Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 29 February 2024
Sec. Nutrition, Psychology and Brain Health
Volume 11 - 2024 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnut.2024.1348918

Association between soybean product consumption and executive function in Chinese Tibetan children and adolescents

Xiaojian Yin1 Feng Zhang2,3 Cunjian Bi4 Yuan Liu5* Yaru Guo2,3 Pengwei Sun2,3 Jun Hong2,3
  • 1Department of Physical Education, Shanghai Institute of Technology, Shanghai, China
  • 2Key Laboratory of Adolescent Health Assessment and Exercise Intervention of Ministry of Education, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
  • 3College of Physical Education and Health, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
  • 4Sports Health Promotion Center, Chizhou University, Chizhou, China
  • 5Physical Education College of Shanghai University, Shanghai, China

Objective: This sought to explore the association between soybean product consumption and executive function (EF) in Chinese Tibetan adolescents in high-altitude areas.

Methods: A total of 1,184 Tibetan children and adolescents were tested on demographic variables, soybean product consumption, and executive function in Lhasa and Nagchu regions of Tibet, China, using stratified whole population sampling. One-way ANOVA, linear regression analysis, and logistic regression analysis were used to analyze the associations existing between soybean product consumption and executive function.

Results: The proportions of Hardly ever, Occasionally, and Often in Soy Consumption among Tibetan children and adolescents in high altitude areas of Tibet, China were 21.7, 50.3, and 28.0%, respectively. The difference in 2 back reaction time among Tibetan children and adolescents with different soybean product consumption was statistically significant (F = 6.374, p = 0.002). The difference in conversion function reaction time was also statistically significant (F = 8.129, p < 0.001). Taking the soybean product consumption ≥6 t/w group as the reference group, after adjusting the relevant factors, those with soybean product consumption ≤1 t/w showed a statistically significant increase in Inhibit Function Dysfunction (OR = 1.844, 95% CI: 1.152, 2.951) and Conversion Function Dysfunction (OR = 2.008, 95% CI: 1.106, 3.646) had an increased risk of Conversion Function Dysfunction (OR = 2.008, 95% CI: 1.106, 3.646), which was significantly different (p < 0.05).

Conclusion: There is an association between soybean product consumption and inhibitory control and translational flexibility of brain executive functions in Chinese Tibetan children and children and adolescents at high altitude.

1 Introduction

Executive functions are critical to child and adolescent development, enabling children and children and adolescents to effectively focus their attention, multitask, understand and manage their emotions, and enhance their social skills (13). Executive functions are highly correlated with child and adolescent development and are important predictors of behavioral functioning, academic achievement, and even other outcomes such as athletic achievement, academic performance, mental health, social well-being, health, wealth, and quality of life (46). It is now widely recognized in the academic community that there are three core functions of executive function: inhibitory, working memory, and translational. On top of the three core executive functions, the brain builds higher-order executive functions, such as logical reasoning, problem solving, and task planning, with the three subcomponents being both independent and interrelated (7). Executive functions are essential skills for adolescents’ healthy physical and mental development, success in school and life, and cognitive, social, and psychological development (819). On the contrary, when children and adolescents have executive function deficits, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and aggressive behaviors, it can cause heavy psychological and physical stress on the family (20, 21). Thus, executive function plays an important role in the healthy development of children and adolescents.

Soy products play an important role in people’s daily diet and bring many health benefits (22). The so-called soy products usually include various kinds of food made from soybeans and mung beans as the main raw materials, such as edamame, fermented bean curd, soybean paste, soy milk, dried beans and other foods. Soy products are rich in high-quality protein and high in unsaturated fatty acids. Research has found that soybeans and their products have a high nutritional value, protein content of about 35%, and is a high-quality protein, containing a variety of essential amino acids needed by the human body (23). The Dietary Guidelines for Chinese Residents (2022) recommends a weekly intake of 105–175 g of soybeans or an equivalent amount of soybean products, while the daily intake of soybeans and soybean products by Chinese residents is only 10.3 g, which is far below the recommended intake (24). This shows that research on soybean products should be emphasized in the future in order to better increase the consumption of soybean products.

