing data was included. Descriptive statistics for all variables are included in Appendix A. Data were weighted with sample weights and are analyzed via linear regression with robust standard errors via the svy: regress command in STATA.
3 Results
Model 1 in Table 1 presents the results from a linear regression with robust standard errors, showing the direct effects of no close relationship and loved by God on happiness. The analysis shows a negative association between lacking close relationships and happiness, highlighting lower happiness levels among those without close personal connections. Conversely, individuals who perceived Love by God reported higher levels of happiness. Model 2 presents analysis that examines whether loved by God moderates the relationship between close relationships and happiness. The results show a positive and significant moderation effect, supporting the hypothesis that perceived love by God moderates the relationship between close personal relationships and happiness.
Figure 1 illustrates predicted happiness based on perceptions of Love by God and the presence of a close relationship, while controlling for all other variables at their mean values. The x-axis categorizes individuals by whether they perceived Love by God, and the y-axis represents predicted happiness scores derived from the analysis in Table 1, Model 2. The grouping variable represents the presence or absence of a close relationship.

Figure 1. Predicted happiness by love by God and close relationship. With 95% confidence intervals; weighted.
This graph illustrates how Love by God moderates the relationship between close social relationships and happiness. When individuals perceived love by God, there is not a significant difference in the effect that a close social relationship has on happiness. Yet, when the individual did not perceive Love by God, individuals who lacked a close social relationship had lower predicted happiness. These findings highlight the protective role of perceiving love by God, particularly for individuals who lacked close social relationships. This moderation effect demonstrates how Love by God can compensate for the absence of close social relations, fostering resilience and generating higher levels of happiness.
While Figure 1 plots predicted mean happiness, Figure 2 plots the predicted distribution of happiness across four groups, based on whether individuals perceived love by God and whether they had a close relationship. The x-axis displays the values of the happiness scale, while the y-axis displays the density of responses. The vertical dashed line marks the weighted mean happiness for the overall sample. Panels A and B depict happiness distributions for individuals who lacked a close social relationship. Panel A includes respondents who perceived Love by God, while Panel B includes those who did not. The differences between these two panels are striking: individuals in Panel A generally reported higher happiness levels than those in Panel B, whose responses were more dispersed and clustered more heavily below the overall mean. Thus, perceptions of Love by God buffer against the negative effects of the absence of close relationships.

Figure 2. Predicted happiness by close relationship and love by God. Dashed line represents overall mean happiness.
In contrast, Panels C and D show the distribution of happiness for individuals who reported a close relationship. Panel C includes those who felt loved by God, while Panel D displays those who did not feel loved by God. These distributions appear more condensed than the distributions for individuals who did not report a close relationship. They both have distributions where the peak is at or above the mean, and the distributions are remarkably similar. This suggests that the presence of a close relationship mitigated differences in happiness regardless of perceptions of love by God.
Yet, there is also a clear cumulative effect. Panel C, representing both a close relationship and love by God, presents the most condensed distribution. Notably, very few cases in the Panel C distribution fall below mean levels of happiness. In contrast, the absence of both close relationships and love by God (Panel B) is associated with the greatest dispersion of happiness with a large skew below mean levels of happiness. This demonstrates the compounding negative effects of lacking both social and spiritual relationships.
4 Discussion
This study, based on U.S. data from the Global Flourishing Study, found that love by God moderates the well-established relationship between close social relationships and happiness. This research offers a novel contribution, as few studies have explored the role of Godly love in shaping happiness in light of a person’s close relationships. Close relationships are, themselves, a manifestation of love that results from both interconnection and a decision to maintain a relationship with the other. Pitirim Sorokin’s complex and often overlooked theory of love (1964), developed decades ago, argued that love—especially Godly love—can influence social cohesion and individual wellbeing.