Previous studies on executive function in children and adolescents have focused on physical activity and executive function, sugary drinks and executive function, and relatively few studies on soybean product consumption and executive function in dietary behavior (25, 26). In addition, no studies have been found on soybean product consumption and executive function in Tibetan children and adolescents at high altitude (27). Tibetan children and adolescents living in Tibet, China, live at high altitude all year round, which has certain effects on brain health and plateau adaptation (28, 29). This reason, it is necessary to conduct research on the brain executive function and soybean product consumption of their Tibetan children and adolescents, in order to promote the healthy development of the brain.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

This study used three stages of stratified whole cluster sampling method for the sampling of subjects. In the first stage, the regions of Lhasa (3,650 meters) and Nagchu (4,450 meters) in the high altitude region of Tibet, China were used as the sampling regions for the subjects of this study. In the second stage, one junior high school and one senior high school in each region among all junior high schools and senior high schools were selected as the survey schools for the subjects of this study by using random whole cluster sampling method. In the third stage, in each school, 2 instructional classes were randomly selected in a whole cluster for each grade using classes as the sampling unit, and the eligible middle school students in the classes served as the subjects of this study. The inclusion criteria for this study were: both the father and mother were Tibetan and had lived in Tibet, China for 3 years or more; the subjects had no physical illnesses and were of normal intelligence; and the subjects volunteered to be surveyed for this study. In the end, a total of 1,209 people from 24 classes in 4 schools were surveyed, and after 25 invalid questionnaires were excluded, 1,184 valid questionnaires were returned (560 boys and 624 girls), with an effective return rate of 97.93%. The mean age of the participants was (15.78 ± 1.68) years. The survey period for this study was April–November 2020. The specific sampling process is shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Sampling flow of Chinese Tibetan children and adolescents subjects in high altitude.

Written informed consent for this study survey was obtained from the students’ parents and themselves, and the survey was conducted after signing a written informed consent form. This study was approved by the Human Ethics Committee of East China Normal University (No.: HR0782020).

2.2 Basic demographic information

The survey of basic demographic information in this study included the information of the subjects’ area, school, grade, class, gender, year of birth, and test date. The subjects were asked to fill in the basic information truthfully according to their actual situation. The age of the subjects was calculated according to their birth date and test date, and the age of the subjects was calculated according to the whole year if it was less than a whole year.

2.3 Soy consumption

The soy products investigated in this study mainly included the frequency of soy products consumed by the subjects in the past week. The standard of soy product consumption is about 200 g for a standard cup of soy milk, 200 g for a standard bowl of tofu brain, and 200 g for a serving of soy product. The respondents were asked to recall the frequency of consumption of soybean products in the past 7 days according to their own situation and fill in the questionnaire (24, 30).

2.4 Executive function

The test of executive function in this study includes subjects’ inhibitory function, refreshing function, and switching function. (1) Inhibitory function refers to a person’s ability to control his or her attention, behavior, thoughts, and emotions in order to override strong internal tendencies or external temptations in order to perform more appropriate or needed tasks. (2) Refreshing function, also known as working memory function, refers to the ability to form deep memories of external information and process the information in a very short period of time. The ability to temporarily hold relevant information in a very short period of time in the relevant specific areas of the brain where the individual can quickly extract it or carry out a step of processing to deal with such information. (3) Switching function refers to the ability to switch smoothly between different cognitive tasks, also known as cognitive flexibility. Switching function helps individuals quickly adjust their cognitive and behavioral styles to adapt to environmental changes.

The inhibitory function in this study was tested using Flanker’s experimental paradigm for the subjects. The refreshing function was tested using 1-back and 2-back experimental paradigms on the subjects. The switching function was tested on subjects using the More-odd Shifting experimental paradigm. The testing of the execution function required the subjects to be tested in the computer room of the school, and the testing program was based on the E-prime 1.1 software system. Participants were asked to react quickly to the letters and colors that appeared on the computer screen and to press the appropriate keyboard keys while ensuring that they were correct. The specific test methods were referred to the specific test methods described in the previously published literature (31).

2.5 Covariates

The covariates tested included father’s education, mother’s education, sleep adequacy, waist circumference, moderate to high intensity physical activity, and maximum oxygen uptake in addition to basic demographic information. Father’s education and mother’s education were categorized as Primary School or Below, secondary and high school, Junior college or above, and sleep adequacy was categorized as Adequate (≥8 h/d) and Insufficient(<8 h/d) (32). Waist circumference was measured according to the testing method and instrument required by the China National Survey on Students’ Physical Fitness and Health, and the results were accurate to 0.1 cm (33). MVPA was calculated according to the time of the items of medium-intensity and high-intensity physical activity in the questionnaire of the China National Survey on Students’ Physical Fitness and Health, and the subjects were mainly tested for the time and frequency of MVPA per day in the past 7 days, so as to calculate the average MVPA per day of the subjects in the past 7 days. VO2max was calculated by using the 20-meter round-trip running test and converted to VO2max by the formula. 20-meter round-trip running was tested according to Léger’s test method (34). The test method is that the tester runs back and forth between two lines at a distance of 20 m, and each completion of 20 m is recorded as 1 lap (times), the running speed is controlled by the music, the initial speed is 8.0 km/h at the beginning, the speed is 9.0 km/h at the second minute, and the speed is accelerated by one speed level every minute, i.e., each time the speed is increased by 0.5 km/h. The tester tries to complete the speed level as much as possible, and the tester can not follow the rhythm to reach the 20 m end of the test for 2 times consecutively. The test is completed when the 20 m endline cannot be reached at tempo for 2 consecutive times, and the speed of the last stage is recorded to obtain the maximal aerobic speed (MAS). The complete 20 m SRT consists of 247 laps in 21 levels, and the maximum aerobic speed is 18.5 km/h. The VO2max level of children and adolescents aged 8–19 years was mainly projected based on the developed equation, which is as follows: VO2max = 31.025 + 3.238 × S − 3.248 × age + 0.1536 × S × age. × age. s denotes the tester’s final maximal speed, and S = 8 + 0.5 × the highest level reached by the tester (35).

2.6 Quality control

The questionnaire survey of the subjects was carried out by professional investigators who were qualified after training and examination. The problems of the subjects in the process of filling out the questionnaire were explained and answered in a timely manner. The questionnaire filling process requires subjects to fill out the questionnaire independently without time constraints, according to their own actual situation. The questionnaire is required to fill in and retrieve the questionnaire on the spot. The returned questionnaires were briefly checked, and the subjects were asked to complete the questionnaires in a timely manner if there were omissions or wrong entries, in order to reduce the number of invalid questionnaires and guarantee the quality of the survey.

2.7 Statistical analysis

The subjects in this study were categorized according to age and gender. Continuous variables were expressed in terms of mean and standard deviation, and categorical variables were expressed in terms of percentage. The frequency of Soy Consumption was classified as hardly ≤1 time/week, occasionally 2–5 time/week, and often ≥6 time/week. Each calculation was based on 1 standard cup of soymilk of about 200 g; 1 standard bowl of tofu brain of about 200 g; 1 serving of soy products of about 200 g; and 1 standard bowl of tofu brain of about 200 g. Each calculation is based on 1 standard cup of soymilk of about 200 g; 1 standard bowl of tofu brain of about 200 g; and 1 serving of soya products of 200 g. The executive function was expressed as mean and standard deviation. Comparisons of executive function between subjects with different Soy Consumption were made by one-way ANOVA, and the relationship between Soy Consumption and executive function was analyzed by linear regression analysis. The executive function reaction time higher than 1 standard deviation was also used as the cut-off value to define the presence of executive dysfunction, which was categorized into two groups. The analysis was performed by means of binary logistic regression analysis. Model 1 controlled for age, gender, father’s education, and mother’s education. Model 2 controlled for factors such as waist circumference, moderate to high intensity physical activity per day, and maximal oxygen uptake (VO2max) on the basis of model 1. Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS 25.0 software (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, United States) with a test level of a = 0.05.

3 Results

In this study, soybean product consumption and executive function were tested and investigated in 1184 (560 boys, 47.3%) Tibetan children and adolescents in the high altitude area of Tibet, China, and the mean age of the investigators was (15.78 ± 1.68) years. The proportion of Father’s education in primary school or below, secondary and high school, Junior college or above was 63.6, 24.4 and 12.0%, respectively, the difference was statistically significant (χ2 = 15.039, p = 0.005). The proportion of mothers whose education was primary school or below, secondary and high school, and Junior college or above was 72.6, 15.3, and 12.1%, respectively, the difference was statistically significant (χ2 = 19.288, p < 0.01). The mean circumference was (68.56 ± 7.21) cm. The results of this study showed that the percentages of Hardly ever, Occasionally, and Often in Soy Consumption among Tibetan children and adolescents in the high altitude area of Tibet, China were 21.7, 50.3, and 28.0%, respectively, and the difference was not statistically significant in comparison (χ2 = 2.029, p = 0.363). The results of this study show that there is a difference in Soy Consumption among Tibetan children and adolescents in terms of Father’s education and Mother’s education, and the difference is statistically significant when compared (χ2 = 15.039, 19.288, p < 0.01) (see Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Basic status of soybean product consumption among Chinese Tibetan children and adolescents.