Notably, the analysis showed that once a close relationship was present, perceptions of love by God do not further enhance happiness. Individuals with close relationships reported similarly high levels of happiness, regardless of whether they perceived Divine love. However, for those without close relationships, feeling loved by God provided resilience, helping individuals maintain happiness despite the absence of close personal connections. This finding contributes to research on social relationships by illustrating a condition under which the lack of a close relationship may not function as a stressor, as manifested in levels of happiness. Importantly, although perceptions of divine love buffer against the negative effects of lacking a close relationship, the most favorable outcomes—the highest and most condensed distribution of happiness—were observed among individuals who report both a close relationship and divine love. This cumulative pattern suggests that close relationships and perceptions of God’s love operate as complementary, rather than fully interchangeable, sources of resilience.
Given that this analysis was based on secondary data, the measurement of love by God was limited to a single measure. Future research should extend this measure of love to fully capture Sorokin’s conceptualization as it is a two-way relationship, suggesting the need for a measure that captures perceptions of love for and by God. Not only was the original conceptualization two-directional, it was multidimensional (60, 61). Levin (39, 62) developed a multidimensional measure of religious love that should be further incorporated into Sociological research. While the present study does not include the broader multidimensional measure as it relies on secondary data, it is important to note that Levin’s measure of religious love had an alpha of 0.92, indicating an extremely high internal consistency between the components, suggesting that the single item measure presented in this study would likely be highly correlated with other items in Levin’s measure. Future research should incorporate a multidimensional measurement of religious love. In the meantime, this study illustrates that perceiving love by God plays a role in offering resilience to individuals who lack close social relationships.
This study highlights the need for future research to further integrate Sorokin’s conceptualization of religious love into sociological inquiry. Sorokin’s proposition that religious love acts as a buffer against societal challenges in modernized contexts is profound and merits deeper exploration. Given the limited existing research on religious love, there is substantial opportunity for future studies to extend this work. For instance, researchers could examine whether religious love fosters resilience in the face of significant life events such as failure, status loss, divorce, job loss, or incarceration. Future studies could also investigate both social isolation and loneliness as potential moderating factors. Comparing the effects of religious love with other well-studied forms of love, such as parental or romantic love, may offer additional insights into the diverse sources of resilience. Moreover, there is potential to expand this analysis by exploring variations in the experience of divine love across different religious traditions and cultural contexts. Refining the conceptualization of close relationships—by considering their length, type, and depth—would also strengthen this line of inquiry. Once broader measures of love and relational dynamics are incorporated, researchers could assess the extent to which different forms of love may serve as substitutes for one another in promoting happiness. Finally, expanding the outcome measures to include additional dimensions of flourishing, such as mental and physical health, would further advance understanding of the broader impacts of religious love.
Data availability statement
Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found at: https://www.cos.io/gfs-faqs.
Ethics statement
Ethical approval was not required for the studies involving humans because the study is based on analysis of secondary data provided by the Global Flourishing Study and does not include sensitive information. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
SM: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author declares that Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript. This work was enhanced using OpenAI’s ChatGPT-4 for editing purposes to improve clarity and coherence. All interpretations and conclusions are the author’s own.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2025.1567701/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Levin, J. A prolegomenon to an epidemiology of love: theory, measurement, and health outcomes. J Soc Clin Psychol. (2000) 19:117–36. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2000.19.1.117
3. Andersen, LMB, Rasmussen, AN, Reavley, NJ, Bøggild, H, and Overgaard, C. The social route to mental health: a systematic review and synthesis of theories linking social relationships to mental health to inform interventions. SSM - Mental Health. (2021) 1:100042. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmmh.2021.100042
4. Baumeister, R, and Leary, M. The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol Bull. (1995) 117:497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497
5. Ryan, RM, and Deci, EL. On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu Rev Psychol. (2001) 52:141–66. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141
6. Green, G, Hayes, C, Dickinson, D, Whittaker, A, and Gilheany, B. The role and impact of social relationships upon well-being reported by mental health service users: a qualitative study. J Ment Health. (2002) 11:565–79. doi: 10.1080/09638230020023912
7. Roberts, S, Arrow, H, Lehmann, J, and Dunbar, R. Close social relationships: an evolutionary perspective In: RIM Dunbar, C Gamble, and JAJ Gowlett, editors. Lucy to language: The benchmark papers. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2014). 180.