Each calculation uses 1 standard cup of soy milk about 200 g; 1 Standard bowl of bean curd about 200 g calculation; One serving of soy products is calculated as 200 g. Descriptive statistics are presented as mean (standard deviation) and number (percentage) for continuous and categorical.

The results of this study showed that the difference in 2 back Reaction Time between different soybean product consumption Tibetan children and adolescents in high altitude areas of Tibet, China, was statistically significant when compared (F = 6.374, p = 0.002). Conversion function reaction time was also statistically significant when compared. The difference was also statistically significant (F = 8.129, p < 0.001). In terms of the subfunctions of the executive function, Consistent reaction time, Inconsistent reaction time, Switching reaction time were worse compared to each other, and the differences were also statistically significant (F = 5.293, 5.563, 8.536, p < 0.05) (see Table 2).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. One-way analysis of executive function status of Tibetan children and adolescents with different soybean product consumption.

Linear regression analysis was performed with Executive Function reaction time as the dependent variable and soybean product consumption as the independent variable among Tibetan children and adolescents at high altitude in Tibet, China. Model 1 was a crude model, model 2 controlled for age, gender, father’s education, mother’s education, etc., and model 3 controlled for waist circumference, daily moderate-high-intensity physical activity, and VO2max on the basis of model 2. The results showed that after adjusting for the relevant factors and using the soybean product consumption ≥6 t/w group as the reference group, the reaction time of those with soybean product consumption ≤1 t/w was analyzed by linear regression. Consistent reaction time and Consistent reaction time were significantly higher in those with soybean product consumption ≤1 t/w, with 16.666 ms and 16.458 ms, respectively, and the differences were statistically significant (p < 0.05). In addition, with the soybean product consumption ≥6 t/w group as the reference group, after adjusting the relevant factors, the Switching reaction time and Conversion function reaction time of those with soybean product consumption ≤1 t/w increased by 43.666 ms and 16.458 ms, respectively, and the difference was statistically significant (p<0.05). After adjusting the relevant factors, the Switching reaction time and Conversion function reaction time of those with soybean product consumption ≤1 t/w increased by 43.765 ms and 56.485 ms, respectively, and the differences were statistically significant (p < 0.05). Overall, with the decrease of soybean product consumption, the Conversion function reaction time showed an increasing trend (see Table 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Linear regression analysis of soybean product consumption and executive function in Tibetan children and adolescents at high altitude.

Binary logistic regression analyses were conducted with the presence of Executive dysfunction as the dependent variable and soybean product consumption as the independent variable among Tibetan children and adolescents in high-altitude areas of Tibet, China. Model 1 was a crude model, model 2 controlled for age, gender, father’s education, mother’s education, etc., and model 3 controlled for waist circumference, daily moderate to high intensity physical activity, and VO2max on the basis of model 2. The results showed that after adjusting for the relevant factors and using the soybean product consumption ≥6 t/w group as the reference group, those with soybean product consumption ≤1 t/w had an increased risk of Inhibit function dysfunction (OR = 1.844, 95% CI: 1.152, 2.951), which was significantly different (p < 0.05). In addition, with the soybean product consumption ≥6 t/w group as the reference group, the risk of Conversion function dysfunction was also increased in those with soybean product consumption ≤1 t/w after adjusting for relevant factors (OR = 2.008, 95% CI: 1.106, 3.646), which was significantly different (p < 0.05). Overall, the risk of Inhibit function dysfunction and Conversion function Dysfunction increased as soybean product consumption decreased (see Table 4). The trend of the association OR between soybean product consumption and executive function in Tibetan children and adolescents at high altitude in Tibet, China is shown in Figure 2.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Logistic regression analysis of soybean product consumption and executive function among Tibetan children and adolescents in high altitude areas of Tibet, China.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Trends in the ORs of the association between soybean product consumption and executive function.

4 Discussion

The results of the present study showed that the differences in 2back Reaction Time of Tibetan children and adolescents with different soybean product consumption in the high altitude area of Tibet, China, were statistically significant when compared with each other. The differences in Conversion function reaction time were also statistically significant when compared with each other. Overall, it can be seen that with the continuous decline of soybean product consumption, the subjects’ refreshing memory function and conversion flexibility showed a tendency to decrease. Relevant studies have confirmed that soybean product intake in children and adolescents is able to guarantee the necessary protein intake of the body, as well as guaranteeing the continuous supply of energy to the brain, which plays an important role in guaranteeing the function of the brain (36). There are also studies confirming that soybeans and soy products are rich in protein, which is a good source of high-quality protein, and that eating some soy products appropriately every day contributes to the health of the brain and enhances the brain’s memory, which is consistent with the findings of this study (22, 37, 38). In addition, research has confirmed that soybean or soybean products contained in soy lecithin, not only can enhance cellular information transfer capacity, improve brain vitality, but also often called “vascular scavenger,” has to reduce serum cholesterol, reduce blood viscosity, promote blood circulation, prevention of cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases and other roles, especially suitable for children and adolescents and the Brain workers (39, 40). Research has also confirmed that soy products are rich in high-quality protein and a variety of amino acids needed by the human body, and these elements help to enhance the function of the cerebrovascular system (41). Soybean fat contains 85.5% of unsaturated fatty acids, including linolenic acid and linoleic acid content, they can reduce cholesterol in the body, the prevention and control of cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases and children and adolescents brain health is particularly beneficial (42). In the statement “A Primary Care Agenda for Brain Health” issued by the American Heart Association (AHA), a dietary pattern known as the brain-healthy diet (MIND diet) is recommended to maintain brain health (43). The MIND diet, which combines the DASH diet and the Mediterranean diet, encourages the consumption of nine foods. Nine foods are encouraged and six foods are restricted, with soy products among the encouraged foods recommended as important foods to consume and play an important role in brain health (44).

The results of this study also showed that overall, the risk of Inhibit Function dysfunction and Conversion function dysfunction increased in Tibetan children and adolescents at high altitude as soybean product consumption decreased. This shows that maintaining appropriate soybean product intake has an important role and significance in the development of brain executive function in Tibetan children and adolescents at high altitude. There are also studies confirming that soybeans have a protein content of up to 35%, making them one of the best sources of plant protein. Soybeans and soy products are relatively low in calories, rich in dietary fiber, and contain high levels of unsaturated fatty acids, trace elements, and beneficial phytochemicals such as soy isoflavones, making them an excellent health food (45). Soy products are rich in protein, but also has calcium, magnesium, potassium, soy isoflavones, B vitamins and other nutrients, many benefits to the body, soy protein can make the adult LDL cholesterol reduced by 3 to 4%, this value, although small, but for the prevention of cardio-cerebral vascular disease and the promotion of brain health play a more significant role. Research also confirms (4648) that soy foods have long been recognized as a source of high-quality protein and healthy fats, such as reducing the risk of coronary heart disease, breast cancer and prostate cancer. It has also been confirmed that the consumption of soy-based foods will have a positive effect on the prevention of neurodegeneration in the brain, thereby effectively contributing to the development of executive functions (49). Yet despite the many benefits of soy, the presence of isoflavones has led to concerns that soy may have adverse effects in some people, concerns that are largely based on animal studies, while human studies support the safety and benefits of soy foods, suggesting a beneficial effect on human health. Of course, the results of studies addressing soy products or soy isoflavones on executive function are not consistent. An experimental study in animals confirmed that repeated daily use of isoflavone metabolites did not alter learning and memory processes in middle-aged rats, with no significant effects (50). In addition, a 6-month experimental study confirmed that 80 mg of soy isoflavone supplementation did not improve performance on standardized neuropsychological tests and overall quality of life in Chinese postmenopausal women (51).

4.1 Strengths and limitations

This study has certain strengths and limitations. In terms of strengths, first, this study analyzed for the first time the association between soybean product consumption and executive function in Tibetan children and adolescents at high altitude in China, which provides a reference for the healthy brain development of Tibetan children and adolescents at high altitude. Secondly, the present study adopted the internationally recognized classical executive function paradigm to test inhibitory function (Flanker experimental paradigm), refreshing function (N-back experimental paradigm), and switching function (More-odd Shifting experimental paradigm), and the test results were objective and accurate, which truly reflected the status of the children and adolescents’ executive function. However, this study also has some limitations. First, the sample size of this study is limited and its representativeness is somewhat restricted. Secondly, the covariates included in this study were limited, and future studies should include more relevant covariates, such as covariates for body mass index, economic status, and micronutrients affecting executive function. Third, this study is a cross-sectional study, which can only understand the correlation relationship but not the causal relationship. In the future, a dyadic study should be conducted to understand the association between soybean product consumption and executive function.