8. Antonucci, TC, Akiyama, H, and Lansford, JE. Negative effects of close social relations. Fam Relat. (1998) 47:379–84. doi: 10.2307/585268
9. Krause, N. Social relationships in late life In: RH Binstock, LK George, SJ Cutler, J Hendricks, and JH Schulz, editors. Handbook of aging and the social sciences. Amsterdam: Elsevier (2006). 181–200.
10. Gallup, I. (2022). How many americans believe in God? Available online at: https://news.gallup.com/poll/268205/americans-believe-god.aspx#:~:text=Most%20recently%2C%20Gallup%20found%2081,in%20the%201950s%20and%201960s (accessed June 6, 2024).
11. Office of the U.S. Surgeon General. Our epidemic of loneliness and isolation: The U.S. surgeon general’s advisory on the healing effects of social connection and community. Washington, DC: Office of the U.S. Surgeon General (2023).
12. Sorokin, P. The ways and power of love: Types, factors, and techniques of moral transformation. West Conshohocken, PA: Templeton Foundation Press (2002).
13. Sorokin, PA. Love: its aspects, production, transformation, and accumulation In: P Sorokin, editor. Explorations in altruistic love and behavior. Gibraltar: The Beacon Press (1950)
14. Mangone, E. From calamities to disasters: Pitirim Aleksandrovič sorokin’s insights. Hum Arenas. (2018) 1:79–85. doi: 10.1007/s42087-018-0001-2
15. Mangone, E, and Dolgov, A. Sorokin’s “altruistic creative love”: genesis, methodological issues, and applied aspects. Hum Arenas. (2020) 3:6–22. doi: 10.1007/s42087-019-00058-w
16. Sorokin, PA. The crisis of our age: The social and cultural outlook. New York, NY: E.P. Dutton & Co., Inc. (1941).
17. Matter, JA. Love, altruism, and world crisis: Challenge of Pitirim Sorokin. New York, NY: Littlefield, Adams (1975).
18. Rusu, MS. Theorising love in sociological thought: classical contributions to a sociology of love. J Class Sociol. (2018) 18:3–20. doi: 10.1177/1468795X17700645
19. Paglione, L. The (epistemological) power of love: from Pitirim a. Sorokin’s integralism to a ‘space for the heart’ in scientific methods. Soc Sci. (2024) 13:482. doi: 10.3390/socsci13090482
20. Kirkpatrick, LA, Shillito, DJ, and Kellas, SL. Loneliness, social support, and perceived relationships with god. J Soc Pers Relat. (1999) 16:513–22. doi: 10.1177/0265407599164006
21. Reis, HT, and Aron, A. Love: what is it, why does it matter, and how does it operate? Perspect Psychol Sci. (2008) 3:80–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00065.x
22. Sternberg, RJ. A triangular theory of love. Psychol Rev. (1986) 93:119–35. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.2.119
23. Cataldi, S. Imagining on the shoulders of giants: a historical selection of the social thought on love In: S Cataldi and G Iorio, editors. Social love and the critical potential of people: When the social reality challenges the sociological imagination. London: Routledge (2022)
24. Sorokin, PA. The ways and power of love: Types, factors, and techniques of moral transformation. Gibraltar: The Beacon Press (1954).
25. Brown, RL, and Ciciurkaite, G. Understanding the connection between social support and mental health In: TL Scheid and ER Wright, editors. A handbook for the study of mental health: Social contexts, theories, and systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2017). 207–23.