5 Conclusion

The results of the present study confirm that there is an association between soybean product consumption and inhibitory control and translational flexibility of brain executive functions in Tibetan children and adolescents at high altitude in China, and that decreased soybean product consumption is associated with an increased risk of developing inhibitory control and translational flexibility dysfunctions. Given the results of this study, it is recommended that effective interventions should be implemented in the future to improve soybean product intake among Tibetan children and adolescents to promote healthy brain function. Meanwhile, this study also provides reference and lessons for future brain health interventions for Tibetan children and adolescents in high altitude areas.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Human Ethics Committee of East China Normal University (No.: HR0782020). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required from the participants or the participants' legal guardians/next of kin in accordance with the national legislation and institutional requirements.

Author contributions

XY: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. FZ: Data curation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. CB: Data curation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. YL: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft. YG: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. PS: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. JH: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (82373595), the Youth Fund Program for Humanities and Social Sciences Research of the Ministry of Education (23YJC890002) and the National Philosophy Social Science Fund of China (21BTY121) project.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all participants and their parents for their cooperation in this research.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Albuquerque, MR, Renno, G, Bruzi, AT, Fortes, LS, and Malloy-Diniz, LF. Association between motor competence and executive functions in children. Appl Neuropsychol Child. (2021) 11:495–503. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2021.1897814

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Alonso-Cabrera, J, Salazar, F, Benavides-Ulloa, J, Parra-Rizo, MA, Zapata-Lamana, R, Diaz-Vargas, C, et al. Students from a public School in the South of Chile with better physical fitness markers have higher performance in executive functions tests-cross-sectional study. Behav Sci (Basel). (2023) 13:191. doi: 10.3390/bs13020191

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Andersen, PN, Skogli, EW, Hovik, KT, Egeland, J, and Oie, M. Associations among symptoms of autism, symptoms of depression and executive functions in children with high-functioning autism: a 2 year follow-up study. J Autism Dev Disord. (2015) 45:2497–507. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2415-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Brocki, KC, and Bohlin, G. Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: a dimensional and developmental study. Dev Neuropsychol. (2004) 26:571–93. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2602_3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Cabral, L, Browne, R, Freire, YA, Schwade, D, Souto, GC, Dantas, M, et al. Cardiorespiratory fitness and performance in multiple domains of executive functions in school-aged adolescents. Front Physiol. (2021) 12:640765. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2021.640765

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Cancela, J, Burgo, H, and Sande, E. Physical fitness and executive functions in adolescents: cross-sectional associations with academic achievement. J Phys Ther Sci. (2019) 31:556–62. doi: 10.1589/jpts.31.556

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Zhang, M, Garnier, H, Qian, G, and Li, S. Effect of 11 weeks of physical exercise on physical fitness and executive functions in children. Children (Basel). (2023) 10:485. doi: 10.3390/children10030485

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Frolli, A, Cerciello, F, Esposito, C, Ciotola, S, De Candia, G, Ricci, MC, et al. Executive functions and foreign language learning. Pediatr Rep. (2022) 14:450–6. doi: 10.3390/pediatric14040053

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Ardila, A. Is intelligence equivalent to executive functions? Psicothema. (2018) 30:159–64. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2017.329

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Barbosa, T, Rodrigues, CC, Mello, CB, Silva, M, and Bueno, O. Executive functions in children with dyslexia. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. (2019) 77:254–9. doi: 10.1590/0004-282x20190033

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Coemans, S, Keulen, S, Savieri, P, Tsapkini, K, Engelborghs, S, Chrispeels, N, et al. Executive functions in primary progressive aphasia: a meta-analysis. Cortex. (2022) 157:304–22. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.10.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Eberhart, J, Paes, TM, Ellefson, MR, and Marcovitch, S. Executive functions and play. Trends Neurosci Educ. (2023) 30:100198. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2023.100198

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Jauregui-Lobera, I. Executive functions in anorexia nervosa. Nutr Hosp. (2014) 29:500–7. doi: 10.3305/nh.2014.29.3.7149

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Keating, L, Kaur, A, Mendieta, M, Gleason, C, Basello, G, Roth, A, et al. Racial discrimination and core executive functions. Stress Health. (2022) 38:615–21. doi: 10.1002/smi.3116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Lucon-Xiccato, T. The contribution of executive functions to sex differences in animal cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2022) 138:104705. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104705

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Mac, KA. Executive functions and aging. Codas. (2016) 28:329–30. doi: 10.1590/2317-1782/20162016056

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Nenadic, I. Narcissistic traits and executive functions. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:707887. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.707887

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Putkinen, V, and Saarikivi, K. Neural correlates of enhanced executive functions: is less more? Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2018) 1423:117–25. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13645

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Riva, D, Cazzaniga, F, Esposito, S, and Bulgheroni, S. Executive functions and cerebellar development in children. Appl Neuropsychol Child. (2013) 2:97–103. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2013.791092

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Contreras-Osorio, F, Guzman-Guzman, IP, Cerda-Vega, E, Chirosa-Rios, L, Ramirez-Campillo, R, and Campos-Jara, C. Anthropometric parameters, physical activity, physical fitness, and executive functions among primary school children. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:3045. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19053045

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Contreras-Osorio, F, Guzmán-Guzmán, IP, Cerda-Vega, E, Chirosa-Ríos, L, Ramírez-Campillo, R, and Campos-Jara, C. Effects of the type of sports practice on the executive functions of schoolchildren. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:3886. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19073886

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Asbaghi, O, Ashtary-Larky, D, Mousa, A, Kelishadi, MR, and Moosavian, SP. The effects of soy products on cardiovascular risk factors in patients with type 2 diabetes: a systematic review and Meta-analysis of clinical trials. Adv Nutr. (2021) 13:455–73. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmab121

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Jayachandran, M, and Xu, B. An insight into the health benefits of fermented soy products. Food Chem. (2019) 271:362–71. doi: 10.1016/j.foodchem.2018.07.158

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Wang, X, Xu, Y, Tan, B, Duan, R, Shan, S, Zeng, L, et al. Development of the Chinese preschooler dietary index: a tool to assess overall diet quality. BMC Public Health. (2022) 22:2428. doi: 10.1186/s12889-022-14672-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Escolano-Pérez, E, and Bestué, M. Academic achievement in Spanish secondary school students: the inter-related role of executive functions, physical activity and gender. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:1816. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18041816

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ishihara, T, Drollette, ES, Ludyga, S, Hillman, CH, and Kamijo, K. Baseline cognitive performance moderates the effects of physical activity on executive functions in children. J Clin Med. (2020) 9:2071. doi: 10.3390/jcm9072071

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wang, Z, Dang, S, Xing, Y, Li, Q, and Yan, H. Dietary patterns and their associations with energy, nutrient intake and socioeconomic factors in rural lactating mothers in Tibet. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. (2017) 26:450–6. doi: 10.6133/apjcn.012016.13

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Yin, X, Zhang, F, Sun, P, Liu, Y, and Guo, Y. The multistage 20-meter shuttle run test reference values for Tibetan children and adolescents in Tibet, China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:12703. doi: 10.3390/ijerph191912703

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Zhang, F, Bi, C, Yin, X, Chen, Q, Liu, Y, Li, Y, et al. Roles of age, sex, and weight status in the muscular fitness of Chinese Tibetan children and adolescents living at altitudes over 3600 m: a cross-sectional study. Am J Hum Biol. (2022) 34:e23624. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.23624

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. C.S.P.F. Group. 2019 China student physical fitness and Health Research report. Beijing: Higher Education Press (2022).

Google Scholar

31. Xia, X, Li, Y, and Chen, S. Association between muscle strength and executive function in Tibetan adolescents at high altitude in China: results from a cross-sectional study at 16–18 years of age. Am J Hum Biol. (2023) 35:e23956. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.23956

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Paruthi, S, Brooks, LJ, D'Ambrosio, C, Hall, WA, Kotagal, S, Lloyd, RM, et al. Consensus statement of the American Academy of sleep medicine on the recommended amount of sleep for healthy children: methodology and discussion. J Clin Sleep Med. (2016) 12:1549–61. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.6288

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. CNSSCH Association. Report on the 2019th National Survey on Students’Constitution and health. Beijing: China College & University Press (2022).

Google Scholar

34. Léger, LA, Mercier, D, Gadoury, C, and Lambert, J. The multistage 20 metre shuttle run test for aerobic fitness. J Sports Sci. (1988) 6:93–101. doi: 10.1080/02640418808729800

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Tomkinson, GR, Lang, JJ, Blanchard, J, Léger, LA, and Tremblay, MS. The 20-m shuttle run: assessment and interpretation of data in relation to youth aerobic fitness and health. Pediatr Exerc Sci. (2019) 31:152–63. doi: 10.1123/pes.2018-0179

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Abernathy, LM, Fountain, MD, Rothstein, SE, David, JM, Yunker, CK, Rakowski, J, et al. Soy Isoflavones promote radioprotection of Normal lung tissue by inhibition of radiation-induced activation of macrophages and neutrophils. J Thorac Oncol. (2015) 10:1703–12. doi: 10.1097/JTO.0000000000000677

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Nakai, S, Fujita, M, and Kamei, Y. Health promotion effects of soy Isoflavones. J Nutr Sci Vitaminol (Tokyo). (2020) 66:502–7. doi: 10.3177/jnsv.66.502