26. Dolan, P, Peasgood, T, and White, M. Do we really know what makes us happy? A review of the economic literature on the factors associated with subjective well-being. J Econ Psychol. (2008) 29:94–122. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2007.09.001
27. Holt-Lunstad, J. Why social relationships are important for physical health: a systems approach to understanding and modifying risk and protection. Annu Rev Psychol. (2018) 69:437–58. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011902
28. Holt-Lunstad, J. Social connection as a public health issue: the evidence and a systemic framework for prioritizing the “social” in social determinants of health. Annu Rev Public Health. (2022) 43:193–213. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-052020-110732
29. Klussman, K, Nichols, AL, Langer, J, and Curtin, N. Connection and disconnection as predictors of mental health and wellbeing. Int J Wellbeing. (2020) 10:89–100. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v10i2.855
30. Pawelski, S. P., and Pawelski, J. (2025). The power of strong social connections: The latest world happiness report finds friendships are key to our happiness. Psychology Today. Available online at: https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/happy-together/202503/the-power-of-strong-social-connections (Accessed July 4, 2025).
31. Pei, R., and Zaki, J. (2025). Connecting with others: how social connections improve the happiness of young adults. In J. F. Helliwell, R. Layard, J. D. Sachs, J.-E. NeveDe, L. B. Aknin, and S. Wang (Eds.), World happiness report 2025. University of Oxford: Oxford
32. Turner, RJ. Social support as a contingency in psychological well-being. J Health Soc Behav. (1981) 22:357–67. doi: 10.2307/2136677
33. Waldinger, R, and Schulz, M. The good life: Lessons from the world's longest scientific study of happiness. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster (2023).
34. Seppala, E, Rossomando, T, and Doty, JR. Social connection and compassion: important predictors of health and well-being. Soc. Res. Special Issue Giving Caring Needs Strangers. (2013) 80:411–30. doi: 10.1353/sor.2013.0027
35. McPherson, M, Smith-Lovin, L, and Brashears, ME. Social isolation in America: changes in core discussion networks over two decades. Am Sociol Rev. (2006) 71:353–75. doi: 10.1177/000312240607100301
36. Parigi, P, and Henson, W. Social isolation in America. Annu Rev Sociol. (2014) 40:153–71. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-071312-145646
37. Umberson, D, Crosnoe, R, and Reczek, C. Social relationships and health behavior across life course. Annu Rev Sociol. (2010) 36:139–57. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-120011
38. Umberson, D, and Donnelly, R. Social isolation: an unequally distributed health hazard. Annu Rev Sociol. (2023) 49:379–99. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-031021-012001
39. Levin, J. Is depressed affect a function of one's relationship with god?: findings from a study of primary care patients. Int J Psychiatry Med. (2002) 32:379–93. doi: 10.2190/3183-WPYV-3KYY-K3V7
41. Dill, LJ. “Wearing my spiritual jacket”: the role of spirituality as a coping mechanism among African American youth. Health Educ Behav. (2017) 44:696–704. doi: 10.1177/1090198117729398
42. Upenieks, L, Bounds, EM, Melton, KK, Glanzer, P, and Schnitker, SA. Trait courage, attachment to god, and mental well-being among U.S. collegiate athletes. J Relig Health. (2024) 63:2941–62. doi: 10.1007/s10943-024-02054-z
43. Crabtree, S, English, C, Johnson, BR, Ritter, Z, and VanderWeele, TJ. Global flourishing study: Questionnaire development report. Washington, DC: Gallup Inc. (2021).
44. Johnson, BR, Ritter, Z, Fogleman, A, Markham, L, Stankov, T, Srinivasan, R, et al. The global flourishing study. (2024) doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/3JTZ8
45. Ritter, Z, Srinivasan, R, Han, Y, Chattopadhyay, M, Honohan, J, Johnson, BR, et al. Global flourishing study methodology. Leicester: Allup Inc (2024).