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Cao, ZH, Green-Johnson, JM, Buckley, ND, and Lin, QY. Bioactivity of soy-based fermented foods: a review. Biotechnol Adv. (2019) 37:223–38. doi: 10.1016/j.biotechadv.2018.12.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Celec, P, Ostatnikova, D, Caganova, M, Zuchova, S, Hodosy, J, Putz, Z, et al. Endocrine and cognitive effects of short-time soybean consumption in women. Gynecol Obstet Investig. (2005) 59:62–6. doi: 10.1159/000081895

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Cui, C, Birru, RL, Snitz, BE, Ihara, M, Kakuta, C, Lopresti, BJ, et al. Effects of soy isoflavones on cognitive function: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Nutr Rev. (2020) 78:134–44. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuz050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Fournier, LR, Ryan, BT, Robison, LM, Wiediger, M, Park, JS, Chew, BP, et al. The effects of soy milk and isoflavone supplements on cognitive performance in healthy, postmenopausal women. J Nutr Health Aging. (2007) 11:155–64.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

42. Gleason, CE, Carlsson, CM, Barnet, JH, Meade, SA, Setchell, KD, Atwood, CS, et al. A preliminary study of the safety, feasibility and cognitive efficacy of soy isoflavone supplements in older men and women. Age Ageing. (2009) 38:86–93. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afn227

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Castellana, F, Zupo, R, Bortone, I, Giannelli, G, Donghia, R, Lampignano, L, et al. Traditional old dietary pattern of Castellana Grotte (Apulia) is associated with healthy outcomes. Nutrients. (2020) 12:3097. doi: 10.3390/nu12103097

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Whitton, C, Rebello, SA, Lee, J, Tai, ES, and van Dam, RM. A healthy Asian a posteriori dietary pattern correlates with a priori dietary patterns and is associated with cardiovascular disease risk factors in a multiethnic Asian population. J Nutr. (2018) 148:616–23. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxy016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Zhang, X, Shu, XO, Gao, YT, Yang, G, Li, Q, Li, H, et al. Soy food consumption is associated with lower risk of coronary heart disease in Chinese women. J Nutr. (2003) 133:2874–8. doi: 10.1093/jn/133.9.2874

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Messina, M. Soy and health update: evaluation of the clinical and epidemiologic literature. Nutrients. (2016) 8:754. doi: 10.3390/nu8120754

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Do, PF, Pagnoncelli, M, de Melo, PG, Karp, SG, and Soccol, CR. Fermented soy products and their potential health benefits: a review. Microorganisms. (2022) 10:1606. doi: 10.3390/microorganisms10081606

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Zhang, M, Lin, L, and Liu, H. Acute effect of soy and soy products on serum uric acid concentration among healthy Chinese men. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. (2018) 27:1239–42. doi: 10.6133/apjcn.201811_27(6).0010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Kim, H, Xia, H, Li, L, and Gewin, J. Attenuation of neurodegeneration-relevant modifications of brain proteins by dietary soy. Biofactors. (2000) 12:243–50. doi: 10.1002/biof.5520120137

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Neese, SL, Pisani, SL, Doerge, DR, Helferich, WG, Sepehr, E, Chittiboyina, AG, et al. The effects of dietary treatment with S-equol on learning and memory processes in middle-aged ovariectomized rats. Neurotoxicol Teratol. (2014) 41:80–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2013.12.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Ho, SC, Chan, AS, Ho, YP, So, EK, Sham, A, Zee, B, et al. Effects of soy isoflavone supplementation on cognitive function in Chinese postmenopausal women: a double-blind, randomized, controlled trial. Menopause. (2007) 14:489–99. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e31802c4f4f

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: association, soybean product consumption, executive function, Chinese Tibetan, children and adolescents

Citation: Yin X, Zhang F, Bi C, Liu Y, Guo Y, Sun P and Hong J (2024) Association between soybean product consumption and executive function in Chinese Tibetan children and adolescents. Front. Nutr. 11:1348918. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2024.1348918

Received: 03 December 2023; Accepted: 15 January 2024;
Published: 29 February 2024.

Edited by:

Mona Vintilă, West University of Timișoara, Romania

Reviewed by:

Shooka Mohammadi, University of Malaya, Malaysia
Fatemeh Gholami, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Iran

Copyright © 2024 Yin, Zhang, Bi, Liu, Guo, Sun and Hong. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yuan Liu, yliu0809@163.com

Download