46. Cavanagh, SE. Family structure history and adolescent adjustment. J Fam Issues. (2008) 29:944–80. doi: 10.1177/0192513X07311232
47. Grevenstein, D, Bluemke, M, Schweitzer, J, and Aguilar-Raab, C. Better family relationships––higher well-being: the connection between relationship quality and health related resources. Mental Health Prev. (2019) 14:200160. doi: 10.1016/j.mph.2019.200160
48. King, V, and Boyd, LM. Factors associated with perceptions of family belonging among adolescents. J Marriage Fam. (2016) 78:1114–30. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12322
49. Osman, F, Mohamed, A, Warner, G, and Sarkadi, A. Longing for a sense of belonging—Somali immigrant adolescents’ experiences of their acculturation efforts in Sweden. Int J Qual Stud Health Well Being. (2020) 15:1784532. doi: 10.1080/17482631.2020.1784532
50. Yoon, E, Chang, C-T, Kim, S, Clawson, A, Cleary, SE, Hansen, M, et al. A meta-analysis of acculturation/enculturation and mental health. J Couns Psychol. (2013) 60:15–30. doi: 10.1037/a0030652
51. Cramer, KM, and Pawsey, H. Happiness and sense of community belonging in the world value survey. Curr Res Ecol Soc Psychol. (2023) 4:100101. doi: 10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100101
52. Wu, Z, and Finnsdottir, M. Perceived racial and cultural discrimination and sense of belonging in Canadian society. Can Rev Sociol. (2021) 58:229–49. doi: 10.1111/cars.12339
53. Cohen, AB, Pierce, JD Jr, Chambers, J, Meade, R, Gorvine, BJ, and Koenig, HG. Intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity, belief in the afterlife, death anxiety, and life satisfaction in young catholics and protestants. J Res Pers. (2005) 39:307–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2004.02.005
54. Fry, PS. Religious involvement, spirituality and personal meaning for life: existential predictors of psychological wellbeing in community-residing and institutional care elders. Aging Ment Health. (2000) 4:375–87. doi: 10.1080/713649965
55. Ismail, Z, and Desmukh, S. Religiosity and psychological well-being. Int J Bus Soc Sci. (2012) 3:20–8. doi: 10.30845/ijbss
56. Jacobi, CJ, Cowden, RG, and Vaidyanathan, B. Associations of changes in religiosity with flourishing during the covid-19 pandemic: a study of faith communities in the United States [brief research report]. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:805785. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.805785
57. Leondari, A, and Gialamas, V. Religiosity and psychological well-being. Int J Psychol. (2009) 44:241–8. doi: 10.1080/00207590701700529
58. Shattuck, EC, and Muehlenbein, MP. Religiosity/spirituality and physiological markers of health. J Relig Health. (2020) 59:1035–54. doi: 10.1007/s10943-018-0663-6
59. Sherkat, DE, and Ellison, CG. Recent developments and current controversies in the sociology of religion. Annu Rev Sociol. (1999) 25:363–94. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.25.1.363
60. Levin, J. The epidemiology of love: historical perspectives and implications for population-health research. J Posit Psychol. (2023) 18:34–43. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2022.2053876
61. Post, SG. Godly love: A rose planted in the desert of our hearts. Conshohocken, PA: Templeton Press (2008).
62. Levin, J. God, love, and health: findings from a clinical study. Rev Relig Res. (2001) 42:277–93. doi: 10.2307/3512570
63. Scheff, TJ. Shame and conformity: The deference-emotion system. Am. Sociol. Rev. (1998) 53:395. doi: 10.2307/2095647
Keywords: love, happiness, close relationship, God, happiness and wellbeing
Citation: Moller S (2025) Close social relationships and happiness in the United States: the moderating role of love by God. Front. Public Health. 13:1567701. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2025.1567701
Edited by:
M. Walid Qoronfleh, Q3 Research Institute, United StatesReviewed by:
Elias L. Khalil, Doha Institute for Graduate Studies, QatarGetrude C. Ah Gang, Universiti Malaysia Sabah, Malaysia
Maria Claudia Scurtu Tura, Sevilla University, Spain
Alexandra Wormley, University of Michigan, United States
Copyright © 2025 Moller. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Stephanie Moller, c3RlcGhhbmllLm1vbGxlckBjaGFybG90dGUuZWR1